Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Behav. Neurosci., 14 September 2023
Sec. Motivation and Reward
This article is part of the Research Topic Dysfunctional Motivation in Eating Behaviors: A Complex Gene x Environment Interplay View all 6 articles

Pharmacological activation of the amygdala, but not single prolonged footshock-induced acute stress, interferes with cue-induced motivation toward food rewards in rats

  • 1Institute of Molecular Medicine, National Tsing Hua University, Hsinchu, Taiwan
  • 2Institute of Systems Neuroscience, National Tsing Hua University, Hsinchu, Taiwan
  • 3Brain Research Center, National Tsing Hua University, Hsinchu, Taiwan

In the face of threats, animals adapt their behaviors to cope with the situation. Under such circumstances, irrelevant behaviors are usually suppressed. In this study, we examined whether food-seeking motivation would decrease under activation of the amygdala, an important nucleus in the regulation of stress response in the central nervous system, or after a physical acute stress session. In Experiment 1, we pharmacologically activated the basolateral nucleus (BLA) or the central nucleus of the amygdala (CeA) before a cue-induced reinstatement test in rats. Our results showed that activation of the BLA or the CeA abolished cue-induced motivation toward food rewards, while locomotor activity and free food intake were not affected. In Experiments 2 and 3, we further assessed anxiety and despair levels, as well as cue-induced reinstatement, after a single prolonged footshock-induced acute stress in rats. Behaviorally, acute stress did not affect anxiety level, despair level, or cue-induced motivation toward food rewards. Physiologically, there was no difference in cellular activities of the amygdala immediately after acute stress. To conclude, our results suggested that pharmacological activation of the amygdala decreased cue-induced motivation toward food reward. However, physiological acute stress did not immediately interfere with the negative emotions, motivation, or amygdala activities of the animals.

1. Introduction

In the face of threats, it is vital for individuals to generate appropriate responses to enhance their chances of survival. As stated in the predatory imminence theory, animals adjust their behaviors according to the potential of threats from predators (Fanselow and Lester, 1988). For example, when predators are likely to appear, the animals stay alert (pre-encounter defensive behavior). However, when predators are already in direct contact, the animals elicit “circa-strike behavior” to escape. In order to adjust to different scenarios, specific neuronal systems are recruited to guide animal behavior (Duval et al., 2015; Meyer et al., 2021), among which the amygdala acts as one of the brain regions involved in such regulation (Davis et al., 1994; Öhman, 2005; Roozendaal et al., 2007, 2009). For example, the basolateral nucleus of the amygdala (BLA), which is one of the important nuclei to regulate emotions, is activated under acute stress (Cullinan et al., 1995; Kavushansky and Richter-Levin, 2006; Reznikov et al., 2008). The central nucleus of the amygdala (CeA) receives robust projection from the BLA (Pitkänen et al., 1995; Savander et al., 1995) and projects to diverse downstream targets in the regulation of behavioral and physiological responses (Janak and Tye, 2015; Zhang et al., 2021), is also activated under acute stress (Dayas et al., 2001; Reznikov et al., 2007). In a recent study, it was shown that the animals shifted their defensive behavioral strategies based on how they perceived the imminence of the threats in a series of behavioral assays (Hoffman et al., 2022). These results led to the likelihood that acute stress prepared the animals to cope with the challenge by adjusting their behavioral strategies instead of merely leading to typical anxiety- or fear-related behaviors. These results also raised another question: that when defensive behaviors were engaged, whether the reward system became suppressed under such circumstances.

In human studies, subjects showed higher amygdala activities during the anticipatory phase of reward processing under acute stress (Kumar et al., 2014). However, acute stress in laboratory settings reduces reward responsiveness, learning, and sensitivity (Bogdan and Pizzagalli, 2006; Morris and Rottenberg, 2015; Burani et al., 2021). In our previous study, animals displayed different anxiety levels based on different task demands under pharmacological activation of the BLA, and there was a down-regulation of dopamine (DA) population activity in the ventral tegmental area (VTA) in the meantime (Lai and Chang, 2019). The DA population activity, defined as the number of spontaneously firing DA neurons, plays a role in the reward system and determines the magnitude of behavioral response (Grace et al., 2007; Arias-Carrián et al., 2010). Although DA activity increased shortly after acute stress, it subsequently decreased after 18 h in rodent studies (Puglisi-Allegra et al., 1991; Valenti et al., 2011; Chang and Grace, 2013; Belujon et al., 2015). As a result, down-regulation of the DA reward system under BLA activation or after acute stress may lead to a decrease in the motivation of the animals. Indeed, researches have shown that DA depletion in the nucleus accumbens (NAc) decreased the effort-demanded response for food rewards (Aberman and Salamone, 1999; Salamone et al., 2001).

The physiological and behavioral response to acute stress is mediated through a complex system, such as activation of the hypothalamic–pituitary–adrenal (HPA) axis (Johnson et al., 1992; Kudielka and Kirschbaum, 2004) and sympathetic division of autonomic nervous system (ANS) (McCorry, 2007). Several brain nuclei of the central nervous system are also involved in stress regulation, such as increased activities in the amygdala (McEwen, 2007; Dedovic et al., 2009). In this study, we focused on how amygdala activation or single prolonged physical acute stress-regulated motivation. We predicted that the appetitive motivation of the animals would be suppressed under these circumstances. Several behavioral tasks can be used to evaluate motivation, such as outcome devaluation tasks, effort-based tasks, and reward-seeking (Markou et al., 2013). In effort-based tasks, the animals are trained with a fixed ratio schedule (Aberman and Salamone, 1999; Salamone et al., 2001; Simmons and Neill, 2009; De Jong et al., 2015) or a progressive ratio schedule (Zhang et al., 2003; De Jong et al., 2015; Randall et al., 2015). These schedules are used to assess how much effort the animals are willing to pay to get the rewards. On the other hand, reinstatement tests are used to examine the motivation of drug-or food-seeking (Ahmed et al., 2000; Yager and Robinson, 2010; Moorman et al., 2017). We are especially interested in cue-induced motivation toward food rewards in reinstatement tests. During instrumental training, the reward-associated cue becomes a feature of incentive stimulus and the incentive salience is attributed to the DA system (Pitchers et al., 2018). In Experiment 1, the rats were tested under pharmacological activation of the BLA or the CeA, the latter serving as the major output nucleus of the amygdaloid complex. We hypothesized that BLA or CeA activation would decrease the cue-induced motivation toward food rewards in reinstatement tests. In Experiment 2, the rats were tested for behavioral assays after a single prolonged footshock-induced acute stress session, to examine if the acute stress affected anxiety and despair level. We hypothesized that footshock-induced acute stress would increase anxiety and despair levels. In Experiment 3, the rats were tested with cue-induced reinstatement after acute stress and cellular activities were assessed with an immediate early gene of c-fos expression in the amygdala, the NAc core (NAcc), and the VTA DA neurons. We hypothesized that footshock-induced acute stress would decrease the cue-induced motivation toward food reward in reinstatement tests accompanied by increased cellular activities in the amygdala. We also hypothesized that reinstatement tests would increase cellular activities in the NAcc and the VTA DA neurons, but the level of increase would be lower following acute stress.

2. Experimental procedures

2.1. Subjects

A total of 90 male Long-Evans rats (250–300 g; National Laboratory Animal Center, Taiwan) were used in the study. The rats were individually housed in a temperature- (21–22°C) and humidity- (60–70%) controlled facility on a 12 h light/dark cycle (lights on at 7:00 a.m.) with food and water available ad libitum. The rats were handled for at least 5 days for 10 s/day before any experimental procedures. All procedures were performed according to the protocols approved by Institutional Animal Care and Use Committees (IACUC) of both National Tsing Hua University and National Yang Ming Chiao Tung University.

2.2. Surgery

In Experiment 1, the rats were anesthetized with ketamine (80–100 mg/kg, i.p.) and xylazine (10 mg/kg, i.p.), and then were placed on a stereotaxic apparatus (Stoelting). The half-life of ketamine is about 1.5 h (White et al., 1975; Veilleux-Lemieux et al., 2013). The core body temperature was maintained at 37°C by a temperature-controlled heating pad (CWE). Burr holes were drilled above bilateral BLA [from bregma: anterior–posterior (AP) -2.8 mm; mediolateral (ML) +5.2 mm; dorsoventral (DV) −7.6 mm] or CeA [from bregma: (AP) −2.1 mm; (ML) +4.0 mm; (DV) −7.2 mm]. Two 26-gauge stainless steel guide cannulae (Plastics One) were implanted to target the BLA or the CeA. Three anchor screws were mounted on the skull and the headstage was covered with dental acrylic. The rats were injected with carprofen (5 mg/kg, s.c.) for analgesic. At least 5 days of recovery were allowed before drug administration or behavioral assays, while dummy cannulae (Plastics One) extended 1.0 mm beyond the end of the guide cannulae were changed daily.

2.3. Drug administration

In Experiment 1, drugs were given immediately before behavioral tests. The drugs were delivered through injectors (33-gauge, Plastic One) protruding 1.0 mm past the end of the guide cannulae. The injectors were attached to polyethylene tubes and connected to Hamilton syringes (5.0 μL) located on a micro-infusion pump (Harvard Apparatus). The rats were infused with N-methyl-D-aspartate (NMDA, 0.05 μg in 0.5 μL/side) or saline as a control. The dosage was chosen based on previous studies (Rezayof et al., 2007; Solati and Hajikhani, 2010) and was much lower than the concentration used for the lesion procedure (Sananes and Davis, 1992; Roozendaal and McGaugh, 1997; Zimmerman et al., 2007). Previous electrophysiology studies using higher (Legault et al., 2000; Floresco et al., 2001) or lower (Pisani et al., 1997; Shen et al., 2007; Zhang et al., 2022) NMDA concentration in comparison to ours have demonstrated physiological effects under their selective dosages. The drugs were delivered at a rate of 0.2 μL/min for 2 min and 30 s. An additional minute was allowed for drug diffusion before the injectors were removed. The series of drug delivery behavioral tests were counterbalanced. Some subjects were excluded from analyses after the experimental procedure due to histology check, and therefore the group size may not be equally distributed in every drug delivery condition.

2.4. Single prolonged stress paradigm

In Experiments 2 and 3, a single prolonged footshock-induced acute stress was conducted before the reinstatement test. The procedure was conducted in sound-attenuating fear conditioning cubicles (Med-Associates). To generate a setting different from the operant chambers, the ceiling lights and the fans attached to the cubicles remained off, while A-frames were inserted. To provide a distinct odor, stainless-steel pans containing a thin layer of 1% ammonium were placed underneath the grid floors before the rats were placed inside. The same solution was used to clean the chambers between squads. The procedure was adapted from the Arakawa study (Arakawa et al., 2014). During the 2 h session, 5-s footshocks (1 mA) were delivered 80 times. The intertrial interval (ITI) was 90 s on average (70–110 s). The video was recorded in Experiment 3 and the freezing behavior was analyzed with Video Freeze (Med-Associates). The freezing behavior was defined as consecutive observed movements below the motion threshold (program setting at 18) for 1 s (video frame sampling at 0.2 s; e.g. at least continuous six frames below threshold). The “Stress” group of animals underwent the above procedure immediately before the behavioral test, while the “Ctrl” group of animals stayed in their home cages.

