- 1Institut des Neurosciences Cellulaires et Intégratives (INCI)—CNRS, UPR 3212, Strasbourg, France
- 2Sorbonne Université, CNRS UMR 8256, Biological Adaptation and Ageing, Paris, France
- 3Neurochlore, Ben-Ari Institute of Neuroarcheology, Campus Scientifique de Luminy, Aix Marseille Université, Marseille, France
Brain development is accompanied by a shift in gamma-aminobutyric acid (GABA) response from depolarizing-excitatory to hyperpolarizing-inhibitory, due to a reduction of intracellular chloride concentration. This sequence is delayed in Autism Spectrum Disorders (ASD). We now report a similar alteration of this shift in the cerebellum, a structure implicated in ASD. Using single GABAA receptor channel recordings in cerebellar Purkinje cells (PCs), we found two conductance levels (18 and 10 pS), the former being dominant in newborns and the latter in young-adults. This conductance shift and the depolarizing/excitatory to hyperpolarizing/inhibitory GABA shift occurred 4 days later in females than males. Our data support a sex-dependent developmental shift of GABA conductance and chloride gradient, leading to different developmental timing in males and females. Because these developmental sequences are altered in ASD, this study further stresses the importance of developmental timing in pathological neurodevelopment.
Introduction
Gamma-aminobutyric acid (GABA) is the main inhibitory neurotransmitter in the central nervous system. By opening GABAA receptor channels and via chloride flow down its electrochemical gradient, GABA hyperpolarizes the membrane potential and thus reduces neuronal excitability in the mature nervous system. Intracellular chloride concentration (Cl−)I is regulated by cation-chloride co-transporters that determine the strength of GABAergic inhibition (Rivera et al., 1999; Watanabe and Fukuda, 2015; Raimondo et al., 2017). If the intracellular chloride concentration is high enough that the equilibrium potential for chloride is positive compared to the resting membrane potential, GABAA receptor activation can depolarize the cell above the action potential threshold, acting like an excitatory neurotransmitter (Ben-Ari et al., 1989; Ben-Ari, 2014). This regulation of (Cl−)I has been reported in a wide range of animal species and brain structures; it is affected by many factors including sex steroids, BDNF and IGF-1 signaling (Galanopoulou, 2008; Tsutsui et al., 2011; Waddell and McCarthy, 2012; Ben-Ari, 2014; Watanabe and Fukuda, 2015). Immature neurons have lower (Cl−)I than neurons of young adults, leading to depolarizing GABA actions which both activate voltage-gated calcium currents and allow NMDA receptor activation, underlying the well-known trophic actions of GABA in development (Ben-Ari, 2002, 2014; Witte et al., 2014; Raimondo et al., 2017). GABAergic signals also generate action potentials in many immature neurons, and the switch between excitatory and inhibitory GABA effects occurs at specific developmental time-points, depending on the neural structure and animal species. For example, the GABA “switch” occurs between E15.5 and E 17.5 in mouse spinal motoneurons (Allain et al., 2011), around P5 in the ventral horn of the rat spinal cord (Stein et al., 2004) and in ganglion and amacrine cells of the retina (Zhang et al., 2006), and from P8–P10 in mouse hippocampal pyramidal cells (Ben-Ari et al., 1989; Tyzio et al., 2007). Changes in the timing of the GABA switch, due to genetic mutations and/or environmental insults, are thought to contribute to neurodevelopmental disorders including autism (Ben-Ari et al., 1989; Ben-Ari, 2017).
GABAA receptors are composed of 2 α subunits, 2 β subunits and 1ϒ subunit, which together form a central ion pore. Excluding splice variants and point mutations, 6 α isoforms, 3 β isoforms and 3 ϒ isoforms have been characterized, as well as some minor subunits (δ, ρ, ε, θ, π and ρ). In theory, therefore, a very large number of GABAA receptor types may be found even in a single cell. The major adult isoform is generally accepted to be composed of α1, β2 and ϒ2 subunits. Whereas some subunits have a broad expression throughout the central nervous system, other subunits show a restricted cellular and subcellular localization (Sigel and Steinmann, 2012). The expression of GABAA receptor subunits is developmentally regulated, notably with a developmental switch in α subunit expression associated with a slow-to-fast shift in the kinetics of GABA-mediated inhibitory postsynaptic potentials (Laurie et al., 1992; Fritschy et al., 1994). Thus, in thalamic reticular neurons, a postnatal switch in GABAA receptor subunits from α5 to α3 is believed to play a role in the early development of the circuit (Pangratz-Fuehrer et al., 2016). Experiments using subunit expression in HEK cells or mouse fibroblasts show that single GABA channels currents have distinct opening conductance levels depending upon subunit composition (Mortensen and Smart, 2006).
Studies on developmental GABA shifts during normal development and in relation to autism spectrum disorders (ASD) have been done primarily in cortical structures. Yet abnormalities of the cerebellum and of cerebellar Purkinje cells (PCs) are frequently described in post mortem studies of humans with autism (Fatemi et al., 2012) as well as in the rodent valproate model of autism (Ingram et al., 2000). In addition, PCs undergo considerable post-natal development, including regression of climbing fiber multi-innervation and dramatic dendritic arbor expansion (Dusart and Flamant, 2012), allowing investigation of these important developmental processes after birth.
We have now measured the changes in somatic GABAA channel properties and chloride gradient in PCs, comparing these changes in male and female mice, since sex differences in the GABA shift have been reported (Galanopoulou, 2008). We report that in normal and valproate-model male and female mice, the dominant GABAA receptor channel shifts from high-conductance in the newborn to low-conductance in juvenile and adult mice. This shift in conductance parallels the GABA switch from depolarizing to hyperpolarizing, as it is sex dependent, being delayed in naïve females compared to males. Furthermore, the GABA switch is delayed in mice of both sexes after prenatal exposure to valproate, supporting a role of cerebellar dysfunction in the pathology of autism.
Materials and Methods
Mice
Pregnant C57 mice were injected intraperitoneally with 600 mg valproate sodium salt (n = 18) or saline (n = 15) at embryonic day 12.5 (Roullet et al., 2013). Either control pups or pups from valproate-treated dams were used to prepare acute cerebellar slices for patch-clamp experiments at ages between postnatal days 5 (P5) and 45 (P45). For the study of climbing fiber synapse elimination, either C57 or Swiss pups (because of large number of pups per litter) were used; no differences were found in synapse elimination between the two strains of mice. All procedures followed guidelines established by le Comité National d’ethique pour les Sciences de la Vie et de la Santé (EU Council Directive 2010/63/EU) and were approved by Institutional Animal Care and Use Committees (CREMAS, Comité Régional d’Ethique en experimentation animale de Strasbourg).
Slice Preparation
Standard procedures were used to prepare 250-μm or 300-μm parasagittal slices from control or valproate-treated mice at P5–P45 following a protocol approved by the European and French guidelines on animal experimentation established by le Comité National d’ethique pour les Sciences de la Vie et de la Santé (EU Council Directive 2010/63/EU) and were approved by Institutional Animal Care and Use Committees (CREMAS, Comité Régional d’Ethique en experimentation animale de Strasbourg). Briefly, mice were killed by decapitation under isoflurane anesthesia. Brains were dissected in ice-cold artificial cerebrospinal fluid (ACSF) and sliced with a vibratome (Leica VT1200S) at 4°C. Slices were maintained for 30 min at 32°C in an interface chamber containing ACSF equilibrated with 95% O2, 5% CO2 and containing (in mM): NaCl 124, KCl 2.7, CaCl2 2, MgCl2 1.3, NaHCO3 26, NaH2PO4 0.4, glucose 10, ascorbate 4, then for at least 1 h at room temperature before being transferred to a superfusing recording chamber.
