Skip to main content

OPINION article

Front. Behav. Neurosci., 13 June 2022
Sec. Learning and Memory
This article is part of the Research Topic Interaction Between Affect and Memory in the Brain: from Basic Mechanisms to Clinical Implications View all 9 articles

Emotional Memory and Amygdala Activation

  • Center for the Study of Emotion and Attention, University of Florida, Gainesville, FL, United States

Introduction

Determining the neural correlates of emotional memory is critical for understanding both healthy and psychopathological emotional processing, and meta-analyses summarizing dozens of individual studies (e.g., Murty et al., 2010; Dahlgren et al., 2020) report enhanced amygdala activity during both encoding and retrieval of emotional, compared to neutral, stimuli, implicating the amygdala as central in enhanced memory for emotional items (see also Phelps and Anderson, 1997). In this Opinion article, we raise a number of issues regarding amygdala activation and emotional episodic memory. First, the majority of fMRI studies investigating emotional episodic memory assess memory for emotional scenes or facial expressions (e.g., Dahlgren et al., 2020), but then generalize findings to memory for the wide variety of emotional events encountered in the natural world. In the brain (and body), however, differential activity due to emotional processing can vary as a function of the specific emotional challenge (Bradley, 2000; Sabatinelli et al., 2011; Bradley and Lang, 2018; Sambuco et al., 2020a), raising questions regarding the generality of amygdala activation in emotional processing. Second, effects of emotion at retrieval are assessed using a number of different fMRI contrasts, with some potentially including differences in functional activity that are related to emotionality, but not necessarily to memory, whereas, for others, excellent memory performance precludes accurate assessment of unique effects of emotion. Below, we more fully consider these issues, and discuss implications for both theory and data.

Emotional Challenge

The extent to which amygdala activation is involved in emotional episodic memory, whether during encoding or retrieval, relies, first of all, on its reliable activation across emotional contexts. Whereas enhanced amygdala activity is a key finding when viewing emotional, compared to neutral, scenes or faces (see Sabatinelli et al., 2011 for a meta-analysis), significant amygdala activation is not reliably obtained when retrieving personal emotional memories, which is central in both healthy and psychopathological functioning. Thus, whereas some studies report amygdala activation during emotional autobiographical retrieval (e.g., Britton et al., 2005; Sharot et al., 2007), others do not (Nadel et al., 2007; Piefke et al., 2008; Svoboda and Levine, 2009; Lanius et al., 2010). To re-address this issue, we asked participants to retrieve and imagine the same pleasant and unpleasant autobiographical events repeatedly (four times) across a scanning session while amygdala activation (Figure 1A) was measured (Bradley et al., 2022). Figure 1B illustrates the pattern of blood-oxygen-level-dependent (BOLD) change in the amygdala across repeated retrieval of emotional autobiographical events, which did not prompt any significant amygdala activation, regardless of repetition.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Enhanced functional brain activity in the amygdala (A) is not found when repeatedly retrieving emotional autobiographical events (B), with no difference between emotional and neutral content [(B), inset]. Amygdala activation is consistently found when [(C), left] encoding emotional, compared to neutral, pictures but is significantly reduced (repetition suppression) when the same scenes are repeatedly retrieved [(C), right].

It is possible that operations of spatial smoothing and/or standardization in previous studies misattributed activation of the anterior hippocampus, which lies just adjacent to the amygdala and is reliably enhanced during autobiographical retrieval (Sambuco et al., 2020a; Bradley et al., 2022), to the amygdala. To test this, we assessed amygdala activation as it varied with emotional challenge in a repeated-measures design in which the same participants viewed emotional (or neutral) scenes and retrieved emotional (or neutral) events (Sambuco et al., 2020a). Identical pre-processing steps of spatial smoothing and standardization were conducted for both sets of functional data, allowing a direct assessment of the extent to which different emotional challenges prompt enhanced activation in the same or different neural regions. Replicating many previous studies, significant enhanced BOLD activity was found in the amygdala when viewing emotional, compared to neutral, scenes, as well as in the inferior frontal gyrus and the visual cortex (for a meta-analysis, see Sabatinelli et al., 2011). On the other hand, ROI analyses confirmed that retrieving personal emotional, compared to neutral, events did not differentially or significantly activate the same amygdala region, but that activation of the adjacent anterior hippocampus was enhanced during emotional autobiographical retrieval (Sambuco et al., 2020a; see also Sambuco et al., 2022).

