- 1Research Institute for Humanity and Nature, Kyoto, Japan
- 2Faculty of Information Networking for Innovation and Design, Toyo University, Tokyo, Japan
- 3Department of Sustainability Studies, Faculty of Engineering, Musashino University, Tokyo, Japan
- 4Faculty of Environmental Studies, Tokyo City University, Yokohama, Japan
- 5School of Human Science and Environment, University of Hyogo, Himeji, Japan
Individual behaviors of citizens are important for implementing the necessary measures for adapting to climate change. However, studies on the adaptive behaviors of ordinary citizens are limited. Therefore, in this study, we developed behavioral models to understand individual behaviors for adaption to and mitigation of climate change. We compared four behavior groups, namely, G1, individual mitigation; G2, individual adaptive; G3, long-term adaptive; and G4, solar-energy system installation behaviors. Following a questionnaire survey for the four behavioral groups, behavioral models were developed using structural equation modeling, which considered psychological factors along with selected attitudes, perceived effectiveness, norms, benefits, practicality, and intention. Furthermore, we considered the recognition and attribution of local climate change as key factors for adaptation behaviors toward local climate change. The behaviors of citizens in four prefectures in Japan, namely, Nagano, Tokyo, Saitama, and Kanagawa were considered. Among them, Nagano Prefecture, wherein the implementation of adaptation measures was at a low level while that of mitigation measures was at a high level, showed a differing trend. In the behavioral models, the recognition of local climate change directly affected the behavior of citizens in the individual adaptive behavior group. In both the individual adaptive and long-term adaptive behavior groups, the impact of benefits was substantial, with significant differences across local areas. Thus, effective adaptation strategies might involve tangible and familiar examples demonstrating the consequences of behavior, thus enhancing citizens' behavior evaluation perception and fostering its acceptance as a desirable behavior. Tailoring adaptation strategies to specific local contexts might also be necessity. To encourage citizens to adapt to climate change and its impacts, we propose two-way efforts, instead of providing one-way information, by supporting citizens to think about self-help and mutual help effectively. Our study can serve as a reference for future studies focusing on citizen behavior about climate change mitigation and enhance their adaptation for locally implemented strategies.
1 Introduction
To address climate change, focusing on measures that deal with the impacts and risks in different regions is as important as developing and adopting mitigation measures for greenhouse gas emission reduction. Hence, implementing both measures at relevant scales is important (Adger et al., 2005; Kirshen et al., 2008; Hijioka et al., 2016; IPCC, 2022).
IPCC (2022) defined adaptation in human systems as “the process of adjustment to actual or expected climate and its effects in order to moderate harm or exploit beneficial opportunities” and stated that “adaptation planning in human systems generally entails a process of iterative risk management.” As climate-related hazards increase in frequency and severity, disaster risk reduction is required at a greater level than ever before. The global priority of climate change adaptation and risk reduction for sustainable development has been recognized at several international conventions, including the Sendai Framework for Disaster Risk Reduction.
To adapt to climate change systematically and comprehensively, the Cabinet of Japan formulated and enacted the first National Plan for Adaptation to the Impacts of Climate Change, as part of the adaptation measures implemented in Japan in November 2015. The plan consisted of five basic strategies: (1) mainstreaming the idea of “adaptation” into government policies, (2) enhancing scientific works and findings, (3) promoting understanding and cooperation among citizens by sharing and providing information about risks related to climate change, (4) supporting adaptation measures in local regions, and (5) developing and enhancing international cooperation and contribution (Cabinet of Japan, 2015). The policy framework provided by the plan was subsequently revised in 2018, 2021, and 2023, per the Climate Change Adaptation Act (Act No. 50 of 2018, Government of Japan, 2018). The Climate Change Adaptation Act enforced adaptation measures in any given region. Following this act, local governments must make efforts to formulate adaptation plans and develop novel systems for collecting and providing data related to climate change.
The measures implemented for adapting to climate change were categorized into three levels: protection (preventive measures to avoid the spread of the negative effects of climate change), adaptation (measures to reduce the negative effects of climate change by adjusting measures based on what has occurred), and transformation (drastic changes to deal with the negative effects to alter their impact before they occur). Currently, Japan is focusing on the protection measures that can be applied to improve the existing facilities and possible response measures responses to different missions (Shirai et al., 2014; Baba et al., 2017). In general, national and local governments focus on enhancing tangible elements related to public assistance; however, self and mutual help have not been thoroughly explored as measures related to climate change.
Climate change adaptation behaviors at the individual level, including climate-related hazard risk reduction behaviors, have been typified by reducing the negative impacts associated with climate change. Therefore, natural disaster risk reduction measures at the individual and household levels can contribute to climate change adaptation. Integrated risk management has also been proposed to reduce the risk of community residents under climate change (Wouter Botzen et al., 2019). Several examples of specific disaster risk behaviors are reported (Grothmann and Reusswig, 2006); however, measures related to climate change adaptive behavior, often address only specific issues, such as agricultural measures for farmers (Arbuckle et al., 2015; Dang et al., 2018). Although research on adaptation in policies and governance systems is underway, only a few studies have comprehensively addressed the adaptive behavior of individuals from the perspective of ordinary citizens (van Valkengoed and Steg, 2019). Most of the current mitigation measures, including government public campaigns, focus on individual behavior. A significant portion of the public is highly interested in such efforts because they recognize that environment-friendly behavior can be beneficial (Stern, 2000; Kollmuss and Agyeman, 2002). Several examples of risk reduction behaviors have been noted when preparing for disasters, and the relationship between risk perception and individual perceptions and behaviors has also been reported (Bubeck et al., 2012; Kellens et al., 2013; Wachinger et al., 2013). The importance of developing self-help behaviors implemented at the individual level for self-protection and community support over the medium to long term as part of disaster preparedness has been highlighted. These studies suggested that individual behavioral choices are important for the effective implementation of adaptation measures (van Valkengoed and Steg, 2019).
This poses the following question: Can people's psychological perceptions explain individual behaviors in adaptation and mitigation measures and their pro-environmental behavior? The relationship between psychological factors and behavior has been reported for pro-environmental behavior practiced by citizens in their daily lives, and psychological models of various behaviors have also been reported. A well-known behavioral model that has been widely applied in its original or derived forms is the theory of planned behavior (TPB). It was originally designed to explain human behavior and not just their pro-environmental behavior. The theory of reasoned action (TRA) was originally proposed by Ajzen and Fishbein (1975, 1980) and contains three general constructs: behavioral intention, attitude toward the behavior, and subjective norms. The TRA is a simple model in which a person would act if he or she had the intention of doing so; it does not evaluate the surrounding circumstances. To address the limitations of the TRA, Ajzen (1991) introduced a modified model, TPB, in which perceived behavioral control (PBC) was added as a new component. The PBC considered other external factors, such as the actor's perceived ability to do what he or she could do, their knowledge of the issue, and time availability. The PBC has been expanded to incorporate more specific evaluation factors, such as cost-benefit and feasibility evaluations or practicality of the behavior. Nevertheless, TPB has been applied to explain several human behaviors, including pro-environmental behavior (Davies et al., 2002), and some examples of TPB application to explain adaptive behaviors have been put forth effectively (Zhang et al., 2020; Zaremohzzabieh et al., 2021). Furthermore, as pro-environmental behavior is a type of altruism, different models of altruistic behavior have also been applied. Schwartz (1977) proposed an altruistic behavior model that included four constructs: personal and social norms, awareness of consequences, and denial of responsibility. The types of social norms, e.g., focusing on the influence of subjective norms (the expectations of others around them), injunctive norms (the perceptions of social morals or social pressure), and descriptive norms (the perceptions of the behavior of others around them), have also been subjected to investigation, owing to the importance of the influence of social norms (Cialdini et al., 1990; Thøgersen, 2006).
The public's awareness on the impacts of climate change and the related adaptation measures is lesser than that on other environmental issues (Hulme, 2009). In general, citizens in developed countries find it difficult to develop mitigation and adaptation strategies as responses because the threats of climate change generally occur away from their locations (Lorenzoni and Pidgeon, 2006; O'Neill and Nicholson-Cole, 2009). The more the people can feel and experience the impacts caused by (or believe that the impacts are caused by) climate change in their local region, the more they can perceive climate change issues as being relevant to them and thus take responsive actions (Blennow et al., 2012). This portrays a strong bias that is dependent on an individual's current situation (Brügger et al., 2015). Adger et al. (2013) suggested that culture and values should be considered when analyzing adaptation behaviors, thus indicating the need for further studies that consider different cultures.
Although a relationship has been established between the perception of climate change and the mitigation and adaptation behaviors of the citizens of Japan (Baba et al., 2011; Shirai et al., 2015), detailed investigations, especially in terms of influential psychological factors, are important for analyzing adaptive behavior as an individual's behavioral choice and exploring the differences between adaptive and mitigation behaviors. The enhancement of intangible elements that support self and mutual help can play a major role in the implementation of local adaptation measures. Therefore, studies and analyses that can promote these measures are of great significance.
In this study, we developed models capable of examining the behavior of individuals for adaption to and mitigation of climate change. We conducted questionnaire surveys to ascertain psychological perceptions and to analyze individual behaviors. This approach facilitated an exploration of the factors that can work as both the barriers and accelerators of the behaviors. The objective was to derive insights based on comparative research results across local areas to aid in the development of policy measures, foster individual adaptive and mitigation behavior, and encourage collaborative efforts within local communities. Furthermore, we conducted comparative analyses of adaptation and mitigation behavior models to offer recommendations on specific areas requiring distinct or integrated implementation approaches, drawing upon previous findings that facilitate mitigation behavior.