Electrophysiological Recordings
Slices were transferred to a recording chamber on an upright microscope. The recording chamber was continuously perfused at room temperature with bath solution containing: (mM) NaCl 124, KCl 2.7, CaCl2 2, MgCl2 1.3, NaHCO3 26, NaH2PO4 0.4, glucose 10, pH 7.4, equilibrated with 95% O2, 5% CO2. For cell-attached recordings (in order to stabilized the resting membrane potential) the bath solution contained tetrodotoxin (TTX) 10−5 M and NBQX 10−5 M. In some experiments isoguvacine or NBQX were applied to the bathing fluid at a concentration of 10−5 M. For experiments recording climbing fiber currents, the ACSF contained 10−4 M picrotoxin.
Most electrophysiological experiments were performed on visually identified PCs using the patch-clamp technique in the cell-attached configuration. Electrodes were filled for single channels recordings with the following solution (mM): KCl 110, NaCl 2, MgCl2 2, CaCl2 2, HEPES 10, tetra-ethyl-ammonium-chloride (TEA) 20, TTX 10−3, CsCl2, 4-aminopyridine (4AP) 1, BaCl2 1, Isoguvacine or Muscimol 10−5, pH 7.4 ; and for spiking activity with the following solution (mM): NaCl 124, KCl 2.7, CaCl2 2, MgCl2 1.3, NaHCO3 26, NaH2PO4 0.4, glucose 10, equilibrated at pH 7.4 with 95%O2, 5% CO2.
For experiments recording climbing fiber currents, patch pipettes were filled with a solution containing (mM): Cs-D-gluconate 120, biocytin 13, 10 HEPES, BAPTA 10, TEACl 3, Na2ATP 2, MgATP 2, NaGTP 0.2, pH 7.3, 290–300 mOsm. Climbing fiber currents were elicited by stimulation in the internal granular layer with a saline-filled glass pipette.
Signals were recorded and filtered at 5 kHz using an Axopatch 200A amplifier (Axon Instrument). Current and voltage signal were digitized at 50 kHz using a Digidata 1322A (Axon Instruments) prior to being recorded directly using Clampex (10.2) software. Analysis was performed off-line using Clampfit (10.2) software. Data were filtered before analysis with a cut off frequency of 1.5 or 1 kHz.
Graphs, Fitting Procedures and Statistics
Sigma plot 12.5 software was used for graphic representations of the data, fitting procedures and statistical analysis. For channels conductance analysis the normality Shapiro-Wilk test, and equal variance test were used before running an un-paired Student’s t-test. Data were considered statistically significant when P < 0.05. * is used for P values between 0.05 and 0.01; ** for P values between P > 0.01 and 0.001. *** is used for P values < 0.001. The quality of the fit was determined using the prediction error.
Results
Two GABAA Conductances in Neonatal PCs
The single-channel properties of GABAA receptors have been extensively studied in neuronal cultures and brain slices. In cerebellar granule cells, three main conductance (28, 17, 12 pS) have been characterized and are attributed to distinct GABAA receptor subtypes (Brickley et al., 1999). We analyzed currents activated by isoguvacine or muscimol, two GABAA receptor agonists, recorded from 108 cell-attached patches from PCs in acute cerebellar slices from male and female mice, between P5 and P45. In 10 patches, two conductance levels were recorded simultaneously (Figure 1). Figure 1A1 illustrates current traces showing two channels subtypes with a slope conductance of 19 pS and 7.3 pS, reversing at the same potential, indicating a similar ionic selectivity (Figure 1A3). The amplitude distribution in Figure 1A2 shows that the dominant channel conductance in this patch is level 1, and that it is not the result of simultaneous openings of two level 2 channels. The mean conductance slope value is 17.7 ± 0.9 pS for level 1 and 8.1 ± 0.5 pS for level 2 (Figure 1B, n = 10); these are significantly different (P < 0.001).
Figure 1. Two levels of type A gamma-aminobutyric acid (GABAA) receptor channel conductances can be recorded from the same Purkinje cell (PC) membrane patch. (A1) Current traces recorded in cell-attached conditions at a potential of 80 mV relative to resting potential (RP). Two amplitude levels of GABAA receptor channels are seen (level 1 in red, level 2 in blue). (A2) Data from the same patch as in panel (A1) is illustrated in a current amplitude histogram. The distribution is fitted by two Gaussians, with peaks at 1.65 pA and 0.74 pA. (A3) The relation between single channel current amplitude and the potential relative to RP for the two conductance levels. Linear regressions give slope conductances of 19 pS (red) and 7.3 pS (blue). The reversal potential for the two conducting levels is −11 mV relative to RP. (B) Bar graphs of level 1 (red) and level 2 (blue) conductances recorded in 7 PC membrane patches. The mean value of level 1 is 17.7 ± 0.9 pA and differs significantly (***p < 0.001) from the mean value of level 2 (8.1 ± 0.5 pA). (C) Histogram distribution of the slope conductance of level 1 (red) and level 2 (blue). Each conductance distribution follows a normal distribution fitted by a Gaussian with a peak value of 17 pS for level 1 and 7.5 pS for level 2.
The histogram distribution of the slope conductance of level 1 (in red) and 2 (in blue) channels are illustrated in Figure 1C. For both channel types the amplitude distribution is normal and fitted with a Gaussian function but with distinct peak values, 17 pS for level 1 and 7.5 pS for level 2.
GABA Conductance Switch During PC Development
A switch in the dominant GABAA receptor channel conductance occurs during development, from primarily level 1 in immature PCs (P5–12) to primarily level 2 in mature (P26–45) PCs in both sexes. Two recordings from PCs (female mice) illustrating this switch are shown in Figure 2A1. At P6, the slope conductance of the dominant GABAA receptor channel is 18 pS, whereas at P24 it is 10.8 pS (Figure 2A2).
Figure 2. Developmental evolution of the main GABAA receptor channel conductance. (A1) Current traces recorded in cell-attached conditions at different potentials relative to RP as indicated, from a female at P6 (left) and at P32 (right). (A2) Current-voltage relationship for the channel recorded at P6 (left) and at P32 (right). Linear regressions yield a conductance of 18 pS (level 1) and a reversal potential at +15 mV relative to RP for the channel recorded at P6; and a conductance of 10.8 pS (level 2) and reversal potential at −8 mV relative to RP for the channel recorded at P32. (B) The left panel shows the conductance of the dominant GABAA receptor channel recorded from males between P5–P12 (n = 14) and P28–45 (n = 15). Each point represents an individual cell. The bar graphs show the mean values ± SD from P5–12 (red) and P28–45 (blue). These mean values (17.3 and 10.1 pS) are significantly different (***p < 0.001). The right panel illustrates the conductance of the dominant GABAA receptor channel recorded from females between P6–P12 (n = 11) and P26–36 (n = 6). Each point represents an individual cell. The bar graphs show the mean values ± SD at P6–12 (red) and P26–36 (blue). These mean values (18.5 and 10.5 pS) are significantly different (***p < 0.001). The conductances at P5–12 in males and at P6–12 in females are not significantly different and neither is significantly different from the mean level 1 conductance. The conductances at P24–45 in males and at P26–36 in females are not significantly different and neither is significantly different from the mean level 2 conductance.
Figure 2B summarizes and compares the conductances of the dominant channels recorded in males (left panel) at P5–12 (newborn) and at P28–45 (young adult) and in females (right panel) at P6–12 and P26–36. In males the mean conductance of the dominant GABAA channel switch from 17.3 ± 1.6 pS (n = 14) at P5–12 to 10.5 pS ± 1.7 pS (n = 15, p < 0.001) at P28–45; and in females from 18.5 ± 3.0 pS (n = 11) at P6–12 to 9.8 ± 8 pS (n = 6, p < 0.001) at P26–36. In newborn mice (both sexes), the mean conductance of the dominant GABAA channel is not significantly different from the level 1 conductance illustrated in Figure 1; similarly, the mean conductance of the dominant GABAA channel in young-adult mice (both sexes) is not significantly different from the level 2 conductance illustrated in Figure 1. Thus GABAA channels in PCs switch from level 1 to level 2 during post-natal development.
