Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Behav. Neurosci., 08 April 2022
Sec. Emotion Regulation and Processing
This article is part of the Research Topic The Effects of Physical Activity and Exercise on Cognitive and Affective Wellbeing View all 13 articles

COVID-19 Quarantine Impact on Wellbeing and Cognitive Functioning During a 10-Week High-Intensity Functional Training Program in Young University Students

  • Sports Research Centre, Department of Sport Sciences, Miguel Hernández University of Elche, Elche, Spain

Physical exercise can improve cognitive functioning and wellbeing; however, the degree of change in either of these two variables seems to be related to the exercise intensity or type. Therefore, new physical training (PT) programs have been developed to increase exercise efficiency. One such example is high-intensity functional training (HIFT), which has proven to be a time-efficient and highly effective strategy to improve physical fitness. This study analyzed whether HIFT can affect reaction time (RT) and vitality, as well as positive and negative affect. Forty-two college students participated in the study, 21 in the experimental group and 21 in the control group. The experimental group completed 10 weeks of training, five of which were supervised, and the remainder consisted of online training during the COVID-19 quarantine. Participants were evaluated at the beginning, at the end of the 5 weeks of supervised training, and after the 5 weeks of online training. HIFT improved RT without changes in psychological wellbeing during the entire period of training supervised and online. Therefore, during the HIFT program, the quarantine situation did not adversely affect this population’s wellbeing, but it did negatively affect adherence to the training program.

Introduction

It is well-known that acute and chronic physical exercise (PE) affects physical and physiological functions and influences aspects related to cognitive functioning. According to Herold et al. (2019), it is crucial to distinguish between physical activity (PA), acute and chronic PE, and physical training (PT). All muscle activity that increases energy expenditure is PA (Herold et al., 2019). PE is a specific, planned and structured PA. PE may include a single bout (acute PE) or repeated bouts over a short-term or long-term period (chronic PE). Finally, when chronic PE is purposed to increase (or maintain) one or multiple dimensions of fitness, this is known as PT (Herold et al., 2019).

Regarding exercise and cognitive functioning, PA and fitness have been linked to the academic performance of young students (Marques et al., 2018), inhibitory function in young and old subjects (Peruyero et al., 2017; Carbonell-Hernández et al., 2019; Pastor et al., 2019), memory (Erickson et al., 2011; Jeon and Ha, 2017), wellbeing (Cervelló et al., 2014), depression (McMahon et al., 2017), and other cognitive outputs. However, it is not well-known how different variables of the exercise session (e.g., exercise intensity, exercise duration, exercise type) can modulate it. The literature on exercise and cognition has traditionally dealt with non-modifiable parameters like sex or genotype. Less research is available on the modifiable parameters, like the individual dose-response relationship (Herold et al., 2019).

Regarding the dose-response relationship of exercise to improve cognitive functioning and wellbeing, it is clear that exercise intensity does matter. According to wellbeing, low-intensity but not high-intensity aerobic exercise seems more effective in producing acute changes in wellbeing in adolescents (Pastor et al., 2019). However, high-intensity chronic exercise in university students is more efficient in reducing stress and improving wellbeing (Leuchter et al., 2022).

Regarding cognitive functioning, high-intensity aerobic exercise seems to produce greater improvements in other age groups and different cognitive domains like choice reaction time (RT) (Chang et al., 2012). Furthermore, in favor of high-intensity acute exercise, it has been shown that high intensity can produce greater increases in neurotrophins like brain-derived neurotrophic factor (BDNF) (Saucedo Marquez et al., 2015). BDNF promotes neurogenesis, synaptic plasticity, dendritic and axonal growth, and cell survival (Walsh et al., 2020). The BDNF increase with exercise intensity may be related to lactate release (Ferris et al., 2007). It is well-known that high-intensity exercise performance produces a considerable release of lactate (Ferguson et al., 2018), and high-intensity functional training (HIFT) implies high-intensity exercise. Moreover, lactate release is also related to cerebral blood flow and improves neurogenesis, neuroprotection, neuronal plasticity, and memory (Hashimoto et al., 2021).

Moreover, resistance training can also improve cognitive functioning (Landrigan et al., 2020); single bouts of resistance exercise showed more significant improvements when higher loads were used (Chang and Etnier, 2009). However, the optimal exercise and training characteristics to effectively enhance cognitive functioning with chronic PE programs are relatively unknown (Soga et al., 2018). So, it seems that a single bout of both high-intensity aerobic or resistance exercise can acutely improve RT. However, we do not know if high-intensity exercise would also be effective when applied in chronic PE programs. It is possible that continuous exposure to lactate, in turn, could induce the release of BDNF to produce such an effect (Hashimoto et al., 2021).

Regarding changes in cognitive functioning, PT and acute PE have been shown to improve attention processes, information processing speed, and executive control (Colcombe and Kramer, 2003; Tsai et al., 2014; Pontifex et al., 2015). Among the various tools generally used to determine the impact of PE on the above-mentioned cognitive variables, RT has been used in the literature to evaluate cognitive and motor functions, as it involves both central and peripheral components (Ozyemisci-Taskiran et al., 2008). RT is understood as the time a person needs to initiate a movement (Magill, 2010). There are different types of RT. On the one hand, simple RT occurs when only one possible stimulus requires a single type of response (Posner and McLeod, 1982), whereas in more complex choice RT, there are different possibilities to choose from during the experiment, as it involves the appearance of different stimuli that require different responses (Neubauer, 1990). More specifically, RT measures processing speed (Mulder and van Maanen, 2013), and this variable reflects a person’s cognitive functioning (Deary et al., 2010). At the morphological level, processing speed may be an indirect measure of white brain substance, as changes in this substance are associated with changes in processing speed (Gunning-Dixon et al., 2009). At the behavioral level, it has recently been shown that medical students with better academic performance had lower RT (Prabhavathi et al., 2017). Not only that, in middle-aged and older people, lower performance in RT tasks can be a predictor of heart disease and stroke mortality (Shimizu et al., 2018).

Like in cognition, PA can improve wellbeing in young students (McMahon et al., 2017). Nowadays, wellbeing is a European priority (Miret et al., 2015), but there is no clear consensus about its definition or operationalization (Korhonen et al., 2014). Positive or negative indicators such as self-esteem, quality of life, positive and negative affect or vitality are usually used (Pastor et al., 2019). Some authors consider two perspectives for studying wellbeing: the hedonic perspective, which considers wellbeing as the presence of positive affect and the absence of negative affect; and the eudaimonic perspective, which associates wellbeing with the possibility of performing or expressing the most valuable human potentials, related to optimal psychological functioning (Ryan et al., 2013). Moreover, in both young and older individuals, it has been seen that cognitive performance changes could be affected by aspects such as mood and social relations (Lieberman et al., 2014). In addition, cardiorespiratory fitness (CRF) is related to vitality in young people (Eddolls et al., 2018), and vitality should correlate with positive and negative affect (Cervelló et al., 2014). Thus, the improvement of CRF with exercise training could improve wellbeing.

