Skip to main content

REVIEW article

Front. Clim. , 11 February 2025

Sec. Climate Mobility

Volume 7 - 2025 | https://doi.org/10.3389/fclim.2025.1514408

This article is part of the Research Topic Managed Retreat in Response to Climate Hazards View all 4 articles

Finding mobility in place attachment research: lessons for managed retreat

Robin Willcocks-Musselman,
Robin Willcocks-Musselman1,2*Julia BairdJulia Baird3Karen FosterKaren Foster4Julia Woodhall-MelnikJulia Woodhall-Melnik5Kate SherrenKate Sherren2
  • 1Faculty of Graduate Studies, Dalhousie University, Halifax, NS, Canada
  • 2Faculty of Science, School for Resource and Environmental Studies, Dalhousie University, Halifax, NS, Canada
  • 3Environmental Sustainability Research Centre, Brock University, St. Catharines, ON, Canada
  • 4Department of Sociology and Social Anthropology, Dalhousie University, Halifax, NS, Canada
  • 5Department of Social Sciences, Faculty of Arts, University of New Brunswick, St. John, NB, Canada

Climate change will affect many global landscapes in the future, requiring millions of people to move away from areas at risk from flooding, erosion, drought and extreme temperatures. The term managed retreat is increasingly used in the Global North to refer to the movement of people and infrastructure away from climate risks. Managed retreat, however, has proven to be one of the most difficult climate adaptation options to undertake because of the complex economic, social-cultural and psychological factors that shape individual and community responses to the relocation process. Among these factors, place attachment is expected to shape the possibilities for managed retreat because relocation disrupts the bonds and identities that individuals and communities have invested in place. Research at the intersection of place attachment and managed retreat is limited, partially because these are complicated constructs, each with confusing terminologies. By viewing the concept of managed retreat as a form of mobility-based climate adaptation, this paper attempts to gain insights from other mobility-related fields. We find that place attachment and mobility research has contributed to the development of a more complex and dynamic view of place attachment: such research has explored the role of place attachment as either constraining or prompting decisions to relocate, and started to explore how the place attachment process responds to disruptions and influences recovery from relocation. Beyond informing managed retreat scholars and practitioners, this research synthesis identifies several areas that need more attention. These needs include more qualitative research to better understand the dualistic role of place attachments in decisions to relocate, more longitudinal research about relocation experiences to fully comprehend the place attachment process during and after relocation, and increased exploration of whether place attachments can help provide stability and continuity during relocation.

1 Introduction

In 2010 at COP16 (Conference of the Parties), the International Panel on Climate Change (IPCC) recognized that one of the effects of climate change would be the increased mobility of people (UNFCCC, 2010). It has been estimated that disasters triggered about 24 million new internal displacements in 2023, most of which were due to storms and floods (IDMC, 2024). These numbers are expected to increase according to researchers working to predict displacement patterns and trends (de Sherbinin et al., 2019; Rossi et al., 2024). For example, under low emissions scenarios 190 million people are predicted to be living below the high tide line by 2100 (Kulp and Strauss, 2019). Accordingly, moving people away from places at high-risk of becoming uninhabitable due to the effects of climate change (high-risk places) is no longer seen as a last resort but as a smart and necessary form of risk management that will reduce residents’ hazard vulnerability and emergency response costs, while promoting restoration of land and ecosystem function (Hino et al., 2017; Siders, 2019). As well, moving people can avoid the high costs of structural protection measures that will increase with more severe and frequent climate change impacts (Doberstein et al., 2020). This “strategic relocation of people, assets and activities to avoid and reduce natural hazard risks and to adapt to impacts of climate change” (Hanna et al., 2019, p. 2) is known, in research and policy, as managed retreat.

Managed retreat is one of the most difficult climate-induced adaptation options to undertake because there is a complex nexus of economic, social-cultural and psychological factors that together shape individual and community responses to the relocation process (Esteban et al., 2020; Hanna et al., 2019; Hino et al., 2017; Steimanis et al., 2021; Thistlethwaite et al., 2018). Even the language of retreat, with its military connotations, can be interpreted as defeatist and intimidating (Koslov, 2016). If managed retreat is to be successfully implemented at the scale that is required due to increasing sea level rise, flooding, drought, and wildfires, then more understanding about the psycho-social dimensions of managed retreat is required (Agyeman et al., 2009; Brunacini, 2023; Fresque-Baxter and Armitage, 2012; Pucker et al., 2023; Raymond, 2013).

Understanding more about the psycho-social aspects of disruptions to place or because of relocation, such as place attachment, is highlighted as a future priority in the emerging research area of climate mobility (Dandy et al., 2019; Seeteram et al., 2023; Simpson et al., 2024). Place attachment merits exploration because relocation will disrupt the bonds and the identities of individuals and communities that are grounded in place (Brunacini, 2023; Devine-Wright, 2014; Relph, 2008; Simpson et al., 2024). Ruptures to people-place bonds create physical and emotional losses that can cause grief (Kothari, 2020) and threaten mental health and well-being (Solecki and Friedman, 2021). Research at the intersection of place attachment and managed retreat is in its infancy, but there are increasing calls to understand how place attachment may inform human responses to retreat (Agyeman et al., 2009; Devine-Wright and Quinn, 2020; Low and Altman, 1992; O’Donnell, 2022; Quinn et al., 2015). There are several potential connections between the two concepts that can be further explored including how place attachments can change over the course of managed retreat; how managed retreat can affect place attachment and vice versa; and how place attachment can impede managed retreat or help in recovery. Increased understanding of these processes has the potential to inform policy and planning initiatives that will ease transitions and limit the negative impact to individual and community well-being (Agyeman et al., 2009; Binder et al., 2019; Cox and Perry, 2011; Fresque-Baxter and Armitage, 2012; Jamali and Nejat, 2016).

The contribution of this paper is to articulate and synthesize the research at the intersection of place attachment and managed retreat. Since research that connects place attachment and managed retreat is limited, in order to obtain useful insights, we take a broader view of managed retreat as a climate-induced mobility, as it requires the movement of people and infrastructure. This has motivated us to conduct a targeted review that incorporates neighboring scholarly fields. For example, the fields of personal mobility, migration and displacement, and forced relocation and resettlement have all incorporated place attachment research and offer insight into the dynamism of place attachments, the role of place attachment in the decision to relocate, and the role of place attachments in recovery. Improved understanding of place attachment and how it may inform human responses to retreat in general will be helpful at both the theoretical and practical levels (Agyeman et al., 2009; Devine-Wright and Quinn, 2020; Low and Altman, 1992; Quinn et al., 2015).

2 Background

The literatures of managed retreat and place attachment show us that both concepts are incompletely conceptualized. Both fields suffer from multiple constructs that are often used interchangeably, and from a wide variety of related research fields which produce an abundant amount of literature about various philosophies, approaches and methods. These issues have been identified in the literature by many researchers and can result in difficulty compiling an accurate body of knowledge that reflects the current state of our understanding (Bukvic, 2015; Nelson et al., 2020).

2.1 Managed retreat

Many terms are used interchangeably in the literature to describe the movement of individual people and communities (Bukvic, 2015). Terms vary in whether they describe movements that are fast or slow, forced or voluntary, planned or unplanned, state-led (managed) or self-governed. For example, the term evacuation describes an abrupt and temporary movement, whereas abandonment may take more time to reconcile but is more permanent. Migration, abandonment, and displacement are seen as primarily un-planned and self-driven, but only displacement is seen as being forced (Burkett et al., 2017; Paul et al., 2024). Likewise, relocation, retreat, and resettlement also tend to refer to forced relocation, but also imply state-led, planned, and the more permanent movement of people (Ajibade et al., 2022; Burkett et al., 2017; Ferris, 2015; Imura and Shaw, 2009; Marter-Kenyon, 2020). Relocation and retreat are also more likely to include the relocation of assets and activities along with people.

Within the climate-induced adaptation literature there are also multiple terms used specifically to describe climate-induced, or climate-related relocation (CRR). For example, in the Global South, the term planned resettlement is most often used, in the United Kingdom and Europe the term managed realignment is often used, and in the United States, Canada and Europe, the terms planned, strategic or managed retreat are used (Doberstein et al., 2020; Hanna et al., 2019; Rupp-Armstrong and Nicholls, 2007). This does not mean you cannot also find references to planned or forced relocation, or resettlement in the CRR literature. Furthermore, newer terms like climigration (climate + migration) are entering the lexicon (Ajibade et al., 2020). Unfortunately, many of these terms have been used interchangeably over the years which is problematic for gathering an accurate state of knowledge in the field. For example, although much of CRR research occurs in the Global South (Ajibade et al., 2022; Marter-Kenyon, 2020), searching for the terms planned or managed retreat will deliver more cases from the Global North (Marter-Kenyon, 2020). Many authors call for more standardized terminology in order to provide consistency in the literature, especially as interest in dialog about climate-related relocation continues to increase (Bukvic, 2015; Paul et al., 2024). New terms like transformative adaptation have been proposed to provide one, all-encompassing term, that invokes fewer negative connotations, and focuses on the positive aspects of relocation (Dundon and Abkowitz, 2021; O’Donnell, 2022). The debate continues about which term to use and when. In this paper we focus on the term managed retreat to represent the forced relocation of individuals, communities and infrastructure due to climate risks, because the term has been widely adopted in academia and policy spheres at this time. This decision however, does affect our search results. Given the linguistic bias discussed above, the focus of this paper is on the movement of people and assets in the Global North.

The construct of managed retreat builds on a long history of both involuntary and voluntary human movement from forced residential relocation as a result of urban expansion and renewal, large infrastructure projects, establishment of protected areas, unsustainable land use patterns that expose people to risks, and man-made and natural disasters (Doberstein and Stager, 2013; Hanna et al., 2019; Marter-Kenyon, 2020; Piggott-McKellar et al., 2020; Wilmsen and Webber, 2015). Some of the earliest managed retreat cases were due to riverine and coastal flooding in the United States and Europe (Koslov, 2016; Pinter, 2021; Pinter and Rees, 2021; Siders, 2019; Tubridy et al., 2021). More recently however, managed retreat has differentiated itself from other types of forced relocation as primarily being driven by climate change hazards. There continues to be managed retreat literature as a result of riverine and coastal flooding (Abel et al., 2011; Dundon et al., 2023; Mayr et al., 2020; Okada et al., 2014; Rupp-Armstrong and Nicholls, 2007), but now, there are also documented responses to extreme weather-related events and sea level rise. For example, there has been much written about relocation after Hurricanes Katrina and Sandy in the United States (Binder et al., 2019; Braamskamp and Penning-Rowsell, 2018; Koslov, 2016; Pinter et al., 2019), and about retreat from low-lying coastal communities (Marter-Kenyon, 2020; Pinter, 2021; Simms et al., 2021). Some of the most well-known cases of managed retreat include the relocation of the island Pacific communities of Kiribati and Fiji, and Indigenous coastal communities in Alaska and the southern United States (Pinter, 2021). Climate change caused wildfires, extreme heat, and permafrost melting are emerging reasons where managed retreat may also be considered necessary (Dundon et al., 2023; Siders, 2019).

Managed retreat may need to occur quickly, such as after a disaster, or may happen more slowly, such as in the case of gradual sea level rise. It is typically a planned, coordinated, and mostly an involuntary process that is overseen by the state through regulations and financial incentives. Depending on the circumstances surrounding the managed retreat, a variety of positive and negative consequences can occur (Simpson et al., 2024). In the best-case scenarios, managed retreat can protect lives, reduce costs related to staying in place, reduce stresses and uncertainty associated with living in high-risk areas, free up land for ecosystems (Ajibade and Siders, 2021; Hanna et al., 2019; Koslov, 2016; Siders, 2019), change historical marginalization (Simpson et al., 2024), and potentially present other life opportunities (Jamali and Nejat, 2016; Siders, 2019). In the worst cases, managed retreat can disconnect people from their communities, culture and livelihoods, exacerbate social inequalities and increase socioeconomic vulnerabilities (Ajibade and Siders, 2021; Koslov, 2016; Simpson et al., 2024), negatively impact mental health and well-being, and disrupt place attachments (Ajibade and Siders, 2021). These positive and negative outcomes may occur simultaneously and fluctuate over time (Ajibade and Siders, 2021).

Despite the increased urgency of needing to move people away from high-risk areas there is still resistance to the use of managed retreat, and considerable difficulties with its implementation (Lawrence et al., 2020; Mallette et al., 2021). Several recent reviews of managed retreat literature address questions about how to undertake managed retreat in ways that deal with the complexities arising from implementation costs, compensation of relocatees, maladaptations from urban and rural planning and the insurance industry, and jurisdictional issues under the law (Ajibade et al., 2022; Dundon and Abkowitz, 2021; Kousky, 2014; Marter-Kenyon, 2020; O’Donnell, 2022; Pinter, 2021; Siders, 2019). Furthermore, finding an equitable or just approach to managed retreat is of increasing interest since it is often the economically, politically, and socially disenfranchised people who are the most vulnerable, and will suffer the most as a result of retreat (Ajibade and Siders, 2021; Loughran and Elliott, 2022; O’Donnell, 2022; Pinter, 2021; Simms et al., 2021; Thaler, 2021). This includes the loss of place.

2.2 Place and place attachment

Interest in place was led by the disciplines of human geography and environmental and social psychology (Williams and Miller, 2020), but it is now studied in almost all aspects of social science and the humanities (Devine-Wright, 2013b; Nelson et al., 2020; Patterson and Williams, 2005). This has resulted in the production of a large and growing body of literature, and a reputation that the field is complex (Relph, 2008). Several reviews—including those by Duggan et al. (2023), Edensor et al. (2020), Erfani (2022), Manzo (2003), Nelson et al. (2020), Patterson and Williams (2005), and Raymond et al. (2021)—capture many of the theories, methods and applications in place research. Here we only give an overview.