2.5. Behavior

2.5.1. Cue-induced reinstatement test

The experiment was conducted in sound-attenuating operant chambers (Med Associates). There were two retractable levers equipped on one of the chamber walls, with stimulus lights above the levers and a food port in between. During behavioral training and tests, the chamber lights and the fans attached to the chambers remained on. To provide a distinct odor, stainless-steel pans containing a thin layer of 1% acetate acid were placed underneath the grid floors before the rats were placed inside. The same solution was used to clean the chambers between squads. The procedure was adapted from the study by McLaughlin and Floresco (2007). Before training, the animals were food deprived to 85–90% of their free-feeding weight. It typically took 5 days to reach this criterion. The food deprivation process continued to the end of all experiments and the rats were weighed daily to ensure they maintained between 85 and 90% of their free-feeding weight. The first 2 days consisted of 30-min behavioral shaping sessions, where reward food pellets (45-mg sucrose pellets, Bio-serv) were dispensed on a fixed-ratio (FR) 1 schedule of reinforcement. On the following days, animals received 20-min lever pressing reinforcement training sessions. During training, both levers were introduced with the left lever designated as the active lever and the right lever as the inactive lever. On the first day of training, the reinforcement schedule was set to a FR1 schedule. Delivery of a food pellet was always paired and preceded by a 5 s light/tone conditioned stimulus (CS), which entailed illumination of the stimulus light above the active lever and presentation of an 80 dB, 3 k Hz tone. This was followed by a 20-s time-out period, where pressing the levers did not result in food pellet/CS delivery. On the second day, the schedule was increased to an FR2 schedule without a 20-s time-out period. A variable-ratio (VR) 5 schedule was implemented on the following 5 days, ensuring that the animals responded reliably to the active lever by the end of training. The rats then underwent daily 20-min extinction sessions, where neither food nor the light/tone CS were presented after responding with either lever. Extinction sessions continued on subsequent days until a cohort of animals reached the criterion, in which the mean number of responses on the active lever was lower than 10% of presses relative to the last day of the VR5 schedule. The animals typically took four to 6 days to reach this criterion. On the day after the extinction criterion was reached, animals were subjected to the 20-min reinstatement test, where the first CS was presented 5 s after the session started. During the test session, pressing the active lever elicited the presentation of the light/tone CS in the absence of food pellets on a VR5 schedule. The lever pressing number was recorded in every session. In Experiment 1, all the animals underwent the reinstatement test procedure. In Experiment 3, the “Rein” group of animals underwent the reinstatement test procedure, while the “No Rein” group of animals stayed in their home cages. The following two parameters were additionally analyzed in Experiment 3. (a) Nosepoke was defined as whenever the rats placed their nose into the food port. (b) Nosepoke latency was defined as the delay from the start of CS to the first nosepoke. If the rat did not nosepoke after the presence of CS, it was defined as an “omission” and was not included in the analysis of latency.

2.5.2. Locomotor

Rats were placed in an open-field arena (Med associates) for 30 min. The total traveling distance (cm) was measured.

2.5.3. Free-feeding

Rats were placed in a 45 × 21 × 20 cm3 cage box with unlimited food pellets (45 mg sucrose pellets, a total of 140–200 g) for 30 min. The consumed food pellets (g) were calculated.

2.5.4. Elevated plus maze

One day before the test, the rats were placed in the behavioral room in their home cages to habituate to the environment for 2 h. On the test day, each rat was placed in the center of the maze facing one of the closed arms, with video recording for 5 min. (a) Time spent in the open arms as the percentage of time spent in open and closed arms, and (b) entries into the open arms as the percentage of entries into open and closed arms, were calculated.

2.5.5. Forced swim test

The elevated plus maze (EPM) and forced swim test (FST) were conducted on the same days. For FST, the pre-exposure and test were performed in a cylinder (50 cm in height × 20 cm in diameter) filled with water (23–25°C) to 30 cm. After the habituation procedure of EPM, each rat underwent the pre-exposure procedure of FST. The rats were individually placed in the cylinder for 15 min and this procedure was to ensure that they would quickly adapt to immobility on the test day. On the next day, after being tested with EPM, each rat was placed in the cylinder for 5 min for FST with video recording. The time spent in immobility was calculated.

2.6. Histology

In Experiment 1, to verify the cannula placements, the rats were deeply anesthetized with CO2, decapitated, and then their brains were removed. The brains were post-fixed at least 48 h in 8% paraformaldehyde (PFA) in 0.2 M phosphate buffer (PB) and cryoprotected with 25% sucrose in 0.1 M PB until saturated. The brains were sectioned coronally by 35 μm and collected sequentially into six adjacent sets and stored in cryoprotectant at −20°C. One tissue section series (containing sections spaced by 210 μm) from each rat was mounted onto gelatin-coated slides and stained with nissl solution (a combination of neutral red and cresyl violet) for histological verification of drug infusion sites (Figure 1).

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Example of cannula placements in the BLA and the CeA in Experiment 1. The location of injector tips on the nissl-stained brain sections was defined as the drug infusion site. LA, lateral nucleus of the amygdala; BLA, basolateral nucleus of the amygdala; CeA, central nucleus of the amygdala.

In Experiment 3, to capture the maximum expression of c-fos, the rats were sacrificed 90 min after the behavioral test. The “No Rein” group was sacrificed along with the “Rein” group. The rats were deeply anesthetized with CO2 and perfused transcardially with 50 mL of saline followed by 100 mL of 4% PFA in 0.1 M PB. Brains were removed and post-fixed for at least 4 hours and then cryoprotected in 25% sucrose in 0.1 M PB. The brains were sectioned coronally by 35 μm and collected sequentially into six adjacent sets and stored in cryoprotectant at −20°C. One tissue section series (containing sections spaced by 210 μm) from each rat was mounted onto gelatin-coated slides and stained with nissl solution. Another section series was used for later immunohistochemical processing.

2.7. Immunohistochemistry

In Experiment 3, the c-fos and tyrosine hydroxylase (TH) visualization were performed with standard immunohistochemical procedures. Free-floating brain sections were incubated overnight in phosphate buffer saline solution (PBS) with mouse anti-c-fos antibody (Santa Cruz, catalog#: sc-271243, 1:200). On the following day, after washing with buffer, the sections were incubated in biotinylated donkey anti-mouse IgG solution (Jackson Immuno Research, catalog#: 715–065-151, 1:500), and then Vectastatin ABC Elite reagents (Vector Laboratories), followed by a solution of nickel sulfate and diaminobenzidine (DAB) with hydrogen peroxide to produce a blue-black reaction product within the nucleus of c-fos + neurons. The sections were then incubated overnight in buffer solution with rabbit anti-TH antibody (Merck Millipore; Catalog#: 6A2907, 1:5,000). On the following day, after washing with buffer, the sections were incubated in biotinylated donkey anti-rabbit IgG solution (Jackson Immuno Research, catalog#: 711–065-152, 1:500), and then Vectastain ABC Elite reagents, followed by a solution of non-intensified DAB with hydrogen peroxide to produce a brown reaction product within the cytosol of TH+ neurons. The sections were then rinsed in the buffer and mounted onto gelatin-coated slides.

2.8. Image analysis

In Experiment 3, the images were taken with a digital camera on a microscope (Nikon ECLIPSE Ni-U). We manually counted the c-fos + neurons in the amygdala, the NAcc, and the VTA. The c-fos+/TH+ neurons were also counted in the VTA. The sampled amygdala regions were chosen at three levels of the LA/BLA [from bregma: (AP) −2.52, −3.00, and −3.48 mm] and the CeA [from bregma: (AP) −2.04, −2.52, and −3.00 mm] (Figure 2A). Examples of c-fos + neurons in sampled amygdala are presented in Figure 2B. The sampled NAcc regions were chosen at three levels [from bregma: (AP) +2.28, +1.80, and +1.32 mm]. The border of the NAcc was hard to define on brain slices, so the sampled areas were four 200 × 200 μm2 (40,000 μm2) squares surrounding the anterior commissure (Figure 3). The signals of c-fos + neurons in the NAcc are of the same quality as the ones shown in the amygdala (Figure 2B). The sampled VTA regions were chosen at two levels [from bregma: (AP) −5.28 and − 5.76 mm] (Figure 4A). Examples of c-fos+/TH+ neurons in sampled VTA are presented in Figure 4B. The sampled area of the respective subregions in the amygdala and the VTA were calculated with image analysis software (Toupview). The sampled area of the NAcc was the sum of sampled squares (480,000 μm2/side). The normalized values (count/μm2) were timed 106 to yield cell density (count/nm2) for easier comprehension and analysis purposes.

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Example of c-fos + labelings in the amygdala. (A) The subregions of the amygdala were sampled at four anterior–posterior (AP) levels relative to bregma (−2.04, −2.52, −3.00, and −3.48 mm). The lines indicated the sampled area of the LA, the BLA, and the CeA. (B) Example of c-fos + neurons in the amygdala. The magnified images illustrate the rectangle areas shown in panel A. The c-fos + neurons were labeled in blue-black (four neurons were pointed with black arrowheads as examples). LA, lateral nucleus of the amygdala; BLA, basolateral nucleus of the amygdala; CeA, central nucleus of the amygdala.

FIGURE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 3. Example of c-fos + labelings in the NAcc. The NAcc was sampled at three anterior–posterior (AP) levels relative to bregma (+2.28, +1.80, and + 1.32 mm). The squares (200 × 200 μm2) represented the sampled area in each AP level. The c-fos + neurons were labeled in blue-black, as the same in Figure 2B. NAcc, nucleus accumbens core.

FIGURE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 4. Example of c-fos + labelings of dopaminergic neurons in the VTA. (A) The VTA was sampled at two anterior–posterior (AP) levels relative to bregma (−5.28 and − 5.76 mm). (B) Example of labeled neurons in the VTA. The magnified images illustrate the rectangle areas shown in panel A. The c-fos+/TH+ neurons were identified as brown cell bodies with blue-black nuclei (three neurons were pointed with black arrowheads as examples). VTA, ventral tegmental area.

2.9. Statistics

In Experiment 1, for cue-induced reinstatement test, lever pressing numbers from animals infused with saline were submitted to ANOVA with “Cannula” (BLA vs. CeA) as between-subject factor, and “Lever” (Active vs. Inactive lever) and “Session” (Last extinction vs. Reinstatement test) as within-subject factors. After confirming that there were no statistical differences in the “Cannula” main effect and related interactions, the animals were pooled into a combined saline group. The combined saline group was abbreviated as “SAL,” while the BLA and CeA activation groups were abbreviated as “BLA” and “CeA,” respectively. The lever pressing numbers were then submitted to ANOVA with “Group” (SAL vs. BLA vs. CeA) as between-subject factor, and “Lever” (Active vs. Inactive lever) and “Session” (Last extinction vs. Reinstatement test) as within-subject factors. For locomotor and free-feeding, the data from animals infused with saline were analyzed with the Student’s t-test. After confirming that there was no statistical difference between saline infusion in the BLA and the CeA, the animals were pooled into a combined saline group. The data were then submitted to ANOVA with “Group” (SAL vs. BLA vs. CeA) as between-subject factors. In Experiment 2, the data of EPM and FST were analyzed with a student’s t-test. In Experiment 3, for the cue-induced reinstatement test, the lever pressing numbers were submitted to ANOVA with “Group” (Ctrl vs. Stress) as between-subject factor, and “Lever” (Active vs. Inactive lever) and “Session” (Last extinction vs. Reinstatement test) as within-subject factors. The nosepoke counts were submitted to ANOVA with “Group” (Ctrl vs. Stress) as between-subject factor and “Session” (Last extinction vs. Reinstatement test) as within-subject factor. The nosepoke latency was analyzed with a student t-test. For cell counts, the cell density was submitted to ANOVA with “Group” (Ctrl vs. Stress) and “Reinstatement” (No Rein vs. Rein) as between-subject factors, and “Hemisphere” (Left vs. Right) as within-subject factor. Student–Newman–Keuls (S-N-K) post hoc was performed if significant p-values (p < 0.05) were obtained. All statistics were calculated using SPSS (IBM) and presented as mean ± SEM.

