Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Behav. Neurosci., 03 January 2025
Sec. Learning and Memory

Are there distinct subtypes of developmental dyslexia?

  • 1Department of Special Education, University of Thessaly, Volos, Greece
  • 2School of Humanities, Hellenic Open University, Patras, Greece

Introduction: The aim of this study was to identify if children with dyslexia can be distinguished into discrete categories based on their domain deficits, indicating various neurocognitive subtypes of developmental dyslexia (DD).

Methods: The sample included 101 students in the 3rd, 4th, 5th, and 6th grades of primary school (mean age 11.15 years) with a diagnosis of dyslexia from a public center and Greek as their native language. The students underwent tests assessing a wide range of abilities, specifically phonological, memory, attention, processing speed abilities, motor, visual, and visual-motor skills.

Results: Cluster analysis revealed that children with dyslexia can be divided into three subtypes. The first subtype includes children identified based on their performance in tasks evaluating the phonological abilities, memory, attention, processing speed, along with visual-motor and visual skills. The second subtype comprises children identified based on their performance in memory abilities, motor, and visual-motor skills. The third subtype includes children identified solely based on their performance in the motor skills domain.

Discussion: In conclusion, our findings suggest that school-aged children with DD can be categorized into different subtypes with distinct neurocognitive characteristics. Furthermore, the results indicate that most children with dyslexia experience difficulties in more than one cognitive, sensory or motor domains, supporting contemporary models regarding the existence of multiple neurocognitive deficits in DD.

1 Introduction

Developmental dyslexia (DD) is a mild neurodevelopmental disorder (Brimo et al., 2021; Centanni, 2020; Ramus, 2004) with neurobiological (Lindgren et al., 1985; Kim, 2021; Vlachos, 2010) and genetic basis (Gialluisi et al., 2021; Schulte-Körne et al., 2001). It manifests as a specific learning difficulty in written language (Beidas et al., 2013), primarily in reading, and is not related to the absence of adequate education, non-typical intelligence, sensory deficits, or a potentially adverse socio-economic environment (Horowitz-Kraus et al., 2014; Nopola-Hemmi et al., 2001; Peterson and Pennington, 2015; Zoubrinetzky et al., 2014). In Greece, its prevalence rate is particularly significant, reaching approximately 5.5% (Vlachos et al., 2013).

Over approximately 50 years of systematic study on this specific disorder, various theoretical approaches have been formulated regarding its causes. Most of these approaches focus on single-deficit and can be distinguished into two levels: the biological and the cognitive.

The supporters of cognitive approaches attempt to identify the underlying deficit that causes difficulties in individuals with dyslexia and interpret them through differences observed in cognitive functions. According to the phonological deficit hypothesis (Stanovich, 1988; Vellutino and Fletcher, 2005), DD is considered the result of a deficit in phonological processing and involves three main domains: phonological awareness, verbal short-term memory, and naming speed. Difficulties in phonological processing appear to lead to problems in written language, particularly in reading. Supporters of this hypothesis argue that phonological processing deficits constitute the core impairment for most individuals with dyslexia, serving as the primary cause of the difficulties they face in both reading and writing (Snowling, 1995, 2000). However, more recent studies report the existence of other deficits, such as visual, auditory, and motor deficits in dyslexics (Heim et al., 2008; Heim and Grande, 2012; White et al., 2006) and argue that the parallel existence of these difficulties with the phonological deficit cannot be fully explained by the hypothesis of a unique deficit, the phonological, as the cause of dyslexia.

According to the hypothesis of temporal processing deficit, individuals with dyslexia seem to struggle with adequate processing of visual and/or auditory stimuli, especially when these stimuli alternate rapidly (Farmer and Klein, 1995; Tallal et al., 1985). Specifically, the various levels of difficulties faced by children with dyslexia could be attributed to a central and fundamental deficit, which is related to the brain’s ability to process the rhythm and temporal characteristics of stimuli (Vlachos, 2010). Consequently, the phonological, visual, and motor difficulties observed in children with dyslexia may be attributed to a more general difficulty in the temporal processing of stimuli (Meilleur et al., 2020; Vlachos, 2010).

Furthermore, according to the double deficit hypothesis, individuals with dyslexia exhibit deficits both in phonology and the speed of word or object naming (Wolf and Bowers, 1999). Advocates of this hypothesis argue that rapid naming is not part of phonological processing (Wolf et al., 2000) and serves as a significant predictive factor for DD (Koponen et al., 2013; Pennington, 2002). It is suggested that individuals with deficits in both domains are more likely to face severe reading problems compared to those with a single deficit (Badian, 1997; Nicolson and Fawcett, 2019). Longitudinal studies by Papadopoulos et al. (2009), as well as research by Constantinidou and Stainthorp (2009) have supported the double deficit hypothesis, demonstrating a connection between phonological deficits and processing speed. Additionally, individuals with dyslexia sometimes seem to exhibit difficulties in a wide range of skills, such as balance, motor skills, phonemic skills, and rapid sensory processing (Vlachos, 2010). These difficulties are consistent with the automaticity deficit hypothesis (Nicolson and Fawcett, 1990) according to which children with dyslexia experience fluency problems for any skill that can be made automatic through extensive practice (Vlachos, 2010). This results in individuals with dyslexia facing significant challenges in skills like reading, requiring more time and practice until they can achieve automaticity (Nicolson and Fawcett, 1990).

Moreover, many studies have linked DD to attention deficits (Bosse et al., 2007; Facoetti and Molteni, 2001). According to this hypothesis, attention deficits impact the letter encoding process, leading individuals with dyslexia to confusion in letters and visual word forms (Valdois et al., 2003). Additionally, a lack of visual attention may reduce perceptual ability (Valdois et al., 2003). The deficit in visual attention often appears to coexist with a deficit in auditory attention, which may contribute to the explanation of DD, as it can lead to difficulties in the development of phonological skills necessary for acquiring reading ability (Facoetti et al., 2003; Sperling et al., 2005). Vidyasagar and Pammer (2010) highlighted the importance of visual-spatial attention in reading. They consider that the attentional mechanisms controlled by the visual component play a significant role in letter scanning. Difficulties and deficits in this domain may cause additional problems, such as issues in the visual processing of graphemes, in the connection of graphemes to phonemes, and more generally in phonological awareness. Phonological deficits appear to stem from a deficit in visual-spatial attention. Overall, research supports the existence of deficits in various attention domains (visual, auditory, and visual-spatial) could be a factor influencing reading skills and being associated with DD.

Working memory is a high-level skill linked to a range of cognitive activities, from the simplest linguistic tasks to verbal comprehension tasks (Cowan and Alloway, 2008). It is used for the storage and processing of new information and appears to play an important role in dyslexia (McLoughlin et al., 2002). Research conducted on working memory in typically developing children has shown high performance in reading skill tasks, which was independent of performance in phonological skill tasks (Swanson, 2006). In contrast, studies examining children with dyslexia have presented findings supporting the presence of a working memory deficit, considering it one of the key characteristics defining the DD (McLoughlin et al., 2002). Children with dyslexia exhibit deficits in working memory, considering it a significant characteristic of DD (McLoughlin et al., 2002). Recent studies (Chalmpe et al., 2017; Gray et al., 2019) have also confirmed the existence of deficits in working memory in children with dyslexia.

In addition, biological hypotheses for DD were based on research, which found that dyslexia may be caused by variations in certain genes (for a review see Vlachos and Nisiotou-Mantelou, 2013), to anatomical or functional variations in certain brain regions (Ramus et al., 2018), or to variations in the ratio of gray and white matter in these regions (Vanderauwera et al., 2017). More specifically, the hypotheses of atypical structure and function in linguistic centers of the brain suggest that morphological and functional differences and abnormalities in cell architecture, mainly in areas related to language function and in the temporal fossa of the left hemisphere (around the Sylvian fissure), is related to the occurrence of DD (Cao et al., 2006). The results of functional imaging studies support this view that the most common form of dyslexia is associated with an atypically structured brain mechanism for reading (Papanicolaou et al., 2003).

Moreover, according to the magnocellular hypothesis, difficulties experienced by individuals with dyslexia in reading arise from the diminished development of magnocellular cells, which are responsible for temporal perception and motor processes (Stein, 2001; Stein and Walsh, 1997). Studies have demonstrated that individuals with dyslexia exhibit a more general magnocellular dysfunction, leading to challenges in processing sensory information, consequently hindering learning and language processing (Stein and Walsh, 1997).