Consequently, it would be interesting to determine how PE can affect wellbeing and processing speed. At the acute level, improvements have been reported in complex RTs after sub-maximum exercise in university students (Audiffren et al., 2008; Ashnagar et al., 2015) and in adolescents’ wellbeing (Pastor et al., 2019). On the other hand, some studies have observed a relationship between fitness and RT in young students (Luque-Casado et al., 2013; Wang et al., 2015; Maghsoudipour et al., 2018), although others did not find this association (Moradi et al., 2019). At the chronic level, the effects of the exercise intensity, duration, and frequency to produce long-term effects on processing speed and wellbeing in this population are not well-established.

Regarding exercise programs, the need to investigate new training methods to improve several fitness components simultaneously and efficiently, without high volumes of training and favoring adherence, has been indicated previously (Heinrich et al., 2014). In this sense, HIFT has recently emerged. HIFT is defined as a training style that incorporates various functional movements performed at high exercise intensity and is designed to improve general physical fitness and performance parameters (Feito et al., 2018). Unlike HIIT, this type of training combines aerobic activities with exercises to improve muscle strength (Feito et al., 2018). Among its benefits are improved body composition, power, muscle strength, and aerobic capacity, the same as or even more so than when performing continuous workouts or HIIT (Feito et al., 2018). However, the term “functional” may not be correct in this training style (Ide et al., 2021), as HIFT can be more clearly described as combined or concurrent training. Therefore, like a high-intensity aerobic and resistance-training regime, it seems a powerful candidate to improve RT and wellbeing in young people. Moreover, high-intensity exercise can release high amounts of lactate, a molecule positively related to cognitive function (Hashimoto et al., 2021).

Therefore, this study’s main objective was to determine whether a supervised HIFT group program of 10 weeks could improve processing speed and wellbeing. However, as a consequence of the COVID-19 pandemic, a national lockdown was announced in the middle of the program (week five), and supervised training had to be terminated. Thus, online training was implemented for the next 5 weeks.

The COVID-19 pandemic impacted the economy and society in many aspects, disturbing lifestyle and health behaviors (Dragun et al., 2021). The lockdown deteriorated daily activities, especially physical activities (Dragun et al., 2021). In addition, as a consequence of the quarantine, there was a negative impact on the population’s wellbeing (Brooks et al., 2020), with higher levels of anxiety and depression (Burke et al., 2020).

The study’s second objective arose as a consequence of the COVID-19 quarantine. We aimed to determine whether the “stay-at-home” orders and the need to continue training at home individually, adapting it to the new situation, would impact cognitive functioning and wellbeing in the participants.

Thus, we hypothesized that the 5-week HIFT supervised training phase would improve participants’ RT and psychological wellbeing and give rise to high adherence rates. Conversely, the phase carried out during the 5 weeks of strict quarantine due to COVID-19 was expected to decrease wellbeing compared to the in-person, supervised program, coupled with lower adherence and a reduction in RT improvement.

Materials and Methods

Participants

The sample consisted of 42 college students, 21 in the experimental group (14 males and 7 females) and 21 in the control group (9 males and 12 females). All the participants were adults over 18 years old, and all of them claimed to be sedentary (one or fewer days of PE a week) during at least the last 6 months. In addition, all participants completed an initial questionnaire to ensure they met the requirements to be part of the study, such as not having recently suffered injuries and not being involved in any other training program. Their sociodemographic characteristics are summarized in Table 1.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Anthropometric and fitness characteristics of the participants (N = 42).

Measurements

Cardiorespiratory Function: Stress Test

Participants of the experimental group performed a maximum incremental test on a treadmill with a measure of oxygen consumption (VO2) and heart rate (HR) throughout the test to determine the maximum VO2 (VO2 max), maximum HR (HR max), and their associated values in the first (VT1) and second ventilatory threshold (VT2). The exchange of respiratory gases was measured using the MasterScreen CPX analyzer (Hoechberg, Germany) breath by breath after being calibrated. The VO2 max was calculated as the highest 30-s mean of VO2. In addition, 15-s means of O2 and CO2 were used to determine VT1 and VT2 (Pettitt et al., 2013). Participants were not allowed to drink or speak during the test and were asked to refrain from intense exercise 24 h earlier.

The incremental test protocol consisted of a 3 min warm-up at 5 km/h with a 1% slope. Then, the test was performed on a 1% slope and began at a speed of 6 km/h and increased 1 km/h per minute until fatigue.

The incremental test protocol could not be repeated at the end of the study due to the COVID-19 lockdown, so no data regarding the possible fitness improvements of the participants was obtained.

Cognitive Functioning

Reaction Time

A test of increasing difficulty was used to measure the time required to respond to a stimulus. All the participants were requested to complete the test in the afternoon at home. The test was carried on a day with no exercise training session in a quiet place. A digital application with three tests was used in the study (AppAndAbout, 2019). Each test consisted of passing ten trials of different stimuli, and between each trial, there was a random rest period between 1,000 and 1,500 ms. The first test, the Training Test (TrT), was used to train the participants at the beginning of each measure. In the TrT, a green or red luminous stimulus was presented, and the subject had to press a green button only if the stimulus presented was of the same color. In the second test, the Choice Reaction Test (CRT) (ICC = 0.78, SEM = 0.05 s), there were also two luminous signals, red and green, and two buttons, red and green. The subject had to press the button with the matching color corresponding to the luminous signal presented (e.g., if the light signal was red, they had to press the red button). The third test, the Interference Test (InT) (ICC = 0.80, SEM = 0.05 s), consisted of introducing interference in the decision process of the second test: the participants had to respond in reverse, such that the signal should not coincide with the color of the button (e.g., if the light signal was green, they must press the red button). The third test was the most complex because it added interference. A familiarization protocol was used at the beginning of the study, repeating all tests five times. The familiarization protocols of the first 15 participants were analyzed to calculate the ICC of the tests, with moderate to strong reliability (Wells et al., 2014).

Psychological Wellbeing

Subjective Vitality

The Subjective Vitality Questionnaire (Ryan and Frederick, 1997) was used to measure the perception of vitality before and after the two phases of the training program, adapted to Spanish by Molina-García et al. (2007). This questionnaire can be considered a measure of psychological wellbeing (Ryan et al., 2013). This instrument comprises seven items that indicate how one feels at present (e.g., I feel alive and vital). The responses are rated on an eight-point Likert-type scale ranging from 0 (not completely true) to 7 (very true). Cronbach’s alpha in the different experimental situations was comprised between 0.88 and 0.95.

Affective State

Following each training program phase, participants were asked to complete the Positive and Negative Affect Schedule (Mackinnon et al., 1999), which assesses their positive and negative feelings. This questionnaire is considered a hedonic measure of wellbeing. The scale is made up of nine adjectives, which are grouped into two factors in response to the item “Indicate how you feel right now….” Four of its items are associated with Positive Affect (“joyful, happy, content, amused”) and five with Negative Affect (“depressed, worried, frustrated, angry, unhappy”). The responses are rated on an eight-point Likert-type scale ranging from 1 (not completely) to 7 (extremely). Cronbach’s alpha for Positive Affect was between 0.89 and 0.95, and for Negative Affect between 0.83 and 0.92.