Differences in emotional reactivity due to the specific emotional challenge are also routinely found in psychophysiological reactions, with, for instance, cardiac deceleration (slowing), a parasympathetically mediated response that facilitates sensory perception, found during aversive visual perception, and cardiac acceleration (speeding), a sympathetically mediated response that supports action preparation, found during aversive mental imagery (Lang, 1979; Vrana et al., 1986; Bradley and Lang, 2018). Thus, finding diverse patterns of neural activity when encoding and/or retrieving different emotional challenges is not surprising, and both human and animal data document a variety of brain regions activated during emotional processing, including the insula, basal ganglia, striatum, cingulate, cerebellum, and more (Gasquoine, 2014; Wang et al., 2016; Adamaszek et al., 2017; Pierce and Péron, 2020). And, whereas early studies reported significant amygdala involvement when retrieving cues associated with the presentation (Phelps et al., 2004) or prediction of electric shock (Phelps et al., 2001; Alvarez et al., 2011), more recent investigations do not find significant amygdala activation in either fear conditioning (Fullana et al., 2016; Visser et al., 2021) or when under threat of shock (Kirlic et al., 2019; Sambuco et al., 2020b,c).

Retrieval Contrast

For emotional challenges (such as scene perception) that include significant amygdala activation, studies assessing retrieval-related functional activity have utilized a number of different contrasts to support the proposed relationship between emotionality and amygdala enhancement. In a recent meta-analysis for example (Dahlgren et al., 2020), the majority of included retrieval studies directly compare functional brain activity at recognition for correctly recognized emotional and neutral items (“hits”), assuming that differences reflect enhanced memory for emotional stimuli. However, functional contrasts that compare emotional and neutral hits can include differential amygdala activation due to differences in emotion, rather than related to retrieval. That is, similar to encoding, these retrieval cues, which are perceptually processed prior to contacting an episodic memory representation, could prompt differential amygdala activation that is unrelated to episodic retrieval.

A second common comparison computes “difference in memory” (DM) maps for emotional (or neutral) items that contrasts functional activation between correctly remembered (“hits”) and forgotten items (“misses”). For emotional scenes, however, immediate recognition is almost perfect (~90% accuracy; Ferrari et al., 2013; Weymar et al., 2018), leaving very few trials available for constructing maps of emotional “misses,” which greatly reduces the reliability of both the statistical contrast and resulting conclusions (Chen et al., 2022). In fact, even when immediate recognition was assessed for hundreds of emotional and neutral scenes, performance was so high that Kalpouzos et al. (2012) were not able to construct DM maps for emotional “misses.” Moreover, in a final step, studies using DM contrasts compare emotional and neutral DM maps, but, as when comparing emotional and neutral hits, differences due to emotionality, rather than memory-related processes, may remain in these functional contrasts.

An alternative way to assess the effects of emotion at retrieval, while controlling for differences in sheer emotionality, is to repeat the same emotional items. In particular, when repetitions are distributed across an imaging session, both theory and data support the hypothesis that these spaced repetitions engage spontaneous episodic retrieval (e.g., Greene, 1989; Hintzman, 2010), in which the re-presentation of a cue, following perceptual processing, activates its prior episodic occurrence. Moreover, because immediate recognition for pictures is almost perfect with just a single prior presentation, repetitive presentation will prompt successful recognition, even in the absence of an explicit performance measure. If amygdala activity is specifically enhanced during episodic retrieval of emotional cues, increased amygdala activation should be found for repeated scenes, compared to when the same emotional cues are novel.

To assess this hypothesis, Bradley et al. (2015) presented novel and repeated emotional and neutral scenes distributed across a session (4x), finding the expected enhanced amygdala activation during initial encoding (novel) of emotional, compared to neutral, scenes (see Figure 1C, left). Moreover, amygdala activation continued to be greater when viewing emotional, compared to neutral, scenes, for repeated stimuli. Importantly, however, as illustrated in Figure 1C (right), compared to encoding, repeated presentation of the same emotional scenes elicited significant repetition suppression, in which amygdala activity was reduced, rather than enhanced, at retrieval. Similar repetition suppression in the amygdala is found during both explicit and implicit scene recognition (Weymar et al., 2018) and following the repetition of emotional and neutral faces (Ishai et al., 2004). In general, repetition suppression effects during episodic retrieval have been variously interpreted as indexing neural priming, perceptual sharpening or information accumulation (e.g., Schott et al., 2005; Yassa and Stark, 2008; Rugg and Vilberg, 2013), raising questions regarding its specific role during episodic retrieval. Taken together, however, although amygdala activation is greater when retrieving emotional, compared to neutral, scenes, this may reflect differences in emotionality, rather than memory, and does not show the expected enhancement, but rather suppression, when compared to encoding.