3. Results

3.1. Experiment 1: activation of the BLA or the CeA decreased cue-induced motivation toward food rewards in reinstatement test

In this experiment, after recovering from surgery, the rats were food-deprived until the end of all behavioral tests. The rats were first tested with locomotor activity, followed by a cue-induced reinstatement test, and free-feeding test (Figure 5A). A total of 42 rats were used in this experiment. Nine subjects were excluded due to cannula misplacements, resulting in correct injector tip placements in the BLA (n = 17) or in the CeA (n = 16) for final analyses. The drug injection sites of subjects included for final analyses are summarized in Figure 5B. The counterbalance of drug administration is summarized in Table 1.

FIGURE 5
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 5. Activation of the BLA or the CeA decreased cue-induced motivation toward food rewards in the reinstatement test. (A) The experimental timeline in Experiment 1. The arrows indicate the time of drug administration. (B) The histology summary in Experiment 1. The numbers indicated anterior–posterior (AP) levels relative to bregma. (C) All groups of animals behaved equivalently on the last VR5 training day. Compared to the control group, activation of the BLA or the CeA decreased the reinstatement effect. (D) The locomotor activity and free-feeding test did not show differences among the three groups. All statistics were presented as mean ± SEM. BLA, basolateral nucleus of the amygdala; CeA, central nucleus of the amygdala; VR5, variable-ratio 5; EXT, extinction; Rein, Reinstatement test.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Counterbalance of drug administration of Experiment 1.

For the animals infused with saline in cue-induced reinstatement test (Figure 5C), there were no statistical differences in the main effect of “Cannula” [F(1,9) = 1.29, p = 0.28], and the related two-way or three-way interactions [all three F(1, 9)s < 1]. Therefore, the animals were pooled into the SAL group. On the last VR5 training day, there was a significant main effect of “Lever” [F(1, 29) = 111.17, p < 0.001], but no statistical differences in the main effect of “Group” [F(2, 29) < 1] or interaction between “Lever” and “Group” [F(2, 29) < 1]. The results showed that the three groups of animals responded equivalently on the last VR5 training day and learned that responding on the active lever led to food rewards. From the last EXT training day to reinstatement test, there was a significant main effect of “Lever” [F(1, 29) = 47.17, p < 0.001], significant two-way interactions between “Lever” and “Session” [F(1, 29) = 7.95, p = 0.009] and between “Session” and “Group” [F(2, 29) = 3.75, p = 0.036], and significant three-way interaction among “Lever,” “Session,” and “Group” [F(2,29) = 3.38, p = 0.048]. The results indicated that the rats responded more to the active lever than to the inactive lever. Post hoc analyses revealed that only the SAL group demonstrated cue-induced reinstatement during the test, these rats showed an increase in the number of lever-pressing compared to the last EXT training session (active lever; p = 0.013). Together, activation of the BLA or the CeA demolished the cue-induced food-seeking behavior in the reinstatement test.

To rule out the possibility that activation of the amygdala may interfere with motor activity or lead to anhedonia in the animals, we tested locomotor activity and free-feeding test before reinforcement training and after the cue-induced reinstatement test, respectively (Figure 5D). For the animals infused with saline, there was no statistical difference between the animals with cannula implanted in the BLA or the CeA in locomotor activity [t(11) = 0.27, p = 0.79] or free-feeding test [t(8) = 0.47, p = 0.65]. Therefore, the animals were pooled into respective SAL groups. For locomotor activity, there was no statistical difference in the traveling distance among the three groups [F(2, 30) < 1]. For the free-feeding test, there was no statistical difference in consumed food pellets (g) among the three groups [F(2, 30) = 2.28, p = 0.12]. Together, activation of the BLA or the CeA did not change the locomotor activity of the animals or free food intake to the animals. Finally, since the animals received drug infusion three times, we checked the nissl-stained sections to rule out excitotoxic lesions. The implantation caused physical tissue damage surrounding the cannula, but the adjacent neurons remained unaffected after pharmacological activation. There were no obvious signs of excitotoxic lesions, including loss of neurons or tissue, proliferation of microglial cells, or pyknotic nuclei (Hembrook and Mair, 2011). The neurons in the CeA are smaller because this nucleus mostly consists of gamma-aminobutyric acid (GABA) neurons, which are smaller than pyramidal neurons (McDonald, 1982a,b; McDonald and Mascagni, 2001). The exemplary nissl-stained sections and magnified images of subjects infused with NMDA or saline for all three behavioral tests are presented in Figure 6.

FIGURE 6
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 6. Example of neuronal morphology after three times of saline or NMDA infusion. For every subject, the brain sections were present with cannula implant location and 0.35 mm anterior or posterior relative to implant location. The magnified images illustrate the square areas shown in low-magnification images. Scale bar, 500 μm in low magnification images, 50 μm in high magnification images. BLA, basolateral nucleus of the amygdala; CeA, central nucleus of the amygdala; S, Saline, N, N-methyl-D-aspartate.

3.2. Experiment 2: single prolonged footshocks-induced acute stress did not affect anxiety-and despair-like behavior

In this experiment, the rats were tested with EPM and FST after a single prolonged footshock-induced stress session (Figure 7A). A total of 16 rats were used in this experiment, with groups assigned as “Ctrl” (n = 8) and “Stress” (n = 8). For EPM, there was no statistical difference in the percentage of time spent in the open arm [t(14) = 0.26, p = 0.80] or the percentage of entries into the open arm [t(14) = 0.59, p = 0.57] (Figure 7B). For FST, there was no statistical difference in the time spent in immobility [t(14) = 0.62, p = 0.55] (Figure 7C). The results suggested that single prolonged footshock-induced stress did not interfere with the anxiety-and despair-like behaviors of the animals.

FIGURE 7
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 7. Footshock-induced acute stress did not affect anxiety-and despair-like behaviors. (A) The experimental timeline in Experiment 2. The arrows indicate the time of a single prolonged footshock-induced acute stress. There was no statistical difference between groups in (B) percentage of time spent in open arms and entries into open arms in EPM and (C) time spent in immobility in FST. All statistics were presented as mean ± SEM. EPM, elevated plus maze; FST, forced swim test.

3.3. Experiment 3: single prolonged footshocks-induced acute stress did not affect cue-induced motivation toward food rewards in the reinstatement test

In this experiment, the rats underwent reinforcement training and extinction, and were food-deprived until the end of all behavioral tests (Figure 8A). Immediately before the cue-induced reinstatement test, the rats underwent a single prolonged footshock-induced stress session. A total of 32 rats were used in this experiment, with group assignment as follows: “Ctrl-Rein” (n = 10), “Stress-Rein” (n = 10), “Ctrl-No Rein” (n = 6), and “Stress-No Rein” (n = 6). For the single prolonged footshocks session (“Stress,” n = 16), the video recordings of three animals were lost due to a program crash, and therefore the freezing level was analyzed based on the remaining 13 subjects. For these rats that received footshocks (Figure 8B), they showed a rapid increase in freezing level during the first 10 min, which remained at a high level toward the end of the 2 h session. The “No Rein” animals (“Ctrl-No Rein” and “Stress-No Rein”) did not undergo the reinstatement test after the stress session. These two groups served as the controls to examine how single prolonged footshock-induced stress may have affected cellular activities.

FIGURE 8
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 8. Footshock-induced acute stress did not interfere with cue-induced motivation toward food rewards in the reinstatement test. (A) The experimental timeline in Experiment 3. The arrow indicates the time of a single prolonged footshock-induced acute stress. (B) The freezing level during the first 10 min and the last 10 min of the total 2 h session. (C) The two groups of animals that underwent the reinstatement procedure behaved equivalently on the last VR5 training day. The footshock-induced acute stress did not decrease the motivation of food-seeking. Both the Stress and Ctrl group of the animals showed reinstatement to cues. (D) The footshock-induced acute stress did not decrease nosepoke counts from the last extinction session to the reinstatement test in both groups. All statistics were presented as mean ± SEM. VR5, variable-ratio 5; EXT, extinction; Rein, Reinstatement test.

For the two groups of animals (“Ctrl-Rein” and “Stress-Rein”) that underwent the reinstatement test (Figure 8C), on the last VR5 training day, there was a significant main effect of “Lever” [F(1, 18) = 108.39, p < 0.001], but no statistical differences in the main effect of “Group” [F(1, 18) < 1] or interaction between “Lever” and “Group” [F(1, 18) < 1]. The results showed that these animals responded equivalently on the last VR5 training day and learned that responding on the active lever led to food rewards. From the last EXT training day to the reinstatement test, there were significant main effects of “Lever” [F(1, 18) = 76.73, p < 0.001] and “Session” [F(1, 18) = 30.19, p < 0.001], and a significant two-way interaction between “Lever” and “Session” [F(1, 18) = 41.71, p < 0.001]. However, there were only a marginal effect of two-way interaction between “Session” and “Group” [F(1, 18) = 3.28, p = 0.087] and a marginal three-way interaction among “Lever,” “Session,” and “Group” [F(1, 18) = 3.60, p = 0.074]. Because of the marginal effects, we further analyzed nosepoke as another index of motivation (Figure 8D). From the last EXT training day to the reinstatement test, there was a significant main effect of “Session” [F(1, 18) = 88.80, p < 0.001], and a significant two-way interaction between “Session” and “Group” [F(1, 18) = 4.90, p = 0.04]. However, post hoc analyses revealed that both “Ctrl” and “Stress” groups showed an increase in nosepoke counts compared to their respective last EXT training session (ps < 0.001) and there was only a marginal difference between the two groups on the reinstatement test day (p = 0.09). The omission rates were very low and there was no difference in nosepoke latency between the two groups [t(18) = 1.20, p = 0.25] with 2.81 ± 0.35 s in the “Ctrl” group and 3.75 ± 0.71 s in the “Stress” group. These results suggested that both groups showed a reinstatement effect and footshocks-induced acute stress did not decrease the motivation in cue-induced food-seeking behavior.