2 Materials and methods
2.1 Hypothetical model and factors
The hypothetical structure of the psychological model was based on the TPB, with three major processes as the basic structure: attitudes toward the behavior, norms, and personal evaluation of the behavior (including the concept of PBC). Additionally, the recognition of local climate change and perceptions regarding attribution of local climate change were assumed to determine specific attitudes such as general attitudinal concern. This is depicted in Figure 1; for example, evaluations of “benefits” and “practicality,” lead to intentions, which then, lead to the practice of the behavior. To contribute to specific future measures for the promotion of such behavior by local governments and other entities, identifying the influence of specific psychological factors that promote or prevent behavior is important. Therefore, we adopted detailed psychological factors that comprise the three basic processes by subdividing them further into more specific factors as in a previous study of construction of psychological models of pro-environmental behavior (Aoki et al., 2013).
Attitude toward the behavior was divided into “attitude” and “perceived effectiveness.” We focused on the “perceived effectiveness” as an important factor for socially desirable behaviors, such as pro-environmental behavior (Hirose, 1994, 2015). The perceived effectiveness also provided an aspect of knowledge about pro-environmental behavior (Kaiser and Fuhrer, 2003). Therefore, we decided to include it as a behavior toward the socially desirable goal of climate change mitigation and adaptation, as a distinct entity as opposed to using only the attitude toward behavior.
Norms included both social and personal norms. Social norms were further divided into “injunctive and descriptive norms,” and indices of “personal norms” were also included. The Ajzen (1991) TPB used subjective norms as determinants of how a person perceives social norms. However, within social norms, injunctive and descriptive norms are factors that have different effects based on distinct components (Cialdini et al., 1990; Kallgren et al., 2000; Rivis and Sheeran, 2003). Furthermore, the process of Schwartz (1977)'s norm-activation model, in which social norms are internalized as personal norms that lead to behavior, has been supported. Hence, injunctive norm was set as an antecedent to personal norm.
PBC embodied “benefits” and “practicality” as personal evaluation of the behavior to determine how the evaluation of the consequences of a behavior, such as costs and benefits evaluation, could be the accelerator of the behavior (Lee et al., 2013) and how the practicality of the behavior may be the barrier to practicing it (Hirose, 1994, 2015; Blake, 1999). Among the evaluations of benefits that could have a strong influence on adaptation measures (Smit and Pilifosova, 2003), “safety” was classified as an independent indicator.
2.2 Study area
Four prefectures in Japan (Nagano, Tokyo, Saitama, and Kanagawa), each known for its unique local characteristics, were chosen as the target areas. Intense adaptation measures have been applied in these subnational regions; with highly aggressive adaptation measures in Nagano and Saitama prefectures. Nagano Prefecture is a cool region; therefore, the impact of rising temperatures on the agricultural sector in the region is of great concern. In addition, Nagano Prefecture is more advanced than the other prefectures, in terms of the positive behavior of its citizens toward the installation of solar energy systems and their pro-environmental perceptions (Aoki et al., 2010). Saitama Prefecture experiences several extremely hot days, and in the Tokyo Metropolitan Area, heatstroke and crop damage occur frequently because of high temperatures. In Tokyo and Kanagawa prefectures (in the metropolitan area), discussions on adaptation measures have been progressing rapidly. These areas were selected to ensure applicability to other local regions that experience comparable effects owing to climate change. Figure 2 is the map showing the targeted four prefectures. Figure 3 shows the current population composition by gender and age group in the four prefectures based on the 2020 Population Census.
2.3 Questionnaire survey: details and design
We performed an online questionnaire survey; online surveys have become a popular method for collecting data in Japan because it helps to reach out to more respondents (Kurisu and Bortoleto, 2011). We designed the questionnaire survey and commissioned a research company to implement the survey. The questionnaire survey is completed online and does not involve any intervention or invasion of the respondents. Therefore, we obtained the respondents' consent online only, following the prescribed criteria set forth by our research institution. The survey was conducted from May 29, 2014 to June 2, 2014. Men and women between the ages of 20 and 60 living in the target areas were equally assigned to 10 groups, comprising of five age groups and two gender groups. To ensure equal consideration of the perceptions of different age-sex groups, we utilized an allocation method of equal distribution by age-sex rather than relying on complete randomness or an allocation method that mirrors the actual age-sex composition of the area.
The questionnaire consisted of five parts: (1) the psychological perceptions of the target behavior groups (Table 1), (2) the practice rates of the target behaviors (Table 2), (3) the recognition of local climate change at the local scale, (4) perceptions regarding the cause of local climate-change effects, and (5) personal attributes. The next four paragraphs describe (1) through (4) in detail and the questionnaire was submitted as Supplementary material.
Table 1. List of indicators of psychological perceptions considered in the behavioral models developed in this study.
Table 2. Results of analysis of variance (ANOVA) for the degree of behavior practice for the four prefectures considered in this study (as a factor).
Table 1 portrays the indicators of the psychological perceptions of the target behavior group. With respect to psychological indicators, we considered “attitude,” “perceived effectiveness,” “social norms (injunctive and descriptive norms),” “personal norms,” “benefits,” and “practicality” as the main factors. The factors were chosen based on previous studies wherein psychological models that expanded the TPB of pro-environmental behaviors and influenced the practice of these behaviors were constructed (Aoki et al., 2013; Kurisu, 2015). We used different descriptions for the indicators of the perceived effectiveness factor, along with the indicators of mitigation and adaptation behaviors. Each indicator was rated on a 6-point scale, ranging from “strongly agree” to “strongly disagree,” and the respondents were asked about their perceptions of each of the four behavior groups: G1, individual mitigation behavior group; G2, individual adaptive behavior group; G3, long-term adaptive behavior group, and G4, group that portrayed readiness to install solar energy systems.
Table 2 shows the classification of the 12 behaviors that individuals could choose into four behavior groups. Respondents were asked to rate the degree of behavioral practice of each of the B1–B12 behaviors on a 6-point scale. The target adaptive behavior consisted of two types (with each portraying four distinct behaviors): one entailed being inclined to carry out information acquisition and daily preparations as self-help behaviors to protect themselves (portraying G2) and the second included four drastic countermeasure behaviors (while considering medium- to long-term preparations and mutual help for the community or G3). For mitigation behaviors, three daily pro-environmental behaviors, such as saving electricity and reducing the use of plastic bags, were considered. In addition, installation renewable energy equipment, such as solar energy systems or G4 were introduced as a mitigation measure and as an adaptive measure by storing for future use as an emergency energy supply.
Furthermore, we investigated their awareness of local climate change through their experience living in the target area. “Perception of what is going on in their surroundings” can have a particularly large impact on adaptation behaviors (Blennow et al., 2012). Local climate change was defined as the changes that respondents experience in their daily lives, and the local administrative regions, which are closely related to their daily lives, were taken as the spatial extent of the changes. The respondents were asked whether or not they felt the following five specific local changes as physical impacts, without considering whether or not the changes themselves were caused by climate change. The respondents were asked about each of the following five items: “Comparing your experience of the past few years with that 10 years ago, do you recognize any changes in the climate in your area?” The options in the questionnaire were as follows: (1) higher temperatures in summer and an increase in the summer, mid-summer, and extremely hot days and hot nights; (2) higher winter temperatures and fewer winter and mid-winter days; (3) increase in localized heavy rains, torrential rains, typhoons, etc.; (4) changes in snowfall (increase or decrease in snow cover or changes in snow quality); and (5) changes in the sense of seasons (shortening of the spring and autumn periods and an ambiguity of the four seasons).
Subsequently, the respondents were asked, “Do you think these types of local climate-change effects are caused by global warming due to an increase in the emissions of carbon dioxide and other substances?” for the same five items, to examine their recognition of effects caused by global warming and climate change. This is because the belief or fear of the impact of climate change and regional climate may be viewed as two different things (Hulme, 2009). In the Japanese questionnaire, we used the terms “global warming” and “climate change” both, rather than using “climate change” alone, because the term “global warming” is more recognized by the general public and indicates the difference between global and local changes. Both questions were answered on a 6-point scale, ranging from “strongly agree” to “strongly disagree.”
2.4 Statistical analysis
Based on the results of the questionnaire survey, we analyzed and constructed behavioral models by structural equation modeling (SEM), using the IBM SPSS Amos ver.24 software. An exploratory factor analysis was also performed, to set the psychological factors as the latent variables for each observation index to better fit the responses rather than confirming them with these hypothetical factors. As SEM could simultaneously realize factor and path analyses, it could visually and quantitatively portray plausible complex relationships, while including factors that were not directly observable in the background of observed indicators and those indicating the relationships among those factors. This statistical modeling technique provides a precise measurement and can thus be used to test hypotheses models (Byrne, 2001; Ullman and Bentler, 2012). In addition, to consider the regional differences between the four prefectures, we conducted a simultaneous analysis of multiple groups, using the four prefectures as variables. Although the relationships between the factors were the same (the model structure remained unchanged), the influences between the factors (magnitude of path coefficient values) were assumed to change depending on the group (prefectures, in this case).
