Skip to main content

EDITORIAL article

Front. Behav. Neurosci., 31 May 2023
Sec. Individual and Social Behaviors
This article is part of the Research Topic Expert Opinion in Environmental and Genetic Factors Impacting Functional Brain Lateralization in Development and Evolution View all 9 articles

Editorial: Expert opinion in environmental and genetic factors impacting functional brain lateralization in development and evolution

  • Department of Psychological, Health and Territorial Sciences, University ‘G. d'Annunzio' of Chieti-Pescara, Chieti, Italy

Functional lateralization of the brain (i.e., the asymmetrical distribution of functions in the two structurally symmetrical cerebral hemispheres), is a research field that has progressed immensely before and after the turn of the Millennium, incorporating inputs from many aspects of biology (e.g., evolution, genetics, and neuroscience) and psychology (e.g., cognition, emotion, and their associated disorders; Ocklenburg and Gunturkun, 2017; Vingerhoets, 2019). However, a sense of stasis might appear to afflict the entire field even to the eyes of the knowledgeable observer, as many of the advancements do not seem to have really added much to the understanding of key questions, for instance in the field of human health (Ocklenburg et al., 2021). We believe that the field indeed epitomizes a most impressive case in which descriptive simplicity (i.e., the search for “simple” left vs. right differences) faces the explanatory complexity of how complex nervous systems originate from complex genes that exert their effects in complex environments. However, sometimes appearances are deceiving, and this is especially true if the simplicity of “left vs. right” is assumed at face value (Marzoli et al., 2022), forgetting that it is a result of that complexity but it might as well be one of its drivers—in evolutionary and developmental terms.

This Research Topic presents an overview of expert opinions in what we deem a righteous recognition of a domain that is by no means simple, and that is succeeding in integrating knowledge on functional lateralization. Such integration takes please by means of the study of individual and social behaviors in humans and non-human species, the computational modeling of their evolutionary constraints, the genetics and epigenetics of typical and atypical neural development—to name but a few disparate sources of evidence that well represent the liveliness of the field.

Pfeifer et al. start from circumscribing the contribution of large-scale genetic and epigenetic association studies on lateralization phenotypes. These have been traditionally oversimplified for the sake of rapid assessment (e.g., left vs. right dominant hand), not doing full justice to the understanding of how the complexity of behavioral asymmetries is linked to genetic and epigenetic causation. Because behavioral lateralization involves a much more articulate pattern, especially when looked through the lens of social interaction, their proposal to expand the laterality phenotype spectrum assessed in GWAS and EWAS studies (e.g., including self-reported preferences in kissing, hugging, cradling, etc.; Packheiser et al., 2020; Malatesta et al., 2021a) comes as a very reasonable and practical suggestion.

Berretz and Packheiser present an interesting point of view on the occurrence of atypical hemispheric asymmetries in clinical conditions with different levels of heritability and susceptibility to environmental stress starting early in life (Berretz et al., 2020). The authors trace a connection between the heterogeneity of environmental causes and the reduced association with alterations of functional brain lateralization observed in major depressive disorder, as compared to the less heterogeneous conditions. Although speculative, their insight might prove useful to differentiate between patients based on the association among symptom clusters, life-history, and brain asymmetry. This would represent a demonstration of the direct application of brain lateralization to novel diagnostic criteria in a field, psychiatry, which has been reluctant to incorporate neuroscientific evidence.

Nelson moves the discourse toward the development of behavioral asymmetries, specifically handedness. Strongly grounded in the tradition that documented how specific prenatal events trigger a series of developmental cascades that end up in establishing handedness directionality in humans (Michel and Harkins, 1986), she urges the adoption of a similar approach in the comparative study of primate handedness. Studies performed on a few primate species are reviewed and re-interpreted in this light, attributing to innate postural constraints the power of channeling visual experience toward one side, thus reinforcing the emergence of hand preference later in life.

Within a similar framework, Malatesta et al. identify the population-level leftward lateralization of maternal cradling during the first post-natal weeks (i.e., a critical period for the neurodevelopment of brain functions) as one of the earliest socio-environmental factors epigenetically canalizing neurodevelopment (Malatesta et al., 2021b). Moreover, they consider the left-cradling bias as a double-exchange platform of “monitoring and exposure”, which benefits both the mother and the infant and is presumably shaped by evolutionary and social pressures. In fact, both phylogenetic and ontogenetic factors are supposed to be involved in the emergence of this interactive side bias, which might subserve a similar function in human infants as that shown by means of light exposure in avians during incubation.

In this regard, Rogers traces a pattern of direct causal relationships between prenatal sensory experience and the establishment of asymmetrical behaviors known to depend on the ontogenesis of neural lateralization. The article revolves around the vast literature accumulated on the avian embryo, because it has proven a superlative animal model especially since the discovery of the effects of light exposure in ovo on chicken brain and behavior by Rogers (1982). The understanding of these environmental factors has expanded including precise tests of the effects of (or lack thereof) auditory and olfactory stimulation, and certainly represents a cornerstone of neuroethological epigenetics.

Comparative research is also under the Giljov and Karenina spotlight. The authors pinpoint that ungulates (saiga antelopes, especially) also might represent a convenient model for social laterality (intended as positional side-bias observation during social interactions; Karenina et al., 2017). For example, these animals do show overtly lateralized social behaviors—generally comparable with those of humans—and do not use forelimbs in social interactions (as is the case of humans and other primates). These factors could make easier the interpretation of this kind of positional and motor behaviors and possibly bring significant insights into the understanding of social laterality.