To analyze the cellular activities during the reinstatement test, we sampled the c-fos + neurons in the amygdala, the NAcc, and the VTA. In the LA (Figure 9A, upper panels), there were only a marginal main effect of “Group” [F(1, 28) = 3.05, p = 0.092] and a marginal two-way interaction between “Hemisphere” and “Reinstatement” [F(1, 28) = 3.11, p = 0.089]. In the BLA (Figure 9A, middle panels), there was only a marginal three-way interaction among “Hemisphere,” “Group,” and “Reinstatement” [F(1, 28) = 3.41, p = 0.076]. In the CeA (Figure 9A, lower panels), there was a significant three-way interaction among “Hemisphere,” “Group” and “Reinstatement” [F(1,28) = 4.47, p = 0.043]. However, post hoc analyses revealed that there was no statistical difference between the “Ctrl” and “Stress” groups in either hemisphere regardless the animals underwent reinstatement or not. Together, we did not find evidence that acute stress and/or reinstatement tests increased the cellular activities in the amygdala.

FIGURE 9
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 9. The cell density in (count/nm2) in (A) the amygdala (the LA, the BLA, and the CeA) and (B) the NAcc. All statistics were presented as mean ± SEM. LA, lateral nucleus of the amygdala; BLA, basolateral nucleus of the amygdala; CeA, central nucleus of the amygdala; NAcc, nucleus accumbens core.

In the NAcc (Figure 9B), the cellular activities did not show any statistical difference among the groups. In the VTA, the cell densities of c-fos + or c-fos+/TH+ neurons were very low. Before normalizing to the sampled VTA area, the mean c-fos + cell count is 15.03 ± 3.79 (range 0–100) in the left VTA and 18.09 ± 4.62 (range 0–125) in the right VTA. The mean c-fos+/TH+ cell count is 5.38 ± 1.24 (range 0–30) in the left VTA and 6.69 ± 1.50 (range 0–32) in the right VTA. Therefore, we did not further analyze the data considering the results were likely biased. The minimum, maximum, and mean cell density in respective brain regions were summarized in Table 2.

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Cell density (count/nm2) of c-fos + or c-fos+/TH+ neurons in the amygdala, the NAcc, and the VTA.

4. Discussion

In this study, we aimed to investigate how amygdala activation or physical acute stress sessions would affect cue-induced motivation in rats. The behavioral results showed that pharmacological activation of the BLA or the CeA decreased cue-induced motivation toward foods. However, single prolonged footshock-induced stress did not immediately affect anxiety-, despair-like behavior, or motivation toward food. The cellular activities assessed in the amygdala subnuclei or the NAcc also showed no difference among hemispheres and groups after acute stress and/or reinstatement test. Together, only intra-cranial pharmacological activation of the amygdala led to the decrease in appetitive motivated behavior.

In Experiment 1, pharmacological activation of the BLA or the CeA decreased the cue-induced motivation of the animals. This result further supported the idea that down-regulation of the DA population activity following BLA activation would suppress the reward system (Belujon and Grace, 2015, 2017). Moreover, activation of either the BLA or the CeA led to similar behavioral outcomes, which was consistent with the concept of serial information flow from the BLA to the CeA that guided many motivated behaviors (Beyeler et al., 2016; Kim et al., 2017). However, we could not rule out the possibility that the BLA and the CeA were under the regulation of a common upstream brain region. Indeed, some previous research has suggested that the BLA and the CeA mediated emotional processing parallelly (Balleine and Killcross, 2006; Moscarello and Ledoux, 2013). Since the BLA and the CeA were adjacent to each other, the drug was delivered at the rate of 0.2 μL/min for 2 min 30 s, which was slower than the procedure of previous studies (0.5 μL/min for 1 min) (Rezayof et al., 2007; Solati and Hajikhani, 2010). However, we cannot rule out the possibility that the drug spreads beyond the targeted region. A better solution is to use chemogenetics in future studies so that the precise expression of the receptors can be confirmed.

The amygdala is not only involved in the regulation of negative emotions but also positive emotions, such as reward processing (Baxter and Murray, 2002; Murray, 2007). The amygdala activity is important during the encoding and retrieval phase of stimulus-reinforcement association (Schoenbaum et al., 1998, 1999; Malvaez et al., 2019; Crouse et al., 2020). In the study of McLaughlin and Floresco, inhibition of caudal BLA enhanced reinstatement in food-seeking (McLaughlin and Floresco, 2007). Our results are in line with this report and showed that activation of the BLA or the CeA abolished cue-induced food-seeking after extinction. Beyond the natural reward, such as sucrose pellets or solution, most of the studies regarding the role of the amygdala in reinstatement focused on drug-seeking (Sharp, 2017). For example, lesion or inhibition of the amygdala attenuated reinstatement of drug-seeking (Kantak et al., 2002; McLaughlin and See, 2003; See et al., 2003; Fuchs et al., 2004a). Inhibition of the BLA to NAcc pathway abolished the reinstatement of drug-seeking as well (Stefanik and Kalivas, 2013). Based on these contradictory results, the underlying mechanisms of reinstatement of drug-and food-seeking are likely different and need further investigation.

In Experiment 2, single prolonged footshock-induced acute stress did not increase the anxiety level in EPM or the despair level in FST. In Experiment 3, we further analyzed the freezing behavior during the acute stress session and confirmed that the animals did show a high level of fear toward the end of the session. However, in our cue-induced reinstatement, there was only a marginal effect of a three-way interaction among “Lever,” “Session,” and “Group” (p = 0.074) in the lever pressing number. Although there was a significant two-way interaction between “Session” and “Group” in nosepoke counts, the count difference between the “Ctrl” and “Stress” groups on the reinstatement test day was only marginal (p = 0.09), suggesting that the animals only showed a trend of decrease in the motivation of food-seeking after acute stress. Considering that the acute stress did not increase the negative emotions at the time point of our assessment, it is possible that the stress effect was weak and therefore resulted in a marginal effect on the reinstatement test. It has been suggested that stress is a risk factor to induce reinstatement of drug-seeking (Shaham et al., 2003; Shalev et al., 2010; Mantsch et al., 2015). For stress-induced reinstatement in food-seeking, the findings remain inconclusive. For example, studies from Shaham’s group demonstrated that yohimbine, which produces a stress-like response in animals, induced reinstatement in food-seeking (Ghitza et al., 2005; Nair et al., 2010; Lê et al., 2011). However, intermittent footshock-induced stress promoted (Chen et al., 2014) or had no effect (Ahmed and Koob, 1997; Buczek et al., 1999) on reinstatement in food-seeking, whereas a recent study revealed that acute footshock-induced stress decreased appetitive learning and motivated behavior (Derman and Matthew Lattal, 2023). In human studies, after acute stress such as threat-of-shock or cold condition (Bogdan and Pizzagalli, 2006; Morris and Rottenberg, 2015; Burani et al., 2021), or in post-traumatic stress disorder (PTSD) patients (Hopper et al., 2008; Nawijn et al., 2015), these subjects displayed dysfunction of reward processing.

There are some operational factors that have led to the limitations of this study. First, the food deprivation process itself increased plasma corticosterone (CORT) levels (Guarnieri et al., 2012; Lenglos et al., 2013) or induced behavioral change, such as an anxiolytic effect in EPM of the animals (Genn et al., 2003; Dietze et al., 2016). However, food restriction is an inevitable step in cue-induced food-seeking tasks. Second, the control groups (“Ctrl,” non-stressed controls and “No Rein,” no reinstatement controls) were left in their home cages while their counterparts underwent corresponding behavioral procedures. In our experimental design, the single prolonged footshock-induced stress served as a distinct experience. This design paralleled other stress protocols using restraint (Kovács et al., 2018; Sousa et al., 2018) or chronic unpredictable mild stress (Nollet et al., 2013; Wiborg, 2013; Chang and Grace, 2014) that the control animals usually were left in their home cages, undisturbed. However, we acknowledged that placing the “Ctrl” animals into fear-conditioning cubicles without footshocks could have ruled out the environmental variables. For the “NoRein” controls, these animals remained in their home cages to examine the cellular activities due to stress per se. Similarly, we acknowledged that having another control group placed in the operant chamber without the presentation of food-related CS could have ruled out environmental variables, and also their lever pressing numbers could have served as a baseline for behavioral comparison.

The single prolonged stress session in this study was adapted from Arakawa’s study (Arakawa et al., 2014). In their study, female Sprague–Dawley rats were used as their subjects, and they demonstrated that the plasma CORT level was significantly increased immediately after footshocks. It has been suggested that females have higher activities of the HPA axis after acute stress and exhibit distinct fear responses from male subjects (Goel et al., 2014; Bangasser and Wicks, 2017; Heck and Handa, 2018). However, as only male Lone-Evans rats were used as our subjects and we did not measure the CORT level of our “Ctrl” and “Stress” animals, there was no direct physiological evidence of stress measurement. Next, the type of stressor is also likely to influence stress response. In studies using footshock as the stressor, the intensity of footshock ranged from 0.5–2.0 mA, and the duration of session time lasted from under 30 min (Haj-Mirzaian et al., 2014; Ness et al., 2022), to 1 h (Shinba et al., 2010), or up to 2 h (Blandino et al., 2006). Therefore, it is hard to directly compare the intensity of stress among all these protocols. To study the animal model of PTSD, the single prolonged stress procedure is usually a combination of several stressors, such as footshock, forced swim, and anesthetization (Knox et al., 2012; Eagle et al., 2013; Souza et al., 2017; Lisieski et al., 2018). It is possible that using only the footshock as in our stress protocol was not intense enough to alter behaviors of the animals. Finally, we noticed that the intermittent footshock of earlier studies was performed in the same operant chamber as the reinstatement test, while in our experiments, the footshocks were performed in other chambers with a different context setting. It is likely that the shift in context between the acute stress session and the reinstatement test was regarded as a safety signal in some of the animals, and therefore, these animals displayed reinstatement behaviors. Indeed, earlier studies have suggested that the context and mental state during training contributed to the renewal phenomenon in operant behaviors (Bouton et al., 2012; Sweeney and Shahan, 2015). Moreover, one study showed that the food-seeking behavior was reinstated after stress only when the stress was also introduced during reinforcement training sessions (Schepers and Bouton, 2019), implying that the reinstatement might be state-dependent.

Since previous studies showed that the amygdala was immediately activated under acute stress (Cullinan et al., 1995; Reznikov et al., 2007, 2008) and also to parallel the experimental design of Experiment 1, we conducted the behavioral tests and sampled the cellular activities with c-fos expression immediately after the acute stress in Experiment 3. Similar to the behavioral results, there were only marginal effects of the cellular activities. In the LA, the “Stress” animals showed a trend of higher cellular activities than “Ctrl” animals. At the level of the BLA and the CeA, the “Stress” animals only showed a trend of higher cellular activities in the right hemisphere when they underwent reinstatement tests. In general, the “Stress” animals tended to have higher cellular activities in the amygdala compared to “Ctrl” ones if they underwent reinstatement tests. One possibility is that the acute stress procedure prepared the animals to be alert, which is similar to the “pre-encounter defensive behavior” as stated in the predatory imminence theory. Under this scenario, the “Stress” animals showed lower motivated behavior during the reinstatement test, accompanied by higher cellular activities in the amygdala. However, earlier studies assessed behavioral performance after acute stress with different temporal delays, from immediately (Masood et al., 2004; Olonode et al., 2018), 1 day (Mitra et al., 2005; Suvrathan et al., 2010), or even 10 days (Mitra et al., 2005; Kedia and Chattarji, 2014) afterward. This temporal delay after the stress procedure could be another factor leading to our marginal effects at both the behavioral and cellular levels.