Adherence to the Training Program

Participation was monitored through a registration sheet in the supervised sessions and a mobile application during the quarantine phase. In addition, each person recorded the session and their perception of effort (RPE: rate of perceived exertion) 30 min after completing the session by the modified RPE-10 scale. A total of 30 sessions was performed, 15 in each of the phases of the program (supervised and online phases). Adherence was considered sufficient if at least 90% of the sessions were completed (Heinrich et al., 2014).

Procedure

The physical evaluation was carried out at the beginning of the program, and RT and wellbeing evaluations were conducted before the start of the training program (January–February 2020), after 5 weeks of the supervised group intervention (March 2020), and after 5 weeks of individual training during the quarantine period due to the COVID-19 global pandemic (April 2020). The experimental procedure for this study followed the latest (7th) Helsinki Declaration and was approved by the University Ethics Committee (UMH.CID.DPC.02.17). Study participants gave their informed written consent before participating in the study and were informed of the confidentiality and anonymity of the results obtained.

The participants’ experimental allocation was distributed as is shown in the CONSORT flow chart (Figure 1). The inclusion criteria were (a) did not participate in any PE program simultaneously, (b) physical inactivity in the last 6 months, and (c) did not suffer injuries in the last 6 months. The participants were randomized using Microsoft Excel software with the “Random” function. There was no allocation concealment.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Consort flow chart.

The sample size was calculated post-hoc using G-Power (G*Power software, ver. 3.1.9.7; Heinrich-Heine-Universität Düsseldorf, Düsseldorf, Germany).1 It was calculated for the RT analysis. For an ANOVA-RM 2 × 3, and with 42 participants, it is necessary an effect size of ηp2 > 0.04 to ensure a statistical power (1–β) > 0.8 (α = 0.05, correlation among repetitive measures = 0.45). Results have a positive power with ηp2 > 0.04.

Training Program

Two research group members prescribed the supervised training program and carried it out. The sessions were 1 h long, 3 days a week (Monday, Wednesday, and Thursday). The sessions were conducted at university facilities, dividing the participants into two different time schedules to ensure the availability of the material. The training sessions during the quarantine phase were sent to participants via a mobile application to be performed at home.

The sessions were divided into four parts: (a) 10 min of a standardized warm-up; (b) a strength-block of about 15 min, occasionally, on a specific movement pattern with load requirements as a function of the manifestations of muscular endurance or hypertrophy depending on the week; (c) HIFT as the primary training block, which ranged from 10 to 30 min; and (d) cool down through mobility exercises and 10 min of static stretches. Repetition methods (completing a series of exercises in the shortest possible time, or maximum repetitions in a scheduled time) or time methods (rest-work ratios similar to the traditional HIIT method) were used in the main HIFT block. This block included cyclic exercises, such as running or jumping rope, and muscle endurance exercises with bodyweight or light loads (e.g., squats, swings, med ball throws, push-ups, pull-ups, jumps, and more) (Feito et al., 2018). The exercise intensity was all-out for all, and the recovery was self-paced, self-selected, and passive (Feito et al., 2018). The training sessions were adapted to the new situation during the quarantine period. Thus, work with loads was prescribed using generic materials that anyone may have at home (bags of rice, bottles of water, milk containers, and other common materials) (Jiménez-Pavón et al., 2020). The days and hours of training were the same during the presential and quarantine periods.

Monitoring and Quantification of Training

The exercise intensity of the presential training sessions was rated with two different methods, one objective and one subjective: HR and subjective RPE-10 scale of the session. Only RPE was used during the sessions during quarantine.

Heart rate was registered during the sessions with the Polar Team2 Pro System (Polar Electro Oy, Kempele, Finland), which includes 20 coded chest straps, allowing HR registration every second. The recorded HR was used to calculate a load of each session, using Lucia’s Training Impulse (TRIMPs) method (Lucía et al., 1999), which uses the HR at the individual ventilatory thresholds obtained in the stress test to establish three training zones: Zone 1 “below VT1,” Zone 2 “between VT1 and VT2,” and Zone 3 “above VT2.” For the calculation of the TRIMP, the time expended in each zone is multiplicated by the zone number (1, 2, or 3).

The modified 10-point scale was used to quantify the internal load through the RPE (Foster et al., 2001). Participants were asked to determine the exercise intensity of the training 30 min after completing it.

Statistical Analysis

Repeated-measures ANOVA was performed, followed by a post-hoc test with Bonferroni adjustment to detect changes between the different time measurements for CRT, InT, positive affect, negative affect, vitality, and attendance. The bilateral significance level was set at p < 0.05. Sphericity was evaluated with Mauchly’s sphericity test. The effect sizes are expressed as partial eta-squared (ηp2) and are grouped as small (≤0.01), medium (≤0.06), and large (≤0.14) (Cohen, 1992). Paired t-test were used to analyze the exercise intensity of the sessions. A Pearson correlation was performed to analyze the relations of the wellbeing questionnaires. The results were analyzed with JASP 0.16 software (Eric-Jan Wagenmakers, Department of the Psychological Methods University of Amsterdam, Nieuwe Achtergracht 129B, Amsterdam, Netherlands).

Results

Perception of effort was maintained with similar values during the online training sessions and the supervised training sessions (Figure 2). Moreover, during the supervised training sessions, the evolution of RPE was similar to LuTRIMP evolution (Figure 2). Despite changes in the structure of the sessions between the two phases, no significant differences in the exercise intensity of the sessions were observed through the recording of RPE between the supervised sessions and online sessions or between sessions (Figure 2), analyzed with paired t-tests (p = 0.39). Therefore, the stabilization of improvements in RT and the decrease in adherence between the two phases of the program were not attributable to load differences in the prescribed sessions, so there may be other underlying factors.

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Weekly (1–5) load in presential sessions (P) and online sessions (O) during quarantine periods expressed in RPE and LuTrimp. n = 21.

The repeated-measures ANOVA shows the interaction between TIME at the three time points [pretest (PRE), after 5 weeks of supervised training (POST 5), and after 5 weeks of online training (POST 10)] and GROUPs [experimental group (exp), control group (con)].

For the CRT, the TIME x GROUP showed a significant difference with large effect size [F(1,40) = 3.602, p = 0.032, ηp2 = 0.085]. The post-hoc Bonferroni analysis showed an improvement only in the experimental group during the intervention [PRE vs. POST5: M = 46.77, SD = 14.46, t(19) = 3.23, p = 0.027; PRE vs. POST10: M = 59.93, SD = 14.47, t(19) = 4.14, p = 0.001] (Figure 3).

FIGURE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 3. (A) Control Group. (B) Experimental Group. Evolution of CRT during the training process. At the beginning of the study (PRE), after 5 weeks of supervised training (POST 5) and after another 5 weeks of online training during quarantine (POST 10). Significant results shown with p-values are for the experimental group. Data are shown as mean ± standard error.