Sexual Dimorphism of the Chloride Gradient Shift
We then determined the reversal potential of the dominant GABAA receptor channel relative to the resting membrane potential (RP) during PC development in males and females. Figure 3 illustrates current recordings obtained in PCs from a P16 male (Figure 3A) and a P16 female (Figure 3B). The reversal potential of the GABAA receptor channel is determined by the linear regression used to fit the current/voltage (I/V) relationship of a single GABAA receptor channel. At P16, the reversal potential is negative to RP (−14 mV) in males (Figure 3A, bottom panel), but positive to RP (+4 mV) in females (Figure 3B, bottom panel).
Figure 3. Developmental evolution of the GABAA receptor channel reversal potential in males and females and climbing fiber synapse elimination. (A) Recordings from a P16 male. Current traces show the GABAA receptor channel recorded from a PC membrane patch at three potentials as indicated on top of each trace. The I/V curve below, a linear regression of the data from this channel, gives a conductance of 15.2 pS and a reversal potential with an extrapolated value of −14 mV relative to RP. (B) Recordings from a P16 female. Current traces show the GABAA receptor channel recorded from a PC membrane patch at three potentials as indicated on top of each trace. The I/V curve below, a linear regression of the data from this channel, gives a conductance of 17.5 pS and a reversal potential with an extrapolated value of + 4 mV relative to RP. Note that at the same post-natal age the GABAA receptor channel reverses at a negative potential relative to RP in the male, but in the female reversal occurs at a positive potential relative to RP. (C) Left graph, evolution of the mean reversal potential of the dominant GABAA receptor channel as a function of the age in males (black) and females (red). The data were best fit (R 0.98) by a sigmoidal hill function with four parameters (f = Y0 + a/(1 + exp(−(X − X0)/b)) between P5 and P48 in males (black line, a (max) = 22, b (slope) = −0.31, X0 = 14.4 and Y0 (min) = −13.5) and in between P8 and P36 in females (red line, a = 24.2, b = −1.25, X0 = 18.5, Y0 = −8.3). The horizontal line at 0 mV shows the depolarizing/hyperpolarizing switch. The bar graphs in inset compare the mean values of the reversal potential relative to RP in males (black) and in females (red) at P8–12, P15–19 and P26–36. A significant difference between males and females (***p < 0.001) was found at P8–12 and P15–19. (D) Right graph: regression of climbing fiber multi-innervation of Purkinje cells (PCs) is similar in males and females, indicating that this basic circuit maturation during this developmental period is not sex-dependent.
The changes in GABA reversal potential relative to RP in cerebellar PCs from males and females is illustrated in Figure 3C. The curve in males can be fitted with a sigmoidal function (four parameters, X0 14.4 days). Between P5–P11 the reversal potential is stable, around +10 mV relative to RP; the reversal potential then rapidly reaches a negative value around P14 and stabilizes at P26 around −10 mV relative to RP (−10.3 ± 5.0 mV, n = 10 at P ≥ 26, see bottom graph in inset). Therefore, GABA exerts depolarizing effects in male PCs prior to P15 and hyperpolarizing effects thereafter.
With the exception of the earliest post-natal days, the developmental curve of the reversal potential in female PCs also follows a sigmoid pattern from P8 to P21 but with a systematically more depolarized value compared to males (Figure 3C, red symbols). Although the reversal potential is not significantly more negative between P5 and P7 in females than in males (+3.5 mV vs. +8.7 mV), it then becomes significantly (p < 0.001) more positive between P8 and P12 (+15.8 ± 3.6 mV, n = 11) than in males (+9.1 ± 4.1 mV, n = 8, see top graph in inset). From this higher plateau value, the reversal potential change over time is best fitted by a sigmoid curve (four parameters, X0 18.7 days) and decreases to −8.5 ± 3.5 mV (n = 8), at P26 and older (see bottom graph in inset). These observations show that GABA exerts more depolarizing actions in female PCs than in male PCs during a period of nearly 2 weeks (P8 to P21). In addition, the GABA switch is delayed in females by 4 days (P18 vs. P14 in males), such that from P15–19 the reversal potential is negative relative to RP in males (−15.9 ± 7.4 mV, n = 17) but still positive in females (+7.6 ± 5 mV, n = 10; see top graph in inset).
An important step in cerebellar synaptic maturation during the early postnatal period is the refinement of climbing fiber synapses on PCs, from a multiple climbing fiber innervation to mono-innervation (Hashimoto and Kano, 2003). To determine possible links between GABA signaling and circuit refinement in terms of climbing fiber synapse elimination, we measured the number of climbing fiber synapses on each PC from males and females at different ages (Figure 3D). The progress of synapse elimination in both sexes was very similar, indicating that sex-dependent differences in GABAergic signaling does not disrupt this fundamental developmental process.
The Developmental Switch of the Chloride Gradient Is Delayed in the Valproate Mouse Model of Autism
In the valproate rat model of autism, the developmental regulation of the chloride gradient, which determines the effects of GABAA receptor activation on neuronal excitability, is disrupted in hippocampal neurons. As a result, the developmental excitatory-inhibitory switch of GABAergic effects is abolished in hippocampal neurons from valproate rats, with depolarizing actions from birth onwards (Tyzio et al., 2014). We used this same model in mice to analyze the developmental changes in GABAA receptor channel conductance and the chloride gradient. We recorded 57 PC-attached recordings of GABAA receptor channels from 35 males and 22 females from valproate-treated dams, at different postnatal ages. As in control mice, recordings from valproate mice revealed two conductance levels (level 1 mean value 15.8 ± 0.4 pS and level 2 mean value 7.8 ± 0.5 pS, p < 0.001) that could occasionally be recorded from the same patch (five patches, Figure 4A). Furthermore, as in control mice, PCs from valproate-treated male or female newborn mice show a dominant level 1 conductance channel, and PCs from juvenile and young adult PCs showed a dominant level 2 conductance channel. In males, the mean conductance switches from 17.1 ± 1.6 pS (n = 13) at P6–10, to 13.1 pS ± 1.8 pS (n = 5) at P24–30 (Figure 4B, p = 0.002). In females, the mean conductance switches from 17.7 ± 3.2 pS (n = 5) at P7–10, to 10.9 pS ± 1.6 pS (n = 7) at P23–30 (Figure 4C, p < 0.001). These conductance levels are not different between the sexes nor are they different from conductance levels 1 and 2 (Figures 1B, 4A) in control mice. Therefore, the conductance shift is not impacted in this ASD model.