Apart from the aforementioned hypotheses, the cerebellar dysfunction hypothesis suggests that the difficulties faced by dyslexic people could be a result of deficient cerebellar function (Nicolson et al., 2001). As information from the language areas of the brain and the magnocellular area in processing passes through the cerebellum, its impaired function can affect reading ability and explain the different types and degrees of dyslexia. According to Nicolson et al. (2001), a cerebellar deficit provides a reasonably satisfactory explanation for a range of problems experienced by children with dyslexia. This hypothesis predicts that a cerebellar abnormality at birth leads to mild motor and articulation problems. The lack of fluency in articulation in turn leads to a poor representation of phonological features of speech, which results in the development of difficulties in phonological awareness at around 5 years of age, leading to later problems in learning to read.

Frith (1999), considering the several hypotheses that have been put forward about dyslexia at the biological and cognitive levels, argues that there are three general causal frameworks for explaining dyslexia, which are expressed at all three levels (biological, cognitive, and behavioral). The framework of phonological deficit and dysfunction of language areas around Sylvius’ fissure suggests that dyslexia is the consequence of difficulties of linguistic origin. The magnocellular deficit framework links the sensory processing deficits exhibited by dyslexics to dysfunction of the magnocellular system. Finally, the cerebellar deficit framework links the difficulties experienced by dyslexics in developing motor and automaticity skills to cerebellar dysfunction.

However, in the last decades the development of genetics and neuroscience and the advances made by various scientific fields in understanding various developmental disorders, such as dyslexia, have challenged the single-deficit hypotheses. In more detail, no single cognitive deficit has been found that can explain all behavioral evidence of all cases of dyslexia (van Bergen et al., 2014a). In fact, research has shown that not all individuals with dyslexia have difficulties in phonological processing, and those who have problems in processing speech sounds do not necessarily have dyslexia (Peterson and Pennington, 2012; Snowling, 2008). Furthermore, as Morton and Frith (1995) pointed out, the single cognitive deficit model does not consider the situation where a biological cause, a gene, can cause a variety of cognitive deficits, which in genetics is called “pleiotropy.” In addition, the single deficit model cannot easily explain the pervasive comorbidity between different disorders, which are not independent of each other but coexist very often. According to research, DD very often coexists with Dyscalculia, attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD), specific language disorder (SLD), articulation disorder, etc. (Nisiotou and Vlachos, 2014; van Bergen et al., 2014a). Such findings have highlighted the need to move from single deficit models to approaches that argue that dyslexia may be the result of multiple cognitive deficits, as a single deficit cannot explain the great heterogeneity that characterizes this disorder, both at the etiological and behavioral level (Pavlidou et al., 2017; Pennington, 2006; Peterson and Pennington, 2015).

Pennington et al. (2012) proposed the multiple deficit model which incorporated all previous assumptions regarding the causes of dyslexia and allows the cognitive profiles of individuals with dyslexia to exhibit either a single deficit or a combination of deficits. Findings from the studies of Borleffs et al. (2018) and Pacheco et al. (2014), supported this specific model as an interpretive framework for DD.

In addition to these advances, Griffiths and Snowling (2002) argue that categorizing DD into subtypes based on reading profiles and the types of errors made by children offers a limited understanding of the characteristics of individuals with dyslexia and fails to provide meaningful insights for informing educational interventions. Contemporary perspectives argue that it would be preferable to focus on categorizing DD in terms of its neurocognitive subtypes (Zoubrinetzky et al., 2014). Studying the spectrum of neurocognitive characteristics in children with dyslexia, rather than examining only one or two hypotheses on its causes, could provide a clearer picture of the overall difficulties these children face. Consequently, it could contribute to a better diagnosis of each dyslexic child’s difficulties and play a crucial role in developing personalized intervention programs (Griffiths and Snowling, 2002).

Numerous studies have examined distinct cognitive abilities and/or sensory-motor skills related to dyslexia, but there are very few studies simultaneously exploring a broad range of cognitive and sensory-motor areas. In literature, three studies in children (Heim et al., 2008; Menghini et al., 2010; White et al., 2006) and two in adults (Ramus, 2003; Reid et al., 2007) were identified.

Specifically, Heim et al. (2008) investigated cognitive subtypes of DD in a sample of 45 students with dyslexia and 48 typically developing children from elementary schools in Germany. The study included assessments of phonological awareness, auditory discrimination, motion detection, visual attention, and automatization. The results identified three subgroups of dyslexic children: children with exclusive phonological deficits, children with combined deficits involving phonological, auditory, and magnocellular dysfunctions, and children with attention deficits. In the study by Menghini et al. (2010), 65 children and adolescents with DD and 60 typically developing peers were examined. The findings supported the presence of phonological deficits in all children with dyslexia, although only 18.3% of them exhibited exclusively this deficit. A total of 76.6% of the children with dyslexia showed multiple deficits, such as in executive functions, visuospatial perception, attention, memory, and motion detection, providing strong support for the notion that dyslexia is often accompanied by various cognitive deficits and is not limited to phonological difficulties. Furthermore, the study by White et al. (2006) examined the role of sensorimotor deficits in dyslexia, investigating the cerebellar, magnocellular, and phonological deficit hypotheses. The sample included 23 children with DD and 22 typically developing children, matched for age and non-verbal intelligence. The findings indicated that all children with dyslexia exhibited deficits in phonological tasks, and a small subgroup of children showed visual difficulties, particularly in visual stress tasks. However, some of these studies were conducted in opaque orthographic systems (White et al., 2006), others in transparent ones (Menghini et al., 2010) and others in intermediate ones (Heim et al., 2008) and this may have influenced their findings. Additionally, the sample of the aforementioned studies was rather small. Examining many children with dyslexia is considered a critical factor in distinguishing subtypes of the disorder as some cases of dyslexia may occur very rarely and may not be detected in small samples.

Therefore, it is important to conduct studies in different linguistic systems, which will fill the above gaps in literature. Given the uniqueness of the Greek language, which is classified as a transparent orthographic system, and the fact that no study in the Greek literature has simultaneously investigated a wide range of cognitive domains, such research is necessary. This study would aim to identify subtypes of dyslexia in the Greek language and explore the possibility of multiple cognitive subtypes of dyslexia in children, as supported by contemporary multiple deficit models (Pennington et al., 2012; van Bergen et al., 2014b). The aim of this study was to identify if Greek-native children with dyslexia can be distinguished into distinct categories based on their performance on various tasks that have been associated with the onset of dyslexia, indicating that dyslexics can be distinguished into distinct subtypes. A further aim of the study was to discuss current theoretical approaches and research findings that support the existence of multiple deficits in children with dyslexia.

Based on the preceding theoretical review, we formulated the general hypothesis of the study, according to which children with dyslexia are expected to present differentiated profiles based on which they can be distinguished into distinct subtypes. This general hypothesis leads to three predictions. According to the phonological deficit hypothesis (Bradley and Bryant, 1983; Snowling, 1995, 2000) and Frith’s (1999) causal framework for dyslexia, we expect that a distinct subtype of children with DD will be characterized based on their performance in the phonological domain (Prediction 1). Based on the other two general causal frameworks for the interpretation of dyslexia proposed by Frith (1999), namely the magnocellular deficit framework and the cerebellar deficit framework, we expect that a distinct subtype of children with DD will emerge based on their performance in processing speed and another subtype based on their performance in motor tasks (Prediction 2). In line with multiple deficit models (Pennington et al., 2012; van Bergen et al., 2014a), we expect that distinct subtypes of children with DD can be identified who will exhibit a combination of neurocognitive deficits (Prediction 3).

2 Materials and methods

2.1 Participants

The sample consisted of 101 children with dyslexia, 63 males and 38 females (age range 8–12 years, M = 11.15 years, SD = 0.88) who were attending the 3rd, 4th, 5th, and 6th grade of Greek primary school. Specifically, the sample consisted of six children aged 8–9 years (4 boys and 2 girls), four children aged 9–10 years (2 boys and 2 girls), 24 children aged 10–11 years (11 boys and 13 girls), and 67 children aged 11–12 years (46 boys and 21 girls). A convenience sampling approach (Creswell, 2012) was employed, as all students had to have been diagnosed with dyslexia by an official public diagnostic center for special educational needs. The selection of students was based on records from the Centers for Interdisciplinary Assessment, Counseling, and Support (KE.DA.SY.), following the acquisition of all necessary approvals from the appropriate authorities. The selection of students was based on records from the Centers for Interdisciplinary Assessment, Counseling, and Support (KE.DA.SY.), following the acquisition of all necessary approvals from the relevant authorities. According to Greek legislation, KE.DA.SY. conducts individual assessments of preschool and school-age students through interdisciplinary teams. The core composition of these teams includes a special education teacher, a psychologist, and a social worker. The special education teacher evaluates reading performance using the standardized Greek version of the Reading Test (Test-A) (Panteliadou and Antoniou, 2008), while the psychologist conducts cognitive assessments using the Greek adaptation of the Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children (WISC-III) (Georgas et al., 1997). The participants in the present study received their diagnosis, according to the discrepancy criterion within a timeframe ranging from 1 to 3 years prior to the implementation of the study. Children who did not have Greek as their native language or lived in bilingual/multilingual family environments were not included in the sample. Additionally, comorbidity with other developmental or behavioral disorders served as a criterion for non-participation in the study.

