Skip to main content

OPINION article

Front. Behav. Neurosci., 05 April 2024
Sec. Emotion Regulation and Processing
This article is part of the Research Topic From Social Wires to Neurobiological Connections: A Neuropsychobiological Focus on Parent-Child Interaction View all 7 articles

In the heat of connection: using infrared thermal imaging to shed new light into early parent-infant co-regulation patterns

  • 1Department of Brain and Behavioral Sciences, University of Pavia, Pavia, Italy
  • 2Developmental Psychobiology Lab, IRCCS Mondino Foundation, Pavia, Italy

1 Introduction

Soon in life, humans' bio-behavioral systems are organized and integrated in response to the social environments (Beeghly and Tronick, 2011; Rothbart et al., 2011). Co-regulation between parent and infant, involving the reciprocal coordination of physiology, emotion, and behaviors during the interaction (Butler and Randall, 2013), plays an important role in these organizational processes. Noteworthy, patterns of parent-infant co-regulation across the first year of life influence several emerging developmental processes such as attachment, socio-emotional development and stress regulation (Feldman, 2007b; Evans and Porter, 2009; Kiel et al., 2024). In this vein, delving into the diverse facets of parent-infant co-regulation is increasingly acknowledged as a key focus in developmental science.

The biological stress response systems, including the Autonomic Nervous System (ANS), are thought to play a key role in co-regulation processes (Feldman, 2007a). Small but growing literature suggests that synchronous interactions may modulate mother-infant autonomic physiology such as cardiac activity (Feldman et al., 2011; Porter et al., 2022). Traditional gold standard physiological methods for the assessments of the ANS functioning include electrocardiography or skin conductance (Ioannou et al., 2014). These classical techniques require the use of contact sensors, restrictions of movements, and high levels of compliance of the participants, resulting in more invasive and less ecological ANS measurements. This can be of particular concern with very young populations where the compliance of participants cannot be assured, and all experimental manipulations might result in biased evaluation of infants' emotional state and of the dyadic interaction.

Infrared thermal imaging (ITI) is an increasingly employed technique that enables to accurately measure cutaneous temperature in a non-invasive, ecological, and contactless manner. ITI devices, also known as thermal cameras, can capture the naturally emitted infrared thermal radiation of the human body and convert it into a radiometric thermal image that digitally maps the superficial temperature distribution of the object of measurement. Recent advances in infrared technology led to up-to-date detectors that guarantee high thermal resolution and accuracy, as well as relatively low-cost mobile thermal cameras options. While ITI use in biomedical fields is growing, there are still sparse applications of this technique to developmental science and particularly, to the assessment of parent-infant interaction. In this opinion, we describe the nature of the thermal response and how it can be molded in real time by social interaction, we review available evidence of ITI as applied to parent-infant interaction, and we outline potentialities and future direction for ITI as a new promising avenue for the accurate and non-invasive investigation of early parent-infant co-regulation.

2 The affective thermal response

ITI techniques allow to examine variations in the ANS activity reflected by cutaneous temperature modulations. Several parameters of the ANS activity exhibit a specific thermal signature (Cardone and Merla, 2017) and can be estimated at a distance employing bioheat transfer models. Thermal directional changes on the skin related to blood flow are the most widely explored thermal indices (Ioannou et al., 2014). Blood flow transfers the heat from the body core to the skin and is regulated by vascular processes under the control of the sympathetic (SNS) and parasympathetic (PNS) nervous systems. Vasoconstriction is mainly controlled by the SNS and induces a local decrease in skin temperature. In contrast, vasodilation has been associated with an increase in cutaneous surface thermal radiation which depends on a greater cutaneous blood perfusion. In a seminal study Kistler et al. (1998) used laser Doppler flowmetry together with ITI to measure, respectively, blood flow and temperature of the fingertip while exposing 30 participants to various stressors like horror movies and acupuncture. They reported a decrease in blood flow (i.e., vasoconstriction), followed by a 15-s delay in fingertip temperature decrease, with over 92% of occasions where the blood flow decrease led to fingertip cooling, suggesting thermal imaging reflects sympathetic activity. Since then, several studies have demonstrated the reliability of ITI as a measure of autonomic activation, by comparing it with simultaneous recordings made with gold-standard methods, including ECG and skin conductance (e.g., Shastri et al., 2009; Kuraoka and Nakamura, 2011). These studies suggest that ITI can reliably capture psychophysiological arousal states while differentiating them from baseline conditions (Shastri et al., 2009; Nhan and Chau, 2010). Furthermore, research has shown that ITI and skin conductance have similar detection power both in human and non-human primates (Coli et al., 2007; Shastri et al., 2009; Kuraoka and Nakamura, 2011; Pavlidis et al., 2012), but different latencies (Merla and Romani, 2007; Kuraoka and Nakamura, 2011). Specifically, a key difference in the onset of the responses was observed, with temperature variations being detected at least 10 s after a stimulus as compared to skin conductance being able to show changes in signal much faster, within 3 s (Merla and Romani, 2007; Kuraoka and Nakamura, 2011).

