- 1Leibniz Institute for Neurobiology, Magdeburg, Germany
- 2Graduate School of Systemic Neurosciences, Munich, Germany
- 3Max Planck Institute of Psychiatry, Munich, Germany
- 4International Max Planck Research School for Translational Psychiatry, Munich, Germany
- 5Weizmann Institute of Science, Rehovot, Israel
Subpopulations of neurons display increased activity during memory encoding and manipulating the activity of these neurons can induce artificial formation or erasure of memories. Thus, these neurons are thought to be cellular engrams. Moreover, correlated activity between pre- and postsynaptic engram neurons is thought to lead to strengthening of their synaptic connections, thus increasing the probability of neural activity patterns occurring during encoding to reoccur at recall. Therefore, synapses between engram neurons can also be considered as a substrate of memory, or a synaptic engram. One can label synaptic engrams by targeting two complementary, non-fluorescent, synapse-targeted GFP fragments separately to the pre- and postsynaptic compartment of engram neurons; the two GFP fragments reconstitute a fluorescent GFP at the synaptic cleft between the engram neurons, thereby highlighting synaptic engrams. In this work we explored a transsynaptic GFP reconstitution system (mGRASP) to label synaptic engrams between hippocampal CA1 and CA3 engram neurons identified by different Immediate-Early Genes: cFos and Arc. We characterized the expression of the cellular and synaptic labels of the mGRASP system upon exposure to a novel environment or learning of a hippocampal-dependent memory task. We found that mGRASP under the control of transgenic ArcCreERT2 labeled synaptic engrams more efficiently than when controlled by viral cFostTA, possibly due to differences in the genetic systems rather than the specific IEG promoters.
Introduction
Memories are thought to be encoded as enduring physical changes in the brain. In fact, in murine models, not only subpopulations of neurons throughout various brain regions show increased neuronal activity during memory formation, but manipulation of the activity of these neurons can induce artificial retrieval or loss of stored memories (Zhou et al., 2009; Liu et al., 2012; Ramirez et al., 2013; Denny et al., 2014; Tanaka et al., 2014a; Vetere et al., 2017; Josselyn and Tonegawa, 2020). This demonstrates that memory storage and retrieval are mediated by subpopulations of neurons which are thus believed to be cellular engrams. According to the Hebbian postulate, connections between neurons with correlated activity patterns are strengthened while connections between neurons whose activity patterns are weakly correlated are depressed or even lost (Hebb, 1949). This phenomenon increases the probability of neural activity patterns occurring during encoding to re-occur at later time points. Therefore, the subset of synapses between coactive neurons can also be considered as a substrate of memory, or a synaptic engram. While an ever-increasing number of studies investigates engrams at the system and circuit levels (Reijmers et al., 2007; Liu et al., 2012; Ramirez et al., 2013, 2015; Denny et al., 2014; Hsiang et al., 2014; Kawashima et al., 2014; Redondo et al., 2014; Tanaka et al., 2014b; Rashid et al., 2016; Kitamura et al., 2017; Vetere et al., 2017; Rao-Ruiz et al., 2019; Visser et al., 2020; Domi et al., 2021), studies focusing on the synaptic level are scarce. This is because investigating the activity of defined synapses and tracking the changes in these synapses through time is technically challenging. Thus, it remains unclear whether memory formation truly enhances synapses between neurons of connected brain regions.