Other than the amygdala, we also assessed the cellular activities of the reward system, including the VTA and the NAcc. However, the cell densities of c-fos + DA and non-DA neurons in the VTA were very low, and the cellular activities in the NAcc did not show statistical differences among groups. Studies suggested that the DA mesolimbic transmission initially increased after acute stress, but subsequently decreased (Puglisi-Allegra et al., 1991; Valenti et al., 2011; Chang and Grace, 2013). Moreover, our previous research showed that BLA activation decreased VTA population activity (Chang and Grace, 2014; Lai and Chang, 2019). Therefore, the alteration of activities in the VTA may depend on several variables. The VTA DA system is important in reward processing, especially stimulus–reward learning and the motivation to obtain reward (Arias-Carrián et al., 2010; Ranaldi, 2014; Salamone et al., 2016). Most of the research focused on drug-seeking behavior, for example, blocking glutamate receptors (McFarland and Kalivas, 2001; Sun et al., 2005; Mahler et al., 2013) or orexin receptors (James et al., 2011; Mahler et al., 2013) in the VTA reduced reinstatement of cocaine-seeking in rats. It has been reported that blocking glutamate receptors in the VTA did not affect reinstatement of food-seeking (Sun et al., 2005), although “food-induced” reinstatement was used in this study. In the VTA, a large proportion of the neurons are DA neurons, with the rest non-DA neurons mostly consisting of GABAergic neurons (Nair-Roberts et al., 2008). The VTA DA neurons project to the medial prefrontal cortex (mPFC), the amygdala, the lateral habenula (LHb), and robustly to the NAc (Yetnikoff et al., 2014; Beier et al., 2015; Morales and Margolis, 2017). The VTA GABAergic neurons exert local inhibition on DA neurons or other VTA GABAergic neurons, as well as long-range inhibition to other brain regions, such as the mPFC, the LHb, and the NAc (Creed et al., 2014; Yetnikoff et al., 2014; Morales and Margolis, 2017; Bouarab et al., 2019). The DA and non-DA long-range projections modulated reward processing and aversive behaviors (Morales and Margolis, 2017; Bouarab et al., 2019). However, previous studies only suggested the importance of the VTA DA neuron in the reinstatement of cocaine-seeking (Tian et al., 2022). Thus, the cell-type specific contribution of the VTA neurons on the reinstatement of food-seeking remains unknown. In the NAc, earlier studies have demonstrated that DA transmission is important for reinforcement-related behaviors, especially in initiating and maintaining instrumental behaviors (Salamone and Correa, 2002; Salamone et al., 2003, 2005). For example, inactivation of the NAcc with GABA agonist reduced food-seeking (Floresco et al., 2008; Lin and Pratt, 2014) and cocaine-seeking (Fuchs et al., 2004b) behavior during the reinstatement test. In our results, footshock-induced stress only resulted in a trend of decrease in cue-induced motivation of the animals. The fact that the stressed animals did show an effect of reinstatement might have explained the lack of difference in cellular activities in the NAcc assessed through c-fos expression.

Our results showed that pharmacological activation of the amygdala during the reinstatement test significantly suppressed the cue-induced motivation of the animals (Experiment 1). The single prolonged footshock-induced acute stress did not affect anxiety-, despair-like behaviors (Experiment 2), or motivation of food-seeking (Experiment 3). Therefore, activation of the amygdala per se did not fully capture the state of the animals being under acute stress. Indeed, multiple systems may be engaged under acute stress, including the HPA axis (Johnson et al., 1992; Kudielka and Kirschbaum, 2004) and the sympathetic division of ANS (McCorry, 2007). Some animal studies suggested that glucocorticoids released under stress enhanced response to rewards (Piazza and Le Moal, 1997; Marinelli and Piazza, 2002; Lemaire et al., 2005), while in human subjects with heightened cortisol levels after acute stress led to deficits in reward processing (Berghorst et al., 2013) and administration of cortisol to human subjects decreased the neuronal activities relative to reward (Montoya et al., 2014; Kinner et al., 2016). The glucocorticoid and noradrenergic activities combined also reduced goal-directed actions in humans (Schwabe et al., 2012). Together, our results further supported the idea that stress response is a complicated coping process that engages many systems beyond the ones mediated through the central nervous systems via the amygdala. The mechanism of how stress affects motivation demands future research.

Data availability statement

The original contributions presented in the study are included in the article/supplementary material, further inquiries can be directed to the corresponding author.

Ethics statement

The animal study was approved by the Institutional Animal Care and Use Committees (IACUC) of both National Tsing Hua University and National Yang Ming Chiao Tung University. The study was conducted in accordance with the local legislation and institutional requirements.

Author contributions

C-WL: conceptualization, methodology, formal analysis, investigation, writing-original draft, visualization, project administration. C-hC: conceptualization, methodology, validation, and resources, writing-review and editing, visualization, supervision, and funding acquisition. All authors contributed to the article and approved the submitted version.

Funding

This work was supported by the National Science and Technology Council (Taiwan) (Grant no. 111-2628-B-007-008-) to C-hC and the Brain Research Center under the Higher Education Sprout Project, co-funded by the Ministry of Education and the National Science and Technology Council (Taiwan).

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher’s note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

References

Aberman, J. E., and Salamone, J. D. (1999). Nucleus accumbens dopamine depletions make rats more sensitive to high ratio requirements but do not impair primary food reinforcement. Neuroscience 92, 545–552. doi: 10.1016/S0306-4522(99)00004-4

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ahmed, S. H., and Koob, G. F. (1997). Cocaine-but not food-seeking behavior is reinstated by stress after extinction. Psychopharmacology 132, 289–295. doi: 10.1007/S002130050347

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ahmed, S. H., Walker, J. R., and Koob, G. F. (2000). Persistent increase in the motivation to take heroin in rats with a history of drug escalation. Neuropsychopharmacology 4, 413–421. doi: 10.1016/s0893-133x(99)00133-5

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Arakawa, K., Arakawa, H., Hueston, C. M., and Deak, T. (2014). Effects of the estrous cycle and ovarian hormones on central expression of interleukin-1 evoked by stress in female rats. Neuroendocrinology 100, 162–177. doi: 10.1159/000368606

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Arias-Carrián, O., Stamelou, M., Murillo-Rodríguez, E., Menéndez-Gonzlez, M., and Pöppel, E. (2010). Dopaminergic reward system: a short integrative review. Int. Arch. Med. 3, 24–26. doi: 10.1186/1755-7682-3-24

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Balleine, B. W., and Killcross, S. (2006). Parallel incentive processing: an integrated view of amygdala function. Trends Neurosci. 29, 272–279. doi: 10.1016/J.TINS.2006.03.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bangasser, D. A., and Wicks, B. (2017). Sex-specific mechanisms for responding to stress. J. Neurosci. Res. 95, 75–82. doi: 10.1002/JNR.23812

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Baxter, M. G., and Murray, E. A. (2002). The amygdala and reward. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 3, 563–573. doi: 10.1038/nrn875

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Beier, K. T., Steinberg, E. E., Deloach, K. E., Xie, S., Miyamichi, K., Schwarz, L., et al. (2015). Circuit architecture of VTA dopamine neurons revealed by systematic input-output mapping. Cells 162, 622–634. doi: 10.1016/J.CELL.2015.07.015

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Belujon, P., and Grace, A. A. (2015). Regulation of dopamine system responsivity and its adaptive and pathological response to stress. Proc. R. Soc. B Biol. Sci. 282:20142516. doi: 10.1098/RSPB.2014.2516

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Belujon, P., and Grace, A. A. (2017). Dopamine system dysregulation in major depressive disorders. Int. J. Neuropsychopharmacol. 20, 1036–1046. doi: 10.1093/IJNP/PYX056

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Belujon, P., Jakobowski, N. L., Dollish, H. K., and Grace, A. A. (2015). Withdrawal from acute amphetamine induces an amygdala-driven attenuation of dopamine neuron activity: reversal by ketamine. Neuropsychopharmacology 41, 619–627. doi: 10.1038/npp.2015.191

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Berghorst, L. H., Bogdan, R., Frank, M. J., and Pizzagalli, D. A. (2013). Acute stress selectively reduces reward sensitivity. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 133. doi: 10.3389/FNHUM.2013.00133

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Beyeler, A., Namburi, P., Glober, G. F., Simonnet, C., Calhoon, G. G., Conyers, G. F., et al. (2016). Divergent routing of positive and negative information from the amygdala during memory retrieval. Neuron 90, 348–361. doi: 10.1016/J.NEURON.2016.03.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Blandino, P., Barnum, C. J., and Deak, T. (2006). The involvement of norepinephrine and microglia in hypothalamic and splenic IL-1β responses to stress. J. Neuroimmunol. 173, 87–95. doi: 10.1016/J.JNEUROIM.2005.11.021

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bogdan, R., and Pizzagalli, D. A. (2006). Acute stress reduces reward responsiveness: implications for depression. Biol. Psychiatry 60, 1147–1154. doi: 10.1016/J.BIOPSYCH.2006.03.037

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bouarab, C., Thompson, B., and Polter, A. M. (2019). VTA GABA neurons at the Interface of stress and reward. Front Neural Circuits 13:78. doi: 10.3389/FNCIR.2019.00078

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bouton, M. E., Winterbauer, N. E., and Todd, T. P. (2012). Relapse processes after the extinction of instrumental learning: renewal, resurgence, and reacquisition. Behav. Process. 90, 130–141. doi: 10.1016/J.BEPROC.2012.03.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Buczek, Y., Lê, A. D., Wang, A., Stewart, J., and Shaham, Y. (1999). Stress reinstates nicotine seeking but not sucrose solution seeking in rats. Psychopharmacology 144, 183–188. doi: 10.1007/S002130050992

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Burani, K., Gallyer, A., Ryan, J., Jordan, C., Joiner, T., and Hajcak, G. (2021). Acute stress reduces reward-related neural activity: evidence from the reward positivity. Stress 24, 833–839. doi: 10.1080/10253890.2021.1929164

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chang, C. H., and Grace, A. A. (2013). Amygdala beta-noradrenergic receptors modulate delayed downregulation of dopamine activity following restraint. J. Neurosci. 33, 1441–1450. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2420-12.2013

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chang, C. H., and Grace, A. A. (2014). Amygdala-ventral pallidum pathway decreases dopamine activity after chronic mild stress in rats. Biol. Psychiatry 76, 223–230. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2013.09.020

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chen, Y. W., Fiscella, K. A., Bacharach, S. Z., and Calu, D. J. (2014). Effect of cafeteria diet history on Cue-, pellet-priming-, and stress-induced reinstatement of food seeking in female rats. PLoS One 9:e102213. doi: 10.1371/JOURNAL.PONE.0102213

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Creed, M. C., Ntamati, N. R., and Tan, K. R. (2014). VTA GABA neurons modulate specific learning behaviors through the control of dopamine and cholinergic systems. Front. Behav. Neurosci. 8:8. doi: 10.3389/fnbeh.2014.00008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Crouse, R. B., Kim, K., Batchelor, H. M., Girardi, E. M., Kamaletdinova, R., Chan, J., et al. (2020). Acetylcholine is released in the basolateral amygdala in response to predictors of reward and enhances the learning of cue-reward contingency. elife 9, 1–31. doi: 10.7554/ELIFE.57335