The concept of place evolved through several key societal movements that bred a diversity of philosophies and approaches, many of which continue to be practiced concurrently (Cresswell, 2009; Morgan, 2010; Williams, 2014a; Williams and Miller, 2020). The humanism movement of the 1970s was one of the most impactful periods in place research. During this time, humanists pushed back on modernistic ideas that neglected human-environment relations, in favor of prioritizing human experiences and meaning (Lewicka, 2011a; Raymond et al., 2021; Williams, 2014a; Williams and Miller, 2020). Later, critical constructivists focussed on how place was socially constructed through narratives, and shaped by politics, power, and social and cultural processes (Manzo and Pinto De Carvalho, 2020; Williams, 2014a; Williams and Miller, 2020). More recently, globalization, migration, and the increased mobility of people in general has resulted in what social science researchers Sheller and Urry (2006) coined the mobilities turn. The mobilities turn has challenged the view that mobility is a threat to place (Di Masso et al., 2019; Lewicka, 2020). Through these historical movements, place has evolved from being considered solely a geographic location, to becoming a place comprised of physical attributes, experiences and meanings (Relph, 2008), to more recently being thought of as complex and dynamic networks of things and connections (Edensor et al., 2020; Raymond et al., 2021; Williams and Miller, 2020). The fact that place research is not grounded in a single research tradition is often blamed for the lack of progress in theoretical coherence and maturity in place research (Herandez et al., 2020; Nelson et al., 2020; Patterson and Williams, 2005; Stedman, 2003).

Many terms have been used in the literature to capture the nature and meaning of connection to place (Masterson et al., 2017; Patterson and Williams, 2005) (Table 1). Sense of place, place attachment, place identity, and place dependence are the main terms used (Manzo, 2003; Stedman, 2003). There are however, many other terms and phrases representing this connection to place found in the literature including topophilia (Tuan, 1974), community sentiment and identity (Low and Altman, 1992), the ancient Roman term genius loci (Patterson and Williams, 2005), sense of belonging, (Nelson et al., 2020), community attachment (Hernandez-Santin et al., 2020), and perception of place (Duggan et al., 2023). There is widespread criticism in the literature about the lack of conceptual clarity about the concept of place attachment. This could be due to the use of so many different terms and the lack of consensus about how they relate to each other (Devine-Wright, 2020; Manzo et al., 2023). Like with managed retreat, this makes it difficult to compare or accumulate research findings because disciplines frequently use different definitions for the same construct (Giuliani and Feldman, 1993; Masterson et al., 2017; Trentelman, 2009), or use terms interchangeably, despite subtle differences in meaning (Nelson et al., 2020; Williams and Miller, 2020).

Table 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Frequently used place terms and definitions.

For the purposes of this paper we adopt the term place attachment because it is the most frequently used term in the literature to represent connection to place (Hernandez-Santin et al., 2020; Nelson et al., 2020; Nicolosi and Corbett, 2018; Trentelman, 2009). Place attachment at its most basic is a bond between humans and their environment (Low and Altman, 1992). Common characteristics found in various definitions include that bonds are affective (emotional), evaluative (positive or negative), and between individuals and or groups and their environment (Lewicka, 2020; Masterson et al., 2017; Scannell and Gifford, 2010).

3 Methods

This review takes a narrative approach, in part due to the challenges of language described above. An initial search for research at the intersection of place attachment and managed retreat using those terms yielded very few results, so conducting a systematic review did not seem beneficial. Knowing there was relevant literature about place attachment and relocation available, we proceeded to broaden the search and consider managed retreat as a mobility-based concept. This allowed us to expand our search and explore other fields related to the movement of humans and assets in relation to place. We conducted our search on the Scopus database using the terms discussed in Section 2.1 to describe the movement of people and assets, and paired them with the main terms used in the literature for connection to place (as discussed in Section 2.2). Over 400 resources were captured in the search which included book chapters, but not books or conference papers. We also found many sources through the reference lists of other papers.

Screening focused on including the literature on relocation that shared similarities to managed retreat. Therefore, of particular interest to our review was literature about place attachment and climate-related relocation, and relocation that was permanent, planned or forced, and state-led. As a result, this excluded some results that focused solely upon human movement, or that was voluntary and self-motivated. The latter results correlated primarily with human movement literature on the Global South, so the reviewed literature ended up being heavily focused on the Global North. However, we did not exclude research about the Global South, nor did we exclude non-climate related relocation, particularly if it referred to how place attachment responded to relocation or place change. The remainder of the paper will review the literature found in these targeted domains.

4 Research fields at the intersection of place attachment and mobility

By taking a broader view of managed retreat as a climate-induced mobility, our search found several neighbouring mobility-based fields that have incorporated place attachment research, including migration and displacement, personal mobility, and forced relocation and resettlement. A brief discussion follows of these fields and how they may be similar or different than managed retreat.

4.1 Migration and displacement

Millions of people migrate within, or between countries each year. Migration can be driven by individual choices, such as seeking better opportunities, or in less voluntary ways, because situations are risky or untenable due to war, politics, or natural hazards (De Sherbinin et al., 2011; Hanna et al., 2019; Paul et al., 2024). Migration is typically unplanned, uncoordinated, and is the result of complex relationships between perceptions, needs, desires, real or perceived risks and benefits, and financial or legal ability to move (Barcus and Brunn, 2010; Koslov, 2016). Involuntary migration is often referred to as displacement and typically affects more vulnerable populations who frequently live in poorer, high risk areas (Paul et al., 2024). Conversely, lack of financial or social resources can lead to immobility, causing some people to be trapped in place (Koslov, 2016; Upadhyay et al., 2024).

Interest in migration caused by climate change is increasing so rapidly it has made its way into popular discourse with recently published non-academic books about the displacement of people away from areas of climate risk (Bittle, 2024; Vince, 2022). The current literature about climate change and migration, or climigration as it is often referred to, mainly focuses on determining the extra impact that climate change contributes to existing socio-economic drivers of migration, how and when climate change will affect which places, and how many people will migrate in the future (Upadhyay et al., 2024). However, the literature also illustrates the similarities between migration and managed retreat, such as when the drivers are climate-driven (Pinter, 2021).

Although there are many opportunities to learn from the field of migration, there are many distinctions between it and managed retreat. Ajibade and Siders (2021) point out that migration is primarily about human mobility and focuses on people in the Global South, whereas managed retreat is a withdrawal of people and the resources they value (e.g., homes, infrastructure, ecosystems, and other assets) (Ajibade and Siders, 2021, p. 102187). Ajibade et al. (2020) also outlines several specific distinctions between managed retreat and climigration, such as their causal mechanisms. Further, according to Ajibade et al. (2020) managed retreat is initiated directly from a climate hazard (i.e., sea level rise), whereas climigration is triggered by indirect climatic effects. For example, extreme heat could lead to drought which causes crop failure and food insecurity. People may thus be motivated to relocate to find areas that have more food resources. The two constructs also differ in their legal protections, rights and funding structures, and discursive effects (Ajibade et al., 2020; Hanna et al., 2019).

4.2 Personal mobility

Research in residential mobility has driven a significant proportion of place attachment research. Moving residences is typically voluntary and peaceful, driven by factors that pull people toward a new place, including benefits such as a better job or improved living conditions. Alternatively, people can be pushed from a place because their needs do not match what a current place offers (Dang and Weiss, 2021), their socioeconomic statues changes, or physical changes may make a place undesirable to live in (Kothari, 2020). In these situations, moving can feel positive, like a search for new opportunities or a liberation from constraints, or it can feel oppressive and out of one’s control (Madanipour, 2020; Williams and Miller, 2024). For example, tenants’ rights research identifies the perils of forced relocation of low to moderate income renters from no-fault evictions such as renovictions (eviction to allow for renovations) and demovictions (eviction to demolish aged housing stock), which are made in the name of housing stock and neighborhood renewal (Ramiller, 2022).

In the Global North personal mobility has become a normal part of people’s lifestyles rather than a one-off experience due to an increase in pleasure travel, commuting for work, second home ownership, and new technologies allowing for virtual travel or telecommuting (Cresswell, 2006; Gustafson, 2014; Lewicka, 2020). There is a tension identified in the literature among mobility, sedentarism, and privilege. This tension is heavily dependent on social position. Currently those who are able to opt into a mobile lifestyle are often considered more privileged than those who may be restricted in their movements, or are forced to move (Cresswell, 2009; Gustafson, 2001). However, it was not long ago that sedentarism was considered a privilege in place attachment research, and those who were able to have a secure and stable place to call home possessed an advantage (Di Masso et al., 2019). These debates have contributed to more interest in relocation and social justice issues which is only expected to increase as climate change intensifies (Ajibade et al., 2022; O’Donnell, 2022; Seeteram et al., 2023).

4.3 Forced relocation and resettlement

Approximately 15 million people are forced to relocate each year as a result of development induced/forced displacement and resettlement (DIDR or DFDR) (Doberstein and Stager, 2013; Piggott-McKellar et al., 2020; Wilmsen and Webber, 2015). This includes those relocated due to urban expansion, gentrification or redevelopment; and large infrastructure development like dams, hydro lines, mines, airports, national parks, and more recently green infrastructure projects such as windfarms (De Sherbinin et al., 2011; Hay, 1998; Vanclay, 2017; Wilmsen and Webber, 2015). Several authors have outlined similarities between DIDIR/DFDR and managed retreat cases, including that they are a result of human actions, can have long lead times (as is the case with sea level rise), and often impact the least powerful (Wilmsen and Webber, 2015). However, a major difference between the two fields, is that DIDR/DFDR projects are typically economically motivated and driven from the top-down (Hsu et al., 2019; Piggott-McKellar et al., 2020).

Natural disasters also force many people to relocate each year and motivates a substantial amount of research on place attachment and mobility. There is clear overlap between the fields of disaster risk and disaster recovery (DRDR) and managed retreat, especially when the drivers are climate-induced storms, floods, fires, and drought. This can sometimes make it difficult to distinguish the fields from each other in the literature. For example, some articles discuss DRDR and climate change adaptation (managed retreat is an adaptation tool) together and treat them the as one and the same (Doberstein et al., 2020). Although there are many similarities between the literature of place attachment and DRDR, and place attachment and managed retreat, there are also points of distinction. For example, the DRDR literature also includes temporary displacements (Iuchi, 2014), people eventually returning to place (Chamlee-Wright and Storr, 2009; Imura and Shaw, 2009; Paul et al., 2024), or situations in which people are not required to move at all, such as after earthquakes and volcanoes (Hsu et al., 2019; Piggott-McKellar et al., 2020; Wilmsen and Webber, 2015).

Forced relocation from DIDR/DFDR or DRDR may be structurally different and hold different implications for people than those experienced under managed retreat but they still provide useful insights into the social dynamics of population displacement (De Sherbinin et al., 2011; Pinter, 2021). For example, it is generally acknowledged in the DRR and DIDR literature that in the past, relocations have often had poor outcomes. Beyond the economic costs, these include exacerbation of historical inequities (Ajibade et al., 2022); the social, psychological, and cultural impact experienced by people when breaking of ties with cultures and communities (Ajibade et al., 2022; Hanna et al., 2019; Hino et al., 2017); top-down approaches that fail to communicate with and involve communities in meaningful ways (Hsu et al., 2019; Tadgell et al., 2018; Wilmsen and Webber, 2015; Yi and Yang, 2014); lack of long-term perspective (Imura and Shaw, 2009); adverse effects of power dynamics and government bureaucracy (Wilmsen and Webber, 2015); unsuitability of new locations; and lack of consideration of impacts on host communities (Perry and Lindell, 1997; Vanclay, 2017; Wilmsen and Webber, 2015). In order to address some of these lessons learned, organizations like the United Nations have produced resettlement policies and performance standards that focus on the protection of human rights of relocatees in developing nations (OECD-DAC, 1992; UN OCHA, 2004; UNHCR, 2015). Other researchers have also provided lessons learned from past relocations (Tubridy et al., 2021; Wilmsen and Webber, 2015), establishing guidelines for future relocations, including for less developed nations (Tadgell et al., 2018) and informal settlements (Doberstein and Stager, 2013).

5 Lessons from the literature

Important lessons about place attachment can be drawn from the various fields that encompass human movements discussed above. Since each mobility field differs in its similarities with managed retreat, some mobility fields contributed more resources to this review than others. The wide range of articles found illustrate how managed retreat affects place attachment and how it is influenced by place attachment. The key lessons are outlined below.

5.1 People have complex and changing attachments to places

5.1.1 People can have attachments to multiple different places

The mobilities turn in particular, has challenged the dominant paradigm of place attachment as sedentary, centred around a primary residence (home), and strengthened by longevity in place (Di Masso et al., 2019; Lewicka, 2020). It has caused place and place attachment researchers to grapple with the once-accepted idea that mobility is a threat (Devine-Wright, 2020; Lewicka, 2020; Williams and Miller, 2020) and to consider whether, instead of eroding place attachments, mobility can actually increase the number and types of attachments people hold. Research on multiple place attachments has centered around attachments to places other than a primary residence, including summer or second homes, wilderness and outdoor recreation areas (Lewicka, 2011a), meeting areas and public places, and sacred structures (Manzo, 2003). This research confirms what Brown and Perkins (1992) proposed long ago, that people can experience multiple attachments with a plurality of place meanings (Gustafson, 2014; Manzo et al., 2023). Devine-Wright and Quinn (2020, p. 226) suggest that with mobility becoming a lifestyle, more than ever people will experience a “mosaic of places” over their lifetime.

Within place attachment research, the life course approach explores how people attach to multiple places as they move through various stages of their lives (Di Masso et al., 2019). The life course paradigm has been part of social psychology research since the 1960s (Elder, 1994). Several studies explore people’s relationships to a multitude of places from childhood to retirement, capturing changes to place attachment over this span of time (Bailey et al., 2021; Di Masso et al., 2019; Giuliani and Feldman, 1993; Manzo, 2003). Bailey et al.’s (2016) research about a proposed power line addition in Bristol, England, builds on the life course approach. These researchers proposed that currently held residential place attachments are influenced by people’s life course trajectories. They proposed five distinct life course trajectories based on mobility patterns associated with different types of attachment, and found those who had a life trajectory characterized by living in one location for a long time were more rooted in place. For these people, power lines were deemed acceptable since they were always part of the landscape. The other extreme described people with life course trajectories that were more mobile. These people often moved to the area in search of places similar to their previous locations. They placed value on landscape aesthetics and viewed power lines as negative, ultimately opposing the power line project. This finding is consistent with research on place-based opposition by new residents. Life course research such as this highlights that people navigating place changes in their lives are often looking for continuity in their attachments and identity (Di Masso et al., 2019).