Studies that investigated the ANS response during social interaction through ITI have typically been focused on measuring facial cutaneous thermal variations both from a temporal and topographic perspective (Ioannou et al., 2014). The face is particularly relevant because it is not obscured, can be easily recorded and is highly involved in social interaction. Facial temperature is related to fluctuations in the distributions of blood in the vessels and is regulated by SNS and PNS activity, being responsive to emotional stimuli in the environment (Kreibig, 2010). As previously mentioned, vasodilation is related to an increase in facial temperature, whereas vasoconstriction implies a decrease in facial temperature. These changes in the infrared emissivity of the facial skin can be captured through ITI and can be monitored over time and across different regions of the face by using simple metrics such as the thermal difference between two time points or two spatial regions. When it comes to the latter, regions of interest (ROIs) within the face are usually employed, with the most studied ROIs being the nose or the nasal tip, the periorbital or supraorbital area, the forehead, the orbicularis oculi, and the maxillary area (Ioannou et al., 2014). In particular, the nasal tip shows the most consistent thermal response to emotional/stressful stimuli with a decrease of the temperature of this ROIs being consistently found in response to emotional and stressful stimuli and being related to sympathetic adrenergic vasoconstrictor activity (Merla and Romani, 2007; Ebisch et al., 2012).

3 The facial thermal response to parent-infant interactions

Thanks to its non-invasiveness, ITI offers the possibility to study humans' emotional and physiological autonomic reactions during social interactions in a more naturalistic setting. Several studies have been conducted on the modulation of facial temperature when interacting with one or more persons in adults (e.g., Hahn et al., 2012; Park et al., 2013; Ponsi et al., 2019). In contrast, only few human studies employed ITI to assess some aspects of parent-infant interactions (Ebisch et al., 2012; Manini et al., 2013; Aureli et al., 2015; Ioannou et al., 2021). Albeit preliminary, evidence indicated that infants' facial temperature is responsive to what happens in the social environment. In particular, Aureli et al. (2015) reported an increase in infants' temperature of the nasal tip and forehead during a face-to-face mother-infant interaction at 3–4 months of age, possibly suggesting a PNS activation during the experimental paradigm. Furthermore, thermal variations were related to infants' behavior, with greater increase in infant's temperature being related to greater engagement with the environment during the still face episode and less negative affectivity. This might suggest that infants' PNS activation might support infants' social engagement with the surrounding environment, as predicted by the Polyvagal Theory (Porges, 2001). Likewise, an increase in 2-month-old infants' facial temperature while interacting with a stranger, as compared to the mother, was reported in another study (Ioannou et al., 2021), being suggestive of a greater involvement of the PNS as social challenges increase. This was paralleled by behavioral data showing greater gaze duration to the stranger as compared to the mother. Although replication of these findings in larger samples is needed, this preliminary evidence indicates that infants' thermal variations during social interaction might be a sensitive marker of PNS activation. Additionally, an activation of the SNS, as indicated by a decrease of the temperature of infants' maxillary area and nasal tip was detected in older children (38–42 months) through ITI during more stressful paradigm, such as the “mishap” paradigm (Ebisch et al., 2012). This paradigm consisted of five episodes, including a “mishap” episode, during which the child is left alone by the experimenter and accidentally “broke” the toy that was given to him. This is followed by the return of the experimenter that did not speak for 30 s and looked at the broken toy and by the “soothing” of the child (Cole et al., 1992). A similar facial thermal modulation was reported in mothers observing their children (Ebisch et al., 2012) and in unrelated women watching unknown children (Manini et al., 2013) during the “mishap” paradigm. Most interestingly, a positive association between maternal and child thermal response during the stressful task was reported (Ebisch et al., 2012) and autonomic attunement was modulated by the degree of relatedness, with a slightly stronger and much faster attunement in thermal responses of mother-child dyads as compared to unknown women-child dyads, as showed by cross-correlation analysis (Manini et al., 2013). This initial evidence suggests that the assessment of mother-infant facial thermal variations might provide further insight into the phenomenon of physiological co-regulation. Further studies using ITI are needed to investigate the degree of physiological synchrony in facial thermal variations among mothers and infants in response to both stressful and non-stressful situations, as well as their role in the development of infant stress regulation.