Studying the stability of structural synaptic connectivity as a proxy for strength of synaptic activity helps circumvent this problem. Stability of dendritic spines in rodents is associated with memory formation and recall (Trachtenberg et al., 2002; Zuo et al., 2005a,b; Xu et al., 2009; Yang et al., 2009, 2016; Fu et al., 2012; Attardo et al., 2015; Castello-Waldow et al., 2020; Chenani et al., 2022; Gallinaro et al., 2022) and increasing stability of neocortical dendritic spines enhances learning while decreasing the size of neocortical dendritic spines leads to impaired performance in a motor task (Hayashi-Takagi et al., 2015; Albarran et al., 2021). Recently, it has become possible to label synapses between neurons, thanks to the GFP Reconstitution Across Synaptic Partners [GRASP (Feinberg et al., 2008)] and its homologous optimized for mammalian expression [mGRASP (Kim et al., 2011; Druckmann et al., 2014)] techniques. Both GRASP and mGRASP use two complementary, non-fluorescent GFP fragments, which are expressed separately on pre- and postsynaptic membranes and reconstitute in the synaptic cleft to form functional GFP, thus pinpointing synapses between specific pre- and postsynaptic neurons. More recently, an enhanced version of the mGRASP system (eGRASP) was targeted to pre- and postsynaptic engram neurons with a genetic method based on the Immediate Early Gene (IEG) cFos (Reijmers et al., 2007) and enabled to visualize the engram at the synaptic level (Choi et al., 2018, 2021; Choi and Kaang, 2022). These advancements could enable to visualize synaptic engrams in the live mouse by using intravital two-photon microscopy and the mGRASP system seems to be better suited to this aim given the lower number of fluorescent proteins involved and their better spectral separation. We thus decide to test whether the mGRASP system could also be used to label synaptic engrams when driven by the IEG cFos. The genetic system based on the IEG cFos, however, provides only transient labeling and it is unclear whether other genetic schemes based on other IEGs (Kawashima et al., 2009, 2013; Guenthner et al., 2013; Sørensen et al., 2016), could improve labeling of structural synaptic engrams. We thus also investigated the use of mGRASP system in the dorsal hippocampus of mice under the control of two different IEG promotors commonly used to identify neuronal engrams: cFos and Arc. We marked cFos-expressing cells by using Adeno-Associated Viruses (AAVs) in which the cFos promoter drives the transcription of a tetracycline Trans Activator (tTA) (Zhang et al., 2015), which in turn drives the transcription of mGRASP. To detect Arc-expressing cells we employed a transgenic mouse line in which the endogenous Arc promoter drives the transcription of a Cre recombinase whose activity is gated by Tamoxifen (CreERT2) (Guenthner et al., 2013), which in turn drives the transcription of mGRASP. We then characterized the expression of the cellular and synaptic labels of the mGRASP system upon exposure to an Enriched Environment (EE) or upon learning of the hippocampal-dependent memory task Trace Fear Conditioning (TFC). mGRASP under the control of transgenic ArcCreERT2 labeled synapses between CA1 and CA3 pyramidal neurons active during EE and TFC more efficiently than when controlled by viral cFostTA. However, we think this difference reflects the difference between the genetic systems we employ rather than between the IEG promoters.
Materials and methods
Subjects
Animals were C57Bl6/N bred in house (for cFostTA-dependent expression), ArcCreERT2-Ai9 double transgenic or ArcCreERT2 single transgenic on C57Bl6/N background (for ArcCreERT2-dependent expression) male and female mice between 3 and 6 months of age with free access to food and water and a 12/12 light/dark cycle. All animal procedures conformed to the guidelines of the Max Planck Society and the local animal authority (Regierung von Oberbayern – Veterinärwesen) and were approved in the License for animal experimentation # ROB-55.2Vet-2532.Vet_02-17-150.
Viral injections
Intracranial injections of Adeno Associated Viral suspensions were carried out according to standard methods. We injected 200–400 nL of a viral suspension (see Supplementary Table 1 for details about viruses) at a rate of 100 nL/min in dCA1 (AP, –2.0 mm; ML, 1.4; DV, 1.4 mm) or in dCA3 (AP, –2.0 mm; ML, 2.2; DV, 2.2 mm). Mice were allowed to recover for a minimum of 10 days.