2.2 Materials and procedure

Participants were given a series of tests assessing a wide range of abilities and skills which have been scientifically documented to be associated with the occurrence of DD. Specifically, the phonological, memory and attention abilities, the processing speed, motor, visual, and visual-motor skills were assessed. All tests were administered individually in a session that lasted 1.5–2 h. The same sequence of administration was followed for all children. All tests were administered and scored according to the instructions of their creators. Below are listed the tests administered by skill area.

Phonological abilities domain: to assess phonological awareness, the Greek version (Kassotaki-Maridaki, 1998) of the Non-word Reading test of “The Children’s Test of Non-word Repetition” (Gathercole et al., 1994) was administered. To evaluate verbal short-term memory (Morris, 1999), the “Forward digit span” subtest of the Greek version of the Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children-Fifth Edition (WISC-V) (Stogiannidou, 2017) was administered.

Attention abilities domain: for the assessment of auditory attention, the “Auditory attention range” task from the psychometric Test of Detection and Investigation of Attention and Concentration for Primary School Students (Simos et al., 2007) was administered. For the assessment of visual-spatial attention, the subtest “Map Mission” from the Greek standardization (Malegiannaki et al., 2015) of the Test of Everyday Attention for Children (TEA-Ch) (Heaton et al., 2001; Malegiannaki et al., 2019; Manly et al., 2002) was administered. Immediate Recall was assessed using the Attention-Enhanced Composite of the Greek-standardized version of the “Detroit Test of Learning Aptitude (DTLA-4)” (Tzouriadou et al., 2008). This composite comprises the subtests: Design sequences, Sentence reproduction, Reversed letters, Design reproduction, Word sequences, and Story sequences.

Memory abilities domain: for the assessment of long-term memory, the subtest “Opposite meanings” from the DTLA-4 was administered (Tzouriadou et al., 2008) and for working memory, the subtest “Backward digit span” from the WISC-V (Stogiannidou, 2017) was administered. For the assessment of immediate verbal memory, the subtest “Word sequences” from the DTLA-4 (Tzouriadou et al., 2008) was administered. For the assessment of auditory memory, the subtest “Reversed letters” from the DTLA-4 was administered (Tzouriadou et al., 2008) and for assessment of visual long-term memory, the Rey-Osterrieth Complex Figure Test was administered using the recall reproduction task (ROCF-Recall) (Lezak, 1995; Osterrieth, 1944; Rey, 1941, 1959).

Motor skills domain: fine motor skills were assessed using the subtest “Design sequences” from the DTLA-4 (Tzouriadou et al., 2008). The “Balance duration” task from the array of paracephalic tests by Dow and Moruzzi (1958) and the “Balance on the dominant leg” task from the Bruininks-Oseretsky Test of Motor Proficiency (BOTMP) (Bruininks, 1978) were administered to assess static balance. For the assessment of dynamic balance, three tasks (Walking forward, Walking forward “heel-toe” in one line of walking, and Walking backward) from the Movement Assessment Battery for Children (M-ABC) (Henderson and Sugden, 1992) were administered.

Processing speed: processing speed was assessed using the Coding subtest from the Greek version of WISC-V (Stogiannidou, 2017).

Visual skills domain: for the assessment of visual processing, the subtest “Symbolic relations1” from DTLA-4 (Tzouriadou et al., 2008) was administered.

Visual-motor skills domain: for the assessment of visual-motor skills, the Rey-Osterrieth Complex Figure Test (ROCF-Copy) (Lezak, 1995; Osterrieth, 1944; Rey, 1941, 1959) and the “Design reproduction” from the DTLA-4 (Tzouriadou et al., 2008) were administered. Visual-motor coordination was assessed with the contrasting composite of the Motor-Enhanced of DTLA-4 (Tzouriadou et al., 2008). This specific composition consists of the subtests: Design sequences, Reversed letters, Design reproduction, and Story sequences.

The psychometric properties of all administered tests were examined through a series of reliability analyses. The calculated Cronbach alpha reliability coefficients ranged from 0.75 to 0.98 indicating high internal consistency in all tests.

Table 1 summarizes by domain the abilities and skills assessed through the 18 tests administered in this study.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. The tests administered in this study by skills domain.

All statistical analyses were conducted using SPSS 26. Descriptive statistics representing the participants’ performance in all 18 tests were calculated, followed by two Hierarchical Cluster Analyses. In these analyses agglomerative clustering was deemed the preferred method as it represents a sophisticated technique that starts with considering individual data points as independent clusters and proceeds with successive mergers thus combining the most similar pairs of clusters together (Murtagh and Legendre, 2014). Specifically, the first hierarchical cluster analysis by variables (i.e., the 18 tests administered) was performed with a view to identifying clusters representing distinct domains of skills. Since the number of clusters cannot be predetermined, we selected the largest ones identified in the dendrogram (Figure 1), which amounted to three. Following this, hierarchical cluster analysis by cases was conducted with a view to allocating the participating students to the three previously identified clusters. In these analyses, we utilized Ward’s method of calculating distance between clusters as it has been advocated as a promising method for establishing highly homogeneous groups (Ward, 1963 as cited in Murtagh and Legendre, 2014). Finally, a series of one-way analyses of variance (ANOVA) were used to compare the identified sets of participants in each of the 18 tests administered.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. The dendrogram of the hierarchical cluster analysis by variable, in which the categorization of the 18 variables into 3 clusters is displayed.

3 Results

3.1 Descriptive analysis

Table 2 shows the means, standard deviations, and the range of scores obtained in all tests administered.

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Means, standard deviations, minimum, and maximum scores per test.

3.2 Hierarchical cluster analysis by variables

Figure 1 represents a dendrogram depicting three distinct clusters. The first cluster includes most of the tests, specifically: Enhanced attention, Motor, Reversed letters, Forward digit span, Backward digit span, Coding, Word sequences, Symbolic relations, Auditory attention range, Map mission, and Reading Greek pseudowords. The second cluster incorporates five tests: Opposite meanings, ROCF-Copy, Dynamic balance, ROCF-Recall, and Design reproduction. The third and final cluster comprises two tests, Design sequences and Static balance.

Table 3 shows the three clusters generated. The first cluster includes six skill domains, namely memory, attention, processing speed, phonological domain, visual-motor, and visual domain. In the second cluster, three skills are included, namely memory, the motor, and the visual-motor domain. Finally, the third cluster includes only one skill domain, the motor. Specifically, the first and second clusters are distinguished by their performance in tasks assessing the domains of memory and visual motor. Additionally, the first cluster also stood out for its performance in the domains of attention, processing speed, phonological, and visual domain. The second cluster, apart from its performance in tasks assessing memory and visual-motor domain, was also distinguished by its performance in the motor domain.

TABLE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. The three clusters by skills domain, that resulted from the hierarchical cluster analysis by variable.

3.3 Hierarchical cluster analysis by cases

Figure 2 presents the dendrogram that emerged which depicts three groups of participants.

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. The dendrogram of the hierarchical cluster analysis by case, displaying the number of dyslexic children in each cluster.

Table 4 shows the number (N) and percentage (%) of children belonging to each of the three clusters that emerged. In the first cluster, which includes the domains of memory, attention, processing speed, phonological, visual-motor, and visual, there are 39 out of 101 children in the sample (38.61%). The second cluster, encompassing three skills domains (memory, motor, and visual-motor), consists of 59 children (58.42%). The third cluster, composed of 3 children (2.97%), includes only the motor domain.

TABLE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Table 4. The number and percentage of participants per cluster.

3.4 Analysis of the characteristics of each cluster

In Figure 3, the performances of students from the first and second clusters are visualized in all administered tests. The performances of students from the third cluster were chosen not to be visualized due to the small number of students included in it (N = 3). These specific students are distinguished in a separate cluster (cluster 3) due to their differentiated performances in the motor domain, specifically in the Design sequences and Static balance tests (see Figure 2) compared to the rest of the students. Students belonging to the first cluster (N = 39) scored lower in all seven domains (phonological, processing speed, attention, memory, visual, motor, and visual-motor) compared to students belonging to the second cluster (N = 59), although the differences in performances between the two clusters were statistically significant in five of seven domains. Specifically, in the domains of memory, motor and visual-motor skills, processing speed, and phonological skills (see Table 5).