4 Discussion

Paralleling the rising interest into the mechanisms underlying the impact of early interactions in human development, there is an urgent need of sensitive and ecologically valid methods that allow to evaluate infants' physiology during real interactions in a naturalistic environment. Traditional physiological parameters, such as heart rate or skin conductance, are now very simple to be collected and analyzed and are increasingly employed in pediatric populations (e.g., Provenzi et al., 2015; Nava et al., 2016; Quadrelli et al., 2019; Reali et al., 2021; Nazzari et al., 2022a). Limitations of these methods include that they are mostly obtained through contact sensors, required participants' compliance to correctly wear the device and a certain amount of time for their accurate application. These drawbacks become particularly relevant when testing challenging populations, such as infants or clinical samples. In the current opinion, we suggest the potential of ITI as a useful tool to investigate parent-infant autonomic co-regulation in the context of early interactions. Thanks to its non-invasiveness and contact-free nature, ITI provides the unique opportunity to study natural social interactions without interfering with spontaneous behaviors in an ecological valid setting. This can be especially important when it comes to the investigation of social interactions early in life or in especially vulnerable samples. For example, ITI might be a useful option to study co-regulation processes in samples of atypically developing children with high skin sensitivity, such as children with autism spectrum disorder. Likewise, children with somatic diseases, such as atopic dermatitis, or conditions related to skin fragility, such as very small premature babies, might benefit for the use of a contact-free techniques, such as ITI, that does not involve the application of electrodes or other devices on their skin.

Available studies showed that, as early as 2 months of age, facial thermal variations are sensitive to social challenges and can represent a marker of autonomic activation (Ebisch et al., 2012; Aureli et al., 2015). It is now widely acknowledged that a complex interplay among genetic and environmental factors is involved in shaping infants' individual differences in stress regulation (Grant et al., 2009; Nazzari et al., 2022b). Thus, future research into the role of individual or caregiving-related influences on early infant thermal regulation is required.

As promising as ITI appears to be, it is equally true that several methodological issues deserve considerations when designing an ITI study. First of all, it is important to keep in mind that despite the advantages offered by ITI, the thermal signal that result from vascular changes is rather slow and has a longer latency as compared to traditional methods such as skin conductance (Kuraoka and Nakamura, 2011). Furthermore, individual and environmental sources of interference with thermal assessment need to be accounted for, as well as technical issues involving distance, motion and angles. An illustrative setting for the assessment of mother-infant thermal regulation through ITI is graphically schematized in Figure 1. We recommend consulting the International Academy of Thermology (IACT) guidelines (Thermography Guidelines, 2002), for a broader discussion. Specific considerations might concern studies on pediatric samples. For example, maternal touching of the face of the child during the ITI recordings might confound the assessment and should thus be avoided. Likewise, a manual tracking system might be recommended to avoid noise related to infants' rapid head movements. Lastly, studies that correlate thermal measures with other well-established physiological measures of stress regulation early in life are needed.