5.1.2 People can have many different types of attachments to a place

Along with research about having attachment to multiple different places, there is research about the idea people can have multiple types of place attachments to a place (Adams-Hutcheson, 2015). The concept of place attachment as singular has changed as researchers have sought language to capture the more nuanced linkages people have with places, and the sense of mobility within and across types of attachment (Bissell, 2020; Lewicka, 2011b; Devine-Wright, 2020b; Van Manen, 1990).

Gustafson (2001) explained that people could have static or dynamic place attachments. They considered traditional and sedentary place attachments in terms of roots, and the more progressive, mobility influenced perspective of attachments as routes. They also emphasized rather than being mutually exclusive, there was room for both static and dynamic place attachments to co-exist. For example, one may still enjoy the stability of having a strong, fixed connection to a primary residence, while mobility in the form of travel can provide an opportunity for connections to other places (Gustafson, 2014; Raymond et al., 2021). In a similar fashion, Di Masso et al. (2019) proposed a fixity and flow framework comprised of six different categories of place attachment that range in dynamism from fixed (representing a place attachment that is centered and anchored in one place), through various ratios of fixity-flow, to flow (which has an absence of anchors and could represent virtual or imaginative travel). Like the life course approach, this framework helps to show that people can have place attachments with different levels of mobility that can change as one navigates across the life course.

Hummon’s (1992) typology was one of the first to capture greater complexity in place attachment bonds by considering their emotional valence (positive or negative), their intensity (strong or weak), and their agency (active or passive). Lewicka (2011b) further developed Hummon’s five-fold typology by renaming the two types of positive rootedness as traditional and active attachments (Bailey et al., 2016; Devine-Wright, 2020; Hummon, 1992; Lewicka, 2020, 2011b). They also included Hummon’s three negative types of weak/non attachments, including alienation (dislike of place and desire to leave), place relativity (ambivalence or conditionally accepting attachment), and placelessness (place indifference or absence of emotional association with place). Of the two rooted types of positive place attachment, traditional attachments are thought to evolve through passive interaction with place (Bailey et al., 2016; Devine-Wright, 2020) by way of long-term rootedness such as familial or cultural ties (Pucker et al., 2023). Active attachments, in contrast, are more consciously made, often when people deliberately seek out a place and relocate there (Lewicka, 2020; Pucker et al., 2023). Overall, this refined typology by Lewicka has become the prevalent form used in the literature (Pucker et al., 2023).

Low (1992) furthered the idea of being able to have different types of place attachment at the same time by stating any of their proposed six types of place attachments could occur simultaneously. The different types of place attachment they proposed include genealogical, narrative, loss and destruction, economic, celebratory cultural events, and cosmological. Hay (1998) elaborated Low’s genealogical type to show these historical linkages have multiple layers, including personal, familial, ancestral, and cultural (Cross, 2015). Cross (2015) also proposes several place attachment types but refers to them more as processes that shape residents’ attachments. They state these processes are dynamic, occurring simultaneously, but each with a unique relationship to time (Cross, 2015). For example, historical processes tend to deepen and expand attachment over time, while the narrative process might either deepen or weaken attachments … commodifying process generally fades over time while the spiritual process is notably static over time (Cross, 2015, p. 515). They also note that these different types, or processes, of attachment can extend for long periods even after a person leaves a place (Cross, 2015).

Researchers have engaged in research that correlates various types of place attachment with different variables such as personality type, education, age, and social relations (Lewicka, 2020). There have also been studies looking for links between an individual’s predominant type of attachment and their response to a disruption (Lewicka, 2020). For example, people with more active attachments may be able to adjust more quickly to a disruption than those whose place attachments are based on long-held daily routines (traditional attachments) (Lewicka, 2020). Other studies have compared types of place attachments to environmental threat response (Sullivan and Young, 2020), coping styles related to climate change (Parreira and Mouro, 2023), responses to community energy projects (Devine-Wright, 2013c; Van Veelen and Haggett, 2017), and to flood preparedness behavior (Mishra et al., 2010). These types of studies help provide more insight into how people may respond to different disruptions based on the types of attachments they have.

5.1.3 People can have attachments to places they visit for short periods of time or visit virtually

Research in residential mobility has explored place attachments and short-term and non-migratory relocation due to work travel and tourism, and in virtual settings (Bailey et al., 2021; Bissell, 2020). Innovations in travel infrastructure have increased mobility for work as well as for tourism (Di Masso et al., 2019; Gustafson, 2001). Employment-related mobility includes daily commuting, travel for work such as meetings and conferences, or relocation for longer periods of time (i.e., seasonal employment and fly-in/fly-out work; Bissell, 2020). Research in tourism explores the idea of having attachments to distant places in which we may not spend much time, especially recreation sites (Devine-Wright, 2020; Di Masso et al., 2019).

Research in such fields has brought attention to the differences between those who feel they belong in a place (i.e., insiders, such as residents who work and live in a place), and those who do not belong (i.e., outsiders, such as tourists and visitors to a place) (Gustafson, 2001; Lewicka, 2011a; Relph, 1976). For example, Gurney et al. (2021) and Marshall et al. (2019) found that after a coral bleaching event at the Great Barrier Reef, both residents and tourists reported a form of climate grief (an expected or real sense of loss due to degradation to places and ecosystems; Allen, 2020; Marshall et al., 2019) and solastalgia (“the inability to derive solace from the present state of one’s environment”; Philippenko et al., 2021, p. 21). They found residents (insiders) who had a more meaningful relationship with the reef had higher rates of solastalgia and climate grief than those who were more interested in the aesthetic value of the reef (outsiders). Adams-Hutcheson (2015) found, among people relocated after an earthquake in New Zealand, that feelings of being an insider or an outsider were not mutually exclusive. Further, they found that strong place attachments could interact with the insider-outsider dynamic such that those who relocated felt like outsiders compared to those who were allowed to stay in place. This may undermine the sense of belonging and ability to make new attachments among the relocated.

Advancements in communication technologies will also require new ways of exploring how connections between people and places are evolving as more people have a ‘virtually’ mobile lifestyle without having to physically relocate (Barcus and Brunn, 2010). Within place attachment research scholars have proposed that virtual mobility may overcome geographical distance, allowing one to be attached to places visited by way of visual images, or virtually online (Di Masso et al., 2019; Gustafson, 2014). In essence, allowing us to be in two places at once (Di Masso et al., 2019). The idea of being transported elsewhere through technology could help recreate, maintain, or change place attachments to places left behind as well as places people have yet to visit (Di Masso et al., 2019).

An example of the potential of technology in this context is the concept ‘place elasticity,’ a form of attachment proposed by Barcus and Brunn (2010) that recognizes how we can stretch our place boundaries. Such stretching is often driven by advances in transportation and technology, allowing for increasingly virtual relationships and connections with places where we used to live. Place elasticity allows for long-term engagement and connections to occur despite not living in, or even visiting, a place. This allows people to take advantage of economic or social opportunities away from the places which they physically live. Di Masso et al. (2019) emphasizes that these types of virtual attachment do not replace conventional place attachments. The research in this field challenges the long-held belief that development of place attachments is a slow process that depends on length of time in a place. Arguing that meaningful place attachments can also be created more quickly, Devine-Wright (2020) suggests follow-on questions including whether more mobile people have more place attachments, and how tangible place must be to foster attachment.

5.2 Place attachments are dynamic

5.2.1 Place attachment is a dynamic process

Place attachment research has traditionally focused more on the existence, strength and shape of attachments rather than the dynamic processes of attaching, detaching, and reattaching to place over time (Cross, 2015; Devine-Wright and Quinn, 2020; Fried, 2000; Grocke et al., 2022; Gustafson, 2014; Scannell and Gifford, 2010). Massey (1991) and Brown and Perkins (1992) were early critics of place being seen as only singular and static, which was a popular stance of humanistic geographers at the time (Dickinson, 2019). As places increasingly began to be considered more complex and dynamic, place attachments evolved along parallel lines (Dandy et al., 2019; Giuliani and Feldman, 1993; Gustafson, 2014; Lewicka, 2020). Place attachments are now thought to be dynamic, continually being (re)constructed, adapted, reshaped, and ebbing and flowing over time (Kim, 2021; Williams and Miller, 2020).

The literature sometimes refers to the process of how people-place bonds form as building place (Million, 1992) or place-making (Kothari, 2020; Williams, 2014b). Phenomenologists, such as Seamon (2014), have long argued that dynamic processes occur simultaneously to make place (Cross, 2015). For example, Seamon proposes that place-making develops through the everyday habitual body movements (i.e., walking to work, picking kids up from school etc.), that make up what he refers to as a body ballet of compounding daily, weekly, and monthly routines (Cresswell, 2009; Cross, 2015; Lewicka, 2011a; Seamon, 2014; Williams and Miller, 2020). Grocke et al. (2022) reiterates the idea that dynamic processes contribute to the place attachment process through “the dynamism of decision-making that constantly evaluates changes to social-environmental settings and the reiteration of the bonding process” (p. 301). Here, it is important to note, that planners, architects, and landscape architects also use the term ‘place-making’ but in a different context; deliberately designing places to improve community well-being and livability (Marshall and Bishop, 2015). Such work is outside the scope of this review.

Conceptual frameworks have been proposed by several authors in attempts to characterize the place attachment process after a disruption to a place or during a relocation (Brown and Perkins, 1992; Devine-Wright and Quinn, 2020; Greene et al., 2011; Inalhan and Finch, 2004; Million, 1992; Prewitt Diaz and Dayal, 2008). Although the frameworks vary, they all have a loose structure of a period before the disruption happens where there may or may not be preparation for change, followed by transition and recovery stages. The process is seen as a continuum but it is not linear in its progression (Kothari, 2020; Whittle et al., 2014). Additionally, the stages do not have clear edges and they may overlap with each other (Whittle et al., 2014). Little research has been done to understand mechanisms within the process (Lewicka, 2020; Manzo and Devine-Wright, 2020b) but it is thought to take considerable work because one must successfully let go of existing attachments (detach), re-establish old attachments, or create new ones in a new location (Brown and Perkins, 1992; Devine-Wright and Quinn, 2020; Dickinson, 2019; Perez Murcia, 2020; Zheng et al., 2019).

A few researchers have explored what happens in the transition stage of the place attachment process, finding it a dynamic time that is disorienting to people (Cox and Perry, 2011; Madanipour, 2020; Prayag et al., 2021; Silver and Grek-Martin, 2015). People feel unmoored during this period because disruption causes the loss of physical and psychological markers and forces changes to people’s routines, both of which in turn affect their sense of continuity and stability (Cox and Perry, 2011; Madanipour, 2020; Silver and Grek-Martin, 2015). Eventually people start to reorientate themselves and find their bearings by reconstructing their identity as they navigate through the psychological, social, and emotional responses to change they have experienced (Binder et al., 2019; Cox and Perry, 2011). Cox and Perry (2011) found place attachment to be important to reorientation because it helped to recreate community and self-identity through repeated cycles of disorientation and reorientation. Similarly, Harms (2015) referred to circuits of displacement and emplacement with each circuit having a role in the cumulative process of place making. These studies contribute to the understanding that place attachment bonds are in a process of continuous instability and renegotiation during a disruption (Madanipour, 2020).

The recovery stage of the place attachment process after a disruption is also not well understood, especially when relocation is required (Adams-Hutcheson, 2015; Dickinson, 2019; Rumbach et al., 2016; Whittle et al., 2014; Zheng et al., 2019). The term emplacement has been used to describe the processes of building or rebuilding connections after disruption or relocation (Kothari, 2020; Perez Murcia, 2020). Similar to place-making, this can be achieved by developing new habits and rituals that will result in new (re)attachments (Prayag et al., 2021) and by “renegotiating conflicts and reconciling losses and trauma” (Zheng et al., 2019, p. 7). Million (1992) also indicates the recovery period is a time when reflective reconciliation begins and displacement recedes to the background. They add that there may be moments that “jolt us back to our loss” (p. 199) which reminds us that the recovery process is recursive and not linear (Million, 1992). As for when this recovery process might be completed, Million (1992) muses that rebuilding place may be done after “the emergence of both the past and the future alongside the present during the course of daily involvements with daily things” (p. 207). Winstanley et al. (2015) indicates that despite irreversible change having happened, recovery is achieved when one “no longer has to renegotiate how to carry out everyday life activities, while acknowledging degrees of adaptation and change in the physical environment, in social interaction and in individual psychologies and behaviours” (p. 128).

5.2.2 Place attachments change during place change/relocation

The research is quiet about the specifics of how and when detachments and reattachments occur but there are several studies illustrating that place attachments do change when there is a disruption to place or a relocation from place. Chow and Healey (2008) are frequently referred to as one of the first to do a longitudinal study showing changing place attachments. Their research captured the transitional process first-year university students experience when they move away from home to a new social and cultural environment. Specifically, they studied students over a five-month period and found that place meanings were constantly being evaluated and re-defined after relocation. They acknowledge this may be influenced by the students’ age and stage of life, both of which present other complexities. Kim (2021) also found people’s attachments to place changed during neighborhood change caused by increasing short term rentals and overcrowding from tourism. In particular, they found that residents experienced fluidity in their attachment to place as they “continuously construct, adapt and reshape their connections to place” (Kim, 2021, p. 129). Further, they showed that place attachments could amplify or attenuate during neighborhood change depending on a variety of factors including proximity to development or how changes impact residents’ quality of life. Cheng and Chou (2015) also found that bonds grew or weakened depending on initial levels of attachment. Considering an environmental corollary to neighborhood change, Gurney et al. (2021, p. 27) found “emotional and intangible elements of sense of place” (including pride, place identity and place attachment) became heightened during reef decline, when more “instrumental elements of senses of place” (lifestyle and aesthetics) declined.