Figure 4. The valproate mouse model of autism alters developmental evolution of GABAA channel reversal potential. (A) Mean slope conductance of the two conductance levels found simultaneously in five cell-attached recordings from PCs of male and female mice prenatally exposed to valproate. The level 1 (red) conductance (15.8 ± 0.4 pS) is significantly different from the level 2 (blue) conductance (7.8 ± 0.5 pS; ***p < 0.001). (B) Conductance of the dominant GABAA receptor channel recorded in PCs from male valproate mice at P6–10 (n = 13) and at P24–30 (n = 5). Each symbol represents a single cell-attached recording. The bar graphs show the mean values ± SD between P6–10 (red) and between P24–30 (blue). These mean values (17.1 and 13.1 pS) are significantly different (**p = 0.002). (C) Conductance of the dominant GABAA receptor channel recorded from female valproate mice at P7–P10 (n = 5) and at P23–30 (n = 7). Each symbol represents a single cell-attached recording. The bar graphs show the mean values ± SD at P7–10 (red) and at P23–30 (blue). These mean values (18.5 and 10.5 pS) are significantly different (***p < 0.001). The mean conductances at P6–10 in males and at P7–10 in females are not significantly different, and neither is significantly different from the level 1 channel conductance. The mean conductances at P24–30 in males and at P23–30 in females are not significantly different and neither is significantly different from the level 2 conductance. (D,E) Reversal potential of GABAA receptor channels during development, in PCs recorded from male (D) and female (E) valproate mice. (D) In males, the depolarizing/hyperpolarizing switch is delayed in valproate animals (black symbols). Evolution of the GABAA reversal potential with age (P5–P30). From P10 to P30 the data are best fit using a sigmoidal hill function with four parameters (black line, a = 21, b = −0.16, X0 = 17.7 and Y0 = −8). The curve in red represents the same curve but for control males. The inset bar graphs compare the mean values of the reversal potential relative to RP in control males (black) and valproate males (red) at P6–8 and P15–19. A significant difference between control males and valproate males (***p < 0.001) is detected. (E) In females, the depolarizing/hyperpolarizing switch is less altered in valproate animals (black symbols) Evolution of the GABAA receptor channel reversal potential with age (P6–P30) in valproate females. Data are best fit by a sigmoidal function four parameters (black curve, a = 17, b = −0.28, X0 = 23 and Y0 = −5). The red curve is a sigmoidal fit of the evolution of the reversal potential in control females and the red open dots the mean value in control females not included in the fit by the red line. The bar graphs in inset compare the mean values of the reversal potential relative to RP in control females (black) and valproate females (red) at P20–23. A significant difference between control females and valproate females (**p = 0.003) is reported.
However, the developmental change in the GABAA receptor channel reversal potential was clearly more complicated in valproate-treated male mice (Figure 4D, black symbols) than in control male mice (red sigmoid, from Figure 3C). At P6–8 the reversal potential is significantly (p < 0.001, see graph in inset) more positive relative to RP (+20.6 ± 6.5 mV, n = 10) in valproate males compared to control males (+7.4 ± 4.6 mV, n = 9), decreases to a plateau value around +12 mV at P10, then reaches a negative value around P20 (sigmoid curve, X0 = P17.7 days). Thus, the switch from depolarizing to hyperpolarizing effect of GABA is delayed by 3 days in valproate male mice compared to control males. Therefore during a relatively long period, GABAergic currents are more depolarizing in valproate mice than in age matched control mice. Indeed, at P15–19 the mean reversal potential of the GABAA channel is still positive in valproate mice (+6.6 ± 4.4 mV, n = 17 compared to −15.9 ± 7.4 mV, n = 11 in control male mice, p < 0.001, see graph in inset). These differences are less evident in females (Figure 4E). Yet at P7 in valproate female mice the reversal potential of GABAA channels is around +20 mV relative to RP whereas it is about +5 mV relative to RP in control females. Furthermore, a comparison between the sigmoidal fit of the data obtained from valproate females (black curve, X0 22.7 days) and the sigmoidal fit of the data obtained in control females (red curve, from Figure 3C) reveals a delay of the GABA switch of almost 4 days. As a consequence, at P20-P23 the mean reversal potential of the GABAA channels is positive relative to RP (+5.8 ± 6.5 mV, n = 8) in valproate treated females but negative (−7.0 ± 3.0 mV, n = 8, p = 0.003) in control females (see inset Figure 4D).
Sex Dependence of the GABA Conductance Shift: Parallel Development of GABAA Channel Reversal Potential and Conductance
To determine whether the GABAA channel conductance and the reversal potential switch occur in parallel, we compared the development of the GABAA channels conductance in males and females (Figure 5). As a first approach, the slope conductance of the dominant GABAA channel was plotted as a function of age in males (Figure 5A1) and females (Figure 5A2). The evolution of the conductance could be fitted in both sexes by a sigmoidal function (black and red lines) but with distinct parameters (slope = −1.33, X0 = 14.7 days for males and slope = −3.3, X0 = 17.7 days for females) indicating that the shift in conductance is slower and later in females compared to males. In both sexes, a single channel with larger slope conductance (25–27 pS, not included in the fit) was recorded in three patches and may indicate the presence of a rarely-occurring third conductance level of GABAA receptor channel, as previously described for cerebellar granule cells (Brickley et al., 1999).
Figure 5. Distinct evolution of the GABA switch between conductance levels 1 and 2 in males, females and valproate-treated males. (A1) Dominant GABAA channel conductance from level 1 to level 2 as a function of the post–natal age in males (each point represents a single cell). Data are best fit with a sigmoidal hill function with four parameters (black line, a = 6.2, b = −1.33, X0 = 14.7 and Y0 = 11.1). (A2) Dominant GABAA channel conductance from level 1 to level 2 as a function of the post–natal age in females (each point represents a single cell). Data are best fit with a sigmoidal hill function with four parameters (red line, a = 7.8, b = −3.3, X0 = 17.7 and Y0 = 10.5). Note the presence of three channels with a conductance around 25 pS. (B1) Histogram distribution of the slope conductance of level 2 channels (pooled data from Figures 1B, 2B, 4A). The distribution is normal and fit by a Gaussian (blue line) with a peak value at 10.9 pA. (B2) Histogram distribution of the slope conductance of level 1 channels (pooled data from Figures 1B, 2B, 4A). The distribution is normal and fit by a Gaussian function (red line) with a peak value at 17.8 pA. (B3) Histogram distribution of the slope conductance of level 1 and level 2 channels (pooled data from B1 and B2). To determine the limit of conductance between level 2 (blue line) and level 1 (red line) the Gaussian fits obtained in (A1, A2) were superimposed. The limit between levels 1 and 2 is 14 pS. Channels with conductances ≥14 pS are classified as level 1 channels and channels with conductances <14 pS are classified as level 2 channels. (C1) Data from male mice. The bar plots show the % of level 1 channels (conductance ≥14 pS) at increasing post-natal age ranges, as indicated on top of each bar. (C2) Data from female mice. The bar plots show the % of level 1 channels (conductance ≥14 pS) at increasing post- natal age ranges (the same range as in C1). (C3) Percentage of conductance level 1 as the function of the age in male mice (black symbols) female mice (red symbols) and valproate treated male mice (blue symbols). Data for control males (black curve) and females (red curve) are best fit using a sigmoidal function with the following parameters: a = 99, b = −1.9, X0 = 15.3 and Y0 = 3.2 for males; and a = 100, b = −2.42, X0 = 18.5, Y0 = 0.43 for females.
In order to quantitatively analyze the conductance switch, we determined the conductance ranges of the two levels. We first constructed separate histogram distributions of conductance levels 1 and 2 channels by pooling conductance values of GABAA channels recorded in both sexes between P5–12 for conductance level 1 (Figure 5B1) and conductance values of GABAA channels recorded at P28–45 in males and P26–36 in females for conductance level 2 (Figure 5B2). The distributions are fitted with distinct Gaussian functions, giving a peak of 17.8 pS for level 1 and 10.9 pS for level 2. Next, we combined the two distributions and associated fits (Figure 5B3), to show that the two fits intercept at a value of 13.9 pS. From this observation, we classified channels with a slope conductance of 14–22 pS as level 1, and channels with a slope conductance <14 pS as level 2.
Then, we determined the developmental alterations of level 1 channel percentage at different post-natal ages (Figures 5C1–C3). In both sexes the proportion of patches displaying dominant level 1 GABAA receptor channel decreases with age; but the histograms in Figures 5C1,C2 show that this drop in level 1 conductance (defined as presence in less than 50% of patches) occurred earlier in males (P14; Figure 5C1) than in females (P17; Figure 5C2). Therefore, during a transitional period (around P15–20), PCs in females have a higher-conductance dominant channel than male PCs.