For the InT, the TIME x GROUP showed a significant difference with a large effect size [F(1,40) = 5.664, p = 0.005, ηp2 = 0.124]. Furthermore, the post-hoc Bonferroni analysis showed an improvement only in the experimental group during the intervention and only at the end of the 10-week training program [PRE vs. POST10: M = 73.61, SD = 23.028, t(19) = 3.19, p = 0.030] (Figure 4).

FIGURE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 4. (A) Control Group. (B) Experimental Group. Evolution of InT during the training process. At the beginning of the study (PRE), after 5 weeks of supervised training (POST 5) and after another 5 weeks of online training during quarantine (POST 10). Significant results shown with p-values are for the experimental group. Data are shown as mean ± standard error.

There were differences in adherence to the sessions between the two periods (supervised vs. online). There was 94% adherence during the supervised period and 71% adherence during the lockdown. An RM ANOVA was done to compare the evolution of the five sessions developed under supervision and online (RM ANOVA 5 × 2, a repeated measure for sessions, and between-subject factor for supervised vs. online training). When we compared the attendance of the participants to the sessions, there was a significant reduction in adherence to the online program [F(1, 58) = 3.179; p = 0.014; ηp2 = 0.052]. The Bonferroni post-hoc analysis showed a significant reduction in attendance to session 3 [M = 0.53, SD = 0.16, t(29) = 3.31, p = 0.049], 4 [M = 0.53, SD = 0.16, t(29) = 3.31, p = 0.049] and 5 [M = 0.83, SD = 0.16, t(29) = 5.17, p < 0.001] compared to the first online session (Figure 5).

FIGURE 5
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 5. (A) Adherence during the supervised period. (B) Adherence during the online period. Weekly adherence in the supervised (weeks 1–5) and online (weeks 6–10) periods. Differences were only present during the online training period.

Finally, the effects of training on wellbeing were analyzed, taking into account three variables (positive affect, negative affect, and subjective vitality). The analysis was done for the three temporal moments (PRE, POST 5, POST 10) and the two groups (experimental vs. control group). The repeated-measures ANOVA revealed the absence of significant differences for the three variables, positive affect [F(1, 40) = 2.34, p = 0.11, ηp2 = 0.11], negative affect [F(1, 40) = 0.21, p = 0.979, ηp2 = 0.001], and subjective vitality, [F(1, 40) = 0.88, p = 0.42, ηp2 = 0.04]. Therefore, the results show that neither the supervised nor the online training phase affected wellbeing. Moreover, we saw a strong correlation between the three variables in the population (Table 2).

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Correlations between wellbeing variables.

All the analyses were repeated considering the participant’s sex, and no differences were found in any variable in the TIME × GROUP × SEX analysis [CRT F(3,37) = 0.431, p = 0.65, ηp2 = 0.01; InT F(3,37) = 0.274, p = 0.76, ηp2 = 0.007; Positive Affect F(3,37) = 1.54, p = 0.22, ηp2 = 0.04; Negative Affect F(3,37) = 1.58, p = 0.21, ηp2 = 0.04; Vitality F(3,37) = 0.783, p = 0.46, ηp2 = 0.02].

Discussion

The objective of this study was to determine whether HIFT could improve cognitive functioning, measured as complex RT, and wellbeing, measured as vitality and positive and negative affects, in young college students. Although the participants declared a sedentary lifestyle during the previous 6 months of the study, their VO2 max were within the normative values provided in the literature (Dwyer et al., 2005). As a result of the COVID-19 lockdown, a second objective, to analyze these variables and adherence during a quarantine period with online training, was included.

Regarding the participants’ processing speed, we observed that 5 weeks of HIFT supervised training improved their RT (CRT and InT). Our findings are in accordance with the literature, providing moderate to strong evidence that exercise being performed at moderate to vigorous exercise intensity can improve processing speed (Erickson et al., 2019).

The different neurophysiological mechanisms that underlie such functional changes might explain the benefits of chronic PE for processing speed. For example, angiogenesis and neurogenesis are structural brain changes mediated by exercise (van Praag et al., 2005). BDNF has been shown to be an essential neurotrophin in this process (Notaras and van den Buuse, 2019). In this sense, a previous study observed that a 3-month HIFT program significantly increased resting BDNF levels in young adults (Murawska-Cialowicz et al., 2015). The chronic changes in the brain must be differentiated from the acute effects. It seems that the benefits of exercise on complex RT may, as an acute response, be mediated by transient phenomena such as the release of catecholamines at the central level (Meeusen and De Meirleir, 1995), which modulate information processing (Robbins and Everitt, 1995).

However, we still do not entirely understand the mechanism related to cognitive improvement (Stillman et al., 2020). In this way, Stillman et al. (2016) proposed a multilevel system to study the underlying changes in cognitive functioning. Thus, cognitive changes can be produced by (a) molecular and cellular modifications (level 1), (b) structural and functional brain changes (level 2), and (c) behavioral and socioemotional changes (level 3). However, traditionally, the literature has focused on the first two levels, and there is a lack of evidence to understand the relations between behavioral and socioemotional changes with cognitive functioning (Stillman et al., 2016). Therefore, our study aimed to analyze the relationship between cognitive functioning and wellbeing, focusing on level 3 changes. Our hypothesis considered that hedonic and eudemonic variables of wellbeing would improve together with the RT variables.

However, the study results show that neither a 5-week HIFT supervised training program nor the online training during the COVID-19 quarantine affected this variable. Regarding the effect of training on these variables, the data contradict previous reports in the literature where acute, chronic PE and PT have been considered an effective tool to improve the wellbeing of young students (Cervelló et al., 2014; Garcia et al., 2015; Eather et al., 2019). However, some studies have found that high-intensity training did not cause significant changes in the wellbeing of this same population (Costigan et al., 2016). Moreover, data suggests an attenuated impact of the COVID-19 quarantine on the wellbeing variables in young students who presumably would not be burdened with responsibilities to the same degree as the adult population and thus experience less emotional distress (Brooks et al., 2020).

It has been shown that changes in cognitive functioning could be influenced by aspects of supervised training linked to mood and social relationships [could be related to level 3, as was proposed by Stillman et al. (2016)], such as (a) creating a structured and collaborative environment that favors positive reinforcement by peers and coaches; (b) a positive perception of social support; (c) achievement of goals; and (d) positive group dynamics (Lieberman et al., 2014). In agreement with these studies, our cognitive functioning improvements were higher during the supervised training than during the online training. However, improvements in processing speed were sustained through online training during the lockdown, unaffected by the reduction in adherence and social interactions. PE was probably sufficient to maintain unaltered mood and wellbeing, and the changes in processing speed could have been mediated by level 1 and 2 mechanisms (Stillman et al., 2016).