FIGURE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 3. Comparison of the performances of children from the 1st and 2nd clusters in the administered tests.

TABLE 5
www.frontiersin.org

Table 5. Means and standard deviations of the scores of the dyslexics on the 18 tests examined by cluster.

As can be seen on Table 5, the analysis of variance revealed that the performances of students in the first cluster are characterized by statistically significant differences compared to the performances of students in the second cluster. Specifically, students in the first cluster did not show statistically significantly lower performances in two out of seven skill domains, namely in the attention domain and visual domain.

More specifically, students in the first cluster recorded statistically significantly lower performances in long-term memory (task “Opposite meanings,” F(2,98) = 22.77, p < 0.05), fine motor skills (task “Design sequences,” F(2,98) = 5.51, p < 0.05), auditory memory (task “Reversed letters,” F(2,98) = 8.44, p < 0.05), visual-motor skills (tasks “Design reproduction,” F(2,98) = 3.62, p < 0.05, and “ROCF-Copy” F(2,98) = 3.12, p < 0.05), static balance (task “Static balance,” F(2,98) = 182.17, p < 0.05), dynamic balance (task “Dynamic balance,” F(2,98) = 4.75, p < 0.05), working memory (task “Backward digit span,” F(2,98) = 4.54, p < 0.05), processing speed (task “Coding,” F(2,98) = 6.44, p < 0.05), phonological awareness (task “Reading Greek pseudowords,” F(2,98) = 8.72, p < 0.05), and visual long-term memory (task “ROCF-Recall,” F(2,98) = 9.36, p < 0.05).

Table 6 presents the means, standard deviations, and range of participants’ age per cluster and overall. ANOVA revealed significant differences between the ages in the three clusters. More specifically, it revealed that the mean age of participants in the first cluster differs statistically significantly from the mean ages of participants in the other two clusters, indicating that majority of younger participants were classified into the first cluster.

TABLE 6
www.frontiersin.org

Table 6. Means, standard deviations, and range of students’ age per cluster and overall.

4 Discussion

According to the general hypothesis of the study, we expected that children with dyslexia would present differentiated profiles based on which they could be distinguished into distinct subtypes. Based on our first prediction we expected that a distinct subtype of children with DD would be characterized based on their performance in the phonological skills area. The results of this study did not confirm this hypothesis. Children belonging to the first cluster, in addition to differential performance in tests assessing the phonological skills domain, showed differential performance in tests assessing memory, attention, processing speed, visual and visual-motor skills domain.

Our results, regarding the presence of additional deficiencies concurrent with variations in dyslexic students’ performance in the phonological skills domain, are in line with recent research findings (Constantinidou and Stainthorp, 2009; Douklias et al., 2009; McGrath et al., 2011; Lewandowska et al., 2014; O’Brien and Yeatman, 2021; Ruffino et al., 2010; Schuchardt et al., 2008). The results indicate the simultaneous existence of other deficits beyond the phonological in children with dyslexia.

However, our results are not in line with the findings of other studies (Perez et al., 2012; Saksida et al., 2016; Soriano-Ferrer et al., 2014), which present phonological deficits as the sole impairment in children with dyslexia. A distinct group of dyslexics with deficits only in the phonological skills domain was also identified in the studies of Borleffs et al. (2018), Menghini et al. (2010), Ramus et al. (2003) and White et al. (2006).

The differences in our results compared to those of other studies could be attributed to various reasons. A possible reason is the transparency of the spelling system between languages, a factor that affects phonological processing. Phonological deficits appear to be more frequent and pronounced in opaque orthographic systems, such as the English language (Martin et al., 2016). In the studies conducted by Ramus et al. (2003) and White et al. (2006), the participants were native English speakers. The English orthographic system is considered one of the opaquest orthographic systems. However, the Greek orthographic system is the second most transparent orthographic system among European languages (Seymour et al., 2003) and studies have shown that in such systems, the rate of phonological deficits is significantly lower as decoding is easier (Ziegler and Goswami, 2005).

An additional factor that may affect the differences between the studies are sample sizes. In the studies of Ramus et al. (2003) and White et al. (2006), the sample size was relatively small, especially considering the administration of numerous diverse tasks. However, in our research we examined a very large sample of dyslexics, which provides higher power to the analysis. Another factor that differentiates the findings could be the variation in the chronological age of the samples. For example, the study by Ramus et al. (2003) focused on university students, while the research by Menghini et al. (2010) included both children and adolescents. The older age of the participants may be associated with the acquisition of more advanced skills, potentially influencing the manifestation of phonological deficits compared to younger children. Recent studies have shown that education affects the brain’s language network, as learning to read supports the development of more advanced phonemic skills through experience (Łuniewska et al., 2019). Therefore, based on all the above findings we could assume that phonological deficits appear to be less pronounced in older children compared to younger ones.

Furthermore, the differentiation of our results from the findings of Borleffs et al. (2018) could be attributed to the sample characteristics in their study. The children participating in their research were at high risk of dyslexia but had not received a formal diagnosis. In contrast, in our study, all children had a confirmed diagnosis of dyslexia.

Additionally, the absence in our study of a distinct cluster of children characterized only by their performance in phonological skills could also be explained from the perspective that reading is a factor influencing an individual’s phonological abilities (Menghini et al., 2010). The improvement of reading with age and progress in school enhances children’s phonological performance. This may explain why phonological deficits are less apparent in older age groups. According to research (Papadimitriou and Vlachos, 2014), phonological awareness is a strong predictor of reading ability in first grade, but its predictive power decreases in second grade as other factors come into play. Our findings seem to align with this perspective. Most of the younger children in our study belong to the cluster characterized, among other factors, by its performance in the phonological domain (cluster 1). In contrast, the other two clusters, which include the older children in the study, did not demonstrate notable performance in phonological skills.

Another factor that might have contributed to the absence of a distinct group of children characterized solely by their performance in phonological skills is the lack of sufficient tasks in our study that assess this specific domain. Based on the extensive literature on the phonological deficit in dyslexia, we expected that such a distinct subtype would be easily distinguishable without the administration of several relevant tests. For this reason, we did not include more tasks assessing this specific domain but chose to focus on tasks investigating other cognitive domains, for which the research evidence is more limited.

The second prediction of this study predicted that a distinct subtype of children with DD would be identified, differing based on their performance in processing speed, and another subtype based on their performance in motor tasks. Our findings partially confirm our second research hypothesis in relation to both aspects.

Regarding the first aspect of the prediction, our results showed that such differentiation is observed in 38.61% of the children comprising the first cluster. This percentage constitutes a distinct subtype of dyslexics that exhibits differences in processing speed simultaneously with variations in memory, visual, visual-motor, attentional, and phonological skills area. Our results are consistent with the findings of recent studies (Moll et al., 2016; Niolaki et al., 2014; O’Brien et al., 2012; O’Brien and Yeatman, 2021) that identified that dyslexics often present additional deficits beyond the processing speed deficit.

As for the second aspect of the second prediction, participants in the third cluster showed differences in their performance in motor tasks. Although this cluster constitutes only 2.97% of the sample, all statistical analyses indicated that it represents a distinct subtype among children with dyslexia, differing in static balance and fine motor skills. Our findings are in line with the findings of previous studies (Getchell et al., 2007; Iversen et al., 2005; Kirby et al., 2008; Petropoulou, 2011) which showed difficulties in motor tasks for children with dyslexia. However, our findings differ partially from those of other studies (Needle et al., 2006; Ramus et al., 2003), possibly due to sample differences. In the study by Needle et al. (2006), the sample consisted of adults with dyslexia, and in the research by Ramus et al. (2003), most participants exhibited comorbidity with another disorder. However, in our study, the sample consisted exclusively of children, and comorbidity with another disorder served as a reason for exclusion from the sample.

The third prediction of the study predicted that distinct subtypes of children with DD would be identified, showing a combination of neurocognitive deficits. This hypothesis was fully confirmed as two large and distinct clusters of dyslexic children with a combination of deficits were identified. In the first cluster, which constitutes 38.61% of the sample, children exhibited difficulties in the domains of memory, attention, processing speed, phonological skills, visual processing, and visual-motor skills. In the second cluster (58.42%), children had difficulties in the skills domains of memory, motor, and visual-motor.