Another comparative study is that of Loconsole et al., who discuss the influence of asymmetrical spatial numerical association on numerical discrimination in chicks by recoding behavioral data from a previous study (Rugani et al., 2022). They speculate on the qualitatively different hemispheric contributions and specializations, as well as on the role of brain functional asymmetries for mapping numbers onto space during evolution.

Finally, Tonello and Vallortigara address the issue of the simulation models which so far have been proposed to account for population-level brain and behavioral asymmetries. According to one of these (Ghirlanda et al., 2009), the unbalanced ratio of left- and right- lateralized individuals can arise from an evolutionary stable strategy based on the balance between competitive (i.e., favoring individuals differently lateralized from the majority, who would be more able to surprise antagonists) and cooperative (i.e., favoring individuals showing the same lateralization) interactions. In this context, the authors provided a new probabilistic and evolutionary perspective by considering the population as a whole, within which the single individual can indirectly communicate with each other through the environmental change in a social way, as a form of “stigmergy” (Theraulaz and Bonabeau, 1999). Therefore, a system can automatically self-organize its own laterality balance by expressing a sort of “swarm intelligence”, but only when operating as a group.

Author contributions

All authors listed have made a substantial, direct, and intellectual contribution to the work and approved it for publication.

Acknowledgments

We would like to thank all authors, editors, and reviewers of the Research Topic for their valuable contributions.

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher's note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

References

Berretz, G., Wolf, O. T., Güntürkün, O., and Ocklenburg, S. (2020). Atypical lateralization in neurodevelopmental and psychiatric disorders: What is the role of stress? Cortex 125, 215–232. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2019.12.019

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ghirlanda, S., Frasnelli, E., and Vallortigara, G. (2009). Intraspecific competition and coordination in the evolution of lateralization. Philos. Trans. R Soc. Lond. B Biol. Sci. 364, 861–866. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2008.0227

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Karenina, K., Giljov, A., Ingram, J., Rowntree, V. J., and Malashichev, Y. (2017). Lateralization of mother–infant interactions in a diverse range of mammal species. Nat. Ecol. Evol. 1, 1–4. doi: 10.1038/s41559-016-0030

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text

Malatesta, G., Marzoli, D., Morelli, L., Pivetti, M., and Tommasi, L. (2021a). The role of ethnic prejudice in the modulation of cradling lateralization. J. Nonverbal. Behav. 45, 187–205. doi: 10.1007/s10919-020-00346-y

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Malatesta, G., Marzoli, D., Prete, G., and Tommasi, L. (2021b). Human lateralization, maternal effects and neurodevelopmental disorders. Front. Behav. Neurosci. 15, 668520. doi: 10.3389/fnbeh.2021.668520

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Marzoli, D., D'Anselmo, A., Malatesta, G., Lucaf,ò, C., Prete, G., and Tommasi, L. (2022). The intricate web of asymmetric processing of social stimuli in humans. Symmetry 14, 1096. doi: 10.3390/sym14061096

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Michel, G. F., and Harkins, D. A. (1986). Postural and lateral asymmetries in the ontogeny of handedness during infancy. Develop. Psychobiol. 19, 247–258. doi: 10.1002/dev.420190310

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ocklenburg, S., Berretz, G., Packheiser, J., and Friedrich, P. (2021). Laterality 2020: entering the next decade. Laterality 26, 265–297. doi: 10.1080/1357650X.2020.1804396

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ocklenburg, S., and Gunturkun, O. (2017). The Lateralized Brain: The Neuroscience and Evolution of Hemispheric Asymmetries. London: Academic Press. doi: 10.1016/B978-0-12-803452-1.00009-6

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Packheiser, J., Schmitz, J., Metzen, D., Reinke, P., Radtke, F., Friedrich, P., et al. (2020). Asymmetries in social touch-motor and emotional biases on lateral preferences in embracing, cradling and kissing. Laterality 25, 325–348. doi: 10.1080/1357650X.2019.1690496

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rogers, L. J. (1982). Light experience and asymmetry of brain function in chickens. Nature 297, 223–225. doi: 10.1038/297223a0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rugani, R., Loconsole, M., Koslowski, M., and Regolin, L. (2022). Processing individually distinctive schematic-faces supports proto-arithmetical counting in the young domestic chicken. Animals 12, 2322. doi: 10.3390/ani12182322

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Theraulaz, G., and Bonabeau, E. (1999). A brief history of stigmergy. Artif. Life 5, 97–116. doi: 10.1162/106454699568700

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vingerhoets, G. (2019). Phenotypes in hemispheric functional segregation? Perspectives and challenges. Phys. Life Rev. 30, 1–18. doi: 10.1016/j.plrev.2019.06.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: lateralization and brain functions, social behavior, environmental factors, epigenetics, comparative & evolutionary neuroscience

Citation: Malatesta G and Tommasi L (2023) Editorial: Expert opinion in environmental and genetic factors impacting functional brain lateralization in development and evolution. Front. Behav. Neurosci. 17:1215176. doi: 10.3389/fnbeh.2023.1215176

Received: 01 May 2023; Accepted: 22 May 2023;
Published: 31 May 2023.

Edited by:

Walter Adriani, National Institute of Health (ISS), Italy

Reviewed by:

Orsola Rosa Salva, University of Trento, Italy

Copyright © 2023 Malatesta and Tommasi. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Gianluca Malatesta, gianluca.malatesta@unich.it

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.