Although wellbeing was not modified due to the lockdown, significant differences in adherence were observed between the supervised and online phases. Self-determination theory (SDT) can explain the decrease in adherence (Deci and Ryan, 1985, 2000). SDT describes three psychological needs (autonomy, competence, and relatedness) that must be satisfied to increase the intrinsic motivation for a given activity. In our online training program, some of these needs were not satisfied, such as relatedness (Deci and Ryan, 1985) with peers and coaches, which has a significant impact on adherence to exercise (Kang et al., 2019), or the supervision of qualified professionals (Garber et al., 2011). In addition, recent studies have shown that people who reduced their PA during lockdown experienced decreased wellbeing (Brand et al., 2020; Faulkner et al., 2021). However, in our results, despite the correlation between positive and negative affect with vitality, the latter was not reduced in parallel with a decrease in adherence. Moreover, CRF is related to vitality in young people (Eddolls et al., 2018), and CRF can be maintained with a lower volume of exercise if the intensity of the activity is conserved (Spiering et al., 2021). Given that there were no changes in the exercise intensity throughout the training program, the reduction in adherence in this study probably did not affect CRF, and hence, vitality was not altered.

Our study demonstrates that HIFT can improve cognitive functioning in young university students and could be successfully applied through online training under lockdown circumstances, maintaining the exercise intensity and improving cognitive functioning. Moreover, as we have demonstrated, it is possible to maintain the subjective exercise intensity (measured by RPE) of the HIFT training in the online approach, with no adverse changes in wellbeing. However, if we want to use this methodology in future lockdowns, it would be interesting to analyze the lockdown training protocols from the SDT perspective, trying to overcome psychological needs deficiencies during the quarantine periods.

Finally, our study did not find any sex differences in the analyses performed. A previous meta-analysis showed discrepancies in sex differences. For example, Barha et al. (2017) found that sex was a strong moderator in the exercise-cognition relationship in their meta-analysis, with more pronounced improvements for women. However, Falck et al. (2019) did not find this relationship in their meta-analysis. In any case, both meta-analyses were done in older people. Some old studies showed that older women participated in lower leisure-time PA than older men (Lee, 2005), or reported lower frequencies of physical activities (Kaplan et al., 2001). In addition, sedentarism has a worse cognitive impact in women than in men (Fagot et al., 2019). Thus, Barha and Liu-Ambrose (2018) proposed that the increase in daily PA could be greater for older women than for older men, and this difference could promote more significant cognitive improvements in women. However, recent studies have shown that, nowadays, older women are less sedentary and do more PA than men (Wennman et al., 2019; Lopez-Valenciano et al., 2020). Suppose PA determines the cognitive functioning improvements after a PT period. In that case, present and future research will find a sex gap with better improvements in older men.

Regarding this study, we do not know any review of sex differences in young university students on the exercise-cognition relationship. Thus, according to the results of our study, sex does not moderate the improvement in processing speed after chronic exercise in young people.

This study investigated whether, in young adults, supervised and unsupervised HIFT can influence wellbeing and cognitive functioning, as empirical evidence regarding the effect of PEs on wellbeing and cognitive functioning is scant in this age group (Stillman et al., 2020). However, we did not find evidence that exercise-related changes in socioemotional parameters are associated with alteration of cognitive functioning, as previously proposed (Stillman et al., 2016). We described our training methodology with HR records and used ventilatory thresholds to control the exercise load. The implications that arise from the findings of our study broaden our knowledge on the influence of HIFT on cognitive functioning and wellbeing in younger adults.

Limitations

As a result of lockdown, the main limitation to this study was that it was impossible to carry out post-training fitness tests, so we do not know if the observed changes in cognitive functioning are related to improvements in fitness. Therefore, it would be interesting for future research to look into the implicit mechanisms related to physical fitness that could potentially explain improvements in complex RT. Moreover, the study did not employ allocation concealment, which indicates a possible source of bias.

Conclusion

On the one hand, HIFT training improves processing speed, measured through two complex RT tasks (CRT and InT) in young college students, without mediation by changes in psychological wellbeing. Also, it was observed that a training program during a quarantine period, forced by the global COVID-19 pandemic, promoted the maintenance of RT improvements previously achieved during a supervised training program. On the other hand, it was observed that this situation did not have adverse effects on this population’s wellbeing. However, it negatively affected adherence to the online training program, possibly because of the suppression of typical factors linked to training supervision and interactions with peers and coaches.

Data Availability Statement

The raw data supporting the conclusions of this article will be made available by the authors, without undue reservation.

Ethics Statement

The studies involving human participants were reviewed and approved by the Miguel Hernández University Ethics Committee (UMH.CID.DPC.02.17). The patients/participants provided their written informed consent to participate in this study.

Author Contributions

JB-F, LC-H, DP, and EC contributed to the conception and design of the study. JB-F organized the database. JB-F, LC-H, and DP performed the statistical analysis and wrote the final manuscript. JB-F and LC-H wrote the first draft of the manuscript. All authors contributed to manuscript revision, read, and approved the submitted version.

Funding

This work was supported by Grant RTI2018-098335-B-I00 funded by MCIN/AEI/10.13039/501100011033/ “ERDF A way of making Europe.”

Conflict of Interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher’s Note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

Footnotes

  1. ^ http://www.gpower.hhu.de/

References

AppAndAbout (2019). Aprenvida App. AppAndAbout. https://play.google.com/store/apps/details?id=com.appandabout.aprenvidaapp (accessed December 15, 2019).

Google Scholar

Ashnagar, Z., Shadmehr, A., and Jalaei, S. (2015). The effects of acute bout of cycling on auditory & visual reaction times. J. Bodyw. Mov. Ther. 19, 268–272. doi: 10.1016/j.jbmt.2014.05.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Audiffren, M., Tomporowski, P. D., and Zagrodnik, J. (2008). Acute aerobic exercise and information processing: energizing motor processes during a choice reaction time task. Acta. Psychol. 129, 410–419. doi: 10.1016/j.actpsy.2008.09.006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Barha, C. K., Davis, J. C., Falck, R. S., Nagamatsu, L. S., and Liu-Ambrose, T. (2017). Sex differences in exercise efficacy to improve cognition: a systematic review and meta-analysis of randomized controlled trials in older humans. Front. Neuroendocrinol. 46:71–85. doi: 10.1016/j.yfrne.2017.04.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Barha, C. K., and Liu-Ambrose, T. (2018). Exercise and the aging brain: considerations for sex differences. Brain Plast. 4, 53–63. doi: 10.3233/BPL-180067

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Brand, R., Timme, S., and Nosrat, S. (2020). When pandemic hits: exercise frequency and subjective well-being during COVID-19 Pandemic. Front. Psychol. 11:570567. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2020.570567

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Brooks, S. K., Webster, R. K., Smith, L. E., Woodland, L., Wessely, S., Greenberg, N., et al. (2020). The psychological impact of quarantine and how to reduce it: rapid review of the evidence. Lancet 395, 912–920. doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736(20)30460-8

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Burke, T., Berry, A., Taylor, L. K., Stafford, O., Murphy, E., Shevlin, M., et al. (2020). Increased psychological distress during COVID-19 and quarantine in Ireland: a national survey. J. Clin. Med. 9:3481. doi: 10.3390/jcm9113481

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Carbonell-Hernández, L., Cervello, E., and Pastor, D. (2019). Two months of combined physical exercise and cognitive training, over cognitive function in older adults: the results of the memtrain project pilot program. Eur. J. Hum. Mov. 42, 59–75.