The evolution of the proportion of level 1 GABAA channel as a function of age (Figure 5C3) is similar in both males and females to the evolution of the chloride reversal potential. Both curves are sigmoidal (four parameters) with similar X0 s (14.4 days for the level 1 plot and 15.3 days for the chloride reversal potential plot in males; and 18.5 days for both the level 1 plot and the chloride reversal potential in females). Interestingly the switch of GABAA channel conductance is also altered in valproate male mice with 25% of level 1 channel dominant in young adult animals.
Sexual Dimorphism of the GABA Excitatory/Inhibitory Shift
Finally, we determined possible links between the chloride driving force and the excitatory or inhibitory actions of the GABAA agonist isoguvacine on PC firing, with or without NBQX. Similar effects of isoguvacine were observed in the presence of NBQX suggesting that glutamatergic neurotransmission was not involved (not shown; n = 80). When the reversal potential of the GABAA channel was positive to RP (at P6), isoguvacine increased the firing rate in males (Figure 6A, n = 3 out of 5) and in females (Figure 6B, n = 3 out of 6). This effect was accompanied by a progressive decrease in the size of the spikes due to the sodium channel inactivation produced by a strong depolarization. In the remaining recordings, PCs were silent and isoguvacine applications led to a depolarization. In older PCs, when the reversal potential of the GABAA channels is negative to RP, isoguvacine decreased spontaneous activity of PCs, without altering the spike amplitude in both males (P27; Figure 7A) and females (P25; Figure 7B).
Figure 6. Activation of GABAA receptors increases PC firing in P6 male and female mice. Cell-attached recordings (voltage clamp) of PC action potentials. (A) PC from a male at P6. The upper trace shows the control recording (in the presence of NBQX), and the lower trace shows the recording in presence of Isoguvacine (10−5 M). Isoguvacine application increases firing frequency and progressively reduced the spike amplitude. (B) PC from a female at P6. Upper trace, control conditions (in the presence of NBQX), lower trace in the presence of Isoguvacine. As in the male, isoguvacine increased firing frequency and progressively reduced spike amplitude.
Figure 7. GABAA receptor activation inhibits PC firing in young adult male and female mice. (A) Cell-attached recording (voltage clamp) of the spontaneous spiking activity of a PC from a male P27 mouse. Upper panel: current traces recorded in control conditions (in the presence of NBQX), in the presence of Isoguvacine, and after washout. Isoguvacine silences the cell, and activity recovers after washout. Lower panel: data from the same cell showing instantaneous action potential frequency (left) and action potential amplitude (right) over time. Isoguvacine application is indicated by the red line. The control spike frequency is about 7 Hz; isoguvacine abolishes the spiking activity of the cell; and this effect is reversible upon washout. Note that spike amplitude is not reduced before the silencing of the cell. (B) As in panel (A), but from a female P25 mouse. Upper panel: current traces recorded in control conditions, in the presence of Isoguvacine, and following washout. Isoguvacine silences the cell, and activity recovers after washout. Lower panel: data from the same cell, showing instantaneous action potential frequency (left) and action potential amplitude (right) over time. Isoguvacine is applied as indicated by the red line. The control spike frequency is around 30 Hz. In presence of isoguvacine the spiking activity of the cell is almost abolished, but the spike amplitude is not altered.
Additional experiments confirmed the sex and age difference in the actions of isoguvacine. In males at P15, after 2 min isoguvacine application, PC firing was blocked by 75 ± 35% in all cells tested (n = 4; example in Figure 8A), and completely blocked (100%) for PCs recorded from P16 to P29 (n = 14; example in Figure 7A). However in females at P17, 2 min of isoguvacine increased PC firing frequency by 220 ± 161%, accompanied by a reduction in spike amplitude in 5 out of 13 PCs (example in Figure 8B), sometimes preceded by a temporary reduction in the firing frequency (n = 2, including the example in Figure 8B). Isoguvacine had no effect on firing frequency in two PCs from female mice, and inhibited firing frequency in seven of these PCs. The excitatory effect of isoguvacine continued to be seen in female PCs (n = 2 out 5) until P19; then at P20–25 isoguvacine had only inhibitory effects (87% ± 8% inhibition, n = 6).
Figure 8. Different effects of GABAA receptor activation in males and females at P15–P17. (A) Cell-attached recording (voltage-clamp) of spontaneous spiking activity of a PC from a P15 male mouse. Upper panel: current traces recorded in control conditions, with isoguvacine, and after washout. Regular action potential firing is reduced in the presence of isoguvacine and recovers after washout. Lower panel: data from the same cell showing instantaneous action potential frequency with time (left) and action potential amplitude with time (right). Isoguvacine application is indicated by the red line. Control firing frequency is about 30 Hz; isoguvacine abolishes firing, and this effect is reversible. Note that the abolition of the discharge is not linked to a decrease in spike amplitude. (B) As in panel (A), but from a P17 female mouse. Upper part: current traces recorded in control conditions, with isoguvacine, and after washout. Regular action potential firing is reduced in the presence of isoguvacine and recovers after washout. Lower graphs, from the same cell, show instantaneous action potential frequency with time (left) and action potential amplitude with time (right) Isoguvacine application is indicated by the red line. Control firing frequency is about 30 Hz; with isoguvacine application, a brief inhibition of the firing frequency is followed by an increase, up to 100 Hz; this effect is reversible. Isoguvacine also reversibly decreased spike amplitude.
To summarize the effects of isoguvacine on spontaneous PC, firing we assigned a value of +1 when isoguvacine produced an increased firing frequency, 0.5 when the increase was preceded by a short inhibition of firing, 0 when isoguvacine had no effect and −1 when isoguvacine inhibited firing. Figure 9 shows the mean valence as a function of age for PCs from male and female mice, and clearly illustrates the different time course of the shift to inhibitory isoguvacine effects between male and females.
Figure 9. Summary of changes in spontaneous action potential frequency following GABAA receptor activation by isoguvacine, in males and females. The mean valence of the isoguvacine effects is plotted as a function of the post-natal age in male and female mice.
Discussion
Development of GABAA Receptor Channel Subunit Composition in Cerebellar PCs
Somatic GABAA receptor channels in adult vertebrate neurons have complex gating and multiple conductance states ranging from 7 to 36 pS, with a single dominant conductance state in each cell type (Newland et al., 1991; Robello et al., 1998; Brickley et al., 1999; Mortensen and Smart, 2006). We found three main conductance levels in PC, with conductances of 7–12 pS, 15–18 pS, and occasionally 25–28 pS. Similar conductance values are observed in adult cerebellar granule cells, but with a shift of the dominant conductance from medium (15–18 pS) to small (7–12 pS) during maturation. This shift likely reflects a reorganization of GABAA channel subunit composition (Moss et al., 1990; Verdoorn et al., 1990; Mortensen and Smart, 2006). Although it is difficult to equate GABA conductance levels with specific subunits combinations (Brickley et al., 1999), it is likely that in young-adult PCs, receptors lack the γ subunit (Verdoorn et al., 1990; Fisher and Macdonald, 1997; Amato et al., 1999; Hörtnagl et al., 2013). The dominant medium conductance level (18 pS) in newborn PCs suggests the presence of an α1β1 GABAA receptor at this developmental stage (Moss et al., 1990). The very rare occurrence of the 25–28 pS conducting level is may be related to a low level of expression of the γ2 subunit (Mortensen and Smart, 2006). Our observations suggest a subunit reorganization during development (but see Nadler et al., 1994; Haghir et al., 2013) but further experiments are required to determine if this is associated with changes in IPSCs kinetics.