5.3 Place attachments play a role in decisions to relocate

5.3.1 Place attachments inhibit more than prompt decisions to relocate

Understanding environmental behavior is a primary focus in place attachment research, including the role of place attachment in the decision to relocate away from a risk (Feng et al., 2022; Solecki and Friedman, 2021). Multiple factors are involved in a person’s decision to relocate including economic, social, environmental, political, and emotional considerations, which make each situation unique (Chan et al., 2022; Dandy et al., 2019; Kothari, 2020). Among these factors, place attachment has been found to play an important role in a person’s decision to relocate (Bukvic and Barnett, 2022; Dandy et al., 2019; Mallette et al., 2021), either inhibiting or prompting a decision to leave (Bukvic et al., 2022; Dandy et al., 2019).

Although there are examples of place attachments prompting a decision to leave (Dandy et al., 2019; Depari and Lindell, 2023; Holley et al., 2022), overall, the majority of research shows strong place attachments are a barrier to relocation (Dandy et al., 2019; Masterson et al., 2017; Steimanis et al., 2021; Swapan and Sadeque, 2021). Several studies have found individual and community place attachments, among other factors, played a role in decisions not to migrate (Adams, 2016; Sengupta and Samanta, 2022; Upadhyay et al., 2024), or not to relocate when exposed to climate risks in particular (Dannenberg et al., 2019; Fattah Hulio et al., 2023; Holley et al., 2022; Phillips et al., 2022; Woodhall-Melnik and Weissman, 2023; Yee et al., 2022). This seems to contradict a common perception that disasters make people devalue place and relocate to safer areas (Oracion, 2021). Nevertheless, as Gurney et al. (2021) found around the Great Barrier Reef, if a place has been threatened or disrupted sometimes people become more aware and appreciative of their environment (McKinzie, 2019), strengthening their attachment to it (Binder et al., 2019; Cox and Perry, 2011; Devine-Wright and Howes, 2010; Lemée et al., 2019; Lewicka, 2020). This phenomenon is often referred to as latent place attachment and can affect relocation decisions, often adding to people’s determination not to relocate (Binder et al., 2019).

Based on the assumption that strong place attachments are a barrier to relocation, Bukvic et al. (2022) and Rey-Valette et al. (2019) mapped the strength of place attachments to predict future willingness to relocate from coastal risks in the eastern US and the south of France, respectively. Bukvic et al. (2022) found rural residents tended to have higher place attachment and determined such residents would therefore be less willing to relocate, and Rey-Valette et al. (2019) found there was a correlation between willingness to relocate and distance from the sea, where those further from the sea were more willing to relocate. Both studies were meant to inform planning and policy decisions by helping to understand who might be more open or resistant to relocation. Bukvic et al. (2022) also developed regional maps displaying exposure to coastal flooding. When compared to maps showing place attachment strength, they could identify which people living in high-risk areas may be more likely to relocate. In this situation, communities with lower place attachment could be targeted first for relocation and act as a signal for others with higher attachment to place who may be more resistant.

5.3.2 Risk perception and place attachment can influence each other in decisions to relocate

The contribution of risk perception to understanding the relationship between place attachment and decision to relocate is complex (Bonaiuto et al., 2016; Lie et al., 2023). In some cases, strong place attachment affects risk perception by making people more aware of environmental risks, resulting in increased risk coping behavior. For example, Woodhall-Melnik and Weissman (2023) found that those who were aware and concerned about future risks openly discussed willingness to relocate. Similarly, Quinn et al. (2018) found people with low place attachment were less likely to perceive flood risk and therefore less likely to feel motivated to choose adaptation measures like moving.

In other cases, strong place attachment diminishes risk perception, leading to actions that contribute to immobility (Jamali and Nejat, 2016; Lie et al., 2023; Steimanis et al., 2021). For example, Costas et al. (2015) and De Dominicis et al. (2015) both studied coastal communities facing risk from sea level rise and flooding, and found that place attachment moderated risk perception by leading to the underestimation of the impact of risk. This in turn weakened communities’ risk-coping intentions. Further, Pucker et al. (2023) found strong place attachment in Hawaiian residents living in vulnerable coastal areas made them feel secure enough to stay and handle changes that might happen to their place. This is often referred to as risk perception normalization (Dang and Weiss, 2021) or optimism bias (Solecki and Friedman, 2021). In these situations, residents seem to accept the risk (i.e., living near the coast) in exchange for benefits including the sense of permanence and stability offered to them by place attachment (Costas et al., 2015). In some circumstances however, diminishing risk perception and coping intentions could restrict other life opportunities (Jamali and Nejat, 2016), or put people in danger if they are in high-risk situations and they do not relocate (Chan et al., 2022; Dannenberg et al., 2019; Lewicka, 2011a; Masterson et al., 2017; Phillips et al., 2012).

Lastly, a few researchers have focused on how specific types of place attachment can affect response to a risk (i.e., stay or move away from a risk) (Devine-Wright, 2013c; Mishra et al., 2010; Sullivan and Young, 2020; Van Veelen and Haggett, 2017). With regards to making the decision to move away from a risk, Quinn et al. (2018) found people with relative (ambivalent) place attachment types at the town scale were less likely to perceive flood risk, which in turn affected their choice not to move. Similarly, Parreira and Mouro (2023) found active (intentional) attachment types were associated with higher risk perception and adoption of coping strategies like moving due to sea level rise. Lie et al. (2023) characterized different dimensions of place attachments and found bonds made through generational ties, historical knowledge, and closeness to nature increased risk awareness of flooding but did not necessarily translate into risk perception. However, Lie et al. (2023) are not alone in finding that place dependency and family bonds influenced people to not relocate (Woodhall-Melnik and Weissman, 2023).

5.4 Place attachments play positive and negative roles during place change/relocation

5.4.1 Disruption to place attachments due to place change/relocation can have negative consequences to health and well-being

Many factors can influence individual and community place attachments after a disruption to place or due to a relocation. These include the existing characteristics of individuals and communities (Jamali and Nejat, 2016; Rumbach et al., 2016) and the characteristics of the disruption itself (Barcus and Brunn, 2010; Di Masso et al., 2019; Solecki and Friedman, 2021).

Disruptions and relocations result in physical changes as well as changes to symbolic meanings and social aspects of place (Manzo et al., 2023). Further, in personal mobility studies, it has been known for some time that frequent relocation negatively affects physical health (Stokols et al., 1983), but there is increasing understanding that relocation can also severely affect psychological health and well-being (Carroll et al., 2009). One of the earliest studies about place attachment and mobility is Fried’s (1966) study on the forced relocation of a Boston neighborhood due to urban renewal. The frequently cited study found residents experienced grief and mourning when relocated. They also found that community ties either provided stability, allowing people to be highly functional, or prevented them from embracing wider life opportunities that mobility may provide (Fried, 1966). Psycho-social responses to disruptions or to a relocation can include profound feelings of loss for both tangible (possessions, homes, infrastructure), and intangible elements (Alston et al., 2018; Kothari, 2020; Prewitt Diaz and Dayal, 2008). Intangible losses can include histories, identity, social cohesion, belonging and community, and place attachments (Alston et al., 2018; Kothari, 2020).

More specifically, a disruption to a place or a relocation can strain or rupture people-place bonds (Quinn et al., 2015). This requires individuals to change or rearrange their old daily routines and habits (Dickinson, 2019; Lewicka, 2011a), and their psychological processes of cognition and affect (Zheng et al., 2019). Changes such as these can cause fear, anxiety and stress (Cheng and Chou, 2015; Phillips et al., 2022; Woodhall-Melnik and Grogan, 2019), a sense of powerlessness (Phillips and Murphy, 2021), and can produce feelings of nostalgia, alienation, or a sense of placelessness (Fullilove, 1996; Zheng et al., 2019). These feelings can undermine the positive aspects of place attachment such as a sense of stability, connectivity, security, and well-being (Fresque-Baxter and Armitage, 2012; Fullilove, 2014; Jamali and Nejat, 2016; Phillips and Murphy, 2021; Woodhall-Melnik and Grogan, 2019; Zheng et al., 2019). Research shows that relocation in particular adds stress to the disaster experience (Adams-Hutcheson, 2015) because being physically separated from your group identity and your sense of security affects mental health more profoundly (Dannenberg et al., 2019; Jamali and Nejat, 2016; McMichael and Powell, 2021; Porter, 2015; Woodhall-Melnik and Grogan, 2019).

Several expressions have been introduced to the lexicon to convey the loss, grief and mourning caused by disruptions to place or relocation from place (Fried, 2000; Phillips and Murphy, 2021; Silver and Grek-Martin, 2015). These include solstalgia (“the inability to derive solace from the present state of one’s environment”; Philippenko et al., 2021, p. 21) and rootshock (a “traumatic stress reaction to the loss of one’s emotional ecosystem”; Fullilove, 2014, p. 149). Both concepts have been found to affect people’s ability to cope and could be exacerbated by place attachment (Fried, 2000; Gurney et al., 2021; Marshall et al., 2019; Silver and Grek-Martin, 2015). The concept of climate grief has also been introduced (an expected or real sense of loss due to degradation to places and ecosystems) and is applicable to disruptions that are specifically climate-related (Allen, 2020; Marshall et al., 2019).

5.4.2 Place attachments have a dualistic role in recovery from place change/ relocation

After a disruption to place or a relocation, emphasis has traditionally been on a return to normal as fast as possible with the focus on physical and economic recovery over social-psychological concerns (Cox and Perry, 2011; Dickinson, 2019). However, rebuilding community, identity and belonging is also important (Perez Murcia, 2020), and increasingly the idea of fostering physical, mental and social wellbeing in parallel is increasingly considered important for recovery (Fresque-Baxter and Armitage, 2012; Prayag et al., 2021; Woodhall-Melnik and Weissman, 2023).

A challenge identified in the literature, however, is that place attachment may have dual roles in recovery, just like it does in decision to relocate (Table 2). On the one hand, place attachments can act as a barrier to, or hindrance to recovery, potentially making it harder to move forward in the recovery process (Binder et al., 2019). For example, change may feel so overwhelming that people want to protect their existing place attachments and identities in order to maintain some stability and security, even if those are only illusory (Fresque-Baxter and Armitage, 2012). This may inhibit people detaching from existing places enough to move them forward in the recovery process. Adams-Hutcheson (2015) also found strong place attachments can undermine relocated individuals’ sense of belonging and ability to make new attachments if they feel like outsiders compared to those who were allowed to stay in place. Lastly, Lewicka (2020) suggested that an individual’s predominant type of attachment may also affect their response to the overall impact of a disruption. Specifically, they proposed people with more traditional place attachments that are based on long-held daily routines may take longer to adjust after a disruption than those with active attachment types.

Table 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Positive and negative roles of place attachment (PA) in decisions to relocate and in recovery after a disruption/relocation.

On the other hand, place attachment has also been found to act as an aid to recovery and to promote healing after a disruption (Jamali and Nejat, 2016; Paul et al., 2024; Prayag et al., 2021; Winstanley et al., 2015; Woodhall-Melnik and Grogan, 2019). For example, latent place attachments that strengthen after a disruption or relocation may inhibit detachment of old bonds, but they may also help with recovery. In particular, research has found that individual and community place attachments play a large role in the development and maintenance of social capital through social ties (Cox and Perry, 2011; Prayag et al., 2021). Social capital has been identified as playing a critical role in disaster recovery and is an indicator of overall well-being (Binder et al., 2019; Jamali and Nejat, 2016; Prayag et al., 2021; Quinn et al., 2021, 2015). However, the buffering effects of social connections may not be possible in situations where residents are relocated permanently and scattered (Quinn et al., 2021). For individuals who stay in or return to place, however, disasters can also lead to communal coping where communities come together during a disruption. This helps provide purpose, identity and fosters reconnection (Adams-Hutcheson, 2015; Silver and Grek-Martin, 2015; Woodhall-Melnik and Grogan, 2019). Understanding how to renew place attachments after a disruption or relocation may be an important factor in recovery to a new normal.

5.4.3 Place attachments may be able to provide stability during place change/relocation

Place attachment can have a positive impact on recovery by providing stability and purposeful direction while people are undergoing change (Di Masso et al., 2019; Manzo et al., 2023; Williams and Miller, 2024) (Table 2). People generally seek security and stability in their lives (Feldman, 1990; Lewicka, 2020), but rapid or ongoing changes to place can lead to feelings of instability and can affect our ontological security. Ontological security is a state that exists when one has safety and stability, both individually and collectively, that allows for the development of personal and group identity (Giddens, 1991; Laing, 1964). Brown and Perkins (1992) and later Manzo et al. (2021) proposed that people are constantly balancing a mixture of stable/fixed and changing/fluid place attachments to maintain a sense of security as they navigate through their lives. When a disruption or change occurs that overwhelms people, they may be able to find a “thread of continuity or stability” via their place attachments (Manzo et al., 2021, p. 282).

Minimizing and restoring broken bonds quickly after a disruption will obviously help maintain or re-establish a sense of security and could be important components of recovery (Lewicka, 2020). However, Brown and Perkins (1992) and Brunacini (2023) argue that the intentional loosening of existing attachments to place and related obligations (detachment), may also be a way to help navigate periods of instability. This implies that we can prepare people by increasing their anticipation of place detachment, and untangle their emotions about a current place while simultaneously imagining a new existence in a new place (Agyeman et al., 2009; Brown and Perkins, 1992; Brunacini, 2023; Giuliani and Feldman, 1993; Tschakert et al., 2017). By encouraging detachment to begin in the pre-disruption phase, perhaps recovery can start prior to relocation and the two processes of detachment and emplacement can occur simultaneously (Kothari, 2020; Tschakert et al., 2017). This could help an individuals’ stability and resilience, and potentially prompt a recovery experience more like those who relocate voluntarily (Brunacini, 2023; Manzo, 2003; Prayag et al., 2021; Quinn et al., 2015). This may work best in situations where there is a long pre-disruption phase, such as for those facing a slowly changing place or anticipating relocating in the future (Agyeman et al., 2009; Brown and Perkins, 1992; Brunacini, 2023; Giuliani and Feldman, 1993; Tschakert et al., 2017).