Google Scholar

Cervelló, E., Peruyero, F., Montero, C., González-Cutre, D., Beltrán-Carrillo, V. J., and Moreno-Murcia, J. A. (2014). Exercise, psychological well-being, sleep quality and situational motivation in physical education students. Cuad. Psicol. 14, 31–38.

Google Scholar

Chang, Y. K., and Etnier, J. L. (2009). Exploring the dose-response relationship between resistance exercise intensity and cognitive function. J. Sport Exerc. Psychol. 31:640.

Google Scholar

Chang, Y. K., Labban, J. D., Gapin, J. I., and Etnier, J. L. (2012). The effects of acute exercise on cognitive performance: a meta-analysis. Brain Res. 1453, 87–101. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2012.02.068

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cohen, J. (1992). Statistical power analysis. Curr. Dir. Psychol. Sci. 1, 98–101. doi: 10.1111/1467-8721.ep10768783

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Colcombe, S., and Kramer, A. F. (2003). Fitness effects on the cognitive function of older adults: a meta-analytic study. Psychol. Sci. 14, 125–130. doi: 10.1111/1467-9280.t01-1-01430

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Costigan, S. A., Eather, N., Plotnikoff, R. C., Hillman, C. H., and Lubans, D. R. (2016). High-intensity interval training on cognitive and mental health in adolescents. Med. Sci. Sports Exerc. 48, 1985–1993.

Google Scholar

Deary, I. J., Johnson, W., and Starr, J. M. (2010). Are processing speed tasks biomarkers of cognitive aging? Psychol. Aging 25, 219–228. doi: 10.1037/a0017750

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Deci, E., and Ryan, R. (1985). Intrıinsic Motivation And Self-Deternination In Human Behaviour. New York:NY: Springer.

Google Scholar

Deci, E. L., and Ryan, R. M. (2000). The “what” and “why” of goal pursuits: human needs and the self-determination of behavior. Psychol. Inq. 11, 227–268. doi: 10.1207/S15327965PLI1104_01

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dragun, R., Veček, N. N., Marendić, M., Pribisalić, A., Ðivić, G., Cena, H., et al. (2021). Have lifestyle habits and psychological well-being changed among adolescents and medical students due to COVID-19 lockdown in croatia? Nutrients 13:97.

Google Scholar

Dwyer, G. B., Davis, S. E., and Medicine, A.C.o.S (2005). ACSM’s Health-Related Physical Fitness Assessment Manual. Philadelphia: Lippincott Williams & Wilkins.

Google Scholar

Eather, N., Riley, N., Miller, A., Smith, V., Poole, A., Vincze, L., et al. (2019). Efficacy and feasibility of HIIT training for university students: the uni-hiit rct. J. Sci. Med. Sport 22, 596–601.

Google Scholar

Eddolls, W. T. B., McNarry, M. A., Lester, L., Winn, C. O. N., Stratton, G., and Mackintosh, K. A. (2018). The association between physical activity, fitness and body mass index on mental well-being and quality of life in adolescents. Qual. Life Res. 27, 2313–2320. doi: 10.1007/s11136-018-1915-3

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Erickson, K. I., Hillman, C., Stillman, C. M., Ballard, R. M., Bloodgood, B., Conroy, D. E., et al. (2019). Physical activity, cognition, and brain outcomes: a review of the 2018 physical activity guidelines. Med. Sci. Sports Exerc. 51, 1242–1251.

Google Scholar

Erickson, K. I., Voss, M. W., Prakash, R. S., Basak, C., Szabo, A., Chaddock, L., et al. (2011). Exercise training increases size of hippocampus and improves memory. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 108, 3017–3022. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1015950108

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fagot, D., Chicherio, C., Albinet, C. T., Andre, N., and Audiffren, M. (2019). The impact of physical activity and sex differences on intraindividual variability in inhibitory performance in older adults. Neuropsychol. Dev. Cogn. B Aging Neuropsychol. Cogn. 26, 1–23. doi: 10.1080/13825585.2017.1372357

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Falck, R. S., Davis, J. C., Best, J. R., Crockett, R. A., and Liu-Ambrose, T. (2019). Impact of exercise training on physical and cognitive function among older adults: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Neurobiol. Aging 79, 119–130. doi: 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2019.03.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Faulkner, J., O’Brien, W. J., McGrane, B., Wadsworth, D., Batten, J., Askew, C. D., et al. (2021). Physical activity, mental health and well-being of adults during initial COVID-19 containment strategies: a multi-country cross-sectional analysis. J. Sci. Med. Sport 24, 320–326. doi: 10.1016/j.jsams.2020.11.016

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Feito, Y., Heinrich, K. M., Butcher, S. J., and Poston, W. S. C. (2018). High-intensity functional training (HIFT): definition and research implications for improved fitness. Sports 6:76. doi: 10.3390/sports6030076

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ferguson, B. S., Rogatzki, M. J., Goodwin, M. L., Kane, D. A., Rightmire, Z., and Gladden, L. B. (2018). Lactate metabolism: historical context, prior misinterpretations, and current understanding. Eur. J. Appl. Physiol. 118, 691–728. doi: 10.1007/s00421-017-3795-6

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ferris, L. T., Williams, J. S., and Shen, C. L. (2007). The effect of acute exercise on serum brain-derived neurotrophic factor levels and cognitive function. Med. Sci. Sports Exerc. 39, 728–734. doi: 10.1249/mss.0b013e31802f04c7

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Foster, J. P., Carl, H., Kara, M., Esten, P. L., and Brice, G. (2001). Differences in perceptions of training by coaches and athletes. S. Afr. J. Sports Med. 8, 3–7.

Google Scholar

Garber, C. E., Blissmer, B., Deschenes, M. R., Franklin, B. A., Lamonte, M. J., Lee, I. M., et al. (2011). American college of sports medicine position stand. Med. Sci. Sports Exerc. 43, 1334–1359. doi: 10.1249/MSS.0b013e318213fefb

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Garcia, D., Jimmefors, A., Mousavi, F., Adrianson, L., Rosenberg, P., and Archer, T. (2015). Self-regulatory mode (locomotion and assessment), well-being (subjective and psychological), and exercise behavior (frequency and intensity) in relation to high school pupils’ academic achievement. Peer. J. 3:e847. doi: 10.7717/peerj.847

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gunning-Dixon, F. M., Brickman, A. M., Cheng, J. C., and Alexopoulos, G. S. (2009). Aging of cerebral white matter: a review of MRI findings. Int. J. Geriatr. Psychiatry 24, 109–117. doi: 10.1002/gps.2087

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hashimoto, T., Tsukamoto, H., Ando, S., and Ogoh, S. (2021). Effect of exercise on brain health: the potential role of lactate as a myokine. Metabolites 11:813. doi: 10.3390/metabo11120813