We also applied the modified index and goodness-of-fit index (GFI) to develop and improve behavioral models. Based on the previous findings on behavior studies and the interpretability of the responses, we modified the arrangement and direction of the paths and added and deleted the covariance repeatedly; finally, a model with the same factor structure but a more plausible fit was selected as the preferred model. This methodology followed those of Ohtomo and Hirose (2007) and Bortoleto et al. (2012). Table 3 presents the model fit indices and formulae. The GFI, adjusted GFI (AGFI), and comparative fit index (CFI) are GFIs; the closer was the value of the index to 1, the better was the fit. Values of GFI and CFI higher than 0.9 suggested a good fit, while values of AGFI higher than 0.8 also suggested a good fit. The root mean square error of approximation (RMSEA) is also a GFI; the smaller was the value of RMSEA, the better was the fit. The values of RMSEA lower than 0.05 indicate a good fit. The χ2-value indicates the result of the χ2-test and is a threshold value of significance; the model is not rejected when the value is smaller than the threshold (Steiger, 1990; Byrne, 2001). A GFI of 0.9 was considered a good fit value; as model complexity increases, the GFI decreases (Schermelleh-Engel et al., 2003). Therefore, a need to increase the number of observed variables does not lead to the rejection of a model only because its GFI value is < 0.9 (Toyoda, 1998).
3 Results
3.1 Practice rates of behavior groups
After the questionnaire survey, we received a total of 2,790 valid responses: 930 responses from Nagano Prefecture and 620 responses from each of Tokyo, Saitama, and Kanagawa prefectures. For each target prefecture, we obtained almost the same number of responses from the 10 groups of men and women, each in their 20–60's. All results thereafter were tabulated and analyzed separately for the four prefectures.
Within the 6-point scale used for analyzing the practice rate of behaviors for each target area, the response rate combining up to three scales on the affirmative side (“strongly agree,” “agree,” and “somewhat agree”) was considered as the practice rate. The practice rates for each behavior group were summarized as follows: the practice rate was 73–90% for three individual mitigation behaviors, 53–72% for four individual adaptive behaviors, 33–48% for four long-term adaptive behaviors, and 22% for the installation of the solar-energy system (planned). The respondents who answered that they strongly agreed with the item “Physically impossible to install due to various reasons, such as rental housing,” were excluded from the analysis. This was done to explore the differences in the perceptions of installation among only those who could take relevant actions. In the subsequent analysis, n = 2,230 was used only for the behavior of installing solar-energy systems, and the practice rate of the installation (and plans for installation) was 28%. However, for planned installation, the respondents were asked for “concrete plans for installation,” and we assumed that the installation process had already begun. This is because the installation process of different systems may differ, owing to the timeframe required between the preparation and actual installation, in addition to the time required for unique selections and fittings on site.
In terms of the differences between the age- and gender-based groups, the older-age group [based on multiple comparisons of Tukey's honestly significant difference (HSD) test, 50/60's > 20/30's; p < 0.01] and the female group portrayed higher practice rates [t-test; p < 0.01] for G1 (individual mitigation behavior group), especially those related to waste management. The same tendency has been observed previously for waste management and pro-environmental behaviors (Barr, 2003; Lee et al., 2013). Although similar trends were observed for G2 (individual adaptive behavior group), no noticeable difference was observed in the G3 (long-term adaptive behavior) or G4 (solar-energy system installation). There were significant differences in the G1 or pro-environmental behaviors of the age- and gender-based groups but no significant differences in adaptive behaviors and energy system installation. In addition, psychological factors have been demonstrated to influence pro-environmental behavior more than socio-demographic factors (Hines et al., 1987; Aoki et al., 2012). Therefore, to better elucidate the outcomes for each specific area, we concentrated on illustrating the relationship between psychological attitudes and behaviors, without controlling for age and gender factors in subsequent analysis.
3.2 Regional and behavioral differences
We conducted an analysis of variance (ANOVA), while considering the prefectures as a factor, to determine whether there were regional differences in the practice rates of the behaviors of the citizens of the four prefectures. As shown in Table 2, the results indicate significant regional differences [df = (3, 2,786); p < 0.05] in six behaviors: (G1) “B1. Refuse plastic bags and excessive packaging,” (G2) three behaviors of the individual adaptive behavior group (excluding B7), (G3) “B8. Considering that the impact will become serious in the future, discuss with family and make preparations, such as improving housing and securing evacuation routes,” and (G4) “B12. Have/plan to install renewable energy equipment, such as solar power generation and solar thermal systems (solar hot water supply, etc.), at home.” Further analysis was conducted to consider what local conditions might be contributing to these differences. Table 4 portrays the prefectures that demonstrated significant differences, based on multiple comparisons carried out using Tukey's HSD test. Significant differences were observed in the same six behaviors as in the previous analysis. For reference, the practice rate is also shown in the table. Regarding B1, the practice rate in Nagano Prefecture was significantly higher than that in Kanagawa Prefecture (p < 0.05). The highest practice rate was observed in Saitama Prefecture and the lowest in Tokyo, but the results differed because the practice rate was a reference value based only on the two values of practicing the behavior or not. For B12, the practice rate in Nagano was significantly higher than that in Tokyo and Kanagawa Prefectures (p < 0.01). The results of Nagano Prefecture's higher behavior practice rate in B1, a mitigating individual behavior related to shopping, and in B12, the installation of energy systems, were consistent with previous findings. Aoki et al. (2010) discussed regional conditions, including socioeconomic conditions such as larger residential areas, higher rates of private cars and marriages than in urban centers, municipal subsidy programs, differences in weather conditions, and regional character or traits based on local cultural identity in Japan (e.g., the regional character of Nagano's residents is often described as hardworking and resilient, although there is no clear evidence to support this). They reported that the higher practice rate in Nagano than in other prefectures was due to factors that cannot be explained solely by socioeconomic statistics and the availability of local government subsidy measures. However, for the three individual adaptive behaviors (B4, B5, and B6) and B8 within the long-term adaptive behaviors, the practice rates in Nagano Prefecture were significantly lower than that in Saitama Prefecture (p < 0.01). Among adaptive behaviors, regional differences were observed in the practice rates of four behaviors related to disaster preparedness, with Saitama Prefecture having a higher rate and Nagano Prefecture having a lower rate. Considering the regional conditions in Japan related to these differences, behaviors to cope with extreme heat may have a significant impact. In Saitama Prefecture, which is one of the regions that often experiences the hottest days, the practice rate of behavior may have been higher because of the frequency of exposure to news concerning extreme heat, which is reported as extraordinary weather. As for heat, the reason may be that in Nagano Prefecture, which is originally a cold region, although adverse effects on agriculture and some cultural events are seen due to this, daily discomfort and damage within proximity to oneself, such as heat stroke, that is visible to everyone in general, are rarely seen. Of the subjects addressed as adaptive behaviors, preparedness for water damage may not have had as great an influence on differences as the heatstroke, since it is not a behavior that is repeated in daily life. This finding that behaviors showing regional differences were most represented in G2 supports the hypothesis that regional differences must be taken more into account in adaptive measures rather than in mitigation measures.
Table 4. Results of multiple comparisons between the prefectures, with significant differences in behavior practice rates.
3.3 Psychological models of behaviors
First, we conducted an exploratory factor analysis to examine the factor structure. As a result, we used “attitude” and “intention” as the same factor (referred to as “attitude and intention”), assuming that people who perceive a behavior as a good thing (attitude) would also intend to implement it (intention). We extended and modified TPB to search for a model that better fits the target behaviors. Hence, attitudes and intentions, which are theoretically similar concepts as internal psychological factors for individuals, were treated as a single factor. Since the early researches, environmental attitude has been recognized to have significant influence on pro-environment behaviors (Van Liere and Dunlap, 1980; Hines et al., 1987). Simultaneously, there is a gap between environmental attitudes and pro-environmental behavior and attitudes and concerns are mediated by intentions before shaping into behavior, as well as the influence of other external factors (Bamberg and Möser, 2007; Gifford and Nilsson, 2014). Thus, attitudes and intentions have played similar roles as individuals' internal psychological factors that antecedent behavior, including a condition-dependent gap between them and behavior (Lord et al., 1984). In this survey, both attitude and intention are asked as cognitions of the specific target behaviors, rather than as attitudes toward the environment or climate change in general. For this reason, it was assumed to not have attitude as antecedents and steps leading to behavior via behavioral intentions but both could be viewed as psychological factors that indicated supportive or negative aspects of the target in the individual's internal beliefs toward the specific behavior. Regarding the recognition of local climate change in the target local area, several respondents expressed that they felt changes in the same manner for all five items. The most common response was “1. High temperatures in summer” (89%), and the least common was “2. High temperatures in winter” (56%). Similarly, several respondents answered affirmatively to the question of whether these types of climate-change effects were caused by global warming; the responses were similar (< 75% for all items). This suggests that the responses were not about individual perceptions and represented a collective perception of climate change in the target local area; this was true even for the perceptions regarding whether the change was caused by global warming. Therefore, we incorporated these perceptions into all psychological models collectively as the factors of “recognition of local climate change” and “attribution of local climate change.” In addition, installing solar-energy systems in each household was considered an effective mitigation measure, as well as an adaptation measure, because the systems could also serve as power sources in emergencies. For this reason, we used an indicator to ask about the perceived effectiveness of mitigation and adaptation measures, but the patterns of the responses were almost the same. Therefore, in the model for G4, the indicators for the perceived effectiveness of both the mitigation and adaptation measures were incorporated as a single factor.