Residential mobility research explains how relocated people often try to (re)establish or maintain a sense of self continuity by choosing similar types of settlements (i.e., suburban, urban, rural, etc.), a phenomenon called settlement identity (Devine-Wright and Quinn, 2020; Feldman, 1990; Gustafson, 2001; Scannell and Gifford, 2010). Choosing similar settlement types may cause less disruption to people’s identity and personal sense of continuity, and it may help them grow roots in new places quicker (Feldman, 1990; Lewicka, 2020). Place congruent continuity is a similar concept that describes matching one’s self-concept to place features (i.e., I am a country person, I am a city person) to help maintain a sense of continuity in one’s life (Feldman, 1990; Lewicka, 2020). Lewicka (2020) describes two other ways people use places to serve as sources of continuity, including place referent continuity, which is triggered by conscious or unconscious memories of place, and perceived continuity, where place is perceived through its continuous history. These concepts can potentially inform future mobility decisions and place attachment formations (Bailey et al., 2021).

Lastly, a new conceptual tool has been proposed that may give insight in how to maintain ontological security through disruption/relocation (Williams and Miller, 2024). Building on the work of early champions for a more holistic and dynamic view of place (Brown and Perkins, 1992; Cresswell, 2009; Williams and Miller, 2020), several researchers suggest that places are uncentered, complex “multi-scaled relations of materials and flows” that normalize change over time and space (Williams and Miller, 2024, p. 298). Massey (1991) refers to these structures as constellations. Others suggest webs (Raymond et al., 2021), assemblages, or rhizomes (Williams and Miller, 2024). Their rationale is that these web-like structures can easily accommodate a more progressive view of place attachment, which considers individuals and communities to have multiple place attachments that: are plural in discourse and practice, vary in type and strength (Kim, 2021; Williams and Miller, 2020), represent a broad range of positive and negative emotions (Dandy et al., 2019; Manzo, 2003), are created by a variety of memories, emotions and experience (Manzo and Devine-Wright, 2020b), and are dynamic, ebbing and flowing over time (Kim, 2021; Williams and Miller, 2020). By considering places and attachments in this way, mobility and transitions can be normalized, allowing for (re)connection of people and communities to a multiplicity of places at various spatial and temporal scales (Berroeta et al., 2021; Manzo et al., 2021; Williams and Miller, 2024). This perspective may provide people a way to feel their ontological security is less impacted through disruption or relocation (Williams and Miller, 2024).

6 Conclusion

Renewed interest in managed retreat is driven by the urgency of climate change responses. Millions of people currently live in low-lying floodplains and high-risk coastal areas, and may need to relocate in the future due to climate-related flooding and sea-level rise. Managed retreat is a difficult climate adaptation option to undertake because it triggers a complex mix of economic, social-cultural and psychological factors. Place attachment is among these psycho-social factors that can contribute to understanding more about managed retreat. Specifically, how can place attachments change over the course of managed retreat, how can managed retreat affect place attachment and vice versa, and how can place attachment impede managed retreat or promote recovery? Research into these questions is currently limited and broadly distributed across fields and vocabularies (Bukvic et al., 2022), which could partially be a result as well as cause of the incomplete conceptualization of both constructs. Taking a broader view of managed retreat as climate-induced mobility has allowed us an opportunity for insight into place attachment research that may help managed retreat researchers and practitioners move the field forward. For example, research on the broader concepts of personal mobility and forced relocation have contributed to our understanding of place attachments by way of concepts like settlement identity, place elasticity and the life course approach. Other specific findings from our review that have improved understanding of place attachment and how it may inform human responses to retreat are summarized below.

Current research supports a more progressive view of place attachment that is complex, dynamic, and frames mobility not as a threat but as something more akin to an opportunity (Dandy et al., 2019; Giuliani and Feldman, 1993; Lewicka, 2020). Growing in acceptance is the idea that places can have multiple contested identities based on different perspectives, and individuals and communities have multiple types of attachments to a place that vary in type, valence, and strength, as well as over time (Kim, 2021; Williams and Miller, 2020). Further, they can have attachments to several different places, even those they have visited temporarily or virtually. More understanding is needed about the characteristics of different type of attachments, their relationships to each other, and how they influence and respond to change (Marshall et al., 2012; Masterson et al., 2017). This web-like view of place attachments raises questions about how the number and diversity of attachments influences our mobility patterns and our response during place change or relocation.

Although research on the dynamic nature of place attachments is sparse, there is greater receptivity to the possibility that they can change over time as they are continually (re)constructed, adapted, and reshaped (Kim, 2021; Williams and Miller, 2020). Disruptions to place, including relocation, reveal there is a non-linear, fluid process that place attachments may follow through pre-transition, transition, and recovery stages. Little is known about the temporal characteristics of each stage or how bonds detach and reattach. However, there is some understanding that the transition period is a tumultuous time when people move through cycles of disorientation and reorientation, which may help them move toward recovery by detaching, reorienting, and renegotiating place attachments (Cox and Holmes, 2000; Harms, 2015; Silver and Grek-Martin, 2015). We concur with researchers who call for more research to understand processes about how bonds to place are formed, developed, and sustained over time, as well as what happens when they are disrupted (Bailey et al., 2016; Brunacini, 2023; Cheng and Chou, 2015; Cross, 2015; Manzo and Devine-Wright, 2020b; Lewicka, 2011a). There is specifically a gap about place attachment changes during relocation (Adams-Hutcheson, 2015).

Making the decision to relocate is complex and is influenced by many factors including place attachment. The literature also indicates that place attachments have a dualistic role, helping or hindering the decision to relocate, and during recovery after a place change or relocation. This dual nature of place attachment is complicated, and there is still a lot of work needed to disentangle the nuances of the nature of place attachment and how it motivates some people to relocate, while constraining others (Woodhall-Melnik and Weissman, 2023).

There is a growing understanding that disruption to place attachments can have negative consequences on people’s physical and mental health and wellbeing. However, the stability and ontological security that place attachments provide, and which can have positive effects, are less understood. The promise that place attachment offers in reinforcing self-continuity should be better studied to understand its utility for promoting recovery and reinforcing self-continuity in persons who experience managed retreat (Brown and Perkins, 1992; Brunacini, 2023; Giuliani and Feldman, 1993; Lewicka, 2020; Masterson et al., 2017; Tschakert et al., 2017).

Full pre-and-post disruption longitudinal studies are presently rare but would be helpful to understand the dynamism of the psycho-social processes, including place attachments, that people undergo following disruption over a long period of time (Cox and Perry, 2011; Cross, 2015; Dang and Weiss, 2021; Devine-Wright and Quinn, 2020; Kothari, 2020; Lewicka, 2011a; Masterson et al., 2017; Zheng et al., 2019). As well, more qualitative studies are needed as they can offer a more nuanced understanding about decisions to relocate and the place attachment process, than can traditional quantitative studies that use surveys and self-report measures to determine the existence and strength of place attachments (Binder et al., 2015; Cheng and Chou, 2015; Devine-Wright and Quinn, 2020; Herandez et al., 2020; Lewicka, 2011a). For example, Simms et al.’s (2021) qualitative study of Isle de Jean Charles residents helped policy makers understand the deep connection residents had with their land and resulted in a change to a standard policy restricting access to properties left behind. By allowing continued access to their old land, residents have been able to maintain some emotional connection to the ‘old’ place, which played a role in their decision to accept a buyout while easing the relocation transition.

Findings from this review have improved our understanding of place attachments and how they may inform human responses to retreat which will be helpful at both the theoretical and practical levels (Agyeman et al., 2009; Devine-Wright and Quinn, 2020; Low and Altman, 1992; Quinn et al., 2015). Further, the findings highlight opportunities where future place attachment research can potentially play a role in easing the stress and trauma caused by the increase in managed retreat expected in the future.

Author contributions

RW-M: Conceptualization, Funding acquisition, Investigation, Methodology, Writing – original draft. JB: Conceptualization, Methodology, Writing – review & editing. KF: Conceptualization, Methodology, Writing – review & editing. JW-M: Methodology, Writing – review & editing. KS: Conceptualization, Funding acquisition, Methodology, Supervision, Writing – review & editing.

Funding

The author(s) declare that financial support was received for the research, authorship, and/or publication of this article. This work has been funded by a Nova Scotia Graduate Research Scholarship, and a Social Sciences and Humanities Research Council of Canada Insight Grant to KS (#435–2021-0221).

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Generative AI statement

The authors declare that no Generative AI was used in the creation of this manuscript.

Publisher’s note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

References

Abel, N., Gorddard, R., Harman, B., Leitch, A., Langridge, J., Ryan, A., et al. (2011). Sea level rise, coastal development and planned retreat: analytical framework, governance principles and an Australian case study. Environ. Sci. Pol. 14, 279–288. doi: 10.1016/j.envsci.2010.12.002

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Adams, H. (2016). Why populations persist: Mobility, place attachment and climate change. Popul. Env. 37, 429–448. doi: 10.1007/s11111-015-0246-3

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Adams-Hutcheson, G. (2015). Voices from the margins of recovery: relocated Cantabrians in Waikato. Kōtuitui N. Z. J. Soc. Sci. 10, 135–143. doi: 10.1080/1177083X.2015.1068184

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Agyeman, J., Devine-Wright, P., and Prange, J. (2009). Close to the edge, down by the river? Joining up managed retreat and place attachment in a climate changed world. Environ. Plan. A 41, 509–513. doi: 10.1068/a41301

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Ajibade, I., Sullivan, M., and Haeffner, M. (2020). Why climate migration is not managed retreat: six justifications. Glob. Environ. Change 65:102187. doi: 10.1016/j.gloenvcha.2020.102187

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Ajibade, I., Sullivan, M., Lower, C., Yarina, L., and Reilly, A. (2022). Are managed retreat programs successful and just? A global mapping of success typologies, justice dimensions, and trade-offs. Glob. Environ. Change 76:102576. doi: 10.1016/j.gloenvcha.2022.102576

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Ajibade, I. J., and Siders, A. R. (2021). “Introduction: climate change and planned retreat” in Global views on climate relocation and social justice (London: Routledge), 1–16. doi: 10.4324/9781003141457-1

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Allen, S., (2020). Is climate grief something new? Available at: https://www.apa.org/members/content/climate-grief (Accessed March 3, 2024).

Google Scholar

Alston, M., Hargreaves, D., and Hazeleger, T. (2018). Postdisaster social work: reflections on the nature of place and loss. Aust. Soc. Work. 71, 405–416. doi: 10.1080/0312407X.2017.1409776

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Bailey, E., Devine-Wright, P., and Batel, S. (2021). Emplacing linked lives: a qualitative approach to understanding the co-evolution of residential mobility and place attachment formation over time. J. Community Appl. Soc. Psychol. 31, 515–529. doi: 10.1002/casp.2533

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Bailey, E., Devine-Wright, P., and Batel, S. (2016). Using a narrative approach to understand place attachments and responses to power line proposals: the importance of life-place trajectories. J. Environ. Psychol. 48, 200–211. doi: 10.1016/j.jenvp.2016.10.006

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Barcus, H. R., and Brunn, S. D. (2010). Place elasticity: exploring a new conceptualization of mobility and place attachment in rural America. Geogr. Ann. Ser. B Hum. Geogr. 92, 281–295. doi: 10.1111/j.1468-0467.2010.00353.x

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Berroeta, H., Pinto De Carvalho, L., and Castillo-Sepulveda, J. (2021). “The place-subjectivity continuum after a disaster: enquiring into the production of sense of place as an assemblage” in Changing senses of place. eds. C. Raymond, L. Manzo, A. Di Masso, and T. Von Wirth (Cambridge: Cambridge University Press), 43–52.

Google Scholar

Binder, S. B., Baker, C. K., and Barile, J. P. (2015). Rebuild or relocate? Resilience and Postdisaster decision-making after hurricane Sandy. Am. J. Community Psychol. 56, 180–196. doi: 10.1007/s10464-015-9727-x

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Binder, S. B., Barile, J. P., Baker, C. K., and Kulp, B. (2019). Home buyouts and household recovery: neighborhood differences three years after hurricane Sandy. Environ. Hazards 18, 127–145. doi: 10.1080/17477891.2018.1511404

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Bissell, D. (2020). “Mobilities and place” in The Routledge handbook of place. eds. T. Edensor, A. Kalandides, and U. Kothari (London: Routledge), 99–108.

Google Scholar

Bittle, J., (2024). The great displacement. Climate change and the next American migration. New York: Simon & Schuster.

Google Scholar

Bonaiuto, M., Alves, S., De Dominicis, S., and Petruccelli, I. (2016). Place attachment and natural hazard risk: research review and agenda. J. Environ. Psychol. 48, 33–53. doi: 10.1016/j.jenvp.2016.07.007

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Braamskamp, A., and Penning-Rowsell, E. C. (2018). Managed retreat: a rare and paradoxical success, but yielding a dismal prognosis. Environ. Manag. Sustain. Dev. 7:108. doi: 10.5296/emsd.v7i2.12851

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Brown, B., and Perkins, D. D. (1992). “Disruptions in place attachment” in Place attachment. eds. I. Altman and S. Low (US, Boston, MA: Springer), 279–303.