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Heinrich, K. M., Patel, P. M., O’Neal, J. L., and Heinrich, B. S. (2014). High-intensity compared to moderate-intensity training for exercise initiation, enjoyment, adherence, and intentions: an intervention study. BMC Public. Health 14:789. doi: 10.1186/1471-2458-14-789

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Herold, F., Muller, P., Gronwald, T., and Muller, N. G. (2019). Dose-response matters! - a Perspective on the exercise prescription in exercise-cognition research. Front. Psychol. 10:2338. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2019.02338

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ide, B. N., Marocolo, M., Santos, C. P. C., Silva, B. V. C., Silvatti, A. P., Simim, M. A. M., et al. (2021). Commentary: “you’re only as strong as your weakest link”: a current opinion about the concepts and characteristics of functional training. Front. Physiol. 12:744144. doi: 10.3389/fphys.2021.744144

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jeon, Y. K., and Ha, C. H. (2017). The effect of exercise intensity on brain derived neurotrophic factor and memory in adolescents. Environ. Health Prev. Med. 22:27. doi: 10.1186/s12199-017-0643-6

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jiménez-Pavón, D., Carbonell-Baeza, A., and Lavie, C. J. (2020). Physical exercise as therapy to fight against the mental and physical consequences of COVID-19 quarantine: special focus in older people. Prog. Cardiovasc. Dis. 63, 386–388. doi: 10.1016/j.pcad.2020.03.009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kang, S., Lee, K., and Kwon, S. (2019). Basic psychological needs, exercise intention and sport commitment as predictors of recreational sport participants’ exercise adherence. Psychology 35, 916–932.

Google Scholar

Kaplan, M. S., Newsom, J. T., McFarland, B. H., and Lu, L. (2001). Demographic and psychosocial correlates of physical activity in late life. Am. J. Prev. Med. 21, 306–312. doi: 10.1016/s0749-3797(01)00364-6

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Korhonen, J., Linnanmäki, K., and Aunio, P. (2014). Learning difficulties, academic well-being and educational dropout: a person-centred approach. Learn. Individ. Differ. 31, 1–10.

Google Scholar

Landrigan, J. F., Bell, T., Crowe, M., Clay, O. J., and Mirman, D. (2020). Lifting cognition: a meta-analysis of effects of resistance exercise on cognition. Psychol. Res. 84, 1167–1183. doi: 10.1007/s00426-019-01145-x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lee, Y. S. (2005). Gender differences in physical activity and walking among older adults. J. Women Aging 17, 55–70. doi: 10.1300/J074v17n01_05

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Leuchter, R. K., Stuber, M. L., McDonald, A. L., and Croymans, D. M. (2022). Relationship between exercise intensity and stress levels among U.S. medical students. Med. Educ. Online 27:2027651. doi: 10.1080/10872981.2022.2027651

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lieberman, H. R., Karl, J. P., Niro, P. J., Williams, K. W., Farina, E. K., Cable, S. J., et al. (2014). Positive effects of basic training on cognitive performance and mood of adult females. Hum. Factors 56, 1113–1123.

Google Scholar

Lopez-Valenciano, A., Mayo, X., Liguori, G., Copeland, R. J., Lamb, M., and Jimenez, A. (2020). Changes in sedentary behaviour in European union adults between 2002 and 2017. BMC Public. Health 20:1206. doi: 10.1186/s12889-020-09293-1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lucía, A., Hoyos, J., Carvajal, A., and Chicharro, J. L. (1999). Heart rate response to professional road cycling: the tour de france. Int. J. Sports Med. 20, 167–172. doi: 10.1055/s-2007-971112

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Luque-Casado, A., Zabala, M., Morales, E., Mateo-March, M., and Sanabria, D. (2013). Cognitive performance and heart rate variability: the influence of fitness level. PLoS One 8:e56935. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0056935

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mackinnon, A., Jorm, A. F., Christensen, H., Korten, A. E., Jacomb, P. A., and Rodgers, B. (1999). A short form of the positive and negative affect schedule: evaluation of factorial validity and invariance across demographic variables in a community sample. Pers. Individ. 27, 405–416. doi: 10.1016/S0191-8869(98)00251-7

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Maghsoudipour, M., Shabani, H., Najafabadi, M. G., Bakhshi, E., and Coh, P. (2018). The relationship between emotional intelligence, reaction time, aerobic capacity and performance in female track and field athletes at the universities of tehran. Work 61, 173–179. doi: 10.3233/WOR-182790

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Magill, R. (2010). Motor Learning and Control. New York:NY: McGraw-Hill Publishing.

Google Scholar

Marques, A., Santos, D. A., Hillman, C. H., and Sardinha, L. B. (2018). How does academic achievement relate to cardiorespiratory fitness, self-reported physical activity and objectively reported physical activity: a systematic review in children and adolescents aged 6–18 years. Br. J. Sports Med. 52, 1039–1039. doi: 10.1136/bjsports-2016-097361

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

McMahon, E. M., Corcoran, P., O’Regan, G., Keeley, H., Cannon, M., Carli, V., et al. (2017). Physical activity in European adolescents and associations with anxiety, depression and well-being. Eur. Child Adolesc. Psychiatry 26, 111–122. doi: 10.1007/s00787-016-0875-9

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Meeusen, R., and De Meirleir, K. (1995). Exercise and brain neurotransmission. Sports Med. 20, 160–188. doi: 10.2165/00007256-199520030-00004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Miret, M., Cabello, M., Marchena, C., Mellor-Marsá, B., Caballero, F. F., Obradors-Tarragó, C., et al. (2015). The state of the art on European well-being research within the area of mental health. Int. J. Clin. Health Psychol. 15, 171–179. doi: 10.1016/j.ijchp.2015.02.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Molina-García, J., Castillo, I., and Pablos, C. (2007). Bienestar psicológico y práctica deportiva en universitarios. Eur. J. Hum. Mov. 18, 79–91.

Google Scholar

Moradi, A., Sadri Damirchi, E., Narimani, M., Esmaeilzadeh, S., Dziembowska, I., Azevedo, L. B., et al. (2019). Association between physical and motor fitness with cognition in children. Medicina 55:7.

Google Scholar

Mulder, M. J., and van Maanen, L. (2013). Are accuracy and reaction time affected via different processes? PLoS One 8:e80222. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0080222

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Murawska-Cialowicz, E., Wojna, J., and Zuwala-Jagiello, J. (2015). Crossfit training changes brain-derived neurotrophic factor and irisin levels at rest, after wingate and progressive tests, and improves aerobic capacity and body composition of young physically active men and women. J. Physiol. Pharmacol. 66, 811–821.