Plasmid and virus production
To produce the TRE-pre-mGRASP construct, an AAV vector backbone with the TRE promoter was obtained from pAAV-TRE-tdTomato-WPRE (#104112, Addgene) via EcoRI and HindIII digestion. The pre-mGRASP-mCerulean encoding sequence was amplified from pAAV-CAG-pre-mGRASP-mCerulean (#34910, Addgene) by polymerase chain reaction (PCR) using two oligonucleotide primers containing EcoRI and HindIII restriction sites (5′-caagaattcATGCCACCTTCTACTAGTC-3′ and 5′- cacaagcttTCACTTGTACAGCTCATC-3′) and inserted into the pAAV-TRE backbone, after digestion with EcoRI and HindIII. For TRE-Post-mGRASP-2A-tdTomato, the Post-mGRASP-2A-tdTomato encoding sequence was amplified from pAAV-CAG-Post-mGRASP-2A-tdTomato (#34912, Addgene) by PCR using two oligonucleotide primers containing EcoRI and HindIII restriction sites (5′- caagaattcATGGCACTTCCTAGATGTATG-3′ and 5′-gatAAGCTTACTTATACAGCTCATCC-3′) and inserted into pAAV-TRE backbone, after digestion with EcoRI and HindIII. The plasmids were transformed into stbl2 E. coli (#10268019, Invitrogen) grown on Ampicillin (100 μg/ml, #A9518-5G, Sigma)–LB agar (#244520, BD DifcoTM) plates. Positive clones were sequenced (Cosmogenetech, Korea), and the results were analyzed with DNASTAR Navigator (DNASTAR, Madison, WI, USA). The viral particles were produced by the Gene Therapy Center Vector Core at the University of North Carolina at Chapel Hill, Chapel Hill, NC (UNC Vector Core) or by the Viral vector facility of the ETH (Zurich).
cFostTA-dependent labeling of neurons
The Tet-off system enables to mark cells expressing the IEG cFos only when Tetracycline or its analogous Doxycycline (DOX) is absent in the organism. Thus, immediately after viral injection, mice were switched to DOX-containing chow (200 mg. DOX/kg. chow, Bio-Serv) to prevent cFostTA-dependent expression. Three weeks after viral injection we switched the mice to normal chow and on the following day we placed mice in a novel EE for 16 h or performed TFC training. A total of 5 h after exploration of EE or TFC mice were switched back to DOX-containing chow until the time they were sacrificed.
ArcCreERT2-dependent labeling of neurons
Mice received a single intraperitoneal injection of tamoxifen (175 mg/kg of body weight) right before being placed into the EE or 30’ before TFC training. Tamoxifen was dissolved in 5% of the final volume in 100% Ethanol and further diluted with corn oil to a final concentration of 10 mg/ml. The solution was heated up to 37°C before injection. After exposure to the EE (16 h) or TFC training mice were transferred back into their HC.
Enriched environment
Enriched environments were created by connecting two rat-cages (37 cm × 60 cm) with an acrylic tunnel (20 cm × 15 cm) resulting in a total area 4,440 cm2. Enriched cages contained tunnels, wooden climbing sticks, wooden shelters, running wheels, seesaws, cotton pads, hair curlers, wooden blocks, swinging hammocks, and toys which mice could open and which contained food pellets. Cages also contained a second level connected to the ground floor by a wooden ladder and consisting of a wooden board and climbing ropes allowing mice to reach the lid grit. Food was hidden in the bedding material and spread around the arena to encourage mice to explore the environment.
Trace fear conditioning
On the training day mice were put into a square conditioning chamber (19 cm × 19 cm, black metal walls, stainless steel grid floor, white light illumination, and ethanol odor) (Panlab) which we defined as Context A. Following 3 min of habituation, mice received 3 pairings of a tone (80 dB, 9 kHz, 20 s duration, CS) and a mild electric foot shock (0.75 mA, 1 s duration, USA) with a trace of 15 s between the tone and the shock and an intra trial interval of 105 s. On probe day mice were tested for their memory recall. To test context memory recall we placed mice into Context A for 3 min. The position of the mouse was tracked automatically and the freezing response was recorded and quantified in real-time with ANY-maze (Stoelting). The amount of freezing was calculated as the percentage of total exploration time during which the mice were immobile. Immobility for more than 250 ms was scored as freezing.