Figure 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Schematic representation of an illustrative setting for the assessment of mother-infant thermal regulation through ITI.

In conclusion, ITI represents a potential versatile and reliable tool for the investigation of physiological responses to social exchanges early in life in both typical and atypical populations which still warrants further explorations. The potential contribution of ITI extends to the examination of early patterns of bio-behavioral synchrony as they unfold in a more ecological and naturalistic setting. Furthermore, the development of small, mobile, low-cost thermal device imaging device holds the promise to further bridge the gap between a constrained laboratory setting and a natural real-life scenario. This opens the door for a more inclusive participation of diverse populations in developmental research. Recognizing that early co-regulation of physiology and behavior between parent and infant lay the ground for later human social interactions and children wellbeing and adaptation (Feldman, 2007a), the use of ITI stands to enrich our understanding of these fundamental processes.

Author contributions

SN: Conceptualization, Supervision, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing. FD: Investigation, Writing – original draft. EJ: Writing – original draft. SL: Investigation, Writing – original draft. AS: Visualization, Writing – original draft. LP: Supervision, Writing – review & editing.

Funding

The author(s) declare that financial support was received for the research, authorship, and/or publication of this article. SN was supported by a co-funding of the European Union-European Social Fund REACT-EU, PON Ricerca e Innovazione 2014–2020. LP was supported by the Italian Health Ministry, Ricerca Corrente 2022–2023 and by #NEXTGENERATIONEU (NGEU) and funded by the Ministry of University and Research (MUR), National Recovery and Resilience Plan (NRRP), project MNESYS (PE0000006) – A Multiscale integrated approach to the study of the nervous system in health and disease (DN.1553 11.10.2022).

Acknowledgments

Authors are thankful to the members of the Developmental Psychobiology (dpb) Lab at IRCCS Mondino Foundation and University of Pavia and to the students attending the lab: discussion within the group has been an invaluable source of inspiration for this work.

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

The author(s) declared that they were an editorial board member of Frontiers, at the time of submission. This had no impact on the peer review process and the final decision.

Publisher's note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

References

Aureli, T., Grazia, A., Cardone, D., and Merla, A. (2015). Behavioral and facial thermal variations in 3-to 4-month-old infants during the Still-Face Paradigm. Front. Psychol. 6, 1–10. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2015.01586

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Beeghly, M., and Tronick, E. (2011). Early resilience in the context of parentinfant relationships: a social developmental perspective. Curr. Probl. Pediatr. Adolesc. Health Care 41, 197–201. doi: 10.1016/j.cppeds.2011.02.005

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Butler, E. A., and Randall, A. K. (2013). Emotional coregulation in close relationships. Emot. Rev. 5, 202–210. doi: 10.1177/1754073912451630

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Cardone, D., and Merla, A. (2017). New frontiers for applications of thermal infrared imaging devices: computational psychopshysiology in the neurosciences. Sensors 17:1042. doi: 10.3390/s17051042

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Cole, P. M., Barrett, K. C., and Zahn-Waxler, C. (1992). Emotion displays in two-year-olds during mishaps. Child Dev. 63, 314–324. doi: 10.2307/1131481

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Coli, M, Fontanella, L., Ippoliti, L., and Merla, A. (2007). “Multiresolution KLE of psycho-physiological signals,” in Proceedings of S.Co. 2007, Book of Short Papers ISBN 978-886129-114-0 Venice (Padova), 116–121.

