- 1Center for Studies and Research in Cognitive Neuroscience, Department of Psychology, University of Bologna, Bologna, Italy
- 2Swiss Center for Affective Sciences and Department of Psychology, University of Geneva, Geneva, Switzerland
- 3Institut de Neurosciences de la Timone, UMR 7289 CNRS & Aix-Marseille Université, Marseille, France
Editorial on the Research Topic
The neurobiological and cognitive underpinnings of appetitive and aversive motivation
Motivation is a complex construct encompassing the processes involved in the initiation, direction, energization, intensity, and persistence of behavior. These processes are central to survival and wellbeing: They help organisms anticipate, adapt, and react to changes in their internal states and in their environment by eliciting multifarious affective responses and appetitive or defensive behaviors (Berridge, 2018; Levy and Schiller, 2021; Stussi and Pool, 2022). For instance, organisms are typically motivated by the opportunity of obtaining rewards and avoiding punishments or threats, which in turn facilitates the execution of approach and avoidance behaviors, respectively (Guitart-Masip et al., 2014; Starita et al., 2022). Whereas, these forms of appetitive and aversive motivation are generally highly adaptive and contribute to healthy functioning, they can sometimes go awry and become maladaptive. This is notably the case in conditions such as apathy, anhedonia, addiction, impulse disorders, or anxiety-related disorders (Berridge and Robinson, 2016; Mogg and Bradley, 2018; Terenzi et al., 2022; Bresin and Hunt, 2023).
In the past decade, a flourishing of studies have examined the behavioral and neural correlates of motivation in healthy individuals as well as the cognitive determinants of motivation and its dysregulation in several psychopathological disorders. With the emergence of affective science (Dukes et al., 2021), there also has been an increased focus and recognition of the prominent influence that motivation exerts on cognition, including attention, learning, memory, and decision-making (Pool et al., 2016; O'Doherty et al., 2017; Stussi et al., 2018; Starita et al., 2019; Garofalo et al., 2020; Terenzi et al., 2021). These two aspects have contributed to pushing the boundaries of knowledge on motivated behavior and the bidirectional links between motivational and cognitive processes.
In this perspective, this Research Topic aims to promote the cognitive neuroscience of human motivation in a 2-fold manner. First, it highlights recent progress in the study of the neural substrates of appetitive and aversive motivation in humans. Second, it showcases how appetitive and aversive motivation impact cognition across various domains. Through these objectives, the present Research Topic seeks to advance the understanding of the neurobiological and psychological mechanisms underlying appetitive and aversive motivation and their influence on cognition.
A key focus of investigation in this Research Topic concerns the role of specific brain regions in reward processing—a core component of appetitive motivation. The brain's reward system involves key structures such as the striatum, the midbrain dopamine neurons, the amygdala, and the prefrontal cortex, including the ventromedial prefrontal cortex (vmPFC; Haber and Knutson, 2010; Berridge and Kringelbach, 2015; Hiser and Koenigs, 2018). While prior research has demonstrated that the vmPFC is involved in the reward network, its precise contribution remains unclear. The study of Rehbein et al. tested the causal role of the vmPFC in predicting and processing rewards by modulating vmPFC excitability with transcranial direct current stimulation (tDCS) before participants performed a gambling task. Results showed that vmPFC excitation through tDCS introduces a positive bias in the reward system, enhancing anticipation, positive outcome appraisal, and improving reward-based learning. This was evident in greater behavioral flexibility following losses and unexpected outcomes, indicating an improved response to feedback. These findings provide evidence delineating the crucial role of the vmPFC in predicting and processing rewards.