Second, we carried out the SEM, and a better-fitting model structure was constructed, with reference to the modification index and GFI. The main change from the hypothetical model was the establishment of a direct influence path from “recognition of local climate change” to “behavior.” The recognition of local climate change was assumed to be an antecedent of attitude, as it may be a cognition that corresponds to general concern or knowledge of environmental issues in the model of pro-environmental behavior. However, as indicated by the strong correlation between adaptive behavior and recognition of local climate change (Blennow et al., 2012), it was more plausible to set up a direct path. In addition, a path was set from “safety” (instead of “benefit”), to influence “perceived effectiveness” and “attitude and intention.” Being safe and secure was assumed to be a form of personal benefit as well as a type of cognition of the consequences of behavior, and thus as a cognition in the latter stage of the behavior model, as shown in Schwartz's (1977) and Stern's (2000) models involving norms. However, it was more appropriate to set it as a type of belief that is an antecedent of attitude, as in Ajzen and Fishbein (1975, 1980), who included it in their explanations even though it was not depicted as an explicit factor in TRA explanations. Furthermore, the path from “perceived effectiveness” to “attitude and intention” was not directly connected and portrayed a structure that led to “intention” and “behavior practice” through behavioral evaluations (e.g., “norms” and “benefits”). Perceived effectiveness was assumed to be an antecedent of more general attitudinal factors, based on the theoretical background that it is a type of attitude (Hirose, 1994) and a form of knowledge (Kaiser and Fuhrer, 2003). However, since more of the target behaviors in this study were not implemented than pro-environmental behavior, the factors in the behavior model were also more significant as inhibiting factors. For this reason, as Kollmuss and Agyeman (2002) constructed a model in which many barriers were made explicit, the model took the form of a model in which even with the initial perception of effectiveness, only those that transcended the many barriers to norms and benefits would behave. In other words, even if one perceives the effectiveness of a behavior, the many barriers of the behavior evaluation process inhibit the behavior and prevent it from being implemented. It goes through the psychological process of recognizing that the behavior is socially required to be practiced (injunctive norm), with the one should practicing it (personal norm), and many others also practicing it (descriptive norm), while bringing benefits to the individual (benefit evaluation) and being feasible and easy to practice (practicality evaluation).
Figures 4–7 portray the structure of each psychological model constructed in this study. The observed indicators and error and disturbance variables have been omitted from the figures. The psychological factors extracted from the indicators are shown by the ellipses. The coefficient values of paths are shown as standardized coefficients. This study emphasizes on the comparisons of the trends of the relationships between behaviors and psychological factors, rather than conducting direct comparisons between the coefficient values of models. Therefore, instead of unstandardized coefficients, we present standardized coefficients, with the relationships being connected by the paths in the models and are indicated by a number between 0 and 1. For simplicity, the coefficient values and R2 of the behaviors for each prefecture are shown separately in Table 5, and the coefficient values for Tokyo Prefecture are shown in Figures 4–7. A symbol is placed in the upper right corner of the coefficient values to indicate the p-value level to provide an understanding of the significance of the paths. The symbols are as follows: ***p < 0.001, **p < 0.01, *p < 0.05. While not considered significant at the 5% level,† for p < 0.10 is also shown for reference as well. The GFIs for each model are shown in Table 5 and the upper part of the figures. The GFIs show the model of G1 [ = 5,886.1, p < 0.01, GFI = 0.866, AGFI = 0.839, CFI = 0.930, RMSEA = 0.033] is better than other models. Because of the same structural constraints for conducting a simultaneous analysis of multiple groups and model complexity, not all indicators necessarily showed good fit, but one indicator in every model showed at least an acceptable criterion. In all three prefectures except Saitama, R2 of the behavior, is also highest in that of G1 (0.46–0.55). In Saitama Prefecture, R2 of G4 (0.52) is slightly higher than that of G1 (0.50).
Figure 4. Model of the “G1, individual mitigation behavior” group. ***p < 0.001, **p < 0.01, *p < 0.05, †p < 0.10. (a–f) Refer to the parts that are explained in the article. Numerical values are standardized coefficient values resulting from the analysis in Tokyo.
Figure 5. Model of the “G2, individual adaptive behavior” group. ***p < 0.001, **p < 0.01, *p < 0.05, †p < 0.10. (a–f) Refer to the parts that are explained in the article. Numerical values are standardized coefficient values resulting from the analysis in Tokyo.
Figure 6. Model of the “G3, long-term adaptive behavior” group. ***p < 0.001, **p < 0.01, *p < 0.05, †p < 0.10. (a–f) Refer to the parts that are explained in the article. Numerical values are standardized coefficient values resulting from the analysis in Tokyo.
Figure 7. Model of the “G4, solar-energy system installation” group. ***p < 0.001, **p < 0.01, *p < 0.05, †p < 0.10. (a–f) Refer to the parts that are explained in the article. Numerical values are standardized coefficient values resulting from the analysis in Tokyo.
Table 5. Coefficient values of different paths (a–f) of behavior models for the “G1. Individual mitigation behavior,” “G2. Individual adaptive behavior,” “G3. Long-term adaptive behavior,” and “G4. Solar-energy system installation” groups.
Table 6 summarizes the relative characteristics of the paths a–f, while comparing the relationships between the psychological factors and the trends in their influence on the behaviors of the different groups. The table shows, in simplified form, whether the target paths are significant at the 5% significance level and whether the target paths that are significant have characteristics such as strong or weak relationships within each model among prefectures. First, in Path a (Figures 4–7 and Table 5), “recognition of local climate change” in the target local area, which is a perception of the surrounding environment, directly affects “behavior,” relatively large coefficient values were observed in G2 (coefficient values for the four prefectures are 0.26–0.30, as shown in Table 5), compared to those in the other two adaptive behavior or G3 and G4. Notably, small values were obtained for the G3 (0.07–0.13) and G4 (ranging from −0.06 to −0.18). As all prefectures portrayed negative values in the case of G4, even if respondents are aware of local climate change, most of them do not install energy systems. This is a representative example of behavior that shows the gap between attitude and behavior, where internal cognitive factors alone do not lead to behavior because of the large influence of external factors. Because of the small value, we could reasonably conclude that system installation is not related to local climate recognition. However, it might be hypothesized that the type of people with lifestyle that makes them aware of changes in their surroundings would prefer small, immediate behaviors that they themselves take the initiative in undertaking, rather than large, single-point investment behaviors such as the system installation. Nevertheless, this hypothesis requires further investigation. Next, the path from “attribution of local climate change” to “perceived effectiveness” (Path b) portrayed a large effect in G1 (0.53–0.58); in the G3, the effect was small in almost all the prefectures (0.11–0.16), excluding Saitama Prefecture. In Saitama, where the practice rate of behaviors was high, the coefficient value was larger than that in other prefectures (0.31). In the G3, the coefficient value of the path from “perceived effectiveness” to “benefits” (Path c) tended to be larger (0.63–0.73) than that in the other behavior groups. The path from “practicality” (Path d) led to “attitude and intention,” and “behavior,” and strength of relationships differed depending on the behavior, as shown in Table 6. In the G1, the coefficient values of the path that affected “attitude and intention” were not large but somehow significant. Even if the behavior evaluation is positive, if attitudes and intentions are not fostered, they won't behave. There are direct correlations among indicators of practicality and behaviors because many people think it is highly practicality. However, in the model, the path became not significant or weak when the effect of attitude-intention was factored in. In the G2, both paths were not significant, excluding the path to “attitude and intention” of Nagano prefecture showed weak relationship. Indicators of practicality in the G2 showed less variation in responses than for the other behaviors, with many responses such as somehow easy to do. Although the evaluation of practicality was on the positive side, it was not as clearly perceived as high as in G1. Practicality is considered high, but because there are so many options in terms of the means of behaviors (e.g., information can be obtained through a smartphone app or by looking in a local magazine), respondents do not have a clear perception of what they are doing, so the direct impact of practicality is not seen. In the G3, the coefficient value of the path to “behavior” were larger or significant. As a result, only G3 showed a direct relationship between practicality and behavior across all prefectures. This is because many respondents, especially those who indicated that they did not practice the behavior, clearly indicated that practicality was low. In the G4, the path from practicality to behavior is significant only in Nagano and Saitama, the two prefectures with higher levels of practicality. In all prefectures, the practicality of system installation is low. However, in the two prefectures with high practice rates, respondents answered somehow in the affirmative, although the practicality was not high, implying that their perception of practicality increased as they planned their own implementation plans and observed the installation of the system by others. The respondents in Tokyo and Kanagawa, where there was no association with practicability, may have thought that the installation of the system was a one-time decision rather than a repeated investment, and therefore, some environmentally conscious people and those who thought it would be beneficial to their families may have installed the system, although they thought the practicality of the system was low. Different impacts were observed depending on the dissemination stage. The path from “attitude and intention” to “behavior” (Path e) had a large coefficient value (0.63–0.69) for the G1, but the paths were not significant for the G3. No direct correlation was found between “attitude and intention” and “behavior” in the G3. The destination of the path from “safety” (Path f) also differed, depending on the behavior. The perceptions of “safety” were linked to “perceived effectiveness,” and “attitude and intention,” with the indicators being related to adaptation behaviors. In the G2, a stronger relationship was observed between “safety” and “attitude and intention,” (0.42–0.56), except for Kanagawa Prefecture. The G2 in Kanagawa Prefecture and the G3 and G4 demonstrated a stronger relationship between “safety” and “perceived effectiveness” (0.43–0.68). As we have seen, by constructing same-structured models in which the psychological factors were progressively related, we have shown differences in the relationships among the factors depending on the behavior and prefecture.
Table 6. Characteristics of the target paths that portrayed different trends depending on the behavior model.