Discussion

As frequently noted, memory performance is generally enhanced for emotional, compared to neutral, information (Bradley et al., 1992; Hamann et al., 1999; Dolcos et al., 2005, 2017; Kensinger and Schacter, 2008), and amygdala activation at encoding and/or retrieval is often proposed as a critical mechanism. Much of the supporting data, however, arise from studies assessing memory for visual scenes or faces, whereas amygdala activation is not a general finding across emotional challenges. Thus, emotional memory accounts that include a key role of the amygdala, based primarily on data from emotional challenges that include significant activation of this region (e.g., amygdala-frontal regulatory circuit, Hartley and Phelps, 2010; Motzkin et al., 2015; amygdala-sensory connections, Mather and Sutherland, 2011; Bowen et al., 2018) will not necessarily generalize to memory in other emotional contexts. Elucidating the neural mechanisms important in emotional episodic memory will instead first need to carefully consider the nature of the emotional challenge, as is generally the case in the study of emotion (Bradley, 2000; Bradley and Lang, 2018), with broader generalities proposed when the data confirm cross-context commonalities.

In addition, regardless of the specific emotion challenge, the nature of the functional contrast used to assess emotional differences at retrieval is also critical, as excellent memory performance for emotional stimuli may rule out contrasts requiring a reasonable number of misses (such as DM contrast). More importantly, comparing functional maps at retrieval for emotional and neutral items is problematic, as these can reflect functional activity related to differences in stimulus emotionality that are not associated with episodic memory or retrieval success. Thus, although amygdala activity is higher at retrieval for emotional, compared to neutral, scenes, repetition suppression, rather than enhancement, is found when compared to initial encoding (Ishai et al., 2004; Bradley et al., 2015; Weymar et al., 2018), which doesn't support a prediction of enhanced amygdala activation related to better episodic memory.

Isolating functional activity specific to emotional retrieval is probably better supported by data indicating that successful emotional memory prompts (1) enhanced functional activation at retrieval, compared to encoding, and/or (2) enhanced functional activation that is only apparent at retrieval. Functional enhancement when retrieving emotional scenes and autobiographical memories is reliably reported in large regions of the posteromedial cortex, including the posterior cingulate cortex and precuneus (Kim, 2010, 2017; Bradley et al., 2022), supporting a central and context-independent role in episodic retrieval (e.g., Wheeler and Buckner, 2004; Wagner et al., 2005; Rugg and Vilberg, 2013). During immediate scene recognition (explicit or implicit; Weymar et al., 2018) or following mere scene repetition (Bradley et al., 2015), however, emotional content does not modulate posteromedial activation (perhaps reflecting excellent immediate memory performance for all scenes), but differential effects have been reported in delayed recognition (Ventura-Bort et al., 2020) as well as during autobiographical retrieval (Sambuco et al., 2022). Future studies assessing similarities and differences in neural activation in different emotional challenges, using appropriate functional contrasts, promise to more fully elucidate the neural mechanisms underlying emotional episodic memory.

Author Contributions

MB and NS conceptualized and wrote initial draft. Both authors contributed to the article and approved the submitted version.

Funding

This research was supported by NIMH grants MH094386 and MH098078.

Conflict of Interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher's Note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

References

Adamaszek, M., D'Agata, F., Ferrucci, R., Habas, C., Keulen, S., Kirkby, K. C., et al. (2017). Consensus paper: Cerebellum and emotion. Cerebellum 16, 552–576. doi: 10.1007/s12311-016-0815-8

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Alvarez, R. P., Chen, G., Bodurka, J., Kaplan, R., and Grillon, C. (2011). Phasic and sustained fear in humans elicits distinct patterns of brain activity. Neuroimage 55, 389–400. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.11.057

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bowen, H. J., Kark, S. M., and Kensinger, E. A. (2018). NEVER forget: negative emotional valence enhances recapitulation. Psychon. Bull. Rev. 25, 870–891. doi: 10.3758/s13423-017-1313-9

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bradley, M. M. (2000). Emotion and motivation, in Handbook of Psychophysiology, eds Cacioppo, J. T., Tassinary, L. G., and Berntson, G, . (New York: Cambridge University Press), 602–642.