Another set of contributions explored the effects of appetitive and aversive motivation on a range of cognitive processes. Gorrino et al. investigated the role of the insular cortex—a key brain region implicated in appetitive and aversive motivated behavior (Ibrahim et al., 2019; Centanni et al., 2021; Horing and Büchel, 2022)—in decision-making and cognitive control. Using high-definition tDCS, they observed no effect of stimulation of the right posterior insula either on loss and risk aversion or on cognitive control. These negative findings highlight the need for further research to establish the effectiveness and sensitivity of insula stimulation in modulating the interplay between motivational and cognitive processes. Yang et al. further examined the influence of aversive motivation on cognitive control. Specifically, they compared the contribution of aversive motivation vs. negative affect to punishment-driven improvement in cognitive control. Using the Stroop Task and manipulating feedback type (punishment vs. neutral) and feedback contingency (performance-contingent vs. non-contingent), they found that negative affect per se did not enhance cognitive control. Instead, aversive motivation improved cognitive control, especially for participants who were aware of the contingency between punishment and performance. These results outline the contribution of aversive motivation to cognitive control and uncover the critical role of metacognition in this process. Finally, Bublatzky et al. investigated whether aversive apprehensions, i.e., trait anxiety, affect spatial navigation in threatening or safe virtual reality contexts. They showed that, relative to individuals with lower trait anxiety, more anxious individuals displayed improved spatial navigation only in the threatening context. This threat-related enhancement in spatial navigation was selectively observed in a route retracing task, but not in a route repetition one. These results suggest that, besides their detrimental effects, aversive apprehensions may support navigational performance when congruent with adaptive avoidance behaviors.
In sum, the present Research Topic offers an overview of the current research on appetitive and aversive motivation that features the diversity of theoretical and methodological approaches within this field. We would like to thank the authors for their work, which provides new insights into the functioning of human motivation. Further exploration into the dynamics of appetitive and aversive motivation may pave the way for informing the development of targeted interventions, thereby fostering a deeper integration of basic and clinical research. This could ultimately contribute to better identifying the role of maladaptive motivational processes in the etiology and maintenance of mental health issues and how these processes can be targeted to improve wellbeing. We hope that this Research Topic will spark interest toward this goal and toward the continuous blossoming of cutting-edge research on human motivation.
Author contributions
FS: Writing—original draft, Writing—review & editing. YS: Writing—original draft, Writing—review & editing. SG: Writing—review & editing. DT: Writing—original draft, Writing—review & editing.
Funding
The author(s) declare financial support was received for the research, authorship, and/or publication of this article. This work was supported by #NEXTGENERATIONEU (NGEU) and funded by the Ministry of University and Research (MUR), National Recovery and Resilience Plan (NRRP), project MNESYS (PE0000006)—A multiscale integrated approach to the study of the nervous system in health and disease (DN. 1553 11.10.2022). FS was supported by a Bial Foundation Grant for Scientific Research 2020/2021 [Grant Number 47/20]. YS was supported by an ERC Starting Grant (INFORL-948671) awarded to Maël Lebreton. SG was supported by the PRIN 2022 PNRR project titled “Individual variations in resisting temptation: the balance between motivational and inhibitory control” funded by the Italian Ministry of University (MUR). DT was supported by Aix-Marseille Université (AMU), Institut de Recherche en Santé Publique (IRESP) and Alliance Aviesan in their call “IRESP-19-ADDICTIONS-02.”
Conflict of interest
The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.
Publisher's note
All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.
References
Berridge, K. C. (2018). Evolving concepts of emotion and motivation. Front. Psychol. 9:1647. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2018.01647
Berridge, K. C., and Kringelbach, M. L. (2015). Pleasure systems in the brain. Neuron 86, 646–664. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2015.02.018
Berridge, K. C., and Robinson, T. E. (2016). Liking, wanting, and the incentive-sensitization theory of addiction. Am. Psychol. 71, 670–679. doi: 10.1037/amp0000059