Histology
We perfused mice intracardially with 1× phosphate-buffered saline (PBS) containing Heparin followed by 4% paraformaldehyde (PFA) in PBS. We then dissected the brains and placed them in 4% PFA in 1× PBS for 24 h, at 4°C. Brains were then transferred to 30% sucrose in PBS for 48 h, at 4°C. Brain slices (50 μm thick) were prepared with a vibratome (Microm HM 650 V, Thermo Scientific). Slices were permeabilized with 0.2% Triton X-100 in PBS for 1 h and later quenched with 150 mM Glycine in ddH20 for 15 min. Slices were incubated with DAPI (1:1000 in PBS, Thermo Fisher) for 5 min washed with PBS and mounted onto slides with mounting medium (Vectashield).
Quantification of the fluorescent markers
To quantify the proportion of RFP-, dTomato, or tdTomato-positive dCA1 cells we used a confocal microscope (Zeiss LSM 800) and acquired image stacks (319.28 μm2 single section area, 5 μm z-step, 8–10 focal planes) of five representative fields in the dCA1 or dCA3 per mouse using a 40× objective [Zeiss Plan-Apochromat 40×/1.4 NA Oil DIC (UV) VIS-IR]. We acquired DAPI (Thermo Fisher, 405 nm excitation and 465 nm emission wavelengths) or Syto60 (Thermo Fisher, 650 nm excitation and 680 nm emission wavelengths) fluorescence to identify neuronal nuclei and red fluorescence (561 nm excitation and 618 nm emission wavelengths) to identify RFP-, dTomato-, or tdTomato-positive cells. We then manually counted DAPI-positive, Syto60-positive and double positive cells using the ImageJ plugin Cell Counter. When the same neuron was visible in more than one z-slice we only counted it once in the z-plane in which the diameter of the soma was the largest.
To identify GFP-positive puncta we used a confocal microscope (Zeiss LSM 800) and acquired image stacks (319.28 μm2 single section area, 1–0.5 μm z-step, 8–30 focal planes) of representative fields in the dCA1 per mouse using a 63× objective [Zeiss Plan-Apochromat 60×/1.5NA Oil DIC (UV) VIS-IR].
Statistical analysis
For statistics, we used the Kruskal–Wallis test with Dunn’s correction for multiple comparisons, paired Wilcoxon Signed-rank, Mann-Whitney U-test, one-way ANOVA with Sidak’s correction for multiple comparisons and unpaired, two-tailed t-test. Prior to performing any statistical test, we tested all distributions to be tested for their likelihood of being Gaussian using the Shapiro–wilk test. We then used non-parametric tests if at least one of the distributions was not Gaussian. Statistical analysis and plotting was done with Prism 8 (GraphPad) software. *p ≤ 0.05, **p ≤ 0.01, ***p ≤ 0.001, ****p < 0.0001.
Results
cFostTA-dependent expression of mGRASP
The promoter of the IEG cFos is extensively used to mark engram neurons in mice (Reijmers et al., 2007; Liu et al., 2012; Ramirez et al., 2013, 2015; Denny et al., 2014; Kawashima et al., 2014; Redondo et al., 2014; Tanaka et al., 2014b; Kitamura et al., 2017; DeNardo et al., 2019; Visser et al., 2020; Hwang et al., 2022) and it has recently been used to label synaptic contacts between engram neurons using the eGRASP system (Choi et al., 2018, 2021; Choi and Kaang, 2022). However, it is unclear whether using other transsynaptic GFP complementation systems such as mGRASP, would also enable to label synaptic contacts between engram neurons. We thus tested whether the mGRASP system would yield labeling of synapses under the control of the cFos promoter. To this aim, we used a construct in which the cFos promoter drives expression of a tTA (Zhang et al., 2015) in combination with two viral constructs in which pre- and post-mGRASP are under the transcriptional control of the Tetracycline Responsive Element (TRE).