Google Scholar

Brunacini, J. (2023). From theory to practice: Participatory approaches to understanding the social dimensions of managed retreat (Publication No. 30248911). [Doctoral dissertation, Michigan State University]. ProQuest Dissertations and Theses Global. Available at: https://www.proquest.com/openview/ea16e20ba1d97a8990d71eccb76b956d/1?pq-origsite=gscholar&cbl=18750&diss=y

Google Scholar

Bukvic, A. (2015). Identifying gaps and inconsistencies in the use of relocation rhetoric: a prerequisite for sound relocation policy and planning. Mitig. Adapt. Strateg. Glob. Change 20, 1203–1209. doi: 10.1007/s11027-013-9532-5

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Bukvic, A., and Barnett, S. (2022). Drivers of flood-induced relocation among coastal urban residents: insight from the US east coast. J. Environ. Manag. 325:116429. doi: 10.1016/j.jenvman.2022.116429

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Bukvic, A., Whittemore, A., Gonzales, J., and Wilhelmi, O. (2022). Understanding relocation in flood-prone coastal communities through the lens of place attachment. Appl. Geogr. 146:102758. doi: 10.1016/j.apgeog.2022.102758

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Burkett, M., Bavishi, J., and Shew, E. (2017). Climate displacement, migration, and relocation—and the United States. Clim. Law 7, 227–231. doi: 10.1163/18786561-00704001

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Carroll, B., Morbey, H., Balogh, R., and Araoz, G. (2009). Flooded homes, broken bonds, the meaning of home, psychological processes and their impact on psychological health in a disaster. Health Place 15, 540–547. doi: 10.1016/j.healthplace.2008.08.009

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Chamlee-Wright, E., and Storr, V. H. (2009). “There’s no place like New Orleans”: sense of place and community recovery in the ninth Ward after hurricane Katrina. J. Urban Aff. 31, 615–634. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9906.2009.00479.x

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Chan, A. Y., Burrows, K., and Bell, M. L. (2022). Why they stayed and why they left—a case study from Ellicott City, MD after flash flooding. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 19:10636. doi: 10.3390/ijerph191710636

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Cheng, C.-K., and Chou, S.-F. (2015). The influence of place change on place bonding: a longitudinal panel study of Renovated Park users. Leis. Sci. 37, 391–414. doi: 10.1080/01490400.2015.1021883

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Chow, K., and Healey, M. (2008). Place attachment and place identity: first-year undergraduates making the transition from home to university. J. Environ. Psychol. 28, 362–372. doi: 10.1016/j.jenvp.2008.02.011

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Costas, S., Ferreira, O., and Martinez, G. (2015). Why do we decide to live with risk at the coast? Ocean coast. Management 118, 1–11. doi: 10.1016/j.ocecoaman.2015.05.015

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Cox, H. M., and Holmes, C. A. (2000). Loss, healing, and the power of place. Hum. Stud. 23, 63–78. doi: 10.1023/A:1005566612706

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Cox, R. S., and Perry, K.-M. E. (2011). Like a Fish out of water: reconsidering disaster recovery and the role of place and social Capital in Community Disaster Resilience. Am. J. Community Psychol. 48, 395–411. doi: 10.1007/s10464-011-9427-0

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Cresswell, T. (2009). “Place” in International encyclopedia of human geography. (Oxford: Elsevier Inc), 169–177. doi: 10.1016/B978-008044910-4.00310-2

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Cresswell, T. (2006). On the move. Mobility in the modern Western world. New York: Routledge.

Google Scholar

Cross, J. E. (2015). Processes of place attachment: an interactional framework. Symb. Interact. 38, 493–520. doi: 10.1002/symb.198

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Dandy, J., Horwitz, P., Campbell, R., Drake, D., and Leviston, Z. (2019). Leaving home: place attachment and decisions to move in the face of environmental change. Reg. Environ. Chang. 19, 615–620. doi: 10.1007/s10113-019-01463-1

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Dang, L., and Weiss, J. (2021). Evidence on the relationship between place attachment and behavioral intentions between 2010 and 2021: a systematic literature review. Sustain. For. 13:13138. doi: 10.3390/su132313138

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Dannenberg, A. L., Frumkin, H., Hess, J. J., and Ebi, K. L. (2019). Managed retreat as a strategy for climate change adaptation in small communities: public health implications. Clim. Chang. 153, 1–14. doi: 10.1007/s10584-019-02382-0

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

De Dominicis, S., Fornara, F., Ganucci Cancellieri, U., Twigger-Ross, C., and Bonaiuto, M. (2015). We are at risk, and so what? Place attachment, environmental risk perceptions and preventive coping behaviours. J. Environ. Psychol. 43, 66–78. doi: 10.1016/j.jenvp.2015.05.010

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

de Sherbinin, A., Bukvic, A., Rohat, G., Gall, M., McCusker, B., Preston, B., et al. (2019). Climate vulnerability mapping: a systematic review and future prospects. WIREs Clim. Change 10:e600. doi: 10.1002/wcc.600

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

De Sherbinin, A., Castro, M., Gemenne, F., Cernea, M. M., Adamo, S., Fearnside, P. M., et al. (2011). Preparing for resettlement associated with climate change. Science 334, 456–457. doi: 10.1126/science.1208821

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Depari, C. D. A., and Lindell, M. K. (2023). “Moving or not?”: factors affecting community responses to environmental disruption. Int. J. Disaster Risk Reduct. 95:103898. doi: 10.1016/j.ijdrr.2023.103898

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Devine-Wright, P. (2020). “Place attachment” in The Routledge handbook of place. eds. T. Edensor, A. Kalandides, and U. Kothari (London: Routledge), 506–517. doi: 10.4324/9780429453267

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Devine-Wright, P., and Quinn, T. (2020). “Dynamics of place attachment in a climate changed world” in Place attachment: Advances in theory, methods and applications. 2nd Edn. eds. L. Manzo and P. Devine-Wright (Abingdon, Oxon: Routledge), 226–242.

Google Scholar

Devine-Wright, P. (2013b). Think global, act local? The relevance of place attachments and place identities in a climate changed world. Glob. Environ. Change 23, 61–69. doi: 10.1016/j.gloenvcha.2012.08.003

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Devine-Wright, P. (2013c). Explaining “NIMBY” objections to a power line: the role of personal, place attachment and project-related factors. Environ. Behav. 45, 761–781. doi: 10.1177/0013916512440435

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Devine-Wright, P., and Howes, Y. (2010). Disruption to place attachment and the protection of restorative environments: a wind energy case study. J. Environ. Psychol. 30, 271–280. doi: 10.1016/j.jenvp.2010.01.008

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Devine-Wright, P. (2014). “Dynamics of place attachment in a climate changed world” in Place attachment: Advances in theory, methods and applications. 1st ed. eds. L. Manzo and P. Devine-Wright (Abingdon, Oxon: Routledge). 165–177. doi: 10.4324/9780203757765

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Di Masso, A., Williams, D. R., Raymond, C. M., Buchecker, M., Degenhardt, B., Devine-Wright, P., et al. (2019). Between fixities and flows: navigating place attachments in an increasingly mobile world. J. Environ. Psychol. 61, 125–133. doi: 10.1016/j.jenvp.2019.01.006

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Dickinson, S. (2019). Changing places: geographies of post-disaster landscapes. Geography 104, 116–124. doi: 10.1080/00167487.2019.12094074

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Doberstein, B., and Stager, H. (2013). Towards guidelines for post-disaster vulnerability reduction in informal settlements. Disasters 37, 28–47. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-7717.2012.01294.x

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Doberstein, B., Tadgell, A., and Rutledge, A. (2020). Managed retreat for climate change adaptation in coastal megacities: a comparison of policy and practice in Manila and Vancouver. J. Environ. Manag. 253:109753. doi: 10.1016/j.jenvman.2019.109753

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Duggan, J., Cvitanovic, C., and van Putten, I. (2023). Measuring sense of place in social-ecological systems: a review of literature and future research needs. Ecosyst. People 19:2162968. doi: 10.1080/26395916.2022.2162968

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Dundon, L. A., and Abkowitz, M. (2021). Climate-induced managed retreat in the U.S.: a review of current research. Clim. Risk Manag. 33:100337. doi: 10.1016/j.crm.2021.100337

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Dundon, L. A., Abkowitz, M., and Camp, J. (2023). Governing transition: case studies in transformative adaptation. Case Stud. Environ. 7:1816908. doi: 10.1525/cse.2023.1816908

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Edensor, T., Kalandides, A., and Kothari, U. (2020). “Introduction. Thinking about place - themes and emergent approaches” in The Routledge handbook of place. eds. T. Edensor, A. Kalandides, and U. Kothari (New York: Routledge), 1–10.

Google Scholar

Elder, G. Jr. (1994). Time, human agency, and social change: perspectives on the life course. Soc. Psychol. Q. 57:4. doi: 10.2307/2786971

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Erfani, G. (2022). Reconceptualising sense of place: towards a conceptual framework for investigating individual-community-place interrelationships. J. Plan. Lit. 37, 452–466. doi: 10.1177/08854122221081109

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Esteban, M., Takagi, H., Jamero, L., Chadwick, C., Avelino, J. E., Mikami, T., et al. (2020). Adaptation to sea level rise: learning from present examples of land subsidence. Ocean Coast. Manag. 189:104852. doi: 10.1016/j.ocecoaman.2019.104852

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Fattah Hulio, A., Varghese, V., and Chikaraishi, M. (2023). Analyzing the preferences of flood victims on post flood public houses (PFPH): application of a hybrid choice model to the floodplains of southern Pakistan. Clim. Risk Manag. 42:100571. doi: 10.1016/j.crm.2023.100571

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Feldman, R. (1990). Settlement-identity: psychological bonds with home places in a Mobile society. Environ. Behav. 22, 183–229. doi: 10.1177/0013916590222002

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Feng, X., Zhang, Z., and Chen, X. (2022). Paper analysis of the relevance of place attachment to environment-related behavior: a systematic literature review. Sustain. For. 14:16073. doi: 10.3390/su142316073

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Ferris, E. (2015). Climate-induced resettlement: environmental change and the planned relocation of communities. SAIS Rev. Int. Aff. 35, 109–117. doi: 10.1353/sais.2015.0001

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Fresque-Baxter, J. A., and Armitage, D. (2012). Place identity and climate change adaptation: a synthesis and framework for understanding. WIREs Clim. Change 3, 251–266. doi: 10.1002/wcc.164

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Fried, M. (2000). Continuities and discontinuities of place. J. Environ. Psychol. 20, 193–205. doi: 10.1006/jevp.1999.0154

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Fried, M. (1966). “Grieving for a lost home: psychological costs of relocation” in Urban renewal: The record and the controversy. ed. J. Q. Wilson (Cambridge, Massachusetts: MIT Press), 359–379.

Google Scholar

Fullilove, M. T. (2014). ““The frayed knot”: what happens to place attachment in the context of serial forced displacement?” in Place attachment: Advances in theory, methods, and applications. 1st ed. eds. L. Manzo and P. Devine-Wright (Abingdon, Oxon: Routledge).

Google Scholar

Fullilove, M. T. (1996). Psychiatric implications of displacement: contributions from the psychology of place. Am. J. Psychiatry 153, 1516–1523. doi: 10.1176/ajp.153.12.1516

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Giddens, A. (1991). Modernity and self-identity: Self and Society in the Late Modern age. Stanford: Stanford University Press.

Google Scholar

Giuliani, M. V., and Feldman, R. (1993). Place attachment in a developmental and cultural context. J. Environ. Psychol. 13, 267–274. doi: 10.1016/S0272-4944(05)80179-3

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Greene, D., Tehranifar, P., Hernandez-Cordero, L. J., and Fullilove, M. T. (2011). I used to cry every day: a model of the family process of managing displacement. J. Urban Health 88, 403–416. doi: 10.1007/s11524-011-9583-4

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Grocke, C. L., Eversole, R., and Hawkins, C. J. (2022). The influence of place attachment on community leadership and place management. J. Place Manag. Dev. 15, 298–315. doi: 10.1108/JPMD-11-2020-0118

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Gurney, G., Marshall, N., Curnock, M., Pert, P., and Thiault, L. (2021). “Coral reef collapse and sense of place in the great barrier reef, Australia” in Changing senses of place. eds. C. Raymond, L. Manzo, A. Di Masso, and T. Von Wirth (Cambridge: Cambridge University Press), 21–31.

Google Scholar

Gustafson, P. (2014). “Place attachment in an age of mobility” in Place attachment: Advances in theory, methods, and applications. eds. L. Manzo and P. Devine-Wright (New York: Routledge), 37–48.

Google Scholar

Gustafson, P. (2001). Roots and routes: exploring the relationship between place attachment and mobility. Environ. Behav. 33, 667–686. doi: 10.1177/00139160121973188

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Hanna, C., White, I., and Glavovic, B. (2019). “Managed retreat in practice: mechanisms and challenges for implementation” in Oxford research encyclopedia of natural Hazard science (Oxford University Press). doi: 10.1093/acrefore/9780199389407.013.350

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Harms, A. (2015). Leaving Lohāchāra: on circuits of emplacement and displacement in the Ganges Delta. Glob. Environ. 8, 62–85. doi: 10.3197/ge.2015.080104

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Hay, R. (1998). Sense of place in developmental context. J. Environ. Psychol. 18, 5–29. doi: 10.1006/jevp.1997.0060

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Herandez, B., Hidalgo, C., and Ruiz, C. (2020). “Theoretical and methodological aspects of research on place attachment” in Place attachment: Advances in theory, methods, and applications. 2nd ed. eds. L. Manzo and P. Devine-Wright (Abingdon, Oxon: Routledge), 95–110.

Google Scholar

Hernandez-Santin, C., Hes, D., Beer, T., and Lo, L. (2020). “Regenerative Placemaking: creating a New model for place development by bringing together regenerative and Placemaking processes” in Contemporary Urban Design thinking. ed. R. Roggema (Cham, Switzerland: Springer), 53–68. doi: 10.1007/978-3-030-54686-1_4

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Hino, M., Field, C. B., and Mach, K. J. (2017). Managed retreat as a response to natural hazard risk. Nat. Clim. Chang. 7, 364–370. doi: 10.1038/nclimate3252

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Holley, J. R., McComas, K. A., Lambert, C. E., Snider, N. P., and Tucker, G. K. (2022). Responding to flood risk in Louisiana: the roles of place attachment, emotions, and location. Nat. Hazards 113, 615–640. doi: 10.1007/s11069-022-05316-9

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Hsu, M., Okada, T., Mori, S., and Howitt, R. (2019). Resettling, disconnecting or displacing? Attending to local sociality, culture and history in disaster settings. Asia Pac. Viewp. 60, 163–174. doi: 10.1111/apv.12230

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Hummon, D. (1992). “Community attachment. Local sentiment and a sense of place” in Place attachment. eds. I. Altman and S. Low (New York: Plenum Press), 253–278.