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cullinan, W. E., Herman, J. P., Battaglia, D. F., Akil, H., and Watson, S. J. (1995). Pattern and time course of immediate early gene expression in rat brain following acute stress. Neuroscience 64, 477–505. doi: 10.1016/0306-4522(94)00355-9

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Davis, M., Rainnie, D., and Cassell, M. (1994). Neurotransmission in the rat amygdala related to fear and anxiety. Trends Neurosci. 17, 208–214. doi: 10.1016/0166-2236(94)90106-6

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dayas, C. V., Buller, K. M., Crane, J. W., Xu, Y., and Day, T. A. (2001). Stressor categorization: acute physical and psychological stressors elicit distinctive recruitment patterns in the amygdala and in medullary noradrenergic cell groups. Eur. J. Neurosci. 14, 1143–1152. doi: 10.1046/j.0953-816x.2001.01733.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

De Jong, J. W., Roelofs, T. J. M., Mol, F. M. U., Hillen, A. E. J., Meijboom, K. E., Luijendijk, M. C. M., et al. (2015). Reducing ventral tegmental dopamine D2 receptor expression selectively boosts incentive motivation. Neuropsychopharmacology 40, 2085–2095. doi: 10.1038/npp.2015.60

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dedovic, K., Duchesne, A., Andrews, J., Engert, V., and Pruessner, J. C. (2009). The brain and the stress axis: the neural correlates of cortisol regulation in response to stress. Neuroimage 47, 864–871. doi: 10.1016/J.NEUROIMAGE.2009.05.074

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Derman, R. C., and Matthew Lattal, K. (2023). Acute stress persistently alters instrumental motivation without affecting appetitive Pavlovian conditioning, extinction, or contextual renewal. Neurobiol. Learn. Mem. 202:107771. doi: 10.1016/J.NLM.2023.107771

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dietze, S., Lees, K. R., Fink, H., Brosda, J., and Voigt, J. P. (2016). Food deprivation, body weight loss and anxiety-related behavior in rats. Animals 6, 6–4. doi: 10.3390/ANI6010004

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Duval, E. R., Javanbakht, A., and Liberzon, I. (2015). Neural circuits in anxiety and stress disorders: a focused review. Ther. Clin. Risk Manag. 11, 115–126. doi: 10.2147/TCRM.S48528

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Eagle, A. L., Fitzpatrick, C. J., and Perrine, S. A. (2013). Single prolonged stress impairs social and object novelty recognition in rats. Behav. Brain Res. 256, 591–597. doi: 10.1016/J.BBR.2013.09.014

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fanselow, M. S., and Lester, L. S. (1988). “A functional behavioristic approach to aversively motivated behavior: predatory imminence as a determinant of the topography of defensive behavior” in Evolution and learning. eds. R. C. Bolles and M. D. Beecher (Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc.), 185–212.

Google Scholar

Floresco, S. B., McLaughlin, R. J., and Haluk, D. M. (2008). Opposing roles for the nucleus accumbens core and shell in cue-induced reinstatement of food-seeking behavior. Neuroscience 154, 877–884. doi: 10.1016/J.NEUROSCIENCE.2008.04.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Floresco, S. B., Todd, C. L., and Grace, A. A. (2001). Glutamatergic afferents from the hippocampus to the nucleus accumbens regulate activity of ventral tegmental area dopamine neurons. J. Neurosci. 21, 4915–4922. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.21-13-04915.2001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fuchs, R. A., Evans, K. A., Ledford, C. C., Parker, M. P., Case, J. M., Mehta, R. H., et al. (2004a). The Role of the dorsomedial prefrontal cortex, basolateral amygdala, and dorsal Hippocampus in contextual reinstatement of cocaine seeking in rats. Neuropsychopharmacology 30, 296–309. doi: 10.1038/sj.npp.1300579

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fuchs, R. A., Evans, K. A., Parker, M. C., and See, R. E. (2004b). Differential involvement of the core and shell subregions of the nucleus accumbens in conditioned cue-induced reinstatement of cocaine seeking in rats. Psychopharmacology 176, 459–465. doi: 10.1007/S00213-004-1895-6

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Genn, R. F., Tucci, S. A., Thomas, A., Edwards, J. E., and File, S. E. (2003). Age-associated sex differences in response to food deprivation in two animal tests of anxiety. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 27, 155–161. doi: 10.1016/S0149-7634(03)00017-4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ghitza, U. E., Gray, S. M., Epstein, D. H., Rice, K. C., and Shaham, Y. (2005). The Anxiogenic drug Yohimbine reinstates palatable food seeking in a rat relapse model: a role of CRF1 receptors. Neuropsychopharmacology 31, 2188–2196. doi: 10.1038/sj.npp.1300964

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Goel, N., Workman, J. L., Lee, T. T., Innala, L., and Viau, V. (2014). Sex differences in the HPA axis. Compr. Physiol. 4, 1121–1155. doi: 10.1002/CPHY.C130054

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Grace, A. A., Floresco, S. B., Goto, Y., and Lodge, D. J. (2007). Regulation of firing of dopaminergic neurons and control of goal-directed behaviors. Trends Neurosci. 30, 220–227. doi: 10.1016/j.tins.2007.03.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Guarnieri, D. J., Brayton, C. E., Richards, S. M., Maldonado-Aviles, J., Trinko, J. R., Nelson, J., et al. (2012). Gene profiling reveals a role for stress hormones in the molecular and behavioral response to food restriction. Biol. Psychiatry 71, 358–365. doi: 10.1016/J.BIOPSYCH.2011.06.028

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Haj-Mirzaian, A., Ostadhadi, S., Kordjazy, N., Dehpour, A. R., and Ejtemaei Mehr, S. (2014). Opioid/NMDA receptors blockade reverses the depressant-like behavior of foot shock stress in the mouse forced swimming test. Eur. J. Pharmacol. 735, 26–31. doi: 10.1016/J.EJPHAR.2014.03.053

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Heck, A. L., and Handa, R. J. (2018). Sex differences in the hypothalamic–pituitary–adrenal axis’ response to stress: an important role for gonadal hormones. Neuropsychopharmacology 44, 45–58. doi: 10.1038/s41386-018-0167-9

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hembrook, J. R., and Mair, R. G. (2011). Lesions of reuniens and rhomboid thalamic nuclei impair radial maze win-shift performance. Hippocampus 21, 815–826. doi: 10.1002/HIPO.20797

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hoffman, A. N., Trott, J. M., Makridis, A., and Fanselow, M. S. (2022). Anxiety, fear, panic: an approach to assessing the defensive behavior system across the predatory imminence continuum. Learn. Behav. 50, 339–348. doi: 10.3758/s13420-021-00509-x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hopper, J. W., Pitman, R. K., Su, Z., Heyman, G. M., Lasko, N. B., Macklin, M. L., et al. (2008). Probing reward function in posttraumatic stress disorder: expectancy and satisfaction with monetary gains and losses. J. Psychiatr. Res. 42, 802–807. doi: 10.1016/J.JPSYCHIRES.2007.10.008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

James, M. H., Charnley, J. L., Levi, E. M., Jones, E., Yeoh, J. W., Smith, D. W., et al. (2011). Orexin-1 receptor signalling within the ventral tegmental area, but not the paraventricular thalamus, is critical to regulating cue-induced reinstatement of cocaine-seeking. Int. J. Neuropsychopharmacol. 14, 684–690. doi: 10.1017/S1461145711000423

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Janak, P. H., and Tye, K. M. (2015). From circuits to behaviour in the amygdala. Nature 517, 284–292. doi: 10.1038/nature14188

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Johnson, E. O., Kamilaris, T. C., Chrousos, G. P., and Gold, P. W. (1992). Mechanisms of stress: a dynamic overview of hormonal and behavioral homeostasis. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 16, 115–130. doi: 10.1016/S0149-7634(05)80175-7

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kantak, K. M., Black, Y., Valencia, E., Green-Jordan, K., and Eichenbaum, H. B. (2002). Dissociable effects of lidocaine inactivation of the rostral and caudal basolateral amygdala on the maintenance and reinstatement of cocaine-seeking behavior in rats. J. Neurosci. 22, 1126–1136. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.22-03-01126.2002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kavushansky, A., and Richter-Levin, G. (2006). Effects of stress and corticosterone on activity and plasticity in the amygdala. J. Neurosci. Res. 84, 1580–1587. doi: 10.1002/jnr.21058

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kedia, S., and Chattarji, S. (2014). Marble burying as a test of the delayed anxiogenic effects of acute immobilisation stress in mice. J. Neurosci. Methods 233, 150–154. doi: 10.1016/J.JNEUMETH.2014.06.012

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kim, J., Zhang, X., Muralidhar, S., LeBlanc, S. A., and Tonegawa, S. (2017). Basolateral to central amygdala neural circuits for appetitive behaviors. Neuron 93, 1464–1479.e5. doi: 10.1016/J.NEURON.2017.02.034

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kinner, V. L., Wolf, O. T., and Merz, C. J. (2016). Cortisol alters reward processing in the human brain. Horm. Behav. 84, 75–83. doi: 10.1016/J.YHBEH.2016.05.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Knox, D., George, S. A., Fitzpatrick, C. J., Rabinak, C. A., Maren, S., and Liberzon, I. (2012). Single prolonged stress disrupts retention of extinguished fear in rats. Learn. Mem. 19, 43–49. doi: 10.1101/LM.024356.111

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kovács, L. Á., Schiessl, J. A., Nafz, A. E., Csernus, V., and Gaszner, B. (2018). Both basal and acute restraint stress-induced c-Fos expression is influenced by age in the extended amygdala and brainstem stress centers in male rats. Front. Aging Neurosci. 10:248. doi: 10.3389/FNAGI.2018.00248

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kudielka, B. M., and Kirschbaum, C. (2004). Sex differences in HPA axis responses to stress: a review. Biol. Psychol. 69, 113–132. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2004.11.009

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kumar, P., Berghorst, L. H., Nickerson, L. D., Dutra, S. J., Goer, F. K., Greve, D. N., et al. (2014). Differential effects of acute stress on anticipatory and consummatory phases of reward processing. Neuroscience 266, 1–12. doi: 10.1016/J.NEUROSCIENCE.2014.01.058

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lai, C. W., and Chang, C. H. (2019). Adaptive anxious states and down-regulation of dopamine activity under amygdala activation in rats. Behav. Brain Res. 361, 1–6. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2018.12.049

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lê, A. D., Funk, D., Juzytsch, W., Coen, K., Navarre, B. M., Cifani, C., et al. (2011). Effect of prazosin and guanfacine on stress-induced reinstatement of alcohol and food seeking in rats. Psychopharmacology 218, 89–99. doi: 10.1007/S00213-011-2178-7

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Legault, M., Rompre, P. P., and Wise, R. A. (2000). Chemical stimulation of the ventral hippocampus elevates nucleus accumbens dopamine by activating dopaminergic neurons of the ventral tegmental area. J. Neurosci. 20, 1635–1642. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.20-04-01635.2000