Our findings are in line with the results of recent studies (Heim et al., 2008; Lewandowska et al., 2014; Menghini et al., 2010; Soriano-Ferrer et al., 2014; O’Brien and Yeatman, 2021) indicating the simultaneous presence of various deficits in several domains (attention, memory, visual, motor, etc.) in dyslexic children. Such findings support the multiple deficits model for DD (Pennington et al., 2012; van Bergen et al., 2014b).

In conclusion, our findings indicate that children with dyslexia can be differentiated into subtypes based on their performance in cognitive tasks. These subtypes are not characterized by a single cognitive deficit but by deficits in various domains, which should be systematically assessed during the diagnostic process. According to neuroscientific research, the diverse clinical manifestations of the disorder can be interpreted by the different case-specific anatomical and/or functional organization of brain networks and the consequences of these neurobiological variations on language and/or other cognitive functions. Additionally, the comorbidity of developmental disorders is explained by multiple deficit models (van Bergen et al., 2014a) through the existence of certain “common risk factors” (Papanikolaou et al., 2017). According to these models, there are genetic and cognitive risk factors as well as protective factors against risks. Some factors are distinct for each of the disorders, while others are shared. Therefore, DD represents a complex and heterogeneous disorder, requiring a systematic investigation of the specific characteristics of each child.

Although the results of this study provided significant information about the neurocognitive deficits exhibited by students with DD, we consider that the present study is subject to few limitations. According to the initial research hypothesis, we expected to identify an unambiguous and distinct subtype of children with dyslexia characterized by their performance in the phonological skills area. This was the reason we did not administer more tests to evaluate this specific area. Instead, this study focused on the co-evaluation of other neurocognitive domains for which there is not as strong a research foundation. This may have influenced the research results and led to the absence of a distinct cluster of students with dyslexia characterized by difficulties in the phonological domain.

An additional limitation is the lack of assessment of the reading ability of children with dyslexia and, consequently, the identification of the specific reading characteristics of participants in each cluster. Due to the large number of tests administered to assess various skills domains, there was not enough time to administer additional tests that would provide information about the reading performance of students in each cluster and the possible reading domains (accuracy, fluency, and comprehension) that are deficient.

The results of recent studies (Heim et al., 2008; Menghini et al., 2010; van Bergen et al., 2014a), as well as the findings from our research, provide evidence for the complex nature of DD. Children with DD, in addition to phonological deficits simultaneously exhibit deficits in other skills domains, such as attention, memory, visual, motor skills, etc. Therefore, there is a pressing need to enhance the diagnostic process by administering tests that assess a broader range of abilities. Administering tests that evaluate not only intelligence and reading abilities, but additional cognitive and motor skills would assist in describing a comprehensive individual neurocognitive profile.

Moreover, when describing the cognitive profile, it is important to consider the influence of the transparency of the orthographic system to which the child with dyslexia belongs, as variations are observed across different orthographic systems (Diamanti, 2010). In transparent systems, such as Greek, where there is a consistent correspondence between letters and phonemes, decoding becomes relatively easier, even for people with dyslexia. However, this may imply that these people show more pronounced difficulties in other domains, such as processing speed, attention, etc. On the contrary, in most opaque systems, such as English, the grapheme-phoneme correspondence is more complex. As a result, students with dyslexia face increased difficulties in reading and spelling.

Additionally, the finding of distinct subtypes in DD highlights the need to design and implement individualized educational interventions that respond to the strengths and weaknesses of each student. Until recently, most interventions focused on the phonological domain. However, the findings of the present study, combined with evidence from other studies (Heim et al., 2008; Menghini et al., 2010; White et al., 2006), indicate that individuals with dyslexia exhibit multiple deficits. Identifying and understanding the individual characteristics of students with dyslexia can contribute to the development of effective and targeted educational intervention programs (Pacheco et al., 2014) and allow the implementation of targeted cognitive and metacognitive strategies, improving reading comprehension and overall school performance of students (Moutsinas et al., 2019). The primary aim of these differentiated interventions based on the identified subtypes must be to prevent the consolidation of difficulties related to dyslexia.

Overall, our findings support the results of recent studies suggesting that DD constitutes a complex and heterogeneous disorder (Heim et al., 2008; Jednoróg et al., 2014; Menghini et al., 2010; O’Brien and Yeatman, 2021; Pennington, 2006; van Bergen et al., 2014a). This disorder is characterized by a multifactorial deficit rather than isolated impairments (Stanovich, 1988; Tallal, 1980; Nicolson and Fawcett, 1990; Nicolson et al., 2001). The majority of individuals with DD seem to exhibit a combination of neurocognitive deficits, with some cases also involving individuals displaying isolated deficiencies. Concurrently, these findings are consistent with recent neurobiological evidence suggesting that various cognitive subtypes of the disorder show differences in the structure and, consequently, the functioning of the brains of individuals with dyslexia. This supports contemporary models of multiple deficits in explaining the etiology of DD.

Data availability statement

The raw data supporting the conclusions of this article will be made available by the authors, without undue reservation.

Ethics statement

The studies involving humans were approved by Ethics Committee of the University of Thessaly. The studies were conducted in accordance with the local legislation and institutional requirements. Written informed consent for participation in this study was provided by the participants’ legal guardians/next of kin.

Author contributions

MC: Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing. FV: Writing – review & editing.

Funding

The author(s) declare financial support was received for the research, authorship, and/or publication of this article. This research was co-financed by Greece and the European Union (European Social Fund – ESF) through the Operational Program “Human Resources Development, Education and Lifelong Learning” in the context of the project “Strengthening Human Resources Research Potential via Doctorate Research” (MIS-5000432), implemented by the State Scholarships Foundation (IKΥ).

Acknowledgments

The authors thank the children who participated in the study, acknowledging their valuable contribution to it.

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Generative AI statement

The authors declare that no Generative AI was used in the creation of this manuscript.

Publisher’s note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

Supplementary material

The Supplementary Material for this article can be found online at: https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fnbeh.2024.1512892/full#supplementary-material

Footnotes

  1. ^ According to the developers of the DTLA-4, the Symbolic Relations subtest assesses non-verbal reasoning. In the present study, it was used to evaluate visual processing, as the ability for non-verbal logic appears to be derived from visual processing (Landy and Goldstone, 2007).

References

Badian, N. A. (1997). Dyslexia and the double deficit hypothesis. Ann. Dyslexia 47, 69–87. doi: 10.1007/s11881-997-0021-y

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Beidas, H., Khateb, A., and Breznitz, Z. (2013). The cognitive profile of adult dyslexics and its relation to their reading abilities. Read. Writ. 26, 1487–1515. doi: 10.1007/s11145-013-9428-5

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Borleffs, E., Jap, B. A. J., Nasution, I. K., Zwarts, F., and Maassen, B. A. M. (2018). Do single or multiple deficit models predict the risk of dyslexia in Standard Indonesian?. Appl. Psycholinguist. 39, 675–702. doi: 10.1017/S0142716417000625

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Bosse, M.-L., Tainturier, M. J., and Valdois, S. (2007). Developmental dyslexia: the visual attention span deficit hypothesis. Cognition 104, 198–230. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2006.05.009

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Bradley, L., and Bryant, P. E. (1983). Categorizing sounds and learning to read: a causal connection. Nature 301, 419–421. doi: 10.1038/301419a0

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Brimo, K., Dinkler, L., Gillberg, C., Lichtenstein, P., Lundström, S., and Åsberg Johnels, J. (2021). The co-occurrence of neurodevelopmental problems in dyslexia. Dyslexia 27, 277–293. doi: 10.1002/dys.1681

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Bruininks, R. (1978). Manual of Bruininks-Oseretsky Test of Motor Proficiency. Circle Pines, MN: American Guidance Service.

Google Scholar

Cao, F., Bitan, T., Chou, T., Burman, D., and Booth, J. (2006). Deficient orthographic and phonological representations in children with dyslexia revealed by brain activation patterns. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry 47, 1041–1050. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2006.01684.x

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Centanni, T. M. (2020). “Neural and genetic mechanisms of dyslexia,” in Translational Neuroscience of Speech and Language Disorders, ed. G. P. D. Argyropoulos (Cham: Springer), 79–103. doi: 10.1007/978-3-030-35687-3_4

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Chalmpe, M., Vlachos, F., Avramidis, H., and Andreou, G. (2017). Researching phonological memory and visual-spatial memory in children with dyslexia. Hellenic Rev. Spec. Educ. 5, 37–58. doi: 10.12681/edusc.3185

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Constantinidou, M., and Stainthorp, R. (2009). Phonological awareness and reading speed deficits in reading disabled Greek-speaking children. Educ. Psychol. 29, 171–186. doi: 10.1080/01443410802613483

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Cowan, N., and Alloway, T. P. (2008). “The development of working memory in childhood,” in Development of Memory in Infancy and Childhood, 2nd Edn, eds M. Courag and N. Cowan (Hove: Psychology Press), 303–342. doi: 10.4324/9780203934654

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Creswell, J. D. (2012). Educational Research: Planning, Conducting, and Evaluating Quantitative and Qualitative Research, 4th Edn. London: Pearson Education, Inc.