Furthermore, we focused on the differences in the paths of the behavioral model among the prefectures that portrayed significant differences in the practice rate of the behavior, as shown in Table 4. In the case of G1, with respect to Nagano and Kanagawa prefectures, the coefficient value of the path from “practicality” to “behavior” (Path d) was significant and negative in Kanagawa Prefecture. This indicated that in Kanagawa Prefecture, even though people recognized the practicality of the behavior, they did not implement relevant actions. Therefore, in Kanagawa Prefecture, social action measures that continue to raise awareness about attitude and intention may be effective in promoting pro-environmental behaviors, e.g., limited use of plastic bags. In the case of G2, with respect to Saitama and Nagano prefectures, in Saitama Prefecture, the coefficient values of Path f and Path e were large. In Nagano Prefecture, the path coefficient values from “injunctive norm” to “descriptive norm,” “benefit” to “practicality” and “behavior,” and “personal norm” to “attitude and intention” were large. This indicated that in Saitama Prefecture, there was a strong recognition that behavior assured safety. This may reflect the situation in Saitama Prefecture, which in recent years has experienced more extremely hot days than other regions in Japan, with the increase in heatstroke and other health problems often making the news. On the other hand, it is assumed that people in Nagano Prefecture do not feel any social requirements and benefits. Similarly, the practice rate of G3 was high in Saitama Prefecture. In Saitama Prefecture, the coefficient value of the Path b and Path c were relatively high. Therefore, “perceived effectiveness” and the “benefit” evaluation stimulated by it are considered to play a significant role in long-term adaptive behavior. In the case of G4, when comparing Nagano with other prefectures, the path coefficient value from “attitude and intention” to “behavior” (Path e) was larger, and the effect of “benefit” on “behavior” was not significant. For this reason, the progress of system installation in Nagano Prefecture was based not on the evaluation of benefits, but on the growing awareness that the installation of such systems was good behavior.
Table 7 portrays the influential factors that had the largest standardized overall effects on the behaviors (all direct and indirect paths combined), along with their effect values. The results showed that the factors do not differ by prefecture for G1 and G3, and that in all prefectures, “attitude and intention” portrayed a large impact on G1, and the evaluation of “benefit” had a large impact on G3 For G2, the factors differed by prefecture, with the largest influence from the evaluation of “benefits” in Nagano and Kanagawa prefectures, “recognition of local climate change” in Tokyo Prefecture, and “attitude and intention” in Saitama Prefecture, wherein the practice rate is the highest. For G4, the influence of “attitude and intention” was large in Nagano Prefecture, wherein the practice rate was high, from “perceived effectiveness” in Tokyo and Saitama prefectures, and from the evaluation of “benefits” in Kanagawa Prefecture.
Table 7. Factors that had the largest influence on behavior in each prefecture and model and their standardized overall effect values.
4 Discussion
In the G1 (individual mitigation behaviors), individual psychological factors, such as “perception of the cause of local climate change,” “perceived effectiveness,” “attitude and intention,” had a great influence on the practice rate of behaviors. Based on the differences in the practice rate in each prefecture and the most influential factors, the results indicate that internal perceptions, such as “attitude and intention,” had a stronger influence on the citizens' behaviors. In the case of G4 (solar-energy system installation), in Nagano Prefecture, where the level of installation was high, there was a high influence of “attitude and intention” on behaviors. In other prefectures, the influence on behaviors was greater from the evaluations of “benefits,” although the influence is less and that direct path was not significant in Nagano prefecture. Nevertheless, in other prefectures, both the perception of benefits and the practice rate of system installation are low. This may be because the psychological stage toward the installation of solar-energy systems was more advanced in Nagano Prefecture than the other prefectures, with “attitude and intention” portraying a direct and significant impact. Confirming the respondents' perceptions in this regard, the descriptive norms were highly recognized in Nagano Prefecture, with its citizens being aware of the diffusion of solar-energy systems. This suggests that it is effective to establish a measurement to raise the perceptions of benefits and effectiveness, as part of the evaluation of installing solar energy systems. Furthermore, the example of Nagano Prefecture also suggests that to raise descriptive norms that can affect the perceptions of benefits, it is effective to have people familiarize themselves with relevant behaviors on not only an individual basis, but also a local community basis, where they can see each other's behaviors. It has been reported that the influence of descriptive norms is particularly significant on behaviors that are visible to others (Cialdini et al., 1990). For G2 (individual adaptive behaviors), such as obtaining information and preparing for climate change, we observed a significant direct influence of the recognition of local climate change on behaviors. Influences of personal evaluations, like “safety” and “benefit,” were also significant. People tended to view these as practical behaviors, adopted based on perceived benefits, contrasting with normative practices. This shows a different trend from that of the pro-environmental behavior (G1), which has been often framed as altruistic behavior. Pro-environmental behavior, beneficial in a global and long-term context, may not offer immediate personal benefits and could entail costs. Such behavior is driven by strong internal attitudes and norms, even with lesser perceived benefits. Conversely, adaptive behaviors primarily aim at enhancing the life of oneself and one's family, providing individual benefits at that time, potentially offering societal benefits too by safeguarding communities and improving wellbeing as inherent aspects of these behaviors. For this reason, regardless of whether an individual is selfish or altruistic, the behavior itself is altruistic in the broadest sense. Pro-environmental behavior is not purely altruistic since it relates to one's own living conditions. However, given the less recognizable benefits to individuals, pro-environmental behavior aligns more closely with true altruism. In contrast, adaptive behaviors align more with reciprocal altruism, focusing on immediate benefits for individuals, their families, and their communities. Therefore, emphasizing the specific benefits of climate change adaptation behaviors, considering temporal and spatial scales, becomes critical. Similarly, although the practice rate of G3 (long-term adaptive behaviors) was overall low, the influential factors were the evaluations of “benefit” and “practicality.” This suggests that specific appeals according to the situation and citizens' lifestyles may be effective in promoting relevant adaptation measures for both individual and long-term adaptive behaviors. For example, it would be effective to set up a place in the community to explore specific examples of behaviors and work together to design examples of behaviors for self-help and systems of mutual assistance as an approach to stimulate the evaluative perceptions related to the consequences of the behavior.
For adaptation measures, a notable variation was observed in both practice rates and influencing factors across the different local contexts within the prefectures. This result is in line with those of previous studies that have focused on regional characteristics, where many studies on adaptive behavior have emphasized risk management at the local community level (Wouter Botzen et al., 2019) or have found that place attachment, including the local community, has influenced adaptive behavior (van Valkengoed and Steg, 2019). However, most direct regional comparisons on adaptation so far have been comparisons on local government regional adaptation plans (Baba et al., 2017). Further research is needed on the context in which specific factors that create regional differences in individual behavior of adaptation are revealed, including cultural differences (Adger et al., 2013; van Valkengoed and Steg, 2019). To change behavior based on regional differences of the results, it is necessary to first establish a system that enables people to recognize climate change in the local area and help perceive the benefits of the behavior. In long-term adaptation behaviors, “benefits” were an influencing factor. However, it was not a positive connection, wherein people behave because the benefits are high, but a negative connection, wherein people don't behave because they don't feel the benefits. Therefore, from a medium- to long-term perspective, it is considered effective to establish measures that enable citizens to envision and feel how their lives and community will benefit by undertaking drastic adaptation behaviors.
Among the different pro-environmental behavior subtypes of G1, only “B1. Refuse plastic bags and excessive packaging” showed regional differences. This is in line with the results of an eco-shopping study, which showed different behavior practice rates by region (Kurisu and Bortoleto, 2011), and with the regional differences owing to the plastic bag fee policy (Aoki et al., 2012; Heidbreder et al., 2019). The regional differences in G4 (installation solar energy systems) were also in line with studies showing the influence of urban policies such as subsidies (Mundaca and Samahita, 2020), as well as previous studies that explained regional differences based on external factors such as regional differences in renewable energy potential and the availability of large houses to install the systems in Japan (Aoki et al., 2012). Nevertheless, no differences were found in the behavior of waste separation for recycling, as most regions in Japan have high practice rates despite some minor policy differences among regions. There was no difference in energy-saving behavior either, which is in line with the findings of previous studies that have shown differences in individual psychological factors to be the major driving and inhibiting factors rather than socioeconomic factors such as regional factors (Hines et al., 1987; Aoki et al., 2010).
In pro-environmental behavior or individual mitigation behavior, recycling behavior is different from waste prevention behavior. Bortoleto et al. (2012) argued that the difference between the two relies on the fact that recycling is a public, procedural, and repetitive behavior, whereas waste prevention is a private and dynamic behavior that depends on the place of implementation. Reportedly, the factors that lead to behavior change differ between behaviors that are invisible to others and those that are seen by others (Cialdini et al., 1990). Similarly, for the adaptive behaviors, the factors influencing the behavior differed in terms of how the behavior was accepted and prevalent in the local community. Pro-environmental behavior is widely recognized as a socially desirable behavior that should be practiced, even though a gap may exist between the attitude and the behavior (Kaiser, 1998; Kollmuss and Agyeman, 2002; Bosone et al., 2022). There are many procedural, concrete, and repetitive behaviors, and the behavior discussed as G1 in this study falls into this category. G2 and G3 are more private and dynamic behaviors than waste prevention behaviors, as they are not visible to others unless a disaster occurs. Adaptive behavior is not the kind of behavior in which a government or other public organization calls on individuals to behave in a uniform manner and make it a social rule to do so. In terms of prevalence, social norms have a positive influence on typical pro-environmental behavior when most people do something about it, but adaptive behavior has a negative influence when there are only a few people who do it and social norms indicate that there is no problem even if they don't do it now. While mitigation behaviors are for the benefit of society, commonly referred to as earth-friendly behaviors or saving the earth, adaptive behaviors, focused on individual and their surroundings' preparedness for damage, were less influenced by social norms and not typically viewed as altruistic so far. Perhaps an abstract question of approval or disapproval of climate change adaptation behavior must yield different results, as it is considered a socially desirable behavior. When behavior is concretized and put down, the difference between adaptation and mitigation, necessarily due to the purpose of the behavior, is not represented as a difference in the factors that influence the behavior. This is implied by the fact that significant differences were observed among the four prefectures analyzed in this study, portraying different levels of diffusion. Although there may be models and characteristic factors that better fit individual specific behaviors, we were able to compare the same structure in other target groups in this study, thus adding to its significance. This will allow us to refer to the results of this study when we disseminate the results to society in the future as behaviors that should also be applied by individuals. Consequently, various methods of palliative interventions have been proposed at the penetration stage of behaviors (Schultz and Kaiser, 2012; Kurisu, 2015). Therefore, our findings on interventions to promote pro-environmental behaviors can be used for inducing behavior changes, to promote adaptive behavior, while focusing on local situations and implications.