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lemaire, V., Piazza, P. V., and Le Moal, M. (2005). Glucocorticoids and motivated behaviour. Tech. Behav. Neural Sci. 15, 341–358. doi: 10.1016/S0921-0709(05)80020-3

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lenglos, C., Mitra, A., Guèvremont, G., and Timofeeva, E. (2013). Sex differences in the effects of chronic stress and food restriction on body weight gain and brain expression of CRF and relaxin-3 in rats. Genes Brain Behav. 12, 370–387. doi: 10.1111/GBB.12028

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lin, P., and Pratt, W. E. (2014). Inactivation of the nucleus Accumbens Core or medial Shell attenuates reinstatement of sugar-seeking behavior following sugar priming or exposure to food-associated cues. PLoS One 9:e99301. doi: 10.1371/JOURNAL.PONE.0099301

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lisieski, M. J., Eagle, A. L., Conti, A. C., Liberzon, I., and Perrine, S. A. (2018). Single-prolonged stress: a review of two decades of progress in a rodent model of post-traumatic stress disorder. Front. Psych. 9:196. doi: 10.3389/FPSYT.2018.00196

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mahler, S. V., Smith, R. J., and Aston-Jones, G. (2013). Interactions between VTA orexin and glutamate in cue-induced reinstatement of cocaine seeking in rats. Psychopharmacology 226, 687–698. doi: 10.1007/S00213-012-2681-5

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Malvaez, M., Shieh, C., Murphy, M. D., Greenfield, V. Y., and Wassum, K. M. (2019). Distinct cortical–amygdala projections drive reward value encoding and retrieval. Nat. Neurosci. 22, 762–769. doi: 10.1038/s41593-019-0374-7

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mantsch, J. R., Baker, D. A., Funk, D., Lê, A. D., and Shaham, Y. (2015). Stress-induced reinstatement of drug seeking: 20 years of progress. Neuropsychopharmacology 41, 335–356. doi: 10.1038/npp.2015.142

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Marinelli, M., and Piazza, P. V. (2002). Interaction between glucocorticoid hormones, stress and psychostimulant drugs*. Eur. J. Neurosci. 16, 387–394. doi: 10.1046/J.1460-9568.2002.02089.X

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Markou, A., Salamone, J. D., Bussey, T. J., Mar, A. C., Brunner, D., Gilmour, G., et al. (2013). Measuring reinforcement learning and motivation constructs in experimental animals: relevance to the negative symptoms of schizophrenia. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 37, 2149–2165. doi: 10.1016/J.NEUBIOREV.2013.08.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Masood, A., Banerji, B., Vijayan, V. K., and Ray, A. (2004). Pharmacological and biochemical studies on the possible role of nitric oxide in stress adaptation in rats. Eur. J. Pharmacol. 493, 111–115. doi: 10.1016/J.EJPHAR.2004.04.018

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

McCorry, L. K. (2007). Physiology of the autonomic nervous system. Am. J. Pharm. Educ. 71:78. doi: 10.5688/aj710478

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

McDonald, A. J. (1982a). Cytoarchitecture of the central amygdaloid nucleus of the rat. J. Comp. Neurol. 208, 401–418. doi: 10.1002/CNE.902080409

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

McDonald, A. J. (1982b). Neurons of the lateral and basolateral amygdaloid nuclei: a golgi study in the rat. J. Comp. Neurol. 212, 293–312. doi: 10.1002/CNE.902120307

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

McDonald, A. J., and Mascagni, F. (2001). Colocalization of calcium-binding proteins and GABA in neurons of the rat basolateral amygdala. Neuroscience 105, 681–693. doi: 10.1016/S0306-4522(01)00214-7

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

McEwen, B. S. (2007). Physiology and neurobiology of stress and adaptation: central role of the brain. Physiol. Rev. 87, 873–904. doi: 10.1152/PHYSREV.00041.2006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

McFarland, K., and Kalivas, P. W. (2001). The circuitry mediating cocaine-induced reinstatement of drug-seeking behavior. J. Neurosci. 21, 8655–8663. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.21-21-08655.2001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

McLaughlin, R. J., and Floresco, S. B. (2007). The role of different subregions of the basolateral amygdala in cue-induced reinstatement and extinction of food-seeking behavior. Neuroscience 146, 1484–1494. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2007.03.025

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

McLaughlin, J., and See, R. E. (2003). Selective inactivation of the dorsomedial prefrontal cortex and the basolateral amygdala attenuates conditioned-cued reinstatement of extinguished cocaine-seeking behavior in rats. Psychopharmacology 168, 57–65. doi: 10.1007/s00213-002-1196-x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Meyer, H. C., Sangha, S., Radley, J. J., LaLumiere, R. T., and Baratta, M. V. (2021). Environmental certainty influences the neural systems regulating responses to threat and stress. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 131, 1037–1055. doi: 10.1016/J.NEUBIOREV.2021.10.014

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mitra, R., Jadhav, S., McEwen, B. S., Vyas, A., and Chattarji, S. (2005). Stress duration modulates the spatiotemporal patterns of spine formation in the basolateral amygdala. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U. S. A. 102, 9371–9376. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0504011102

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Montoya, E. R., Bos, P. A., Terburg, D., Rosenberger, L. A., and van Honk, J. (2014). Cortisol administration induces global down-regulation of the brain’s reward circuitry. Psychoneuroendocrinology 47, 31–42. doi: 10.1016/J.PSYNEUEN.2014.04.022

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Moorman, D. E., James, M. H., Kilroy, E. A., and Aston-Jones, G. (2017). Orexin/hypocretin-1 receptor antagonism reduces ethanol self-administration and reinstatement selectively in highly-motivated rats. Brain Res. 1654, 34–42. doi: 10.1016/J.BRAINRES.2016.10.018

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Morales, M., and Margolis, E. B. (2017). Ventral tegmental area: cellular heterogeneity, connectivity and behaviour. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 18, 73–85. doi: 10.1038/nrn.2016.165

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Morris, B. H., and Rottenberg, J. (2015). Heightened reward learning under stress in generalized anxiety disorder: a predictor of depression resistance? J. Abnorm. Psychol. 124, 115–127. doi: 10.1037/A0036934

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Moscarello, J. M., and Ledoux, J. E. (2013). The contribution of the amygdala to aversive and appetitive Pavlovian processes. Emot. Rev. 5, 248–253. doi: 10.1177/1754073913477508

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Murray, E. A. (2007). The amygdala, reward and emotion. Trends Cogn. Sci. 11, 489–497. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2007.08.013

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nair, S. G., Navarre, B. M., Cifani, C., Pickens, C. L., Bossert, J. M., and Shaham, Y. (2010). Role of dorsal medial prefrontal cortex dopamine D1-family receptors in relapse to high-fat food seeking induced by the anxiogenic drug Yohimbine. Neuropsychopharmacology 36, 497–510. doi: 10.1038/npp.2010.181

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nair-Roberts, R. G., Chatelain-Badie, S. D., Benson, E., White-Cooper, H., Bolam, J. P., and Ungless, M. A. (2008). Stereological estimates of dopaminergic, GABAergic and glutamatergic neurons in the ventral tegmental area, substantia nigra and retrorubral field in the rat. Neuroscience 152, 1024–1031. doi: 10.1016/J.NEUROSCIENCE.2008.01.046

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nawijn, L., van Zuiden, M., Frijling, J. L., Koch, S. B. J., Veltman, D. J., and Olff, M. (2015). Reward functioning in PTSD: a systematic review exploring the mechanisms underlying anhedonia. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 51, 189–204. doi: 10.1016/J.NEUBIOREV.2015.01.019

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ness, T. J., DeWitte, C., and DeBerry, J. J. (2022). Spinal neurochemical mechanisms of acute stress-induced visceral hypersensitivity in healthy rats. Neurosci. Lett. 770:136401. doi: 10.1016/J.NEULET.2021.136401

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nollet, M., Le Guisquet, A. M., and Belzung, C. (2013). Models of depression: unpredictable chronic mild stress in mice. Curr. Protoc. Pharmacol. 61:Unit 5.65. doi: 10.1002/0471141755.PH0565S61

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Öhman, A. (2005). The role of the amygdala in human fear: automatic detection of threat. Psychoneuroendocrinology 30, 953–958. doi: 10.1016/J.PSYNEUEN.2005.03.019

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Olonode, E. T., Aderibigbe, A. O., Adeoluwa, O. A., and Ajayi, A. M. (2018). Protective effects of Morin hydrate on acute stress-induced behavioral and biochemical alterations in mice. Basic Clin. Neurosci. 9, 195–208. doi: 10.29252/NIRP.BCN.9.3.195

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Piazza, P. V., and Le Moal, M. (1997). Glucocorticoids as a biological substrate of reward: physiological and pathophysiological implications. Brain Res. Rev. 25, 359–372. doi: 10.1016/S0165-0173(97)00025-8

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pisani, A., Calabresi, P., Centonze, D., and Bernardi, G. (1997). Enhancement of NMDA responses by group I metabotropic glutamate receptor activation in striatal neurones. Br. J. Pharmacol. 120, 1007–1014. doi: 10.1038/SJ.BJP.0700999

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pitchers, K. K., Sarter, M., and Robinson, T. E. (2018). The hot ‘n’ cold of cue-induced drug relapse. Learn. Mem. 25, 474–480. doi: 10.1101/LM.046995.117

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pitkänen, A., Stefanacci, L., Farb, C. R., Go, G.-G., Ledoux, J. E., and Amaral, D. G. (1995). Intrinsic connections of the rat amygdaloid complex: projections originating in the lateral nucleus. J. Comp. Neurol. 356, 288–310. doi: 10.1002/cne.903560211

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Puglisi-Allegra, S., Imperato, A., Angelucci, L., and Cabib, S. (1991). Acute stress induces time-dependent responses in dopamine mesolimbic system. Brain Res. 554, 217–222. doi: 10.1016/0006-8993(91)90192-X

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ranaldi, R. (2014). Dopamine and reward seeking: the role of ventral tegmental area. Rev. Neurosci. 25, 621–630. doi: 10.1515/revneuro-2014-0019

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Randall, P. A., Lee, C. A., Podurgiel, S. J., Hart, E., Yohn, S. E., Jones, M., et al. (2015). Bupropion increases selection of high effort activity in rats tested on a progressive ratio/chow feeding choice procedure: implications for treatment of effort-related motivational symptoms. Int. J. Neuropsychopharmacol. 18, pyu017–pyu011. doi: 10.1093/IJNP/PYU017

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rezayof, A., Golhasani-Keshtan, F., Haeri-Rohani, A., and Zarrindast, M. R. (2007). Morphine-induced place preference: involvement of the central amygdala NMDA receptors. Brain Res. 1133, 34–41. doi: 10.1016/J.BRAINRES.2006.11.049

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Reznikov, L. R., Grillo, C. A., Piroli, G. G., Pasumarthi, R. K., Reagan, L. P., and Fadel, J. (2007). Acute stress-mediated increases in extracellular glutamate levels in the rat amygdala: differential effects of antidepressant treatment. Eur. J. Neurosci. 25, 3109–3114. doi: 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2007.05560.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Reznikov, L. R., Reagan, L. P., and Fadel, J. R. (2008). Activation of phenotypically distinct neuronal subpopulations in the anterior subdivision of the rat basolateral amygdala following acute and repeated stress. J. Comp. Neurol. 508, 458–472. doi: 10.1002/cne.21687