Google Scholar

Neubauer, A. C. (1990). Selective reaction times and intelligence. Intelligence 14, 79–96. doi: 10.1016/0160-2896(90)90015-L

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Notaras, M., and van den Buuse, M. (2019). Brain-derived neurotrophic factor (BDNF): novel insights into regulation and genetic variation. Neuroscientist 25, 434–454. doi: 10.1177/1073858418810142

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ozyemisci-Taskiran, O., Gunendi, Z., Bolukbasi, N., and Beyazova, M. (2008). The effect of a single session submaximal aerobic exercise on premotor fraction of reaction time: an electromyographic study. Clin. Biomech. 23, 231–235. doi: 10.1016/j.clinbiomech.2007.08.027

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pastor, D., Cervelló, E., Peruyero, F., Biddle, S., and Montero, C. (2019). Acute physical exercise intensity, cognitive inhibition and psychological well-being in adolescent physical education students. Curr. Psychol. 40, 5030–5039. doi: 10.1007/s12144-019-00454-z

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Peruyero, F., Zapata, J., Pastor, D., and Cervello, E. (2017). The acute effects of exercise intensity on inhibitory cognitive control in adolescents. Front. Psychol. 8:921. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2017.00921

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pettitt, R. W., Clark, I. E., Ebner, S. M., Sedgeman, D. T., and Murray, S. R. (2013). Gas exchange threshold and v[combining dot above]o2max testing for athletes: an update. J. Strength. Cond. Res. 27, 549–555. doi: 10.1519/JSC.0b013e31825770d7

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pontifex, M. B., Parks, A. C., Henning, D. A., and Kamijo, K. (2015). Single bouts of exercise selectively sustain attentional processes. Psychophysiology 52, 618–625. doi: 10.1111/psyp.12395

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Posner, M. I., and McLeod, P. (1982). Information processing models-in search of elementary operations. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 33, 477–514.

Google Scholar

Prabhavathi, K., Hemamalini, R. V., Thilip, K. G., Christopher, A., Maruthy, K. N., and Saravanan, A. (2017). A correlational study of visual and auditory reaction time with their academic performance among the first year medical students. Natl. J. Physiol. Pharm. Pharmacol. 7, 371–374. doi: 10.5455/njppp.2017.7.1131828112016

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Robbins, T. W., and Everitt, B. J. (1995). “Arousal systems and attention,” in The Cognitive Neurosciences, ed. M. S. Gazzaniga (Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press), 703–720.

Google Scholar

Ryan, R. M., and Frederick, C. (1997). On energy, personality, and health: subjective vitality as a dynamic reflection of well-being. J. Pers 65, 529–565.

Google Scholar

Ryan, R. M., Huta, V., and Deci, E. (2013). “Living well: a self-determination perspective on eudaumonic,” in The Exploration Of Happiness, Happiness Studies Book Series, ed. A. Delle-Fave (Netherlands: Springer), 117–139.

Google Scholar

Saucedo Marquez, C. M., Vanaudenaerde, B., Troosters, T., and Wenderoth, N. (2015). High-intensity interval training evokes larger serum BDNF levels compared with intense continuous exercise. J. Appl. Physiol. 119, 1363–1373. doi: 10.1152/japplphysiol.00126.2015

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Shimizu, M., Misumi, M., Yamada, M., Ohishi, W., Yamamoto, H., and Kihara, Y. (2018). Choice reaction time and grip strength as predictors of cardiovascular mortality in middle-aged and elderly japanese: from the radiation effects research foundation adult health study. Intern. Med. J. 48, 1331–1336. doi: 10.1111/imj.14002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Soga, K., Masaki, H., Gerber, M., and Ludyga, S. (2018). Acute and Long-term effects of resistance training on executive function. J. Cogn. Enhanc. 2, 200–207. doi: 10.1007/s41465-018-0079-y

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Spiering, B. A., Mujika, I., Sharp, M. A., and Foulis, S. A. (2021). Maintaining physical performance: the minimal dose of exercise needed to preserve endurance and strength over time. J. Strength. Cond. Res. 35, 1449–1458. doi: 10.1519/JSC.0000000000003964

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Stillman, C. M., Cohen, J., Lehman, M. E., and Erickson, K. I. (2016). Mediators of physical activity on neurocognitive function: a review at multiple levels of analysis. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 10:626. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2016.00626

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Stillman, C. M., Esteban-Cornejo, I., Brown, B., Bender, C. M., and Erickson, K. I. (2020). Effects of Exercise on brain and cognition across age groups and health states. Trends Neurosci. 43, 533–543. doi: 10.1016/j.tins.2020.04.010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tsai, C. L., Chen, F. C., Pan, C. Y., Wang, C. H., Huang, T. H., and Chen, T. C. (2014). Impact of acute aerobic exercise and cardiorespiratory fitness on visuospatial attention performance and serum BDNF levels. Psychoneuroendocrinology 41, 121–131. doi: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2013.12.014

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

van Praag, H., Shubert, T., Zhao, C., and Gage, F. H. (2005). Exercise enhances learning and hippocampal neurogenesis in aged mice. J. Neurosci. 25, 8680–8685. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1731-05.2005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Walsh, E. I., Smith, L., Northey, J., Rattray, B., and Cherbuin, N. (2020). Towards an understanding of the physical activity-BDNF-cognition triumvirate: a review of associations and dosage. Ageing Res. Rev. 60:101044.

Google Scholar

Wang, C.-H., Liang, W.-K., Tseng, P., Muggleton, N. G., Juan, C.-H., and Tsai, C.-L. (2015). The relationship between aerobic fitness and neural oscillations during visuo-spatial attention in young adults. Exp. Brain Res. 233, 1069–1078. doi: 10.1007/s00221-014-4182-8

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wells, A. J., Hoffman, J. R., Beyer, K. S., Jajtner, A. R., Gonzalez, A. M., Townsend, J. R., et al. (2014). Reliability of the dynavision d2 for assessing reaction time performance. J. sports sci. med. 13, 145–150.

Google Scholar

Wennman, H., Pietila, A., Rissanen, H., Valkeinen, H., Partonen, T., Maki-Opas, T., et al. (2019). Gender, age and socioeconomic variation in 24-hour physical activity by wrist-worn accelerometers: the finhealth 2017 survey. Sci. Rep. 9:6534. doi: 10.1038/s41598-019-43007-x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: reaction-time, exercise, COVID-19, wellbeing, HIFT, quarantine

Citation: Ballester-Ferrer JA, Carbonell-Hernández L, Pastor D and Cervelló E (2022) COVID-19 Quarantine Impact on Wellbeing and Cognitive Functioning During a 10-Week High-Intensity Functional Training Program in Young University Students. Front. Behav. Neurosci. 16:822199. doi: 10.3389/fnbeh.2022.822199

Received: 25 November 2021; Accepted: 10 March 2022;
Published: 08 April 2022.

Edited by:

Toru Ishihara, Kobe University, Japan

Reviewed by:

Fabian Herold, German Center for Neurodegeneratives, Helmholtz Association of German Research Centers (HZ), Germany
Francisco Javier Santos-Rosa, Universidad Pablo de Olavide, Spain

Copyright © 2022 Ballester-Ferrer, Carbonell-Hernández, Pastor and Cervelló. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Diego Pastor, ZHBhc3RvckB1bWguZXM=

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.