Google Scholar

Bradley, M. M., Costa, V. D., Ferrari, V., Codispoti, M., Fitzsimmons, J. R., and Lang, P. J. (2015). Imaging distributed and massed repetitions of natural scenes: spontaneous retrieval and maintenance. Hum. Brain Map. 36, 1381–1392. doi: 10.1002/hbm.22708

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bradley, M. M., Greenwald, M. K., Petry, M. C., and Lang, P. J. (1992). Remembering pictures: Pleasure and arousal in memory. J. Exp. Psychol. Learn. Mem. Cognit. 18, 379–390. doi: 10.1037/0278-7393.18.2.379

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bradley, M. M., and Lang, P. J. (2018). Emotion in body and brain: Context-dependent action and reaction, in The Nature of Emotion, 2nd Edn, eds Davidson, R., Shackman, A., Fox, A., and Lapate, R, . (Oxford University Press), 280–282.

Google Scholar

Bradley, M. M., Sambuco, N., and Lang, P. J. (2022). Neural correlates of repeated retrieval of emotional autobiographical events. Neuropsychologia 169, 108203. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2022.108203

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Britton, J. C., Phan, K. L., Taylor, S. F., Fig, L. M., and Liberzon, I. (2005). Corticolimbic blood flow in posttraumatic stress disorder during script-driven imagery. Biol. Psychiatry 57, 832–840. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2004.12.025

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chen, G., Pine, D. S., Brotman, M. A., Smith, A. R., Cox, R. W., Taylor, P. A., et al. (2022). Hyperbolic trade-off: The importance of balancing trial and subject sample sizes in neuroimaging. NeuroImage 247, 118786. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2021.118786

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dahlgren, K., Ferris, C., and Hamann, S. (2020). Neural correlates of successful emotional episodic encoding and retrieval: an SDM meta-analysis of neuroimaging studies. Neuropsychologia 143, 107495. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2020.107495

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dolcos, F., Katsumi, Y., Weymar, M., Moore, M., Tsukiura, T., and Dolcos, S. (2017). Emerging directions in emotional episodic memory. Front. Psychol. 8, 1867. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2017.01867

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dolcos, F., LaBar, K. S., and Cabeza, R. (2005). Remembering one year later: Role of the amygdala and the medial temporal lobe memory system in retrieving emotional memories. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U. S. A. 102, 2626–2631. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0409848102

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ferrari, V., Bradley, M. M., Codispoti, M., Karlsson, M., and Lang, P. J. (2013). Repetition and brain potentials when recognizing natural scenes: task and emotion differences. Soc. Cognit. Affect. Neurosci. 8, 847–854. doi: 10.1093/scan/nss081

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fullana, M. A., Harrison, B. J., Soriano-Mas, C., Vervliet, B., Cardoner, N., Àvila-Parcet, A., et al. (2016). Neural signatures of human fear conditioning: an updated and extended meta-analysis of fMRI studies. Mol. Psychiatry 21, 500–508. doi: 10.1038/mp.2015.88

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gasquoine, P. G. (2014). Contributions of the insula to cognition and emotion. Neuropsychol. Rev. 24, 77–87. doi: 10.1007/s11065-014-9246-9

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Greene, R. L. (1989). Spacing effects in memory: Evidence for a two-process account. J. Exp. Psychol. Learn. Mem. Cognit. 15, 371–377. doi: 10.1037/0278-7393.15.3.371

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hamann, S. B., Ely, T. D., Grafton, S. T., and Kilts, C. D. (1999). Amygdala activity related to enhanced memory for pleasant and aversive stimuli. Nat. Neurosci. 2, 289–293. doi: 10.1038/6404

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hartley, C. A., and Phelps, E. A. (2010). Changing fear: the neurocircuitry of emotion regulation. Neuropsychopharmacology 35, 136–146. doi: 10.1038/npp.2009.121

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hintzman, D. L. (2010). How does repetition affect memory? Evidence from judgments of recency. Mem. Cognit. 38, 102–115. doi: 10.3758/MC.38.1.102