Google Scholar

Ebisch, S. J., Aureli, T., Bafunno, D., Cardone, D., Romani, G. L., and Merla, A. (2012). Mother and child in synchrony: thermal facial imprints of autonomic contagion. Biol. Psychol. 89, 123–129. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2011.09.018

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Evans, C. A., and Porter, C. L. (2009). The emergence of mother-infant co-regulation during the first year: Links to infants' developmental status and attachment. Infant Behav. Dev. 32, 147–158. doi: 10.1016/j.infbeh.2008.12.005

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Feldman, R. (2007a). Parent-infant synchrony: Biological foundations and developmental outcomes. Curr. Dir. Psychol. Sci. 16, 340–345. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8721.2007.00532.x

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Feldman, R. (2007b). Parent-infant synchrony and the construction of shared timing; physiological precursors, developmental outcomes, and risk conditions. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry Allied Discipl. 48, 329–354. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2006.01701.x

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Feldman, R., Magori-Cohen, R., Galili, G., Singer, M., and Louzoun, Y. (2011). Mother and infant coordinate heart rhythms through episodes of interaction synchrony. Infant Behav. Dev. 34, 569–577. doi: 10.1016/j.infbeh.2011.06.008

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Grant, K. A., McMahon, C., Austin, M. P., Reilly, N., Leader, L., and Ali, S. (2009). Maternal prenatal anxiety, postnatal caregiving and infants' cortisol responses to the still-face procedure. Dev. Psychobiol. 51, 625–637. doi: 10.1002/dev.20397

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Hahn, A. C., Whitehead, R. D., Albrecht, M., Lefevre, C. E., and Perrett, D. I. (2012). Hot or not? Thermal reactions to social contact. Biol. Lett. 8, 864–867. doi: 10.1098/rsbl.2012.0338

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Ioannou, S., Gallese, V., and Merla, A. (2014). Thermal infrared imaging in psychophysiology: potentialities and limits. Psychophysiology 51, 951–963. doi: 10.1111/psyp.12243

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Ioannou, S., Morris, P., Hassanain, H., Baker, M., Alkattan, F., Almakadma, A. H., et al. (2021). Under the skin: exploring 2-month-olds' thermal reactions in different social interactions with mother and stranger. Infancy 26, 352–368. doi: 10.1111/infa.12390

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Kiel, N., Samdan, G., Wienke, A. S., Reinelt, T., Pauen, S., Mathes, B., et al. (2024). From co-regulation to self-regulation: Maternal soothing behaviour and self-efficacy related to infant regulation at 3 and 7 months. Infant Behav. Dev. 45, 135–152. doi: 10.1002/imhj.22098

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Kistler, A., Mariauzouls, C., and von Berlepsch, K. (1998). Fingertip temperature as an indicator for sympathetic responses. Int. J. Psychophysiol. 29, 35–41. doi: 10.1016/S0167-8760(97)00087-1

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Kreibig, S. D. (2010). Autonomic nervous system activity in emotion: a review. Biol. Psychol. 84, 394–421. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2010.03.010

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Kuraoka, K., and Nakamura, K. (2011). The use of nasal skin temperature measurements in studying emotion in macaque monkeys. Physiol. Behav. 102, 347–355. doi: 10.1016/j.physbeh.2010.11.029

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Manini, B., Cardone, D., Ebisch, S. J. H., Bafunno, D., Aureli, T., and Merla, A. (2013). Mom feels what her child feels: thermal signatures of vicarious autonomic response while watching children in a stressful situation. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 7:299. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2013.00299

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Merla, A., and Romani, G. L. (2007). Thermal signatures of emotional arousal: a functional infrared imaging study. Annu. Int. Conf. IEEE Eng. Med. Biol. Soc. 2007, 247–249. doi: 10.1109/IEMBS.2007.4352270

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Nava, E., Romano, D., Grassi, M., and Turati, C. (2016). Skin conductance reveals the early development of the unconscious processing of emotions. Cortex 84, 124–131. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2016.07.011

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Nazzari, S., Fearon, P., Rice, F., Molteni, M., and Frigerio, A. (2022b). Maternal caregiving moderates the impact of antenatal maternal cortisol on infant stress regulation. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry Allied Discip. 63, 871–880. doi: 10.1111/jcpp.13532

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Nazzari, S., Reali, P., Ceppi, E., Giorda, R., Piazza, C., Bianchi, A. M., et al. (2022a). Respiratory Sinus Arrhythmia (RSA) stress response in preschool age varies by serotonin transporter polymorphism (5-HTTLPR): a preliminary report. J. Exp. Child Psychol. 219:105413. doi: 10.1016/j.jecp.2022.105413