Sexual Dimorphism of GABA Driving Force in Wild Type Mice and in the Valproate Model of Autism
In a wide range of brain structures, the polarity of GABAA receptor effects is regulated by the expression and activity of two chloride membrane transporters: NKCC1 and KCC2. NKCC1 is expressed in immature neurons and actively transports chloride ions into the cell; KCC2 is expressed in mature neurons and actively transports chloride ions out of the cell. This changes in transporter expression leads to depolarizing effects of GABA in immature neurons and hyperpolarizing effects in mature neurons (Ben-Ari, 2014; Watanabe and Fukuda, 2015; Raimondo et al., 2017) with a developmental switch around birth (Tyzio et al., 2014; Watanabe and Fukuda, 2015). However, PCs do not express NKCC1 (Mikawa et al., 2002), suggesting that alternative mechanisms are required to regulate chloride homeostasis, including chloride channels (Zhang et al., 2015; Rahmati et al., 2016) and KCC2. The developmental GABA switch occurs in PCs at the end of the second post-natal week (see also Eilers et al., 2001), when profound morphological and functional alterations occur in PCs (Dusart and Flamant, 2012). The timing of the switch is strongly dependent on the specific cerebral structure; it is similar in the antero-ventral cochlear nucleus (Song et al., 2012) and substantia nigra pars reticulata (SNR; Kyrozis et al., 2006), and earlier in other brain structures (Stein et al., 2004; Tyzio et al., 2007; Allain et al., 2011; Witte et al., 2014). Our results also suggest that the regulation of chloride homeostasis is sex dependent, since it is delayed in female mice. A similar sex dependence of chloride homeostasis has been observed in the substantia nigra (Galanopoulou, 2008).
The mechanisms underlying these sex differences are poorly understood. The testosterone surge in males that occurs during late gestation and shortly after birth (Dean and McCarthy, 2008) has been suggested to promote the expression and the activity of NKCC1 and to depress the synthesis and activity of KCC2 (Waddell and McCarthy, 2012), and thus make the GABA switch occurs later in males than in females. This effect has been shown in hippocampal cultures (Nuñez and McCarthy, 2007), SNR neurons in acute slices (Kyrozis et al., 2006), and embryonic hypothalamic neurons in culture (Mir et al., 2017). However, the GABA switch in PCs is delayed in females, rather than in males, despite this testosterone effect on KCC2, suggesting that alternative mechanisms must be present. PCs synthesize estradiol from cholesterol in the neonatal period and the enzymes involved in this synthesis are developmentally regulated with a different profile in males and in females (Dean and McCarthy, 2008). In PCs, estradiol promotes BDNF-mediated dendritic growth, spinogenesis and synaptogenesis during neonatal life (Tsutsui et al., 2011), and BDNF inhibits KCC2 activity (Huang et al., 2013). Furthermore, the development of the cerebellar expression of insulin receptors (IR) and insulin-like growth factor-1 receptors (IGF-1R) differs in males and females. IR and IGF-1R expression increase between P0–P7 and is down regulated at P14 in males, whereas in females IR and IGF-1R are stable between P0–P7 and up-regulated at P14 (Haghir et al., 2013). IGF-1 accelerates the developmental switch between NKCC1 and KCC2 chloride transporters in the visual cortex (Baroncelli et al., 2017). Collectively, these observations converge to suggest multiple mechanisms to explain the differences between chloride homeostasis regulation in males and females.
Using the valproate mouse model of autism, we observed that the chloride reversal potential in P5–6 valproate males is more positive than in controls (by about 10–20 mV), and that the GABA switch is delayed by 3–4 days in both males and females, extending this experimental model to the cerebellum. Increased excitatory-inhibitory ratio is suggested to underlie the pathogenesis of autism and successful clinical trials have shown that the NKCC1 antagonist bumetanide, which restores low (Cl−)I and GABAergic inhibition, also attenuates the severity of autism (Rubenstein and Merzenich, 2003; Lemonnier and Ben-Ari, 2010; Tyzio et al., 2014; Lee et al., 2016; Uzunova et al., 2016; Lemonnier et al., 2017). It is now accepted that the cerebellum is involved in higher order functions (Schmahmann and Sherman, 1998) including perceptual processes (Baumann et al., 2015), in addition to its roles in balance, posture, and motor control. The cerebellum has been implicated in many psychiatric disorders (Phillips et al., 2015) and cerebellar abnormality is associated with ASD (Tsai et al., 2012). Several reports in individuals with autism or animal models of autism describe alterations in PC density and properties (Skefos et al., 2014; Tsai, 2016). Thus, chloride gradient modifications and alterations in the GABA switch in cerebellar PCs that we show here may be particularly relevant for further studies of autism and the role of the cerebellum.
Parallel Development of GABAA Receptor Channel Subunit Composition and Chloride Gradient
Our data show that the kinetics of the shift of the dominant conductance levels during development, from medium (15–18 pS) to small conductance (7–12 pS), are similar to the kinetics of the development of the chloride gradient. Both phenomena followed a similar sigmoidal process, are delayed in females, and are altered in valproate mice, suggesting interactions between the subunit composition of GABAA channels and intracellular chloride concentration (Succol et al., 2012).
Interestingly, in PCs, it has been suggested that an interaction between KCC2 and a specific subunit of the GABAA receptor represents a fundamental mechanism of regulation of GABAergic synapses (Huang et al., 2013). Furthermore, membrane expression of the β3 GABAA receptor subunit at different postnatal developmental stages is observed in rats exposed prenatally to valproic acid (Li et al., 2017). Finally, alterations in the efficacy of neuronal inhibition mediated by GABAA receptors containing β3 subunits have been implicated in autism (Vien et al., 2015). Collectively, these observations raise the possibility of parallel convergent developmental alterations of GABAergic signals, linking chloride gradient and subunit composition.
Conclusions and Perspectives
The conductance properties of GABAA receptors as well as the chloride equilibrium potential and the polarity of GABA effects in PCs are developmentally regulated in parallel, occurring later than in other brain structures, which is coherent with the delayed development of cerebellar PCs to the postnatal period. The switch is also delayed by 4 days in females, indicating sexual dimorphism in keeping with recently reported sex differences in synaptic excitation, inhibition and intrinsic properties (Mercer et al., 2016). Finally, we show that the valproate model of autism produced a shift in the chloride gradient in developing PCs from males and females, providing important potential implications for the cerebellum in the pathology of autism.
Author Contributions
SR, AL, YB-A, BP and J-LB wrote the manuscript. SR, AL and J-LB designed the research, performed the experiments and analyzed the data.
Conflict of Interest Statement
YB-A is CEO of Neurochlore, a biotech company devoted to treat autism.
The remaining authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.