Google Scholar

IDMC (2024). Global report on internal displacement 2023. Geneva: Internal Displacement Monitoring Centre.

Google Scholar

Imura, M., and Shaw, R. (2009). Challenges and potentials of post-disaster relocation. Asian J. Environ. Disaster Manag. AJEDM – focus. -act. Risk Reduct. Asia 1, 197–221. doi: 10.3850/S1793924009000029

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Inalhan, G., and Finch, E. (2004). Place attachment and sense of belonging. Facilities 22, 120–128. doi: 10.1108/02632770410540333

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Iuchi, K. (2014). Planning Resettlement After Disasters. J. Am. Plan. Assoc. 80, 413–425. doi: 10.1080/01944363.2014.978353

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Jamali, M., and Nejat, A. (2016). Place attachment and disasters: knowns and unknowns. J. Emerg. Manag. 14, 349–364. doi: 10.5055/jem.2016.0299

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Kim, M. (2021). Plural and fluid place attachment amid tourism-induced neighborhood change in a disadvantaged neighborhood in South Korea. Geoforum 121, 129–137. doi: 10.1016/j.geoforum.2020.12.017

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Koslov, L. (2016). The case for retreat. Public Cult. 28, 359–387. doi: 10.1215/08992363-3427487

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Kothari, U. (2020). “Displacement, loss and emplacement” in Edensor, T. eds. A. Kalandides and U. Kothari (The Routledge Handbook of Place: Routledge & CRC Press), 443–447.

Google Scholar

Kousky, C. (2014). Managing shoreline retreat: a US perspective. Clim. Chang. 124, 9–20. doi: 10.1007/s10584-014-1106-3

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Kulp, S. A., and Strauss, B. H. (2019). New elevation data triple estimates of global vulnerability to sea-level rise and coastal flooding. Nat. Commun. 10:4844. doi: 10.1038/s41467-019-12808-z

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Laing, R. D. (1964). “The divided self” in An existential study in sanity and madness (London: Peguin Books).

Google Scholar

Lawrence, J., Boston, J., Bell, R., Olufson, S., Kool, R., Hardcastle, M., et al. (2020). Implementing pre-Emptive managed retreat: constraints and novel insights. Curr. Clim. Chang. Rep. 6, 66–80. doi: 10.1007/s40641-020-00161-z

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Lemée, C., Fleury-Bahi, G., and Navarro, O. (2019). Impact of place identity, self-efficacy and anxiety state on the relationship between coastal flooding risk perception and the willingness to cope. Front. Psychol. 10:499. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2019.00499

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Lewicka, M. (2020). “In search of roots: restoring continuity in a mobile world” in Place attachment: Advances in theory, methods, and applications. eds. L. Manzo and P. Devine-Wright (New York: Routledge), 61–76. doi: 10.4324/9780429274442-4

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Lewicka, M. (2011a). Place attachment: how far have we come in the last 40 years? J. Environ. Psychol. 31, 207–230. doi: 10.1016/j.jenvp.2010.10.001

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Lewicka, M. (2011b). On the varieties of People’s relationships with places: Hummon’s typology revisited. Environ. Behav. 43, 676–709. doi: 10.1177/0013916510364917

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Lie, L. B., de Korte, L., and Pursiainen, C. H. (2023). “Here, I will stay until I die”—exploring the relationship between place attachment, risk perception, and coping behavior in two small Norwegian communities. Reg. Environ. Chang. 115. doi: 10.1007/s10113-023-02106-2

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Loughran, K., and Elliott, J. R. (2022). Unequal retreats: how racial segregation shapes climate adaptation. Hous. Policy Debate 32, 171–189. doi: 10.1080/10511482.2021.1931928

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Low, S. (1992). “Symbolic ties that bind. Place attachment in the plaza” in Place Attachment. eds. I. Altman and S. Low (New York: Plenum Press), 165–185.

Google Scholar

Madanipour, A. (2020). “Temporary places. Moving people and changing spaces” in The Routledge handbook of place. eds. T. Edensor, A. Kalandides, and U. Kothari (London: Routledge), 449–458.

Google Scholar

Mallette, A., Smith, T. F., Elrick-Barr, C., Blythe, J., and Plummer, R. (2021). Understanding preferences for coastal climate change adaptation: a systematic literature review. Sustain. For. 13:8594. doi: 10.3390/su13158594

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Manzo, L. (2003). Beyond house and haven: toward a revisioning of emotional relationships with places. J. Environ. Psychol. 23, 47–61. doi: 10.1016/S0272-4944(02)00074-9

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Manzo, L., and Devine-Wright, P. (2020). “Introduction” in Place attachment: Advances in theory, methods, and applications. 2 nd Ed. eds. L. Manzo and P. Devine-Wright (Abingdon, Oxon: Routledge), 1–12.

Google Scholar

Manzo, L., and Pinto De Carvalho, L. (2020). “The role of qualitative approaches to place attachment research” in Place attachment: Advances in theory, methods, and applications. 2nd ed. eds. L. Manzo and P. Devine-Wright (Abingdon, Oxon: Routledge), 111–126.

Google Scholar

Manzo, L., Williams, D., Di Masso, A., Raymond, C., and Gulsrud, N. (2023). Using senses of place to help communities navigate place disruption and uncertainty. Landsc. J. 42, 37–52. doi: 10.3368/lj.42.1.37

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Manzo, L., Williams, D. R., Masso, A. D., Raymond, C., Von Wirth, T., and Devine-Wright, P. (2021). “Navigating the spaciousness of uncertainties posed by global challenges. A senses of place perspective” in Changing senses of place. eds. C. Raymond, L. C. Manzo, D. R. Williams, A. Di Masso, and T. von Wirth (Cambridge University Press), 331–347.

Google Scholar

Marshall, N., and Bishop, K. (2015). Is there any correlation between Placemaking and place attachment? A Challenge for Interdisciplinary Research. Int. J. Interdiscip. Soc. Community Stud. 9, 1–10. doi: 10.18848/2324-7576/CGP/v09i3-4/59397

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Marshall, N., Adger, W. N., Benham, C., Brown, K., Curnock, M. I., Gurney, G. G., et al. (2019). Reef grief: investigating the relationship between place meanings and place change on the great barrier reef, Australia. Sustain. Sci. 14, 579–587. doi: 10.1007/s11625-019-00666-z

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Marshall, N. A., Park, S. E., Adger, W. N., Brown, K., and Howden, S. M. (2012). Transformational capacity and the influence of place and identity. Environ. Res. Lett. 7:034022. doi: 10.1088/1748-9326/7/3/034022

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Marter-Kenyon, J. (2020). Origins and functions of climate-related relocation: an analytical review. Anthr. Rev. 7, 159–188. doi: 10.1177/2053019620915633

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Massey, D. (1991). A global sense of place. Marx. Today. 38, 24–29.

Google Scholar

Masterson, V. A., Stedman, R. C., Enqvist, J., Tengö, M., Giusti, M., Wahl, D., et al. (2017). The contribution of sense of place to social-ecological systems research: a review and research agenda. Ecol. Soc. 22:49. doi: 10.5751/ES-08872-220149

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Mayr, B., Thaler, T., and Hübl, J. (2020). Successful small-scale household relocation after a millennial flood event in Simbach, Germany 2016. Water 12:156. doi: 10.3390/w12010156

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

McKinzie, A. E. (2019). You don’t miss it ‘til it’s gone: insecurity, place, and the social construction of the environment. Environ. Sociol. 5, 232–242. doi: 10.1080/23251042.2019.1574463

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

McMichael, C., and Powell, T. (2021). Planned relocation and health: a case study from Fiji. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 18:4355. doi: 10.3390/ijerph18084355

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Million, M. L. (1992). “IT WAS HOME”: A phenomenology of place and involuntary displacement as illustrated by the forced dislocation of five southern Alberta families in the Oldman River dam flood area (Publication No. 9328546) [Doctoral dissertation, Saybrook University]. ProQuest Dissertations and Theses Global. Available at: https://www.proquest.com/docview/304030314?pq-origsite=gscholar&fromopenview=true&sourcetype=Dissertations%20&%20Theses.

Google Scholar

Mishra, S., Mazumdar, S., and Suar, D. (2010). Place attachment and flood preparedness. J. Environ. Psychol. 30, 187–197. doi: 10.1016/j.jenvp.2009.11.005

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Morgan, P. (2010). Towards a developmental theory of place attachment. J. Environ. Psychol. 30, 11–22. doi: 10.1016/j.jenvp.2009.07.001

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Nelson, J., Ahn, J. J., and Corley, E. A. (2020). Sense of place: trends from the literature. J. Urban. Int. Res. Placemaking Urban Sustain. 13, 236–261. doi: 10.1080/17549175.2020.1726799

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Nicolosi, E., and Corbett, J. B. (2018). Engagement with climate change and the environment: a review of the role of relationships to place. Local Environ. 23, 77–99. doi: 10.1080/13549839.2017.1385002

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

OECD-DAC, (1992). Guidelines for aid agencies on involuntary displacement and Resettlment in development projects. Paris: OECD.

Google Scholar

O’Donnell, T. (2022). Managed retreat and planned retreat: a systematic literature review. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. B Biol. Sci. 377:20210129. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2021.0129

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Okada, T., Haynes, K., Bird, D., van den Honert, R., and King, D. (2014). Recovery and resettlement following the 2011 flash flooding in the Lockyer Valley. Int. J. Disaster Risk Reduct. 8, 20–31. doi: 10.1016/j.ijdrr.2014.01.001

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Oracion, E. G. (2021). Flood disaster risk perception and sense of place among households along the ocoy river in Negros oriental. Philippines. J. Environ. Sci. Manag. 24, 56–67. doi: 10.47125/jesam/2021_1/06

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Parreira, N., and Mouro, C. (2023). Living by the sea: place attachment, coastal risk perception, and eco-anxiety when coping with climate change. Front. Psychol. 14: 1155635. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2023.1155635

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Patterson, M. E., and Williams, D. R. (2005). Maintaining research traditions on place: diversity of thought and scientific progress. J. Environ. Psychol. 25, 361–380. doi: 10.1016/j.jenvp.2005.10.001

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Paul, N., Galasso, C., and Baker, J. (2024). Household displacement and return in disasters: a review. Nat. Hazards Rev. 25:03123006. doi: 10.1061/NHREFO.NHENG-1930

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Perez Murcia, L. E. (2020). “Remaking a place called home following displacement” in Edensor, T. eds. A. Kalandides and U. Kothari (The Routledge Handbook of Place: Routledge & CRC Press), 443–447. doi: 10.4324/9780429453267-41

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Perry, R. W., and Lindell, M. K. (1997). Principles for managing community relocation as a Hazard mitigation measure. J. Contingencies Crisis Manag. 5, 49–59. doi: 10.1111/1468-5973.00036

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Philippenko, X., Goeldner-Gianella, L., Le Cozannet, G., Grancher, D., and De La Torre, Y. (2021). Perceptions of climate change and adaptation: a subarctic archipelago perspective (Saint-Pierre-and-Miquelon, North America). Ocean Coast. Manag. 215:105924. doi: 10.1016/j.ocecoaman.2021.105924

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Phillips, B., Stukes, P. A., and Jenkins, P. (2012). Freedom Hill is not for Sale—and neither is the Lower ninth Ward. J. Black Stud. 43, 405–426. doi: 10.1177/0021934711425489

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Phillips, C., and Murphy, C. (2021). Solastalgia, place attachment and disruption: insights from a coastal community on the front line. Reg Env. Change 21:46. doi: 10.1007/s10113-021-01778-y

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Phillips, C., Murphy, C., and Bresnihan, P. (2022). The impacts of and responses to place loss in a coastal community in Ireland. Local Environ. Int. J. Justice Sustain. 27, 879–896. doi: 10.1080/13549839.2022.2078294

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Piggott-McKellar, A. E., Pearson, J., McNamara, K. E., and Nunn, P. D. (2020). A livelihood analysis of resettlement outcomes: lessons for climate-induced relocations. Ambio 49, 1474–1489. doi: 10.1007/s13280-019-01289-5

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Pinter, N. (2021). The lost history of managed retreat and community relocation in the United States. Elem. Sci. Anthr. 9:00036. doi: 10.1525/elementa.2021.00036

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Pinter, N., Ishiwateri, M., Nonoguchi, A., Tanaka, Y., Casagrande, D., Durden, S., et al. (2019). Large-scale managed retreat and structural protection following the 2011 Japan tsunami. Nat. Hazards 96, 1429–1436. doi: 10.1007/s11069-019-03602-7

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Pinter, N., and Rees, J. C. (2021). Assessing managed flood retreat and community relocation in the Midwest USA. Nat. Hazards 107, 497–518. doi: 10.1007/s11069-021-04592-1

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Porter, R. R. (2015). Looking homeward: Place attachment and forced relocation (Publication No. 3738098) [Doctoral dissertation, Pacifica Graduate Institute]. ProQuest Dissertations and Theses Global. Available at: https://www.proquest.com/docview/1738947396/70962F8492C45C3PQ/1?accountid=10406&sourcetype=Dissertations%20&%20Theses

Google Scholar

Prayag, G., Ozanne, L. K., and Spector, S. (2021). A psychological wellbeing perspective of long-term disaster recovery following the Canterbury earthquakes. Int. J. Disaster Risk Reduct. 63:102438. doi: 10.1016/j.ijdrr.2021.102438

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Prewitt Diaz, J., and Dayal, A. (2008). Sense of place: a model for community based psychosocial support programs. Australas. J. Disaster Trauma Stud. 63, 820–827. Available at: https://trauma.massey.ac.nz/issues/2008-1/prewitt_diaz.htm

Google Scholar

Pucker, I., McCauley, M., Fox, K., and Jacobel, A. (2023). Residents of Hawaiʻi’s climate decisions to remain or migrate. Ecopsychology 15, 56–68. doi: 10.1089/eco.2022.0027

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Quinn, T., Adger, W. N., Butler, C., and Walker-Springett, K. (2021). Community resilience and well-being: an exploration of Relationality and belonging after disasters. Ann. Am. Assoc. Geogr. 111, 577–590. doi: 10.1080/24694452.2020.1782167

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Quinn, T., Bousquet, F., Guerbois, C., Sougrati, E., and Tabutaud, M. (2018). The dynamic relationship between sense of place and risk perception in landscapes of mobility. Ecol. Soc. 23:39. doi: 10.5751/ES-10004-230239

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Quinn, T., Lorenzoni, I., and Adger, W. N. (2015). “Place attachment, identity, and adaptation” in The adaptive challenge of climate change. eds. K. O’Brien and E. Selboe (New York: Cambridge University Press), 160–170.