Google Scholar

Diamanti, V. (2010). “Orthographic skills and difficulties in children with dyslexia,” in Orthography: Learning and Disorders, eds A. Mouzaki and A. Protopapas (Athens: Gutenberg), 275–304. (In Greek).

Google Scholar

Douklias, S. D., Masterson, J., and Hanley, J. R. (2009). Surface and phonological DD in Greek. Cogn. Neuropsychol. 26, 705–723. doi: 10.1080/02643291003691106

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Dow, R. S., and Moruzzi, G. (1958). The Physiology and Pathology of the Cerebellum. Minneapolis, MN: Minnesota Press.

Google Scholar

Facoetti, A., and Molteni, M. (2001). The gradient of visual attention in developmental dyslexia. Neuropsychologia 39, 352–357. doi: 10.1016/S0028-3932(00)00138-X

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Facoetti, A., Lorusso, M. L., Paganoni, P., Cattaneo, C., Galli, R., Umiltà, C., et al. (2003). Auditory and visual automatic attention deficits in developmental dyslexia. Cogn. Brain Res. 16, 185–191. doi: 10.1016/S0926-6410(02)00270-7

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Farmer, M., and Klein, R. (1995). The evidence for a temporal processing deficit linked to dyslexia: a review. Psychon. Bull. Rev. 2, 460–493. doi: 10.3758/BF03210983

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Frith, U. (1999). Paradoxes in the definition of dyslexia. Dyslexia 5, 192–214. doi: 10.1002/(SICI)1099-0909(199912)5:4<192::AID-DYS144<3.0.CO.2-N

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Gathercole, S. E., Willis, C. S., Baddeley, A. D., and Emslie, H. (1994). The children’s test of nonword repetition: a test of phonological working memory. Memory 2, 103–127. doi: 10.1080/09658219408258940

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Georgas, D., Paraskevopoulos, I., Bezevegis, E., and Giannitsas, N. (1997). Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children - Third Edition (WISC-III): Greek Version Manual. Athens: Ellinika Grammata.

Google Scholar

Getchell, N., Padreja, P., Needle, K., and Carrio, V. (2007). Comparing children with and without dyslexia on the movement assessment battery for children and the test of gross motor development. Percept. Motor Skills 105, 207–214. doi: 10.2466/pms.105.1.207-214

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Gialluisi, A., Andlauer, T. F. M., Mirza-Schreiber, N., and Schulte-Körne, G. (2021). Genome-wide association study reveals new insights into the heritability and genetic correlates of developmental dyslexia. Mol. Psychiatry 26, 3004–3017. doi: 10.1038/s41380-020-00898-x

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Gray, S., Fox, A. B., Green, S., Alt, M., Hogan, T. P., Petscher, Y., et al. (2019). Working memory profiles of children with dyslexia, developmental language disorder, or both. J. Speech Lang. Hear. Res. 62, 1839–1858. doi: 10.1044/2019_JSLHR-L-18-0148

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Griffiths, Y. M., and Snowling, M. J. (2002). Predictors of exception word and nonword reading in dyslexic children: the severity hypothesis. J. Educ. Psychol. 94, 34–43. doi: 10.1037/0022-0663.94.1.34

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Heaton, S. C., Reader, S. K., Preston, A. S., Fennell, E. B., Puyana, O. E., Gill, N., et al. (2001). The Test of Everyday Attention for Children (TEA-Ch): patterns of performance in children with ADHD and clinical controls. Child Neuropsychol. 7, 251–264. doi: 10.1076/chin.7.4.251.8736

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Heim, S., and Grande, M. (2012). Fingerprints of developmental dyslexia. Trends Neurosci. Educ. 1, 10–14. doi: 10.1016/j.tine.2012.09.001

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Heim, S., Tschierse, J., Amunts, K., Vossel, S., Wilms, M., Willmes, K., et al. (2008). Cognitive subtypes of dyslexia. Acta Neurobiol. Exp. 68, 73–82.

Google Scholar

Henderson, S., and Sugden, D. (1992). The Movement Assessment Battery for Children. London: The Psychological Corporation.

Google Scholar

Horowitz-Kraus, T., Vannest, J. J., Kadis, D., Cicchino, N., Wang, Y. Y., and Holland, S. K. (2014). Reading acceleration training changes brain circuitry in children with reading difficulties. Brain Behav. 4, 886–902. doi: 10.1002/brb3.281

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Iversen, S., Berg, K., Ellertsen, B., and Tønnessen, F. E. (2005). Motor coordination difficulties in a municipality group and in a clinical sample of poor readers. Dyslexia 11, 217–231. doi: 10.1002/dys.297

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Jednoróg, K., Gawron, N., Marchewka, A., Heim, S., and Grabowska, A. (2014). Cognitive subtypes of dyslexia are characterized by distinct patterns of grey matter volume. Brain Struct. Funct. 219, 1697–1707. doi: 10.1007/s00429-013-0595-6

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Kassotaki-Maridaki, A. (1998). Short term memory of phonological information and reading achievement: an attempt to investigate their relationship. Psychologia 5, 44–52. (In Greek).

Google Scholar

Kim, S. K. (2021). Recent update on reading disability (dyslexia) focused on neurobiology. Clin. Exp. Pediatr. 64, 497–503. doi: 10.3345/cep.2020.01543

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Kirby, A., Sugden, D., and Beveridge, S. (2008). Dyslexia and developmental co-ordination disorder in further and higher education—similarities and differences. Does the “Label” influence the support given? Dyslexia 14, 197–213. doi: 10.1002/dys.367

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Koponen, T., Salmi, P., Eklund, K., and Aro, T. (2013). Counting and RAN: predictors of arithmetic calculation and reading fluency. J. Educ. Psychol. 105, 162–175. doi: 10.1037/a0029285

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Landy, D., and Goldstone, R. L. (2007). How abstract is symbolic thought? J. Exp. Psychol. 33, 720–733. doi: 10.1037/0278-7393.33.4.720

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Lewandowska, M., Milner, R., Ganc, M., Włodarczyk, E., and Skarżyński, H. (2014). Attention dysfunction subtypes of developmental dyslexia. Med. Sci. Monit. 20, 2256–2268. doi: 10.12659/MSM.890969

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Lezak, M. D. (1995). Neuropsychological Assessment, 3rd Edn. New York, NY: Oxford University Press.

Google Scholar

Lindgren, S. D., de Renzi, E., and Richman, L. C. (1985). Cross-national comparisons of DD in Italy and the United States. Child Dev. 56, 1404–1417. doi: 10.2307/1130460

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Łuniewska, M., Chyl, K., Dêbska, A., Banaszkiewicz, A., Żelechowska, A., Marchewka, A., et al. (2019). Children with dyslexia and familial risk for dyslexia present atypical development of the neuronal phonological network. Front. Neurosci. 13. doi: 10.3389/fnins.2019.01287

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Malegiannaki, A. C., Aretouli, E., Metallidou, P., Messinis, L., Zafeiriou, D., and Kosmidis, M. H. (2019). Test of Everyday Attention for Children (TEA-Ch): Greek normative data and discriminative validity for children with combined type of attention deficit-hyperactivity disorder. Dev. Neuropsychol. 44, 189–202. doi: 10.1080/87565641.2019.1578781

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Malegiannaki, A. C., Metallidou, P., and Kiosseoglou, G. (2015). Psychometric properties of the Test of Everyday Attention for Children in Greek-speaking school children. Eur. J. Dev. Psychol. 12, 234–242. doi: 10.1080/17405629.2014.973842

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Manly, T., Anderson, V., Nimmo-Smith, I., Turner, A., Watson, P., and Robertson, I. (2002). The differential assessment of children’s attention: the Test of Everyday Attention for Children (TEA-Ch), normative sample and ADHD performance. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry Allied Discipl. 42, 1065–1081. doi: 10.1017/S0021963001007909

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Martin, A., Kronbichler, M., and Richlan, F. (2016). Dyslexic brain activation abnormalities in deep and shallow orthographies: a meta-analysis of 28 functional neuroimaging studies. Hum. Brain Mapp. 37, 2676–2699. doi: 10.1002/hbm.23202

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

McGrath, L. M., Pennington, B. F., Shanahan, M. A., Santerre-Lemmon, L. E., Barnard, H. D., Willcutt, E. G., et al. (2011). A multiple deficit model of reading disability and attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: searching for shared cognitive deficits. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry 52, 547–557. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2010.02346.x

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

McLoughlin, D., Leather, C., and Stringer, P. (2002). The Adult Dyslexic. Interventions and Outcomes. London: Whurr.