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ishai, A., Pessoa, L., Bikle, P. C., and Ungerleider, L. G. (2004). Repetition suppression of faces is modulated by emotion. PNAS 101, 9827–9832 doi: 10.1073/pnas.0403559101

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kalpouzos, G., Fischer, H., Rieckmann, A., MacDonald, S. W. S., and Bäckman, L. (2012). Impact of negative emotion on the neural correlates of long-term recognition in younger and older adults. Front. Integr. Neurosci. 6, 74. doi: 10.3389/fnint.2012.00074

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kensinger, E. A., and Schacter, D. L. (2008). Memory and emotion, in Handbook of Emotions, eds Lewis, M., Haviland-Jones, J. M., and Barrett, L. F, . (New York, NY: The Guilford Press), 601–617.

Google Scholar

Kim, H. (2010). Dissociating the roles of the default-mode, dorsal, and ventral networks in episodic memory retrieval. Neuroimage 50,1648–1657. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.01.051

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kim, H. (2017). Brain regions that show repetition suppression and enhancement: a meta-analysis of 137 neuroimaging experiments. Hum. Brain Map. 38, 1894–1913. doi: 10.1002/hbm.23492

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kirlic, N., Aupperle, R. L., Rhudy, J. L., Misaki, M., Kuplicki, R., Sutton, A., et al. (2019). Latent variable analysis of negative affect and its contributions to neural responses during shock anticipation. Neuropsychopharmacology 44, 695–702. doi: 10.1038/s41386-018-0187-5

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lang, P. J. (1979). A bio-informational theory of emotional imagery. Psychophysiology 16, 495–512. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-8986.1979.tb01511.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lanius, R. A., Bluhm, R. L., Coupland, N. J., Hegadoren, K. M., Rowe, B., Théberge, J., et al. (2010). Default mode network connectivity as a predictor of post-traumatic stress disorder symptom severity in acutely traumatized subjects. Acta Psychiatr. Scand. 121, 33–40. doi: 10.1111/j.1600-0447.2009.01391.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mather, M., and Sutherland, M. R. (2011). Arousal-biased competition in perception and memory. Perspect. Psychol. Sci. 6, 114–133. doi: 10.1177/1745691611400234

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Motzkin, J. C., Philippi, C. L., Wolf, R. C., Baskaya, M. K., and Koenigs, M. (2015). Ventromedial prefrontal cortex is critical for the regulation of amygdala activity in humans. Biol. Psychiatry 77, 276–284. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2014.02.014

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Murty, V. P., Ritchey, M., Adcock, R. A., and LaBar, K. S. (2010). fMRI studies of successful emotional memory encoding: a quantitative meta-analysis. Neuropsychologia 48, 3459–3469. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2010.07.030

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nadel, L., Campbell, J., and Ryan, L. (2007). Autobiographical memory retrieval and hippocampal activation as a function of repetition and the passage of time. Neural Plast. 2007, 90472. doi: 10.1155/2007/90472

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Phelps, E. A., and Anderson, A. K. (1997). Emotional memory: What does the amygdala do? Curr. Biol. 7, R311–R314. doi: 10.1016/S0960-9822(06)00146-1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Phelps, E. A., Delgado, M. R., Nearing, K. I., and LeDoux, J. E. (2004). Extinction learning in humans role of the amygdala and vmPFC. Neuron 43, 897–905. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2004.08.042

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Phelps, E. A., O'Connor, K. J., Gatenby, J. C., Gore, J. C., Grillon, C., and Davis, M. (2001). Activation of the left amygdala to a cognitive representation of fear. Nat. Neurosci. 4, 437–441. doi: 10.1038/86110

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Piefke, M., Pestinger, M., Arin, T., Kohl, B., Kastrau, F., Schnitker, R., et al. (2008). The neurofunctional mechanisms of traumatic and non-traumatic memory in patients with acute PTSD following accident trauma. Neurocase. 13, 342–357. doi: 10.1080/13554790701851494

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pierce, J. E., and Péron, J. (2020). The basal ganglia and the cerebellum in human emotion. Soc. Cogn. Affect. Neurosci. 15, 588–613. doi: 10.1093/scan/nsaa076