Kinetics of cFostTA-dependent expression in dorsal hippocampal CA1 and CA3
The tTA system leads to transient cFos-dependent gene expression and synaptic mGRASP should be most evident at the peak of cFostTA-dependent gene expression. We thus quantified the kinetics of cFostTA-dependent gene expression in dorsal hippocampal CA1 and CA3 (dCA1 and dCA3, respectively) upon exploration of an EE. To this aim, we injected the right dCA1 and left dCA3 of C57Bl6 animals each with two AAVs encoding for cFostTA and TRE-RFP (Red Fluorescent Protein), respectively (Figures 1A, B). Immediately after viral transduction, we switched the mice to DOX-containing food to prevent cFostTA-dependent RFP expression. Three weeks after viral transduction we switched the mice to normal chow to enable RFP expression and on the following day we placed five groups of mice in EE for 16 h (Figure 1C). After EE, we switched back the mice to DOX-containing food. We sacrificed different groups at different time points after induction and processed brain slices for confocal microscopy to quantify cFostTA-dependent RFP appearance in dCA1 and dCA3 (Figures 1D, E). One additional group of mice was housed in their Home Cage (HC) and served as a control for baseline expression (Figure 1C). In dCA1 the percentage of RFP-positive cells peaked at 3 days after induction (Figure 1F). In dCA3 the percentage of RFP-positive cells at 3 and 5 days post induction was significantly higher than 0 h only by pairwise comparison (Figure 1G). Altogether, these data show a trend toward peak expression for cFostTA-dependent RFP between 3 and 5 days after induction in both dCA1 and dCA3.
Figure 1. Kinetics of cFostTA-dependent expression of RFP in dorsal hippocampus. Schematic description of the viral injection sites in WT animals (A), the viral constructs injected (B), and the experimental design (C). Confocal pictures, single Z-planes, of dCA1 (D) and dCA3 (E) at different time points after induction of c-FOS-dependent expression of RFP upon exposure to EE. Red, RFP; blue, DAPI. White triangles indicate RFP-positive cells. D, Dorsal, V, Ventral. Scale bar = 20 μm. (F) The percentage of RFP-expressing over DAPI-positive cells in dCA1 at 3 days was significantly higher than 0 h and HC (p0h–24h > 0.999, *p0h–3d = 0.02, p0h–5d > 0.999, p0h–7d > 0.89, p0h–HC > 0.99; n0h = 5, n24h = 5, n3d = 7, n5d = 3, n7d = 5, nHC = 5; Kruskal–Wallis test after Dunn’s correction for multiple comparisons). (G) The percentages of RFP-expressing over DAPI-positive cells in dCA3 at 3 and 5 days were significantly higher than 0 h when compared directly (p3d–0h = 0.019 and p5d–0h = 0.035; n3d = 7, n0h = 5, nHC = 5; Mann–Whitney U-test) but showed only a trend after correction for multiple comparisons (p = 0.076; Kruskal–Wallis test after Dunn’s correction for multiple comparisons).
cFostTA-dependent expression of mGRASP upon exposure to enriched environment or trace fear conditioning labels a subset of dCA1 cells without apparent GFP reconstitution
To characterize the peak of cFostTA-dependent mGRASP expression, we quantified the kinetics of dTomato expression in the dCA1 upon exposure to EE. To this aim, we injected the right dCA1 of C57Bl6 mice with two AAVs encoding for cFostTA and TRE-post-mGRASP and the left dCA3 of the same animals with two AAVs encoding for cFostTA and TRE-pre-mGRASP (Figures 2A, B) and switched the mice to DOX-containing food. Three weeks after viral transduction we switched to normal chow and on the following day we placed mice in a novel EE for 16 h (Figure 2C). After EE, we switched the mice back to DOX-containing food. We sacrificed each group at a different time point after induction to quantify cFos-dependent dTomato appearance in dCA1 (Figure 2D). One group of mice was housed in their Home Cage (HC) and served as a control for baseline expression (Figure 2C). In dCA1 the percentage of dTomato-positive cells peaked at 3 days after induction (Figures 2D, E), consistently with the previous experiment. We also detected mCerulean-positive cells in the contralateral CA3 at 3 days after induction (Supplementary Figures 1A, B). However, we could not detect any GFP reconstitution on dendrites or somas of dTomato-expressing dCA1 neurons (Supplementary Figure 1C).