Bresin, K., and Hunt, R. A. (2023). Appetitive and aversive motivation in dysregulated behaviors: a meta-analysis. Personal. Soc. Psychol. Bullet. 2023:1461672231185509. doi: 10.1177/01461672231185509
Centanni, S. W., Janes, A. W., Haggerty, D. L., Atwood, B., and Hopf, F. W. (2021). Better living through understanding the insula: why subregions can make all the difference. Neuropharmacology 198:108765. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropharm.2021.108765
Dukes, D., Abrams, K., Adolphs, R., Ahmed, M. E., Beatty, A., Berridge, K. C., et al. (2021). The rise of affectivism. Nat. Hum. Behav. 5, 816–820. doi: 10.1038/s41562-021-01130-8
Garofalo, S., Sagliano, L., Starita, F., Trojano, L., and di Pellegrino, G. (2020). Subliminal determinants of cue-guided choice. Sci. Rep. 10:11926. doi: 10.1038/s41598-020-68926-y
Guitart-Masip, M., Duzel, E., Dolan, R., and Dayan, P. (2014). Action versus valence in decision making. Trends Cogn. Sci. 18, 194–202. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2014.01.003
Haber, S. N., and Knutson, B. (2010). The reward circuit: linking primate anatomy and human imaging. Neuropsychopharmacology 35, 4–26. doi: 10.1038/npp.2009.129
Hiser, J., and Koenigs, M. (2018). The multifaceted role of the ventromedial prefrontal cortex in emotion, decision making, social cognition, and psychopathology. Biol. Psychiatr. 83, 638–647. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2017.10.030
Horing, B., and Büchel, C. (2022). The human insula processes both modality-independent and pain-selective learning signals. PLoS Biol. 20:e3001540. doi: 10.1371/journal.pbio.3001540
Ibrahim, C., Rubin-Kahana, D. S., Pushparaj, A., Musiol, M., Blumberger, D. M., Daskalakis, Z. J., et al. (2019). The insula: a brain stimulation target for the treatment of addiction. Front. Pharmacol. 10:720. doi: 10.3389/fphar.2019.00720
Levy, I., and Schiller, D. (2021). Neural computations of threat. Trends Cogn. Sci. 25, 151–171. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2020.11.007
Mogg, K., and Bradley, B. P. (2018). Anxiety and threat-related attention: cognitive-motivational framework and treatment. Trends Cogn. Sci. 22, 225–240. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2018.01.001
O'Doherty, J. P., Cockburn, J., and Pauli, W. M. (2017). Learning, reward, and decision making. Ann. Rev. Psychol. 68, 73–100. doi: 10.1146/annurev-psych-010416-044216
Pool, E., Brosch, T., Delplanque, S., and Sander, D. (2016). Attentional bias for positive emotional stimuli: a meta-analytic investigation. Psychol. Bullet. 142, 79–106. doi: 10.1037/bul0000026
Starita, F., Garofalo, S., Dalbagno, D., Degni, L. A. E., and di Pellegrino, G. (2022). Pavlovian threat learning shapes the kinematics of action. Front. Psychol. 13:1005656. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2022.1005656
Starita, F., Kroes, M. C. W., Davachi, L., Phelps, E. A., and Dunsmoor, J. E. (2019). Threat learning promotes generalization of episodic memory. J. Exp. Psychol. 148, 1426–1434. doi: 10.1037/xge0000551
Stussi, Y., and Pool, E. R. (2022). Multicomponential affective processes modulating food-seeking behaviors. Curr. Opin. Behav. Sci. 48:101226. doi: 10.1016/j.cobeha.2022.101226
Stussi, Y., Pourtois, G., and Sander, D. (2018). Enhanced Pavlovian aversive conditioning to positive emotional stimuli. J. Exp. Psychol. 147, 905–923. doi: 10.1037/xge0000424
Terenzi, D., Catalan, M., Polverino, P., Bertolotti, C., Manganotti, P., Rumiati, R. I., et al. (2022). Effects of tDCS on reward responsiveness and valuation in Parkinson's patients with impulse control disorders. J. Neurol. 269, 1557–1565. doi: 10.1007/s00415-021-10733-0
Keywords: appetitive motivation, aversive motivation, reward, learning, decision-making, cognitive control, ventromedial prefrontal cortex (vmPFC), behavioral neuroscience
Citation: Starita F, Stussi Y, Garofalo S and Terenzi D (2024) Editorial: The neurobiological and cognitive underpinnings of appetitive and aversive motivation. Front. Behav. Neurosci. 18:1383393. doi: 10.3389/fnbeh.2024.1383393
Received: 07 February 2024; Accepted: 12 February 2024;
Published: 21 February 2024.
Edited and reviewed by: Richard G. Hunter, University of Massachusetts Boston, United States
Copyright © 2024 Starita, Stussi, Garofalo and Terenzi. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.
*Correspondence: Francesca Starita, ZnJhbmNlc2NhLnN0YXJpdGEyJiN4MDAwNDA7dW5pYm8uaXQ=; Yoann Stussi, eW9hbm4uc3R1c3NpJiN4MDAwNDA7dW5pZ2UuY2g=; Damiano Terenzi, ZGFtaWFuby50ZXJlbnppJiN4MDAwNDA7dW5pdi1hbXUuZnI=
†These authors have contributed equally to this work and share first and senior authorship
‡ORCID: Francesca Starita orcid.org/0000-0002-5623-0708
Yoann Stussi orcid.org/0000-0002-8601-6737
Sara Garofalo orcid.org/0000-0002-6023-2802
Damiano Terenzi orcid.org/0000-0002-8789-6158