Furthermore, adaptation measures can be implemented at various scales; notably, human behavior at a particular local scale (e.g., at an individual or local-community scale) is important (Adger, 2001). Wilson et al. (2020) suggested that to consider the behavior of an individual, one should not only consider the individual, but also the collective impact of the individual's surroundings. As suggested by the results of this study, individual adaptive behaviors are directly affected by “recognition of local climate change.” Individual perceptions can be changed based on not only the numerical values of observational data but also the information that can be linked to the benefits of the citizens that they can experience themselves. Therefore, to promote the implementation of adaptation measures in local communities, we propose introducing prescribed actions as specific adaptation measures and supporting citizens to study and analyze appropriate local behaviors and weather information, so that they can develop their own specific action plans. In addition, identifying the local problems and developing relevant countermeasures within local communities can lead to positive changes in the local social norms and foster an attitude that enables citizens to harmonize with their community members.
While TRA has shown that intentions are direct antecedents of behavior, Fishbein and Ajzen (1975) pointed to both causal relationships and discrepancies between intentions and behavior, highlighting a more complex relationship. Several different types of intentions exist, and in this study, we focused on goal-oriented intentions, similar to TRA and TPB. Other, less completely divisive and highly correlated, but certainly independent predictors are behavioral expectations, which predict one's own behavior in the future; implementation intention or behavioral planning, which is a more embodied plan of future behavior; and behavioral willingness is openness to or acceptance of the circumstances conductive to socially undesirable behavior (Gibbons et al., 1998; Ohtomo and Hirose, 2007). The difficulty in distinguishing between these issues is that even similar behavioral intentions require different understandings. The TPB, on which this study is based, indicates that goal-oriented intentions can be predicted using attitudes and subjective norms as antecedents, and that the correlation between intentions and behavior then depends on the extent to which people can control their behavior themselves (Ajzen, 2020). Ajzen (2020) explained three problems with the research methodology regarding the discrepancy between intention and behavior: restriction of range, lack of compatibility, and hypothetical bias; and three problems with the features of individuals' behavior regarding forgetting, change of mind, and low control over the behavior. Regarding the problem of discrepancy between intention and behavior as features of individuals' behavior, it is regarded as a gap between intention and behavior, and the influence of external factors and what kind of reinforcement is necessary for behavior change have been studied (Kollmuss and Agyeman, 2002; Webb and Sheeran, 2006; Hirose, 2015; Sheeran and Webb, 2016). This study does not aim to utilize behavioral intentions as future behaviors or to retrospectively uncover the reasons and causal factors that led to the initial execution of the target behavior. Instead, it seeks to illustrate the connection between whether or not the behavior was enacted at the time of the survey and the respondents' current perceptions regarding the behavior. By doing so, the study has identified whether any discrepancies between current behavior and intentions exist and, if so, what factors are responsible for these disparities. Although measuring behavioral intentions and behavior can be challenging, it is valuable to examine the relationship between the two as they are currently perceived by the respondents. These relationships may be highly or poorly correlated, and it is crucial to comprehend the context of these relationships in terms of the subject, target behavior, and social context.
The gap between intention to engage in pro-environmental behavior and actual behavior could be associated with a blend of moral values, habituation, rational decision making, emotions, ethics, and cognitive thinking (Ajzen, 2002; Ziegelmann et al., 2007; Bosone et al., 2022). Ajzen (2002) emphasizes how the interaction of rational behavior with past behavior and habituation can affect the consistency of behavior over time. Bosone et al. (2022) also investigate the impact of cognitive dissonance and dissonance on pro-environmental intentions, showing that cognitive dynamics and internal conflict play a role in the mismatch between intentions and actual behavior. Webb and Sheeran (2006), Sheeran and Webb (2016) studies reveal that the link between intention and behavior is complex. They found that significant changes in intention often lead to only modest changes in behavior, particularly when strong habits exist. Additionally, they suggest that intentions are more likely to translate into actions when the behaviors are easier to perform. Ziegelmann et al. (2007) also mention different types of intentions through a longitudinal study indicating that as behaviors become more routine over time, the role of implementation intentions becomes more significant in determining actual behavior, while the influence of goal intentions diminishes. In this study, G1, individual mitigation behavior, is a routine pro-environmental behavior; hence, it is expected to be influenced by habit and past behavior. However, the influence of attitudes and intentions on behavior was greater than in the other behavior groups, and the gap between intentions and behavior was not as large. This finding is consistent with those from previous research (Ajzen, 2002; Webb and Sheeran, 2006; Sheeran and Webb, 2016) that behaviors that are easier to perform, i.e., those with fewer external disincentives, are more likely to bridge the gap between intention and behavior. Conversely, the correlation between intention and behavior in the case of climate change adaptation behaviors was not as strong. This could be attributed to the fact that the behavior is not a routine, repetitive action and that external factors such as benefit as perceived consequences and perceived effectiveness as antecedents of attitudes and intentions, have not been sufficiently fostered, resulting in a low practice rate of the behavior within the target area of the study.
Regarding hypothetical bias, the influence of social desirability has been noted in prosocial behaviors such as pro-environmental behavior. Individuals tend to report better behavior than they actually engage in activities such as answering questionnaires, to appear more socially desirable (Scott and Willits, 1994; Ajzen, 2020). Furthermore, some research raises questions about the accuracy of measuring behavioral intentions, which may pose challenges in assessment (Geller, 1981; Norman and Smith, 1995). However, subjective self-reports are often considered valid, though not entirely accurate, indicators (Webb and Sheeran, 2006), and their use in an effort to minimize the influence of social desirability can be an economical and parsimonious method (Kaiser, 1998). To avoid response bias due to social desirability in this survey, we carefully designed the wording in the questionnaire to be neutral and did not present climate change action as a socially desirable behavior. Additionally, abstract situations tend to have a greater impact on social desirability (Brown et al., 2003), so we concretized the target behaviors in the questions. Finally, we clearly stated that the respondents' answers would be anonymous and confidential.
This study has several limitations. As this study captured the perceptions only at a single point in time, the changes in the actual behaviors of the respondents were not captured. In addition, while our analysis measured how individuals perceive social norms, it did not measure the interactions between citizens of a local community. Given the study's employment of an age-gender balanced sample, the uncovered regional disparities could be attributed to the varied perceptions among different age-gender groups within each local area rather than to demographic shifts in age-gender composition owing to factors like rural depopulation. However, the sample did not follow the actual age-gender composition of the area and did not include minors or elderly people in their 70's or older. It should be noted that since this is not a large-scale survey representative of the broader area, there are limitations in generalizing the results of the analysis as differences specific to the localized target region. Furthermore, our study did not account for the relationship between subjective perceptions of climate change and the actual political measures implemented in the four prefectures.
5 Conclusion
In this research, we analyzed distinct behavior groups in response to climate change adaptation and mitigation: G1 (individual mitigation), G2 (individual adaptive), G3 (long-term adaptive), and G4 (solar-energy system installation). Cognitive psychological models of the same structure but reflecting unique influences of varied psychological factors were constructed for each group. Notably, while pro-environmental behaviors were predominantly driven by internal attitudes and normative beliefs, aligning with social desirability, adaptation behaviors were more significantly influenced by external factors, particularly evaluative perceptions related to the consequences of the behavior in implementation contexts.
Furthermore, our findings highlight the necessity of tailoring adaptation strategies to specific local contexts, given the substantial variation in psychological characteristics of adaptive behaviors across different local areas. Effective adaptation strategies might involve demonstrating the consequences of behavior, thus enhancing behavior evaluation perception and fostering its acceptance as a desirable behavior through concrete and relatable means. The findings propose that interactive efforts that enable citizens to think about both self and mutual help on their own are more effective than unidirectional information dissemination.
For advancing climate change adaptation and mitigation efforts, further research should concentrate on the individuals as behavioral actors and the local communities as distinct contextual entities. They are imperative to delineate the determinants of behavioral change, assessing the magnitude and nature of change across various levels.
Data availability statement
The raw data supporting the conclusions of this article will be made available by the authors, without undue reservation.
Ethics statement
Ethical reviews and approvals were not required for this study in accordance with the local legislation and institutional requirements. In this questionnaire survey, the survey is completed online and does not involve any intervention or invasion of the respondents. Therefore, we obtained the respondents' consent online only, following the prescribed criteria set forth by the research institution in accordance with “Ethical Guidelines for Medical and Biological Research Involving Human Subjects” by the Ministry of Education, Culture, Sports, Science and Technology, the Ministry of Health, Labor and Welfare, and the Ministry of Economy, Trade and Industry of Japan.
Author contributions
EA: Conceptualization, Formal analysis, Investigation, Methodology, Visualization, Writing – original draft. NS: Conceptualization, Investigation, Methodology, Writing – review & editing. KB: Supervision, Writing – review & editing. NM: Project administration, Writing – review & editing. MT: Funding acquisition, Writing – review & editing.