Figure 2. cFostTA-dependent expression of mGRASP in dCA1 upon exposure to an Enriched Environment or Trace Fear Conditioning. Schematic description of the viral injection sites in WT animals (A), the viral constructs injected (B) and the experimental design (C) to determine the expression kinetics of post-mGRASP in dCA1 under the control of cFos upon exposure to EE or HC. (D) Confocal pictures, single Z-planes, of dCA1 at different time points after induction of post-mGRASP expression. Red, dTomato; blue, DAPI. White triangles indicate RFP-positive cells. D, Dorsal, V, Ventral. Scale bar = 20 μm. (E) Percentage of dTomato-expressing over DAPI-positive cells in dCA1 after exposure to EE or HC. The levels of dTomato reached a peak at 3 days (p3d–24h = 0.01, p3d–5d = 0.7, p3d–7d = 0.3, p3d–10d = 0.037, p3d–HC = 0.048; all n = 5; Kruskal–Wallis test after Dunn’s correction for multiple comparisons). (F) Schematic description of the experimental design to determine the dCA1 expression levels of post-mGRASP under the control of cFos upon exposure to TFC, EE, or HC. (G) Confocal pictures, single Z-planes, of dCA1 at different time points after induction of post-mGRASP. Red, dTomato; blue, DAPI. White triangles indicate dTomato-positive cells. D, Dorsal, V, Ventral. Scale bar = 20 μm. (H) The percentage of dTomato-expressing over DAPI-positive cells in dCA1 was not significantly different after exposure to TFC, EE or HC (pTFC–HC = 0.41 and pEE–HC = 0.26, all n = 6; Kruskal–Wallis test after Dunn’s correction for multiple comparisons). (I) Percentage of freezing time during the context probe 3 days after TFC training (p = 0.03, n = 6; Wilcoxon Signed-rank test). *p < 0.05.
Next, we investigated the differences in cFostTA-driven dTomato induction upon exploration of an EE in comparison to learning of the hippocampal-dependent learning task Trace Fear Conditioning (TFC). To this aim we injected the brains of WT mice with AAVs and fed the mice with DOX-containing food as in the previous experiment (Figures 2A, B). Three weeks after viral transduction we switched to normal chow and on the following day one group of mice underwent TFC, another group of mice explored an EE for 16 h and a third group of mice was housed in HC and served as a control group (Figure 2F). The dTomato expression levels of the EE and HC mice were consistent with the previous experiment. However, we found virtually no dTomato-positive cell after TFC with only 1 out of 5 mice showing a single cell labeled in a single field of view (Figures 2G, H), despite a significant recall of the association between context and shock (Figure 2I). Under these conditions we could not detect any GFP reconstitution on dTomato-expressing dCA1 neurons.
ArcCreERT2-dependent labeling of synaptic engrams
The promoter of the IEG Arc has also been used to mark engram neurons in mice (Kawashima et al., 2009, 2013; Denny et al., 2014; Attardo et al., 2018; Castello-Waldow et al., 2020) but it has not been used to label synaptic contacts between engram neurons. We thus tested whether the mGRASP system could work under the control of Arc promotor. To this aim, we employed a transgenic mouse line previously used to label neurons active during a defined time window (Guenthner et al., 2013; Castello-Waldow et al., 2020). As in this line the promoter of the Arc gene drives expression of a Cre recombinase gated by Tamoxifen (TAM), we used viral constructs in which pre- and post-mGRASP were under the transcriptional control of Cre (Kim et al., 2011).