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Roozendaal, B., Barsegyan, A., and Lee, S. (2007). Adrenal stress hormones, amygdala activation, and memory for emotionally arousing experiences. Prog. Brain Res. 167, 79–97. doi: 10.1016/S0079-6123(07)67006-X

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Roozendaal, B., McEwen, B. S., and Chattarji, S. (2009). Stress, memory and the amygdala. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 10, 423–433. doi: 10.1038/nrn2651

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Roozendaal, B., and McGaugh, J. L. (1997). Basolateral amygdala lesions block the memory-enhancing effect of glucocorticoid Administration in the Dorsal Hippocampus of rats. Eur. J. Neurosci. 9, 76–83. doi: 10.1111/J.1460-9568.1997.TB01355.X

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Salamone, J. D., and Correa, M. (2002). Motivational views of reinforcement: implications for understanding the behavioral functions of nucleus accumbens dopamine. Behav. Brain Res. 137, 3–25. doi: 10.1016/S0166-4328(02)00282-6

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Salamone, J. D., Correa, M., Mingote, S., and Weber, S. M. (2003). Nucleus Accumbens dopamine and the regulation of effort in food-seeking behavior: implications for studies of natural motivation, psychiatry, and drug abuse. J. Pharmacol. Exp. Ther. 305, 1–8. doi: 10.1124/JPET.102.035063

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Salamone, J. D., Correa, M., Mingote, S. M., and Weber, S. M. (2005). Beyond the reward hypothesis: alternative functions of nucleus accumbens dopamine. Curr. Opin. Pharmacol. 5, 34–41. doi: 10.1016/J.COPH.2004.09.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Salamone, J. D., Pardo, M., Yohn, S. E., López-Cruz, L., San Miguel, N., and Correa, M. (2016). “Mesolimbic dopamine and the regulation of motivated behavior” in Behavioral neuroscience of motivation. eds. E. H. Simpson and P. D. Balsam (Cham: Springer International Publishing), 231–257.

Google Scholar

Salamone, J. D., Wisniecki, A., Carlson, B. B., and Correa, M. (2001). Nucleus accumbens dopamine depletions make animals highly sensitive to high fixed ratio requirements but do not impair primary food reinforcement. Neuroscience 105, 863–870. doi: 10.1016/S0306-4522(01)00249-4

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sananes, C. B., and Davis, M. (1992). N-methyl-D-aspartate lesions of the lateral and basolateral nuclei of the amygdala block fear-potentiated startle and shock sensitization of startle. Behav. Neurosci. 106, 72–80. doi: 10.1037/0735-7044.106.1.72

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Savander, V., Go, C.-G., Ledoux, J. E., and Pitkänen, A. (1995). Intrinsic connections of the rat amygdaloid complex: projections originating in the basal nucleus. J. Comp. Neurol. 361, 345–368. doi: 10.1002/cne.903610211

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schepers, S. T., and Bouton, M. E. (2019). Stress as a context: stress causes relapse of inhibited food seeking if it has been associated with prior food seeking. Appetite 132, 131–138. doi: 10.1016/J.APPET.2018.10.016

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schoenbaum, G., Chiba, A. A., and Gallagher, M. (1998). Orbitofrontal cortex and basolateral amygdala encode expected outcomes during learning. Nat. Neurosci. 1, 155–159. doi: 10.1038/407

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schoenbaum, G., Chiba, A. A., and Gallagher, M. (1999). Neural encoding in orbitofrontal cortex and basolateral amygdala during olfactory discrimination learning. J. Neurosci. 19, 1876–1884. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.19-05-01876.1999

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schwabe, L., Tegenthoff, M., Höffken, O., and Wolf, O. T. (2012). Simultaneous glucocorticoid and noradrenergic activity disrupts the neural basis of goal-directed action in the human brain. J. Neurosci. 32, 10146–10155. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1304-12.2012

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

See, R. E., Fuchs, R. A., Ledford, C. C., and McLaughlin, J. (2003). Drug addiction, relapse, and the amygdala. Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci. 985, 294–307. doi: 10.1111/J.1749-6632.2003.TB07089.X

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Shaham, Y., Shalev, U., Lu, L., De Wit, H., and Stewart, J. (2003). The reinstatement model of drug relapse: history, methodology and major findings. Psychopharmacology 168, 3–20. doi: 10.1007/S00213-002-1224-X

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Shalev, U., Erb, S., and Shaham, Y. (2010). Role of CRF and other neuropeptides in stress-induced reinstatement of drug seeking. Brain Res. 1314, 15–28. doi: 10.1016/J.BRAINRES.2009.07.028

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sharp, B. M. (2017). Basolateral amygdala and stress-induced hyperexcitability affect motivated behaviors and addiction. Transl. Psychiatry 7:e1194. doi: 10.1038/tp.2017.161

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Shen, Y., Kishimoto, K., Linden, D. J., and Sapirstein, A. (2007). Cytosolic phospholipase A2 alpha mediates electrophysiologic responses of hippocampal pyramidal neurons to neurotoxic NMDA treatment. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U. S. A. 104, 6078–6083. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0605427104

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Shinba, T., Ozawa, N., Yoshii, M., and Yamamoto, K. I. (2010). Delayed increase of brain noradrenaline after acute footshock stress in rats. Neurochem. Res. 35, 412–417. doi: 10.1007/s11064-009-0070-1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Simmons, D. A., and Neill, D. B. (2009). Functional interaction between the basolateral amygdala and the nucleus accumbens underlies incentive motivation for food reward on a fixed ratio schedule. Neuroscience 159, 1264–1273. doi: 10.1016/J.NEUROSCIENCE.2009.01.026

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Solati, J., and Hajikhani, R. (2010). Microinjection of NMDA receptor agents into the central nucleus of the amygdale alters water intake in rats. Iran. J. Basic Med. Sci. 13, 238–241. doi: 10.22038/IJBMS.2010.5069

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sousa, F. S. S., Birmann, P. T., Balaguez, R., Alves, D., Brüning, C. A., and Savegnago, L. (2018). α-(phenylselanyl) acetophenone abolishes acute restraint stress induced-comorbid pain, depression and anxiety-related behaviors in mice. Neurochem. Int. 120, 112–120. doi: 10.1016/J.NEUINT.2018.08.006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Souza, R. R., Noble, L. J., and McIntyre, C. K. (2017). Using the single prolonged stress model to examine the pathophysiology of PTSD. Front. Pharmacol. 8:615. doi: 10.3389/FPHAR.2017.00615

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Stefanik, M. T., and Kalivas, P. W. (2013). Optogenetic dissection of basolateral amygdala projections during cue-induced reinstatement of cocaine seeking. Front. Behav. Neurosci. 7:213. doi: 10.3389/FNBEH.2013.00213

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sun, W., Akins, C. K., Mattingly, A. E., and Rebec, G. V. (2005). Ionotropic glutamate receptors in the ventral tegmental area regulate cocaine-seeking behavior in rats. Neuropsychopharmacology 30, 2073–2081. doi: 10.1038/sj.npp.1300744

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Suvrathan, A., Tomar, A., and Chattarji, S. (2010). Effects of chronic and acute stress on rat behaviour in the forced-swim test. Stress 13, 533–540. doi: 10.3109/10253890.2010.489978

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sweeney, M. M., and Shahan, T. A. (2015). Renewal, resurgence, and alternative reinforcement context. Behav. Process. 116, 43–49. doi: 10.1016/J.BEPROC.2015.04.015

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tian, G., Hui, M., Macchia, D., Derdeyn, P., Rogers, A., Hubbard, E., et al. (2022). An extended amygdala-midbrain circuit controlling cocaine withdrawal-induced anxiety and reinstatement. Cell Rep. 39:110775. doi: 10.1016/J.CELREP.2022.110775

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Valenti, O., Lodge, D. J., and Grace, A. A. (2011). Aversive stimuli alter ventral tegmental area dopamine neuron activity via a common action in the ventral hippocampus. J. Neurosci. 31, 4280–4289. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5310-10.2011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Veilleux-Lemieux, D., Castel, A., Carrier, D., Beaudry, F., and Vachon, P. (2013). Pharmacokinetics of ketamine and xylazine in young and old Sprague-Dawley rats. J. Am. Assoc. Lab. Anim. Sci. 52, 567–570.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

White, P. F., Johnston, R. R., and Pudwill, C. R. (1975). Interaction of ketamine and halothane in rats. Anesthesiology 42, 179–186. doi: 10.1097/00000542-197502000-00011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wiborg, O. (2013). Chronic mild stress for modeling anhedonia. Cell Tissue Res. 354, 155–169. doi: 10.1007/s00441-013-1664-0

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Yager, L. M., and Robinson, T. E. (2010). Cue-induced reinstatement of food seeking in rats that differ in their propensity to attribute incentive salience to food cues. Behav. Brain Res. 214, 30–34. doi: 10.1016/J.BBR.2010.04.021

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Yetnikoff, L., Lavezzi, H. N., Reichard, R. A., and Zahm, D. S. (2014). An update on the connections of the ventral mesencephalic dopaminergic complex. Neuroscience 282, 23–48. doi: 10.1016/J.NEUROSCIENCE.2014.04.010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zhang, M., Balmadrid, C., and Kelley, A. E. (2003). Nucleus accumbens opioid, GABAergic, and dopaminergic modulation of palatable food motivation: contrasting effects revealed by a progressive ratio study in the rat. Behav. Neurosci. 117, 202–211. doi: 10.1037/0735-7044.117.2.202

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zhang, H., Sheng, Z. F., Wang, J., Zheng, P. R., Kang, X. L., Chang, H. M., et al. (2022). Signaling pathways involved in NMDA-induced suppression of M-channels in corticotropin-releasing hormone neurons in central amygdala. J. Neurochem. 161, 478–491. doi: 10.1111/JNC.15647

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zhang, W.-H., Zhang, J.-Y., Holmes, A., and Pan, B.-X. (2021). Amygdala circuit substrates for stress adaptation and adversity. Biol. Psychiatry 89, 847–856. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2020.12.026

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zimmerman, J. M., Rabinak, C. A., McLachlan, I. G., and Maren, S. (2007). The central nucleus of the amygdala is essential for acquiring and expressing conditional fear after overtraining. Learn. Mem. 14, 634–644. doi: 10.1101/lm.607207

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: basolateral nucleus of the amygdala, central nucleus of the amygdala, acute stress, motivated behavior, food-seeking

Citation: Lai C-W and Chang C-h (2023) Pharmacological activation of the amygdala, but not single prolonged footshock-induced acute stress, interferes with cue-induced motivation toward food rewards in rats. Front. Behav. Neurosci. 17:1252868. doi: 10.3389/fnbeh.2023.1252868

Received: 04 July 2023; Accepted: 25 August 2023;
Published: 14 September 2023.

Edited by:

Charles L. Pickens, Kansas State University, United States

Reviewed by:

Kevin Thomas Ball, Bloomsburg University, United States
Erik Joseph Garcia, University of Nebraska Omaha, United States

Copyright © 2023 Lai and Chang. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Chun-hui Chang, changch@life.nthu.edu.tw

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.