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Nhan, B. R., and Chau, T. (2010). Classifying affective states using thermal infrared imaging of the human face. IEEE Trans. Biomed. Eng. 57, 979–987. doi: 10.1109/TBME.2009.2035926

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Park, K. K., Suk, H. W., Hwang, H., and Lee, J. H. (2013). A functional analysis of deception detection of a mock crime using Infrared Thermal Imaging and the Concealed Information Test. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 7, 1–17. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2013.00070

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Pavlidis, I., Tsiamyrtzis, P., Shastri, D., Wesley, A., Zhou, Y., Lindner, P., et al. (2012). Fast by nature - how stress patterns define human experience and performance in dexterous tasks. Sci. Rep. 2:305. doi: 10.1038/srep00305

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Ponsi, G., Monachesi, B., Panasiti, V., Aglioti, S. M., and Panasiti, M. S. (2019). Physiological and behavioral reactivity to social exclusion: a functional infrared thermal imaging study in patients with psoriasis. J. Neurophysiol. 121, 38–49. doi: 10.1152/jn.00555.2018

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Porges, S. W. (2001). The polyvagal theory: phylogenetic substrates of a social nervous system. Int. J. Psychophysiol. 42, 123–146. doi: 10.1016/S0167-8760(01)00162-3

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Porter, C. L., Yang, C., Jorgensen, N. A., and Evans-Stout, C. (2022). Development of mother-infant co-regulation: the role of infant vagal tone and temperament at 6, 9, and 12 months of age. Infant Behav. Dev. 67:101708. doi: 10.1016/j.infbeh.2022.101708

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Provenzi, L., Casini, E., de Simone, P., Reni, G., Borgatti, R., and Montirosso, R. (2015). Mother–infant dyadic reparation and individual differences in vagal tone affect 4-month-old infants' social stress regulation. J. Exp. Child Psychol. 140, 158–170. doi: 10.1016/j.jecp.2015.07.003

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Quadrelli, E., Geangu, E., and Turati, C. (2019). Human action sounds elicit sensorimotor activation early in life. Cortex 117, 323–335. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2019.05.009

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Reali, P., Piazza, C., Tacchino, G., Songia, L., Nazzari, S., Reni, G., et al. (2021). Assessing stress variations in children during the strange situation procedure: comparison of three widely used respiratory sinus arrhythmia estimation methods. Physiol. Meas. 42:ac18ff. doi: 10.1088/1361-6579/ac18ff

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Rothbart, M. K., Sheese, B. E., Rueda, M. R., and Posner, M. I. (2011). Developing mechanisms of self-regulation in early life. Emot. Rev. 3, 207–213. doi: 10.1177/1754073910387943

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Shastri, D., Merla, A., Tsiamyrtzis, P., and Pavlidis, I. (2009). Imaging facial signs of neurophysiological responses. IEEE Trans. Biomed. Eng. 56, 477–484. doi: 10.1109/TBME.2008.2003265

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Thermography Guidelines (2002). Standards and Protocols in Clinical Thermographic Imaging. Available online: http://www.iact-org.org/professionals/thermog-guidelines.html

Google Scholar

Keywords: infrared thermal imaging, emotion regulation, Autonomic Nervous System, parent-infant interaction, synchrony

Citation: Nazzari S, Darvehei F, Jensen EN, Lucchin S, Samoukina A and Provenzi L (2024) In the heat of connection: using infrared thermal imaging to shed new light into early parent-infant co-regulation patterns. Front. Behav. Neurosci. 18:1388886. doi: 10.3389/fnbeh.2024.1388886

Received: 20 February 2024; Accepted: 26 March 2024;
Published: 05 April 2024.

Edited by:

Serge Campeau, University of Colorado Boulder, United States

Reviewed by:

Henriette Acosta, University of Marburg, Germany

Copyright © 2024 Nazzari, Darvehei, Jensen, Lucchin, Samoukina and Provenzi. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Sarah Nazzari, c2FyYWgubmF6emFyaSYjeDAwMDQwO3VuaXB2Lml0

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.