Kinetics of ArcCreERT2 -dependent expression in hippocampal dCA1 and dCA3
To identify the peak of Arc-dependent expression, we quantified the kinetics of tdTomato appearance in the hippocampal dCA1 and dCA3 upon exposure to EE. To this aim, we crossed the ArcCreERT2 transgenic mouse line with the Ai9 transgenic mouse line to obtain a double transgenic line (ArcCreERT2-Ai9) in which the onset of Arc-dependent tdTomato expression was gated by TAM injection (Figure 3A). We had previously determined the appearance kinetics upon a 16 h-long exposure to an EE in the dCA1 and found a significant increase in tdTomato-positive cells plateauing at 7 days after induction [Figures 3B–D, modified from Castello-Waldow et al. (2020)]. Now, we used five additional groups of the ArcCreERT2-Ai9 double transgenic mice to perform the same quantification in the dCA3. We injected a single dose of TAM intraperitoneally in all groups right before exploration of EE and quantified tdTomato appearance at different time points (Figures 3E, F). The number of tdTomato-positive cells was higher than 0 at all time points, from 24 h to 10 days, with peak expression at 10 days after induction (Figure 3G). Expression levels in dCA1 were higher than in dCA3 within the 10 days time window (Figure 3H). Altogether, these data show a peak expression for ArcCreERT2-driven tdTomato between 7 and 10 days after induction in both dCA1 and dCA3.
Figure 3. Kinetics of ArcCreERT2-dependent expression of tdTomato in dorsal hippocampus upon exposure to an Enriched Environment. (A) Schematic description of ArcCreERT2—Ai9 double transgenic animals. (B) Confocal pictures, single Z-planes, of dCA1 at different time points after induction of tdTomato expression. Red, tdTomato; blue, DAPI. White triangles indicate tdTomato-positive cells. D, Dorsal, V, Ventral. Scale bar = 20 μm. Modified from Castello-Waldow et al. (2020). (C) Percentage of tdTomato-expressing over DAPI-positive cells in dCA1 at different time points after exposure to EE. The levels of tdTomato at 7 and 10 days were significantly higher than baseline (0 h) (p7d–0h = 0.0012 and p10d–0h = 0.0007, all other p > 0.14, all n = 5; Kruskal–Wallis test after Dunn’s correction for multiple comparisons). Modified from Castello-Waldow et al. (2020). (D) Single exponential fit to the time course of ArcCreERT2-dependent tdTomato expression shown in C. Plateau = 60%, R2 = 0.96. Circles represent single datapoints (percentage of tdTomato-expressing over DAPI-positive cells in dCA1 at different time points after exposure to EE per mouse), dashed line is the best fit curve. (E) Schematic description of the experimental design to determine the expression kinetics of tdTomato in dCA3 under the control of endogenous Arc upon exposure to EE. (F) Confocal pictures, single Z-planes, of dCA3 at different time points after induction of tdTomato expression. Red, tdTomato; blue, DAPI. White triangles indicate tdTomato-positive cells. D, Dorsal, V, Ventral. Scale bar = 20 μm. (G) Percentage of tdTomato-expressing over DAPI-positive cells in dCA1 at different time points after exposure to EE. The levels of tdTomato were significantly higher than baseline (0 h) on all days (p24–0h = 0.03, p48h–0h = 0.03, p7d–0h < 0.0001, p10d–0h = 0.0091; n0h = 1, n24h = 2, n48h = 2, n7d = 5, n10d = 4; One-sample t-test). Comparison to the 24 h timepoint retrieved a trend at time point 7 days and a significant increase at time point 10 (p48h–24h = 0.93, p7d–24h = 0.0719, p10d–24h = 0.025; n24h = 2, n48h = 2, n7d = 5, n10d = 4; One-way ANOVA with Sidak correction for multiple comparisons). (H) Ratio of dCA1 over dCA3 tdTomato expression levels at different time points after induction. *p < 0.05, **p < 0.01, ****p < 0.0001.