References
Allain, A. E., Le Corronc, H., Delpy, A., Cazenave, W., Meyrand, P., Legendre, P., et al. (2011). Maturation of the GABAergic transmission in normal and pathologic motoneurons. Neural Plast. 2011:905624. doi: 10.1155/2011/905624
Amato, A., Connolly, C. N., Moss, S. J., and Smart, T. G. (1999). Modulation of neuronal and recombinant GABAA receptors by redox reagents. J. Physiol. 517, 35–50. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7793.1999.0035z.x
Baroncelli, L., Cenni, M. C., Melani, R., Deidda, G., Landi, S., Narducci, R., et al. (2017). Early IGF-1 primes visual cortex maturation and accelerates developmental switch between NKCC1 and KCC2 chloride transporters in enriched animals. Neuropharmacology 113, 167–177. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropharm.2016.02.034
Baumann, O., Borra, R. J., Bower, J. M., Cullen, K. E., Habas, C., Ivry, R. B., et al. (2015). Consensus paper: the role of the cerebellum in perceptual processes. Cerebellum 14, 197–220. doi: 10.1007/s12311-014-0627-7
Ben-Ari, Y. (2002). Excitatory actions of GABA during development: the nature of the nurture. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 3, 728–739. doi: 10.1038/nrn920
Ben-Ari, Y. (2014). The GABA excitatory/inhibitory developmental sequence: a personal journey. Neuroscience 279, 187–219. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2014.08.001
Ben-Ari, Y. (2017). NKCC1 chloride importer antagonists attenuate many neurological and psychiatric disorders. Trends Neurosci. 40, 536–554. doi: 10.1016/j.tins.2017.07.001
Ben-Ari, Y., Cherubini, E., Corradetti, R., and Gaiarsa, J. L. (1989). Giant synaptic potentials in immature rat CA3 hippocampal neurones. J. Physiol. 416, 303–325. doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.1989.sp017762
Brickley, S. G., Cull-Candy, S. G., and Farrant, M. (1999). Single-channel properties of synaptic and extrasynaptic GABAA receptors suggest differential targeting of receptor subtypes. J. Neurosci. 19, 2960–2973. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.19-08-02960.1999
Dean, S. L., and McCarthy, M. M. (2008). Steroids, sex and the cerebellar cortex: implications for human disease. Cerebellum 7, 38–47. doi: 10.1007/s12311-008-0003-6
Dusart, I., and Flamant, F. (2012). Profound morphological and functional changes of rodent Purkinje cells between the first and the second postnatal weeks: a metamorphosis? Front. Neuroanat. 6:11. doi: 10.3389/fnana.2012.00011
Eilers, J., Plant, T. D., Marandi, N., and Konnerth, A. (2001). GABA-mediated Ca2+ signalling in developing rat cerebellar Purkinje neurones. J. Physiol. 536, 429–437. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7793.2001.0429c.xd
Fatemi, S. H., Aldinger, K. A., Ashwood, P., Bauman, M. L., Blaha, C. D., Blatt, G. J., et al. (2012). Consensus paper: pathological role of the cerebellum in autism. Cerebellum 11, 777–807. doi: 10.1007/s12311-012-0355-9
Fisher, J. L., and Macdonald, R. L. (1997). Single channel properties of recombinant GABAA receptors containing γ 2 or delta subtypes expressed with α 1 and β 3 subtypes in mouse L929 cells. J. Physiol. 505, 283–297. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7793.1997.283bb.x
Fritschy, J. M., Paysan, J., Enna, A., and Mohler, H. (1994). Switch in the expression of rat GABAA-receptor subtypes during postnatal development: an immunohistochemical study. J. Neurosci. 14, 5302–5324. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.14-09-05302.1994
Galanopoulou, A. S. (2008). Sexually dimorphic expression of KCC2 and GABA function. Epilepsy Res. 80, 99–113. doi: 10.1016/j.eplepsyres.2008.04.013
Haghir, H., Rezaee, A. A., Nomani, H., Sankian, M., Kheradmand, H., and Hami, J. (2013). Sexual dimorphism in expression of insulin and insulin-like growth factor-I receptors in developing rat cerebellum. Cell. Mol. Neurobiol. 33, 369–377. doi: 10.1007/s10571-012-9903-6
Hashimoto, K., and Kano, M. (2003). Functional differentiation of multiple climbing fiber inputs during synapse elimination in the developing cerebellum. Neuron 38, 785–796. doi: 10.1016/s0896-6273(03)00298-8
Hörtnagl, H., Tasan, R. O., Wieselthaler, A., Kirchmair, E., Sieghart, W., and Sperk, G. (2013). Patterns of mRNA and protein expression for 12 GABAA receptor subunits in the mouse brain. Neuroscience 236, 345–372. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2013.01.008
Huang, Y., Wang, J. J., and Yung, W. H. (2013). Coupling between GABA-A receptor and chloride transporter underlies ionic plasticity in cerebellar Purkinje neurons. Cerebellum 12, 328–330. doi: 10.1007/s12311-013-0453-3
Ingram, J. L., Peckham, S. M., Tisdale, B., and Rodier, P. M. (2000). Prenatal exposure of rats to valproic acid reproduces the cerebellar anomalies associated with autism. Neurotoxicol. Teratol. 22, 319–324. doi: 10.1016/s0892-0362(99)00083-5
Kyrozis, A., Chudomel, O., Moshé, S. L., and Galanopoulou, A. S. (2006). Sex-dependent maturation of GABAA receptor-mediated synaptic events in rat substantia nigra reticulata. Neurosci. Let. 398, 1–5. doi: 10.1016/j.neulet.2005.12.018
Laurie, D. J., Wisden, W., and Seeburg, P. H. (1992). The distribution of thirteen GABAA receptor subunit mRNAs in the rat brain: III. Embryonic and postnatal development. J. Neurosci. 11, 4151–4172. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.12-11-04151.1992
Lee, K., Goodman, L., Fourie, C., Schenk, S., Leitch, B., and Montgomery, J. M. (2016). AMPA receptors as therapeutic targets for neurological disorders. Adv. Protein Chem. Struct. Biol. 103, 203–261. doi: 10.1016/bs.apcsb.2015.10.004
Lemonnier, E., and Ben-Ari, Y. (2010). The diuretic bumetanide decreases autistic behaviour in five infants treated during 3 months with no side effects. Acta Paediatr. 99, 1885–1888. doi: 10.1111/j.1651-2227.2010.01933.x
Lemonnier, E., Villeneuve, N., Sonie, S., Serret, S., Rosier, A., Roue, M., et al. (2017). Effects of bumetanide on neurobehavioral function in children and adolescents with autism spectrum disorders. Transl. Psychiatry 7:e1124. doi: 10.1038/tp.2017.101
Li, Y., Zhou, Y., Peng, L., and Zhao, Y. (2017). Reduced protein expressions of cytomembrane GABAARβ3 at different postnatal developmental stages of rats exposed prenatally to valproic acid. Brain Res. 1671, 33–42. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2017.06.018
Mercer, A. A., Palarz, K. J., Tabatadze, N., Woolley, C. S., and Raman, I. M. (2016). Sex differences in cerebellar synaptic transmission and sex-specific responses to autism-linked Gabrb3 mutations in mice. Elife 5:e07596. doi: 10.7554/eLife.07596
Mikawa, S., Wang, C., Shu, F., Wang, T., Fukuda, A., and Sato, K. (2002). Developmental changes in KCC1, KCC2 and NKCC1 mRNAs in the rat cerebellum. Dev. Brain Res. 136, 93–100. doi: 10.1016/s0165-3806(02)00345-0
Mir, F. R., Carrer, H. F., and Cambiasso, M. J. (2017). Sex differences in depolarizing actions of GABAA receptor activation in rat embryonic hypothalamic neurons. Eur. J. Neurosci. 45, 521–527. doi: 10.1111/ejn.13467
Mortensen, M., and Smart, T. G. (2006). Extrasynaptic αβ subunit GABAA receptors on rat hippocampal pyramidal neurons. J. Physiol. 577, 841–856. doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.2006.117952
Moss, S. J., Smart, T. G., Porter, N. M., Nayeem, N., Devine, J., Stephenson, F. A., et al. (1990). Cloned GABA receptors are maintained in a stable cell line: allosteric and channel properties. Eur. J. Pharmacol. 189, 77–88. doi: 10.1016/0922-4106(90)90232-m