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rugg, M. D., and Vilberg, K. L. (2013). Brain networks underlying episodic memory retrieval. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 23, 255–260. doi: 10.1016/j.conb.2012.11.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sabatinelli, D., Fortune, E. E., Li, Q., Siddiqui, A., Krafft, C., Oliver, W. T., et al. (2011). Emotional perception: Meta-analyses of face and natural scene processing. NeuroImage 54, 2524–2533. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.10.011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sambuco, N., Bradley, M. M., Herring, D. R., and Lang, P. J. (2020a). Common circuit or paradigm shift? The functional brain in emotional scene perception and emotional imagery. Psychophysiology 57, 1–14. doi: 10.1111/psyp.13522

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sambuco, N., Bradley, M. M., and Lang, P. J. (2022). Narrative imagery: emotional modulation in the default mode network. Neuropsychologia 164, 108087. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2021.108087

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sambuco, N., Costa, V. D., Lang, P. J., and Bradley, M. M. (2020b). Assessing the role of the amygdala in fear of pain: Neural activation under threat of shock. J. Affect. Disord. 276, 1142–1148. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2020.07.110

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sambuco, N., Costa, V. D., Lang, P. J., and Bradley, M. M. (2020c). Aversive perception in a threat context: Separate and independent neural activation. Biol. Psychol. 154, 107926. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2020.107926

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schott, B. H., Henson, R. N., Richardson-Klavehn, A., Becker, C., Thoma, V., Heinze, H.-J., et al. (2005). Redefining implicit and explicit memory: The functional neuroanatomy of priming, remembering, and control of retrieval. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U. S. A. 102, 1257–1262. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0409070102

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sharot, T., Riccardi, A. M., Raio, C. M., and Phelps, E. A. (2007). Neural mechanisms mediating optimism bias. Nature 450, 102–105. doi: 10.1038/nature06280

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Svoboda, E., and Levine, B. (2009). the effects of rehearsal on the functional neuroanatomy of episodic autobiographical and semantic remembering: a functional magnetic resonance imaging study. J. Neurosci. 29, 3073–3082. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3452-08.2009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ventura-Bort, C., Wendt, J., Wirkner, J., König, J., Lotze, M., Hamm, A. O., et al. (2020). Neural substrates of long-term item and source memory for emotional associates: an fMRI study. Neuropsychologia 147, 107561. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2020.107561

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Visser, R. M., Bathelt, J., Scholte, H. S., and Kindt, M. (2021). Robust BOLD Responses to faces but not to conditioned threat: challenging the amygdala's reputation in human fear and extinction learning. J. Neurosci. 41, 10278–10292. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0857-21.2021

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vrana, S. R., Cuthbert, B. N., and Lang, P. J. (1986). Fear imagery and text-processing. Psychophysiology 23, 247–253. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-8986.1986.tb00626.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wagner, A. D., Shannon, B. J., Kahn, I., and Buckner, R. L. (2005). Parietal lobe contributions to episodic memory retrieval. Trends Cogn. Sci. 9, 445–453. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2005.07.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wang, X., Xie, H., Cotton, A. S., Brickman, K. R., Lewis, T. J., Wall, J. T., et al. (2016). Early changes in cortical emotion processing circuits after mild traumatic brain injury from motor vehicle collision. J. Neurotrauma 34,273–280. doi: 10.1089/neu.2015.4392

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Weymar, M., Bradley, M. M., Sege, C. T., and Lang, P. J. (2018). Neural activation and memory for natural scenes: Explicit and spontaneous retrieval. Psychophysiology 55, e13197. doi: 10.1111/psyp.13197

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wheeler, M. E., and Buckner, R. L. (2004). Functional-anatomic correlates of remembering and knowing. NeuroImage 21, 1337–1349. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2003.11.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Yassa, M. A., and Stark, C. E. L. (2008). Multiple signals of recognition memory in the medial temporal lobe. Hippocampus 18, 945–954. doi: 10.1002/hipo.20452

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: emotion, amygdala, hippocampus, episodic memory, perception, imagery, repetition suppression, repetition enhancement

Citation: Bradley MM and Sambuco N (2022) Emotional Memory and Amygdala Activation. Front. Behav. Neurosci. 16:896285. doi: 10.3389/fnbeh.2022.896285

Received: 14 March 2022; Accepted: 16 May 2022;
Published: 13 June 2022.

Edited by:

Lycia D. de Voogd, Radboud University Nijmegen, Netherlands

Reviewed by:

Jennifer Strafford Stevens, Emory University, United States

Copyright © 2022 Bradley and Sambuco. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Margaret M. Bradley, YnJhZGxleSYjeDAwMDQwO3VmbC5lZHU=

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.