Funding
The author(s) declare that financial support was received for the research, authorship, and/or publication of this article. This work was supported by the MEXT-Program for Research and Development for Social Transformation to Accelerate Local Decarbonization (Grant Number: JPJ010039) and Environmental Research and Technology Development Fund (S-8) of the Ministry of the Environment, Japan.
Acknowledgments
We would like to thank Emeritus Mitsuru Tanaka (Hosei University), who provided useful advice and supervision.
Conflict of interest
The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.
Publisher's note
All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.
Supplementary material
The Supplementary Material for this article can be found online at: https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fclim.2024.1283946/full#supplementary-material
Abbreviations
TPB, Theory of planned behavior; TRA, Theory of reasoned action; PBC, Perceived behavioral control; SEM, Structural equation modeling; HSD, Honestly significant difference; GFI, Goodness-of-fit index; ANOVA, Analysis of variance; AGFI, Adjusted goodness-of-fit index; CFI, Comparative fit index; RMSEA, Root mean square error of approximation.
References
Adger, W. N. (2001). Scales of governance and environmental justice for adaptation and mitigation of climate change. Int. J. Dev. 13, 921–931. doi: 10.1002/jid.833
Adger, W. N., Arnell, N. W., and Tompkins, E. L. (2005). Successful adaptation to climate change across scales. Glob. Environ. Change 15, 77–86. doi: 10.1016/j.gloenvcha.2004.12.005
Adger, W. N., Barnett, J., Brown, K., Marshall, N., and O'Brien, K. (2013). Cultural dimensions of climate change impacts and adaptation. Nat. Clim. Change 3, 112–117. doi: 10.1038/nclimate1666
Ajzen, I. (1991). The theory of planned behavior. Organ. Behav. Hum. Decis. Process 50, 179–211. doi: 10.1016/0749-5978(91)90020-T
Ajzen, I. (2002). Residual effects of past on later behavior: habituation and reasoned action perspectives. Personal. Soc. Psychol. Rev. 6, 107–122. doi: 10.1207/S15327957PSPR0602_02
Ajzen, I. (2020). The theory of planned behavior: frequently asked questions. Hum. Behav. Emerg. Technol. 2, 314–324. doi: 10.1002/hbe2.195
Ajzen, I., and Fishbein, M. (1975). Attitude-behavior relations: a theoretical analysis and review of empirical research. Psychol. Bull. 84, 888–918. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.84.5.888
Ajzen, I., and Fishbein, M. (1980). Understanding Attitudes and Predicting Social Behaviour. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall; American Psychological Association.
Aoki, E., Kurisu, K., and Hanaki, K. (2012). Analysis of factors contribute to citizen's environmental behavior in local government. J. Jpn. Soc. Civ. Eng. G 68, 165–176. doi: 10.2208/jscejer.68.II_165
Aoki, E., Kurisu, K., and Hanaki, K. (2013). Exploring determinants and models of citizen's environmental behaviors. J. Jpn. Soc. Civ. Eng. G 69, 93–104. doi: 10.2208/jscejer.69.II_93
Aoki, E., Kurisu, K., Nakatani, J., and Hanaki, K. (2010). Current state and interregional comparison of citizen's environmental behavior by 47 prefectures. J. Jpn. Soc. Civ. Eng. G 38, 17–26. Available online at: http://library.jsce.or.jp/jsce/open/00013/2010/38-0017.pdf
Arbuckle, J. G., Morton, L. W., and Hobbs, J. (2015). Understanding farmer perspectives on climate change adaptation and mitigation: the roles of trust in sources of climate information, climate change beliefs, and perceived risk. Environ. Behav. 47, 205–234. doi: 10.1177/0013916513503832
Baba, K., Matsuura, M., Kudo, T., Watanabe, S., Kawakubo, S., Chujo, A., et al. (2017). Oxford research encyclopedia of climate science. Clim. Change Adapt. Strateg. Local Gov. Jpn. doi: 10.1093/acrefore/9780190228620.013.597
Baba, K., Sugimoto, T., Kubota, H., Hijioka, Y., and Tanaka, M. (2011). Factors to determine risk perception of climate change, and attitude toward adaptation policy of the public. J. Jpn. Soc. Civ. Eng. G 67, 405–413. doi: 10.2208/jscejer.67.II_405
Bamberg, S., and Möser, G. (2007). Twenty years after Hines, Hungerford, and Tomera: a new meta-analysis of psycho-social determinants of pro-environmental behaviour. J. Environ. Psychol. 27, 14–25. doi: 10.1016/j.jenvp.2006.12.002
Barr, S. (2003). Strategies for sustainability: citizens and responsible environmental behaviour. Area 35, 227–240. doi: 10.1111/1475-4762.00172
Blake, J. (1999). Overcoming the 'value-action gap' in environmental policy: tensions between national policy and local experience. Local Environ. 4, 257–278. doi: 10.1080/13549839908725599
Blennow, K., Persson, J., Tomé, M., and Hanewinkel, M. (2012). Climate change: believing and seeing implies adapting. PLoS ONE 7:e50182. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0050182
Bortoleto, A. P., Kurisu, K. H., and Hanaki, K. (2012). Model development for household waste prevention behaviour. Waste Manag. 32, 2195–2207. doi: 10.1016/j.wasman.2012.05.037
Bosone, L., Chevrier, M., and Zenasni, F. (2022). Consistent or inconsistent? The effects of inducing cognitive dissonance vs. cognitive consonance on the intention to engage in pro-environmental behaviors. Front. Psychol. 13:902703. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2022.902703
Brown, T. C., Ajzen, I., and Hrubes, D. (2003). Further tests of entreaties to avoid hypothetical bias in referendum contingent valuation. J. Environ. Econ. Manag. 46, 353–361. doi: 10.1016/S0095-0696(02)00041-4
Brügger, A., Dessai, S., Devine-Wright, P., Morton, T. A., and Pidgeon, N. F. (2015). Psychological responses to the proximity of climate change. Nat. Clim. Change 5, 1031–1037. doi: 10.1038/nclimate2760
Bubeck, P., Botzen, W. J. W., and Aerts, J. C. J. H. (2012). A review of risk perceptions and other factors that influence flood mitigation behavior. Risk Anal. 32, 1481–1495. doi: 10.1111/j.1539-6924.2011.01783.x
Byrne, B. M. (2001). Structural equation modeling with AMOS, EQS, and LISREL: comparative approaches to testing for the factorial validity of a measuring instrument. Int. J. Test. 1, 55–86. doi: 10.1207/S15327574IJT0101_4
Cabinet of Japan (2015). National Plan for Adaptation to the Impacts of Climate Change. Available online at: https://www.env.go.jp/content/900451302.pdf (accessed August 8, 2023).
Cialdini, R. B., Reno, R. R., and Kallgren, C. A. (1990). A focus theory of normative conduct: recycling the concept of norms to reduce littering in public places. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 58, 1015–1026. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.58.6.1015
Dang, H. L., Li, E., Nuberg, I., and Bruwer, J. (2018). Vulnerability to climate change and the variations in factors affecting farmers' adaptation: a multi-group structural equation modelling study. Clim. Dev. 10, 509–519. doi: 10.1080/17565529.2017.1304885
Davies, J., Foxall, G. R., and Pallister, J. (2002). Beyond the intention-behaviour mythology: an integrated model of recycling. Mark. Theor. 2, 29–113. doi: 10.1177/1470593102002001645
Fishbein, M., and Ajzen, I. (1975). Belief, Attitude, Intention, and Behavior: An Introduction to Theory and Research. Reading, PA: Addison-Wesley.
Geller, E. S. (1981). Evaluating energy conservation programs: is verbal report enough? J. Con. Res. 8, 331–335. doi: 10.1086/208872
Gibbons, F. X., Gerrard, M., Blanton, H., and Russell, D. W. (1998). Reasoned action and social reaction: willingness and intention as independent predictors of health risk. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 74, 1164–1180. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.74.5.1164
Gifford, R., and Nilsson, A. (2014). Personal and social factors that influence pro-environmental concern and behaviour: a review. Int. J. Psychol. 49, 141–157. doi: 10.1002/ijop.12034
Government of Japan (2018). Climate Change Adaptation Act (Act No. 50 of 2018). Available online at: https://www.japaneselawtranslation.go.jp/en/laws/view/3212/en (accessed August 8, 2023).
Grothmann, T., and Reusswig, F. (2006). People at risk of flooding: why some residents take precautionary action while others do not. Nat. Hazards 38, 101–120. doi: 10.1007/s11069-005-8604-6
Heidbreder, L. M., Bablok, I., Drews, S., and Menzel, C. (2019). Tackling the plastic problem: a review on perceptions, behaviors, and interventions. Sci. Tot. Environ. 668, 1077–1093. doi: 10.1016/j.scitotenv.2019.02.437
Hijioka, Y., Takano, S., Oka, K., Yoshikawa, M., Ichihashi, A., Baba, K., et al. (2016). Potential of existing policies of the Tokyo Metropolitan Government for implementing adaptation to climate change. Reg. Environ. Change 16, 967–978. doi: 10.1007/s10113-015-0809-y
Hines, J. M., Hungerford, H. R., and Tomera, A. N. (1987). Analysis and synthesis of research on responsible environmental behavior: a meta-analysis. J. Environ. Educ. 18, 1–8. doi: 10.1080/00958964.1987.9943482
Hirose, Y. (2015). Two-phase decision-making model of environmental conscious behavior and its application for the waste reduction behavior. Saf. Sci. Rev. 5, 81–91.