Nadler, L. S., Guirguis, E. R., and Siegel, R. E. (1994). GABAA receptor subunit polypeptides increase in parallel but exhibit distinct distributions in the developing rat cerebellum. J. Neurobiol. 25, 1533–1544. doi: 10.1002/neu.480251206
Newland, C. F., Colquhoun, D., and Cull-Candy, S. G. (1991). Single channels activated by high concentrations of GABA in superior cervical ganglion neurones of the rat. J. Physiol. 432, 203–233. doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.1991.sp018382
Nuñez, J. L., and McCarthy, M. M. (2007). Evidence for an extended duration of GABA-mediated excitation in the developing male versus female hippocampus. Dev. Neurobiol. 67, 1879–1890. doi: 10.1002/dneu.20567
Pangratz-Fuehrer, S., Sieghart, W., Rudolph, U., Parada, I., and Huguenard, J. R. (2016). Early postnatal switch in GABAA receptor α-subunits in the reticular thalamic nucleus. J. Neurophysiol. 115, 1183–1195. doi: 10.1152/jn.00905.2015
Phillips, J. R., Hewedi, D. H., Eissa, A. M., and Moustafa, A. A. (2015). The cerebellum and psychiatric disorders. Front. Public Health 3:66. doi: 10.3389/fpubh.2015.00066
Rahmati, N., Vinueza Veloz, M. F., Xu, J., Barone, S., Rodolfo Ben Hamida, N., Schonewille, M., et al. (2016). SLC26A11 (KBAT) in purkinje cells is critical for inhibitory transmission and contributes to locomotor coordination. eNeuro 3:ENEURO.0028–16. doi: 10.1523/ENEURO.0028-16.2016
Raimondo, J. V., Richards, B. A., and Woodin, M. A. (2017). Neuronal chloride and excitability—the big impact of small changes. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 43, 35–42. doi: 10.1016/j.conb.2016.11.012
Rivera, C., Voipio, J., Payne, J. A., Ruusuvuori, E., Lahtinen, H., Lamsa, K., et al. (1999). The K+/Cl− co-transporter KCC2 renders GABA hyperpolarizing during neuronal maturation. Nature 397, 251–255. doi: 10.1038/16697
Robello, M., Amico, C., and Cupello, A. (1998). Cerebellar granule cell GABAA receptors studied at the single-channel level: modulation by protein kinase G. Biochem. Biophys. Res. Commun. 253, 768–773. doi: 10.1006/bbrc.1998.9830
Roullet, F. I., Lai, J. K. Y., and Foster, J. (2013). In utero exposure to valproic acid and autism—a current review of clinical and animal studies. Neurotoxicol. Teratol. 36, 47–56. doi: 10.1016/j.ntt.2013.01.004
Rubenstein, J. L. R., and Merzenich, M. M. (2003). Model of autism: increased ratio of excitation/inhibition in key neural systems. Genes Brain Behav. 2, 255–267. doi: 10.1034/j.1601-183x.2003.00037.x
Schmahmann, J. D., and Sherman, J. C. (1998). The cerebellar cognitive affective syndrome. Brain 121, 561–579. doi: 10.1093/brain/121.4.561
Sigel, E., and Steinmann, M. E. (2012). Structure, function, and modulation of GABAA receptors. J. Biol. Chem. 287, 40224–40231. doi: 10.1074/jbc.R112.386664
Skefos, J., Cummings, C., Enzer, K., Holiday, J., Weed, K., Levy, E., et al. (2014). Regional alterations in purkinje cell density in patients with autism. PLoS One 9:e81255. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0081255
Song, N. Y., Shi, H. B., Li, C. Y., and Yin, S. K. (2012). Interaction between taurine and GABAA/glycine receptors in neurons of the rat anteroventral cochlear nucleus. Brain Res. 1472, 1–10. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2012.07.001
Stein, V., Hermans-Borgmeyer, I., Jentsch, T. J., and Hübner, C. A. (2004). Expression of the KCl cotransporter KCC2 parallels neuronal maturation and the emergence of low intracellular chloride. J. Comp. Neurol. 468, 57–64. doi: 10.1002/cne.10983
Succol, F., Fiumelli, H., Benfenati, F., Cancedda, L., and Barberis, A. (2012). Intracellular chloride concentration influences the GABAA receptor subunit composition. Nat. Commun. 3:738. doi: 10.1038/ncomms1744
Tsai, P. T. (2016). Autism and cerebellar dysfunction: evidence from animal models. Semin. Fetal Neonatal Med. 21, 349–355. doi: 10.1016/j.siny.2016.04.009
Tsai, P. T., Hull, C., Chu, Y., Greene-Colozzi, E., Sadowski, A. R., Leech, J. M., et al. (2012). Autistic-like behavior and cerebellar dysfonction in Purkinje cell Tsc1 mutant mice. Nature 488, 647–651. doi: 10.1038/nature11310
Tsutsui, K., Ukena, K., Sakamoto, H., Okuyama, S., and Haraguchi, S. (2011). Biosynthesis, mode of action, and functional significance of neurosteroids in the purkinje cell. Front. Endocrinol. 2:61. doi: 10.3389/fendo.2011.00061
Tyzio, R., Holmes, G. L., Ben-Ari, Y., and Khazipov, R. (2007). Timing of the developmental switch in GABAA mediated signaling from excitation to inhibition in CA3 rat hippocampus using gramicidin perforated patch and extracellular recordings. Epilepsia 48, 96–105. doi: 10.1111/j.1528-1167.2007.01295.x
Tyzio, R., Nardou, R., Ferrari, D. C., Tsintsadze, T., Shahrokhi, A., Eftekhari, S., et al. (2014). Oxytocin-mediated GABA inhibition during delivery attenuates autism pathogenesis in rodent offspring. Science 343, 675–679. doi: 10.1126/science.1247190
Uzunova, G., Pallanti, S., and Hollander, E. (2016). Excitatory/inhibitory imbalance in autism spectrum disorders: implications for interventions and therapeutics. World J. Biol. Psychiatry 17, 174–186. doi: 10.3109/15622975.2015.1085597
Verdoorn, T. A., Draguhn, A., Ymer, S., Seeburg, P. H., and Sakmann, B. (1990). Functional properties of recombinant rat GABAA receptors depend upon subunit composition. Neuron 4, 919–928. doi: 10.1016/0896-6273(90)90145-6
Vien, T. N., Modgil, A., Abramian, A. M., Jurd, R., Walker, J., Brandon, N. J., et al. (2015). Compromising the phosphodependent regulation of the GABAAR β3 subunit reproduces the core phenotypes of autism spectrum disorders. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U S A 112, 14805–14810. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1514657112
Waddell, J., and McCarthy, M. M. (2012). Sexual differentiation of the brain and ADHD: what is a sex difference in prevalence telling us? Curr. Top. Behav. Neurosci. 9, 341–360. doi: 10.1007/7854_2010_114
Watanabe, M., and Fukuda, A. (2015). Development and regulation of chloride homeostasis in the central nervous system. Front. Cell. Neurosci. 9:371. doi: 10.3389/fncel.2015.00371
Witte, M., Reinert, T., Dietz, B., Nerlich, J., Rübsamen, R., and Milenkovic, I. (2014). Depolarizing chloride gradient in developing cochlear nucleus neurons: underlying mechanism and implication for calcium signaling. Neuroscience 261, 207–222. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2013.12.050
Zhang, L., Pathak, H. R., Coulter, D. A., Freed, M. A., and Vardi, N. (2006). Shift of intracellular chloride concentration in ganglion and amacrine cells of developing mouse retina. J. Neurophysiol. 95, 2404–2416. doi: 10.1152/jn.00578.2005
Keywords: cerebellum, Purkinje neuron, chloride gradient, autism, sexual dimorphism, GABAA channels
Citation: Roux S, Lohof A, Ben-Ari Y, Poulain B and Bossu J-L (2018) Maturation of GABAergic Transmission in Cerebellar Purkinje Cells Is Sex Dependent and Altered in the Valproate Model of Autism. Front. Cell. Neurosci. 12:232. doi: 10.3389/fncel.2018.00232
Received: 04 May 2018; Accepted: 16 July 2018;
Published: 30 July 2018.
Edited by:
Enrico Cherubini, Scuola Internazionale Superiore di Studi Avanzati (SISSA), ItalyReviewed by:
Tommaso Pizzorusso, Consiglio Nazionale delle Ricerche (CNR), ItalyAndrea Barberis, Fondazione Istituto Italiano di Technologia, Italy
Copyright © 2018 Roux, Lohof, Ben-Ari, Poulain and Bossu. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.
*Correspondence: Jean-Louis Bossu, jlbossu@inci-cnrs.unistra.fr