Google Scholar

Meilleur, A., Foster, N. E. V., Coll, S.-M., Brambati, S. M., and Hyde, K. L. (2020). Unisensory and multisensory temporal processing in autism and dyslexia: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 116, 44–63. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2020.06.013

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Menghini, D., Finzi, A., Benassi, M., Bolzani, R., Facoetti, A., Giovagnoli, S., et al. (2010). Different underlying neurocognitive deficits in developmental dyslexia: a comparative study. Neuropsychologia 48, 863–872. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2009.11.003

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Moll, K., Göbel, S. M., Gooch, D., Landerl, K., and Snowling, M. J. (2016). Cognitive risk factors for specific learning disorder: processing speed, temporal processing, and working memory. J. Learn. Disabil. 49, 272–281. doi: 10.1177/0022219414547221

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Morris, R. G. M. (1999). Brain Research Bulletin. Amsterdam: Elsevier Science.

Google Scholar

Morton, J., and Frith, U. (1995). “Causal modeling: a structural approach to developmental psychopathology.” in Developmental psychopathology, Vol: 1, Theory and methods, eds D. Cicchetti and D. J. Cohen (John Wiley & Sons), 357–390.

Google Scholar

Moutsinas, G., Ntziavida, A., and Machia, A. (2019). The neuropsychological profile and enhancement of reading comprehension in students with dyslexia through cognitive and metacognitive strategies: effective school interventions for 11- to 12-year-olds. Sci. Yearbook Dep. Early Childh. Educ. Univ. Ioannina 12, 26–116. doi: 10.12681/jret.19000

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Murtagh, F., and Legendre, P. (2014). Ward’s hierarchical agglomerative clustering method: which algorithms implement ward’s criterion? J. Classif. 31, 274–295. doi: 10.1007/s00357-014-9161-z

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Needle, J. L., Fawcett, A. J., and Nicolson, R. I. (2006). Balance and dyslexia: an investigation of adults’ abilities. J. Cogn. Psychol. 18, 909–936. doi: 10.1080/09541440500412304

Google Scholar

Nicolson, R. I., and Fawcett, A. J. (1990). Automaticity: a new framework for dyslexia research? Cognition 35, 159–182. doi: 10.1016/0010-0277(90)90013-A

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Nicolson, R. I., and Fawcett, A. J. (2019). Development of dyslexia: the delayed neural commitment framework. Front. Behav. Neurosci. 13:112. doi: 10.3389/fnbeh.2019.00112

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Nicolson, R. I., Fawcett, A. J., and Dean, P. (2001). Developmental dyslexia: the cerebellar deficit hypothesis. Trends Neurosci. 24, 508–511. doi: 10.1016/S0166-2236(00)01896-8

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Niolaki, G. Z., Terzopoulos, A. R., and Masterson, J. (2014). Varieties of DD in Greek children. Writ. Syst. Res. 6, 230–256. doi: 10.1080/17586801.2014.893862

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Nisiotou, I., and Vlachos, F. (2014). Neurodevelopmental disorders: is there a common biological base? Child Adolesc. Psychiatry 2, 31–41. (In Greek).

Google Scholar

Nopola-Hemmi, J., Myllyluoma, B., Haltia, T., Taipale, M., Ollikainen, V., Ahonen, T., et al. (2001). A dominant gene for developmental dyslexia on chromosome 3. J. Med. Genet. 38, 658–664. doi: 10.1136/jmg.38.10.658

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

O’Brien, B. A., Wolf, M., and Lovett, M. W. (2012). A taxometric investigation of developmental dyslexia subtypes. Dyslexia 18, 16–39. doi: 10.1002/dys.1431

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

O’Brien, G., and Yeatman, J. D. (2021). Bridging sensory and language theories of dyslexia: toward a multifactorial model. Dev. Sci. 24:e13039. doi: 10.1111/desc.13039

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Osterrieth, P. A. (1944). Le test de copie d’une figure complexe. Arch. Psychol. 30, 286–356. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2005.10.012

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Pacheco, A., Reis, A., Araújo, S., Inácio, F., Petersson, K. M., and Faísca, L. (2014). Dyslexia heterogeneity: cognitive profiling of Portuguese children with dyslexia. Read. Writ. 27, 1529–1545. doi: 10.1007/s11145-014-9504-5

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Panteliadou, S., and Antoniou, F. (2008). Test-A: Reading Assessment Tool. Athens: Modern Times. (In Greek).

Google Scholar

Papadimitriou, A., and Vlachos, F. (2014). Which specific skills developing during preschool years predict the reading performance in the first and second grade of primary school? Early Child Dev. Care 184, 1706–1722. doi: 10.1080/03004430.2013.875542

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Papadopoulos, T. C., Georgiou, G. K., and Kendeou, P. (2009). Investigating the double-deficit hypothesis in Greek: findings from a longitudinal study. J. Learn. Disabil. 42, 528–547. doi: 10.1177/0022219409338745

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Papanicolaou, A. C., Simos, P. G., Breier, J. I., Fletcher, J. M., Foorman, B. R., Francis, D., et al. (2003). Brain mechanisms for reading in children with and without dyslexia: a review of studies of normal development and plasticity. Dev. Neuropsychol. 24, 593–612. doi: 10.1080/87565641.2003.9651912

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Papanikolaou, A. S., Tzakou, E. T., and Vlachos, F. (2017). Modern multiple deficit models for developmental dyslexia. Hellenic Rev. Spec. Educ. 5, 79–97. (In Greek).

Google Scholar

Pavlidou, M., Teliou, E., and Vlachos, F. (2017). Heterogeneity in the cognitive profiles of dyslexic people. Hellenic Rev. Spec. Educ. 5, 17–36. (In Greek).

Google Scholar

Pennington, B. F. (2002). The Development of Psychopathology: Nature and Nurture. New York, NY: Guildford Press.

Google Scholar

Pennington, B. F. (2006). From single to multiple deficit models of developmental disorders. Cognition 101, 385–413. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2006.04.008

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Pennington, B. F., Santerre-Lemmon, L., Rosenberg, J., MacDonald, B., Boada, R., Friend, A., et al. (2012). Individual prediction of dyslexia by single versus multiple deficit models. J. Abnorm. Psychol. 121, 212–224. doi: 10.1037/a0025823

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Perez, T. M., Majerus, S., Mahot, A., and Poncelet, M. (2012). Evidence for a specific impairment of serial order short-term memory in dyslexic children. Dyslexia 18, 94–109. doi: 10.1002/dys.1438

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Peterson, R. L., and Pennington, B. F. (2012). Developmental dyslexia. Lancet 379, 1997–2007. doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736(12)60198-6

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Peterson, R. L., and Pennington, B. F. (2015). Developmental dyslexia. Annu. Rev. Clin. Psychol. 11, 283–307. doi: 10.1146/annurev-clinpsy-032814-112842

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Petropoulou, P. (2011). The investigation of balance skills in children with dyslexia: counseling suggestions [Postgraduate thesis, University of Thessaly]. Available online at: https://ir.lib.uth.gr/xmlui/bitstream/handle/11615/41348/10189.pdf?sequence=1

Google Scholar

Ramus, F. (2003). Developmental dyslexia: specific phonological deficit or general sensorimotor dysfunction? Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 13, 212–218. doi: 10.1016/S0959-4388(03)00035-7

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Ramus, F. (2004). Neurobiology of dyslexia: a reinterpretation of the data. Trends Neurosci. 27, 720–726. doi: 10.1016/j.tins.2004.10.004

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Ramus, F., Altarelli, I., Jednoróg, K., Zhao, J., and Scotto di Covella, L. (2018). Neuroanatomy of developmental dyslexia: pitfalls and promise. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 84, 434–452. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2017.08.001

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Ramus, F., Rosen, S., Dakin, S. C., Day, B. L., Castellote, J. M., White, S., et al. (2003). Theories of developmental dyslexia: insights from a multiple case study of dyslexic adults. Brain 126, 841–865. doi: 10.1093/brain/awg076

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Reid, A., Szczerbinski, M., Iskierka-Kasperek, E., and Hansen, P. (2007). Cognitive profiles of adult developmental dyslexics: theoretical implications. Dyslexia 13, 1–24. doi: 10.1002/dys.321

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Rey, A. (1941). L’examen psychologique dans les cas d’encéphalopathie traumatique. (Les problems.). Arch. Psychol. 28, 215–285.