Hulme, M. (2009). Why We Disagree About Climate Change: Understanding Controversy, Inaction and Opportunity. New York, NY: Cambridge University Press.
IPCC (2022). “2022: impacts, adaptation and vulnerability,” in Contribution of Working Group II to the Sixth Assessment Report of the Intergovernmental Panel on Climate Change, eds. H. O. Pörtner, D. C. Roberts, M. Tignor, E. S. Poloczanska, K. Mintenbeck, A. Alegría, et al. (Cambridge; New York, NY: Cambridge University Press), 3056.
Kaiser, F. G. (1998). A general measure of ecological Behavior. J. Appl. Soc. Psychol. 28, 395–422. doi: 10.1111/j.1559-1816.1998.tb01712.x
Kaiser, F. G., and Fuhrer, U. (2003). Ecological behavior's dependency on different forms of knowledge. Appl. Psychol. 52, 598–613. doi: 10.1111/1464-0597.00153
Kallgren, C. A., Reno, R. R., and Cialdini, R. B. (2000). A focus theory of normative conduct: when norms do and do not affect behavior. Pers. Soc. Psychol. Bull. 26, 1002–1012. doi: 10.1177/01461672002610009
Kellens, W., Terpstra, T., and De Maeyer, P. (2013). Perception and communication of flood risks: a systematic review of empirical research. Risk Anal. 33, 24–49. doi: 10.1111/j.1539-6924.2012.01844.x
Kirshen, P., Ruth, M., and Anderson, W. (2008). Interdependencies of urban climate change impacts and adaptation strategies: a case study of Metropolitan Boston USA. Clim. Change 86, 105–122. doi: 10.1007/s10584-007-9252-5
Kollmuss, A., and Agyeman, J. (2002). Mind the gap: why do people act environmentally and what are the barriers to pro-environmental behavior? Environ. Educ. Res. 8, 239–260. doi: 10.1080/13504620220145401
Kurisu, K. H., and Bortoleto, A. P. (2011). Comparison of waste prevention behaviors among three Japanese megacity regions in the context of local measures and socio-demographics. Waste Manag. 31, 1441–1449. doi: 10.1016/j.wasman.2011.03.008
Lee, H., Kurisu, K., and Hanaki, K. (2013). Influential factors on pro-environmental behaviors—a case study in Tokyo and Seoul. Low Carbon Econ. 4, 104–116. doi: 10.4236/lce.2013.43011
Lord, C. G., Lepper, M. R., and Mackie, D. (1984). Attitude prototypes as determinants of attitude-behavior consistency. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 46, 1254–1266. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.46.6.1254
Lorenzoni, I., and Pidgeon, N. F. (2006). Public views on climate change: European and USA perspectives. Clim. Change 77, 73–95. doi: 10.1007/s10584-006-9072-z
Mundaca, L., and Samahita, M. (2020). What drives home solar PV uptake? Subsidies, peer effects and visibility in Sweden. Energy Res. Soc. Sci. 60:101319. doi: 10.1016/j.erss.2019.101319
Norman, P., and Smith, L. (1995). The theory of planned behaviour and exercise: an investigation into the role of prior behaviour, behavioural intentions and attitude variability. Eur. J. Soc. Psychol. 25, 403–415. doi: 10.1002/ejsp.2420250405
Ohtomo, S., and Hirose, Y. (2007). The dual-process of reactive and intentional decision-making involved in eco-friendly behavior. J. Environ. Psychol. 27, 117–125. doi: 10.1016/j.jenvp.2007.01.005
O'Neill, S. J., and Nicholson-Cole, S. (2009). “Fear Won't Do It”: promoting positive engagement with climate change through visual and iconic representations. Sci. Commun. 30, 355–379. doi: 10.1177/1075547008329201
Rivis, A., and Sheeran, P. (2003). Descriptive norms as an additional predictor in the theory of planned behaviour: a meta-analysis. Curr. Psychol. 22, 218–233. doi: 10.1007/s12144-003-1018-2
Schermelleh-Engel, K., Moosbrugger, H., and Müller, H. (2003). Evaluating the fit of structural equation models: tests of significance and descriptive goodness-of-fit measures. Methods Psychol. Res. 8, 23–74.
Schultz, P. W., and Kaiser, F. G. (2012). “The Oxford handbook of environmental and conservation psychology,” in Promoting Pro-environmental Behavior, ed. S. D. Clayton (Oxford: Oxford University Press), 556–580.
Schwartz, S. H. (1977). “Normative influences on altruism,” in Adv. Exp. Soc. Psychol. ed. L. Berkowitz (Cambridge, MA: Academic Press), 221–279.
Scott, D., and Willits, F. K. (1994). Environmental attitudes and behavior: a Pennsylvania survey. Environ. Behav. 26, 239–260. doi: 10.1177/001391659402600206
Sheeran, P., and Webb, T. L. (2016). The intention-behavior gap. Soc. Pers. Psychol. Compass 10, 503–518. doi: 10.1111/spc3.12265
Shirai, N., Tanaka, M., and Aoki, E. (2015). Analysis of the structure of consciousness of mitigation and adaptation behavior to climate change. Jpn. J. Environ. Edu 25, 62–71. doi: 10.5647/jsoee.25.2_62
Shirai, N., Tanaka, M., Tamura, M., Yasuhara, K., Harasawa, H., and Komatsu, T. (2014). Building and verification of a theoretical framework for climate change adaptation. Environ. Sci. 27, 313–323. doi: 10.11353/sesj.27.313
Smit, B., and Pilifosova, O. (2003). Adaptation to climate change in the context of sustainable development and equity. Sustain. Dev. 8:9. doi: 10.1142/9781860945816_0002
Steiger, J. H. (1990). Structural model evaluation and modification: an interval estimation approach. Multivar. Behav. Res. 25, 173–180. doi: 10.1207/s15327906mbr2502_4
Stern, P. C. (2000). New environmental theories: toward a coherent theory of environmentally significant behavior. J. Soc. Iss. 56, 407–424. doi: 10.1111/0022-4537.00175
Thøgersen, J. (2006). Norms for environmentally responsible behaviour: an extended taxonomy. J. Environ. Psychol. 26, 247–261. doi: 10.1016/j.jenvp.2006.09.004
Ullman, J. B., and Bentler, P. M. (2012). “Structural equation modeling,” in Handbook of Psychology, 2nd Edn. (Hoboken, NJ: John Wiley & Sons, Ltd). doi: 10.1002/9781118133880.hop202023
Van Liere, K. D., and Dunlap, R. E. (1980). The social bases of environmental concern: a review of hypotheses, explanations and empirical evidence. Publ. Opin. Q. 46, 181–197. doi: 10.1086/268583
van Valkengoed, A. M., and Steg, L. (2019). Meta-analyses of factors motivating climate change adaptation behaviour. Nat. Clim. Change 9, 158–163. doi: 10.1038/s41558-018-0371-y
Wachinger, G., Renn, O., Begg, C., and Kuhlicke, C. (2013). The risk perception paradox-Implications for governance and communication of natural hazards. Risk Anal. 33, 1049–1065. doi: 10.1111/j.1539-6924.2012.01942.x
Webb, T. L., and Sheeran, P. (2006). Does changing behavioral intentions engender behavior change? A meta-analysis of the experimental evidence. Psychol. Bull. 132, 249–268. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.132.2.249
Wilson, R. S., Herziger, A., Hamilton, M., and Brooks, J. S. (2020). From incremental to transformative adaptation in individual responses to climate-exacerbated hazards. Nat. Clim. Change 10, 200–208. doi: 10.1038/s41558-020-0691-6
Wouter Botzen, W. J., Bouwer, L. M., Scussolini, P., Kuik, O., Haasnoot, M., Lawrence, J., et al. (2019). “Integrated disaster risk management and adaptation,” in Loss and Damage from Climate Change: Concepts, Methods and Policy Options, Climate Risk Management, Policy and Governance, eds. R. Mechler, L. M. Bouwer, T. Schinko, S. Surminski, and J. Linnerooth-Bayer (Cham: Springer International Publishing), 287–315.
Zaremohzzabieh, Z., Samah, A. A., Roslan, S., Shaffril, H. A. M., D'Silva, J. L., Kamarudin, S., et al. (2021). Household preparedness for future earthquake disaster risk using an extended theory of planned behavior. Int. J. Disaster Risk Reduc. 65:102533. doi: 10.1016/j.ijdrr.2021.102533
Zhang, L., Ruiz-Menjivar, J., Luo, B., Liang, Z., and Swisher, M. E. (2020). Predicting climate change mitigation and adaptation behaviors in agricultural production: a comparison of the theory of planned behavior and the value-belief-norm theory. J. Environ. Psychol. 68:101408. doi: 10.1016/j.jenvp.2020.101408
Keywords: behavioral model, pro-environmental behavior, adaptation, mitigation, climate change, disaster risk behavior, psychological factors, Japan
Citation: Aoki E, Shirai N, Baba K, Masuhara N and Taniguchi M (2024) Developing behavioral models of citizens for adapting to and mitigating climate change: a study on four prefectures in Japan. Front. Clim. 6:1283946. doi: 10.3389/fclim.2024.1283946
Received: 27 August 2023; Accepted: 19 February 2024;
Published: 11 March 2024.
Edited by:
Bao-Jie He, Chongqing University, ChinaReviewed by:
Junsong Wang, South China University of Technology, ChinaAna Paula Bortoleto, State University of Campinas, Brazil
Copyright © 2024 Aoki, Shirai, Baba, Masuhara and Taniguchi. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.
*Correspondence: Eri Aoki, YW9raSYjeDAwMDQwO2NoaWt5dS5hYy5qcA==