Google Scholar

Ramiller, A. (2022). Displacement through development? Property turnover and eviction risk in Seattle. Urban Stud. 59, 1148–1166. doi: 10.1177/00420980211004214

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Raymond, C. (2013). Review of place attachment: advances in theory, methods and applications (edited by Lynne Manzo and Patrick Devine-Wright). Estud. Psicol. 34, 345–348. doi: 10.1174/021093913808349235

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Raymond, C. M., Williams, D. R., Masso, A. D., Manzo, L. C., and Von Wirth, T. (2021). “Introduction: senses of place in the face of global challenges” in Changing senses of place. eds. C. Raymond, L. Manzo, D. Williams, A. Di Masso, and T. von Wirth (Cambridge: Cambridge University Press), 1–18.

Google Scholar

Relph, E. (2008). “Sense of place and emerging social and environmental challenges” in Sense of place, health and quality of life. eds. A. Williams and J. Eyles (London: Routledge), 51–64.

Google Scholar

Relph, E. (1976). Place and Placelessness. London: Pion.

Google Scholar

Rey-Valette, H., Robert, S., and Rulleau, B. (2019). Resistance to relocation in flood-vulnerable coastal areas: a proposed composite index. Clim. Pol. 19, 206–218. doi: 10.1080/14693062.2018.1482823

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Rossi, L., Ponserre, S., Trasforini, E., Ottonelli, D., Campo, L., Libertino, A., et al. (2024). A new methodology for probabilistic flood displacement risk assessment: the case of Fiji and Vanuatu. Front. Clim. 6:1345258. doi: 10.3389/fclim.2024.1345258

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Rumbach, A., Makarewicz, C., and Németh, J. (2016). The importance of place in early disaster recovery: a case study of the 2013 Colorado floods. J. Environ. Plan. Manag. 59, 2045–2063. doi: 10.1080/09640568.2015.1116981

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Rupp-Armstrong, S., and Nicholls, R. J. (2007). Coastal and estuarine retreat: a comparison of the application of managed realignment in England and Germany. J. Coast. Res. 236, 1418–1430. doi: 10.2112/04-0426.1

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Scannell, L., and Gifford, R. (2010). Defining place attachment: a tripartite organizing framework. J. Environ. Psychol. 30, 1–10. doi: 10.1016/j.jenvp.2009.09.006

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Seamon, D. (2014). “Place attachment and phenomenology. The synergistic dynamism of place” in Place attachment: Advances in theory, methods, and applications. eds. L. Manzo and P. Devine-Wright (New York: Routledge), 11–22.

Google Scholar

Seeteram, N. A., Ash, K., Sanders, B. F., Schubert, J. E., and Mach, K. J. (2023). Modes of climate mobility under sea-level rise. Environ. Res. Lett. 18:114015. doi: 10.1088/1748-9326/acfe22

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Sengupta, A., and Samanta, G. (2022). Understanding immobility of a highly vulnerable coastal village in the Indian Sundarban. Reg. Environ. Chang. 22:90. doi: 10.1007/s10113-022-01931-1

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Sheller, M., and Urry, J. (2006). The New Mobilities paradigm. Environ. Plan. Econ. Space 38, 207–226. doi: 10.1068/a37268

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Siders, A. R. (2019). Managed retreat in the United States. One Earth 1, 216–225. doi: 10.1016/j.oneear.2019.09.008

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Silver, A., and Grek-Martin, J. (2015). “Now we understand what community really means”: Reconceptualizing the role of sense of place in the disaster recovery process. J. Environ. Psychol. 42, 32–41. doi: 10.1016/j.jenvp.2015.01.004

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Simms, J. R. Z., Waller, H. L., Brunet, C., and Jenkins, P. (2021). The long goodbye on a disappearing, ancestral island: a just retreat from isle de Jean Charles. J. Environ. Stud. Sci. 11, 316–328. doi: 10.1007/s13412-021-00682-5

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Simpson, N. P., Mach, K. J., Tebboth, M. G. L., Gilmore, E. A., Siders, A. R., Holden, P., et al. (2024). Research priorities for climate mobility. One Earth 7, 589–607. doi: 10.1016/j.oneear.2024.02.002

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Solecki, W., and Friedman, E. (2021). At the Water’s edge: coastal settlement, transformative adaptation, and well-being in an era of dynamic climate risk. Annu. Rev. Public Health 42, 211–232. doi: 10.1146/annurev-publhealth-090419-102302

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Stedman, R. C. (2003). Is it really just a social construction?: the contribution of the physical environment to sense of place. Soc. Nat. Resour. 16, 671–685. doi: 10.1080/08941920309189

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Steimanis, I., Mayer, M., and Vollan, B. (2021). Why do people persist in sea-level rise threatened coastal regions? Empirical evidence on risk aversion and place attachment. Clim. Risk Manag. 34:100377. doi: 10.1016/j.crm.2021.100377

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Stokols, D., Shumaker, S. A., and Martinez, J. (1983). Residential mobility and personal well-being. J. Environ. Psychol. 3, 5–19. doi: 10.1016/S0272-4944(83)80018-8

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Sullivan, D., and Young, I. F. (2020). Place attachment style as a predictor of responses to the environmental threat of water contamination. Environ. Behav. 52, 3–32. doi: 10.1177/0013916518786766

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Swapan, M. S. H., and Sadeque, S. (2021). Place attachment in natural hazard-prone areas and decision to relocate: research review and agenda for developing countries. Int. J. Disaster Risk Reduct. 52:101937. doi: 10.1016/j.ijdrr.2020.101937

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Tadgell, A., Doberstein, B., and Mortsch, L. (2018). Principles for climate-related resettlement of informal settlements in less developed nations: a review of resettlement literature and institutional guidelines. Clim. Dev. 10, 102–115. doi: 10.1080/17565529.2017.1291401

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Thaler, T. (2021). Just retreat—how different countries deal with it: examples from Austria and England. J. Environ. Stud. Sci. 11, 412–419. doi: 10.1007/s13412-021-00694-1

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Thistlethwaite, J., Henstra, D., Brown, C., and Scott, D. (2018). How flood experience and risk perception influences protective actions and Behaviours among Canadian homeowners. Environ. Manag. 61, 197–208. doi: 10.1007/s00267-017-0969-2

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Trentelman, C. K. (2009). Place attachment and community attachment: a primer grounded in the lived experience of a community sociologist. Soc. Nat. Resour. 22, 191–210. doi: 10.1080/08941920802191712

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Tschakert, P., Barnett, J., Ellis, N., Lawrence, C., Tuana, N., New, M., et al. (2017). Climate change and loss, as if people mattered: values, places, and experiences. WIREs Clim. Change 8:e476. doi: 10.1002/wcc.476

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Tuan, Y.-F. (1974). Topophilia: A study of environmental perceptions, attitudes, and values. New Jersey: Prentice-Hill.

Google Scholar

Tubridy, F., Scott, M., and Lennon, M. (2021). Managed retreat in response to flooding: lessons from the past for contemporary climate change adaptation. Plan. Perspect. 36, 1249–1268. doi: 10.1080/02665433.2021.1939115

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

UN OCHA, (2004). Guiding principles on internal displacement. Geneva: United Nations.

Google Scholar

UNFCCC (2010). Report of the conference of the parties on its 16th session (no. FCCC/CP/2010/7/add.1). Cancun, Mexico: UNFCCC.

Google Scholar

UNHCR, (2015). Guidance on protecting people from disaster and environmental change through planned relocation. UNHCR. Available at: https://www.refworld.org/policy/opguidance/unhcr/2015/en/117656

Google Scholar

Upadhyay, H., Vinke, K., and Weisz, H. (2024). “We are still here” climate change, gender and immobility in highly mobile Himalayan communities. Clim. Dev. 16, 443–457. doi: 10.1080/17565529.2023.2230176

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Van Manen, M. (1990). Researching lived experience human science for an action sensitive pedagogy, SUNY series in the philosophy of education. Albany, NY: State University of New York Press.

Google Scholar

Van Veelen, B., and Haggett, C. (2017). Uncommon ground: the role of different place attachments in explaining community renewable energy projects. Sociol. Rural. 57, 533–554. doi: 10.1111/soru.12128

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Vanclay, F. (2017). Project-induced displacement and resettlement: from impoverishment risks to an opportunity for development? Impact assess. Proj. Apprais. 35, 3–21. doi: 10.1080/14615517.2017.1278671

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Vince, G. (2022). Nomad Century: How to Survive the Climate Upheaval. London: Penguin Books.

Google Scholar

Whittle, R., Medd, W., Mort, M., Deeming, H., Walker, M., Twigger-Ross, C., et al. (2014). “Placing the flood recovery process” in Displaced heritage. Responses to disaster, trauma and loss. eds. I. Convery, G. Corsane, and P. Davis (Woodbridge, Suffolk: Boydell and Brewer Limited), 199–206. doi: 10.1017/9781782044109.020

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Williams, D. (2014a). Making sense of ‘place’: reflections on pluralism and positionality in place research. Landsc. Urban Plan. 131, 74–82. doi: 10.1016/j.landurbplan.2014.08.002

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Williams, D. (2014b). “Beyond the commodity metaphor,” revisited: some methodological reflections on place attachment research” in Place attachment. Advances in theory, methods, and applications. 1st ed. eds. L. Manzo and P. Devine-Wright (Abingdon, Oxon: Routledge), 89–99.

Google Scholar

Williams, D., and Miller, B. (2024). “From roots to rhizomes: place, transitions, translocality in a less stationary world” in Opening windows: Embracing New perspectives and practices in natural resource social sciences. eds. K. Sherren, G. Thondhlana, and D. Jackson-Smith (Denver: University of Colorado Press).

Google Scholar

Williams, D., and Miller, B. (2020). “Metatheoretical moments in place attachment research: seeking clarity in diversity” in Place attachment: Advances in theory, methods, and applications. 2nd. eds. L. Manzo and P. Devine-Wright (Abingdon, Oxon: Routledge), 12–28.

Google Scholar

Wilmsen, B., and Webber, M. (2015). What can we learn from the practice of development-forced displacement and resettlement for organised resettlements in response to climate change? Geoforum 58, 76–85. doi: 10.1016/j.geoforum.2014.10.016

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Winstanley, A., Hepi, M., and Wood, D. (2015). Resilience? Contested meanings and experiences in post-disaster Christchurch, New Zealand. Kōtuitui N. Z. J. Soc. Sci. 10, 126–134. doi: 10.1080/1177083X.2015.1066402

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Woodhall-Melnik, J., and Grogan, C. (2019). Perceptions of mental health and wellbeing following residential displacement and damage from the 2018 St. John River flood. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 16:4174. doi: 10.3390/ijerph16214174

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Woodhall-Melnik, J., and Weissman, E. P. (2023). Living with disaster: exploring complex decisions to stay in or leave flood prone areas. Hous. Stud. 38, 747–769. doi: 10.1080/02673037.2021.1900794

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Yee, M., Piggott-McKellar, A. E., McMichael, C., and McNamara, K. E. (2022). Climate change, voluntary immobility, and place-belongingness: insights from Togoru. Fiji. Climate 10:46. doi: 10.3390/cli10030046

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Yi, H., and Yang, J. (2014). Research trends of post disaster reconstruction: the past and the future. Habitat Int. 42, 21–29. doi: 10.1016/j.habitatint.2013.10.005

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Zheng, C., Zhang, J., Yongrui, G., Zhang, Y., and Qian, L. (2019). Disruption and reestablishment of place attachment after large-scale disasters: the role of perceived risk, negative emotions, and coping. Int. J. Disaster Risk Reduct. 40:101273. doi: 10.1016/j.ijdrr.2019.101273

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: climate adaptation, climate migration, disaster recovery, forced relocation, managed retreat, mobility, place attachment, sense of place

Citation: Willcocks-Musselman R, Baird J, Foster K, Woodhall-Melnik J and Sherren K (2025) Finding mobility in place attachment research: lessons for managed retreat. Front. Clim. 7:1514408. doi: 10.3389/fclim.2025.1514408

Received: 20 October 2024; Accepted: 06 January 2025;
Published: 11 February 2025.

Edited by:

Charles Krishna Huyck, ImageCat, United States

Reviewed by:

Bina Desai, Independent researcher, Geneva, Switzerland
Rebecca Jane McConnell, Queen’s University Belfast, United Kingdom

Copyright © 2025 Willcocks-Musselman, Baird, Foster, Woodhall-Melnik and Sherren. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Robin Willcocks Musselman, cndpbGxjb2NAZGFsLmNh

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

Research integrity at Frontiers

Man ultramarathon runner in the mountains he trains at sunset

94% of researchers rate our articles as excellent or good

Learn more about the work of our research integrity team to safeguard the quality of each article we publish.


Find out more