Google Scholar

Rey, A. (1959). Manuel: Test de Copie d’une Figure Complexe. Paris: Centre de Psychologie Apliquee.

Google Scholar

Ruffino, M., Trussardi, A. N., Gori, S., Finzi, A., Giovagnoli, S., Menghini, D., et al. (2010). Attentional engagement deficits in dyslexic children. Neuropsychologia 48, 3793–3801. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2010.09.002

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Saksida, A., Iannuzzi, S., Bogliotti, C., Chaix, Y., Démonet, J.-F., Bricout, L., et al. (2016). Phonological skills, visual attention span, and visual stress in developmental dyslexia. Dev. Psychol. 52, 1503–1516. doi: 10.1037/dev0000184

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Schuchardt, K., Maehler, C., and Hasselhorn, M. (2008). Working memory deficits in children with specific learning disorders. J. Learn. Disabil. 41, 514–523. doi: 10.1177/0022219408317856

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Schulte-Körne, G., Deimel, W., Bartling, J., and Remschmidt, H. (2001). Speech perception deficit in dyslexic adults as measured by mismatch negativity (MMN). Int. J. Psychophysiol. 40, 77–87. doi: 10.1016/S0167-8760(00)00152-5

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Seymour, P. H. K., Aro, M., and Erskine, J. M. (2003). Foundation literacy acquisition in European orthographies. Br. J. Psychol. 94, 143–174. doi: 10.1348/000712603321661859

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Simos, P., Mouzaki, A., and Sideridis, G. (2007). Test of Detection and Investigation of Attention and Concentration for Elementary School Students. Athens: Greek Ministry of Education. (In Greek).

Google Scholar

Snowling, M. J. (1995). Phonological processing and developmental dyslexia. J. Res. Read. 18, 132–138. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9817.1995.tb00079.x

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Snowling, M. J. (2000). Dyslexia, 2nd Edn. Hoboken, NJ: Blackwell Publishing.

Google Scholar

Snowling, M. J. (2008). Specific disorders and broader phenotypes: the case of dyslexia. Q. J. Exp. Psychol. 61, 142–156. doi: 10.1080/17470210701508830

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Soriano-Ferrer, M., Nievas-Cazorla, F., Sánchez-López, P., Félix-Mateo, V., and González-Torre, J. A. (2014). Reading-related cognitive deficits in Spanish developmental dyslexia. Procedia Soc. Behav. Sci. 132, 3–9. doi: 10.1016/j.sbspro.2014.04.270

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Sperling, A. J., Lu, Z.-L., Manis, F. R., and Seidenberg, M. S. (2005). Deficits in perceptual noise exclusion in developmental dyslexia. Nat. Neurosci. 8, 862–863. doi: 10.1038/nn1474

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Stanovich, K. E. (1988). Explaining the differences between the dyslexic and the garden-variety poor reader: the phonological-core variable-difference model. J. Learn. Disabil. 21, 590–604. doi: 10.1177/002221948802101003

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Stein, J. (2001). The magnocellular theory of developmental dyslexia. Dyslexia 7, 12–36. doi: 10.1002/dys.186

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Stein, J., and Walsh, V. (1997). To see but not to read: the magnocellular theory of dyslexia. Trends Neurosci. 20, 147–152. doi: 10.1016/S0166-2236(96)01005-3

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Stogiannidou, A. (2017). WISC-V GR (Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children - 5th Edition). Athens: Motivo Ekdotiki. (In Greek).

Google Scholar

Swanson, H. L. (2006). “Working memory and learning disabilities: both phonological and executive processing deficits are important,” in Working Memory and Neurodevelopmental Disorders, eds T. P. Alloway and S. E. Gathercole (London: Psychology Press), 59–88.

Google Scholar

Tallal, P. (1980). Language and reading: some perceptual prerequisites. Bull. Orton Soc. 30, 170–178. doi: 10.1007/BF02653716

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Tallal, P., Stark, R., and Mellits, E. (1985). Identification of language-impaired children on the basis of rapid perception and production skills. Brain Lang. 25, 314–322. doi: 10.1016/0093-934X(85)90087-2

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Tzouriadou, M., Anagnostopoulou, E., Toutountzi, E., and Psoinou, M. (2008). Detroit Test of Learning Aptitude (DTLA, DTLA-P: 3, DTLA-4). Thessaloniki: Aristotle University of Thessaloniki, Ministry of Greek Education. (In Greek).

Google Scholar

Valdois, S., Bosse, M.-L., Ans, B., Carbonnel, S., Zorman, M., David, D., et al. (2003). Phonological and visual processing deficits can dissociate in developmental dyslexia: evidence from two case studies. Read. Writ. 16, 541–572. doi: 10.1023/A:1025501406971

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

van Bergen, E., de Jong, P. F., Maassen, B., and van der Leij, A. (2014b). The effect of parents’ literacy skills and children’s preliteracy skills on the risk of dyslexia. J. Abnorm. Child Psychol. 42, 1187–1200. doi: 10.1007/s10802-014-9858-9

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

van Bergen, E., van der Leij, A., and de Jong, P. F. (2014a). The intergenerational multiple deficit model and the case of dyslexia. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 8:346. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2014.00346

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Vanderauwera, J., Wouters, J., Vandermosten, M., and Ghesquière, P. (2017). Early dynamics of white matter deficits in children developing dyslexia. Dev. Cogn. Neurosci. 27, 69–77. doi: 10.1016/j.dcn.2017.08.003

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Vellutino, F. R., and Fletcher, J. M. (2005). The Science of Reading: A Handbook. Malden, MA: Blackwell Publishing, 362–378.

Google Scholar

Vidyasagar, T. R., and Pammer, K. (2010). Dyslexia: a deficit in visuo-spatial attention, not in phonological processing. Trends Cogn. Sci. 14, 57–63. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2009.12.003

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Vlachos, F. (2010). Dyslexia: a synthetic approach to causal theories. Hellenic J. Psychol. 7, 205–240.

Google Scholar

Vlachos, F., and Nisiotou-Mantelou, I. (2013). Dyslexia gene? Paediatriki 76, 20–25. (In Greek).

Google Scholar

Vlachos, F., Avramidis, E., Dedousis, G., Chalmpe, M., Ntalla, I., and Giannakopoulou, M. (2013). Prevalence and gender ratio of dyslexia in Greek adolescents and its association with parental history and brain injury. Am. J. Educ. Res. 1, 22–25.

Google Scholar

Ward, J. H. (1963). Hierarchical grouping to optimize an objective function. J. Am. Stat. Assoc. 58, 236–244. doi: 10.1080/01621459.1963.10500845

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

White, S., Milne, E., Rosen, S., Hansen, P., Swettenham, J., Frith, U., et al. (2006). The role of sensorimotor impairments in dyslexia: a multiple case study of dyslexic children. Dev. Sci. 9, 237–255. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-7687.2006.00483.x

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Wolf, M., and Bowers, P. G. (1999). The double-deficit hypothesis for the developmental dyslexias. J. Educ. Psychol. 91, 415–438. doi: 10.1037/0022-0663.91.3.415

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Wolf, M., Bowers, P. G., and Biddle, K. (2000). Naming-speed processes, timing, and reading: a conceptual review. J. Learn. Disabil. 33, 387–407. doi: 10.1177/002221940003300409

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Ziegler, J. C., and Goswami, U. (2005). Reading acquisition, developmental dyslexia, and skilled reading across languages: a psycholinguistic grain size theory. Psychol. Bull. 131, 3–29. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.131.1.3

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Zoubrinetzky, R., Bielle, F., and Valdois, S. (2014). New insights on developmental dyslexia subtypes: heterogeneity of mixed reading profiles. PLoS One 9:e99337. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0099337

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: developmental dyslexia, subtypes, multiple deficits models, primary-aged students, neurocognitive deficits

Citation: Chalmpe M and Vlachos F (2025) Are there distinct subtypes of developmental dyslexia? Front. Behav. Neurosci. 18:1512892. doi: 10.3389/fnbeh.2024.1512892

Received: 17 October 2024; Accepted: 12 December 2024;
Published: 03 January 2025.

Edited by:

Christos A. Frantzidis, University of Lincoln, United Kingdom

Reviewed by:

Ioanna Talli, Aristotle University of Thessaloniki, Greece
Kyriaki Neophytou, Johns Hopkins University, United States
Barbara Piotrowska, Edinburgh Napier University, United Kingdom

Copyright © 2025 Chalmpe and Vlachos. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Maria Chalmpe, bWFoYWxiZUB1dGguZ3I=

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.