Arc-dependent expression of mGRASP upon exposure to enriched environment or trace fear conditioning labels a subset of dCA1 cells and yields GFP reconstitution
While cFostTA-dependent dTomato expression peaked at 3 days after induction (Figure 2E), ArcCreERT2-dependent labeling followed longer time scales (Figure 3C). Thus to compare cFostTA- versus ArcCreERT2-dependent mGRASP expressions, we quantified ArcCreERT2-dependent dTomato appearance in the hippocampal dCA1 at an earlier (3 days) and a later (7 days) time points after overnight exposure to EE. To this aim, we injected the right dCA1 and the left dCA3 of two groups of ArcCreERT2 mice with AAVs encoding for Cre-dependent-post-mGRASP and Cre-dependent-pre-mGRASP, respectively (Figures 4A, B). Three weeks after viral transduction we injected a single dose of TAM (175 mg/kg) intraperitoneally in all groups right before exploration of EE and quantified ArcCreERT2-dependent dTomato appearance 3 or 7 days later (Figure 4C). We detected dTomato-positive cells in dCA1 (Figure 4D) and the percentage of dTomato-expressing cells was higher at 7 days than at 3 days after induction (Figure 4E). Interestingly, while the proportion of cells positive for cFostTA- and ArcCreERT2-dependent dTomato was similar at 3 days after induction (7.5 and 9.2% of all DAPI cells, respectively), at 7 days the number of cells positive for ArcCreERT2-dependent dTomato was significantly higher than the number of cells positive for cFostTA-dependent dTomato (3 and 23% of all DAPI cells, respectively) (Figure 4F).
Figure 4. ArcCreERT2-dependent expression of mGRASP in dCA1 upon exposure to an Enriched Environment or Trace Fear Conditioning. Schematic description of the viral injection sites in ArcCreERT2 transgenic animals (A) and the viral constructs injected (B). (C) Experimental design to determine the expression levels of post-mGRASP in dCA1 under the control of Arc at two different time points after exposure to EE. (D) Confocal pictures, single Z-planes, of dCA1 at different time points after induction of post-mGRASP expression. Red, dTomato; blue, DAPI. White triangles indicate dTomato-positive cells. D, Dorsal, V, Ventral. Scale bar = 20 μm. (E) Percentage of dTomato-expressing over DAPI-positive cells in dCA1 3 and 7 days after exposure to EE. The level of dTomato at 7 days was significantly higher than at 3 days (p = 0.0095; n3 = 4, n7 = 6; Mann–Whitney U-test). (F) Percentage of dTomato-expressing over DAPI-positive cells in dCA1 3 and 7 days after exposure to EE in the cFostTA (striped) or ArcCreERT2 (solid) systems. The level of dTomato at 7 days was significantly higher in the ArcCreERT2 system than at 3 and 7 days in the cFostTA system (p7dArc7dFOS = 0.0005, p7dArc3dFOS = 0.017, all other p > 0.31; n7dArc = 6, n7dFOS = 5, n3dArc = 4, n3dFOS = 10; Kruskal–Wallis test after Dunn’s correction for multiple comparisons). (G) Confocal pictures, single Z-planes, of apical (right) and basal (left) dendritic segments of dCA1 pyramidal neurons at 7 days after induction of post-mGRASP expression showing GFP-fluorescent puncta colocalizing with dTomato-positive dendritic segments. Red, dTomato; green, GFP. White triangles indicate synaptic GFP reconstitution. D, Dorsal, V, Ventral. Scale bars = 5 μm. (H) Experimental design to determine the dCA1 and dCA3 expression levels of post-mGRASP under the control of Arc after TFC. (I) Confocal picture, single Z-plane, of dCA1 7 days after induction of post-mGRASP expression. Red, dTomato; blue, DAPI. White triangles indicate dTomato-positive cells. D, Dorsal, V, Ventral. Scale bar = 20 μm. (J) Percentage of dTomato- (dCA1) or mCerulean- (dCA3) expressing over DAPI- (dCA1) or Syto60- (dCA3) positive cells 7 days after exposure to FC. (K) Percentage of freezing time during the context probe 7 days after TFC training (p = 0.015, n = 7; Wilcoxon Signed-rank test). (L) Confocal pictures, single Z-planes, of a dCA1 pyramidal neuron and dendritic segments (insets) 7 days after induction of post-mGRASP expression showing GFP-fluorescent puncta colocalizing with dTomato-positive dendritic segments. Red, dTomato; green, GFP. White triangles indicate synaptic GFP reconstitution. Inset number 2 is rotated 90 degrees from the original orientation. Scale bars: left = 10 μm, middle and right = 5 μm. *p < 0.05, **p < 0.01, ***p < 0.001.
Importantly, at 7 days–but not at 3 days–after induction we detected GFP reconstitution on approximately 40% of apical and basal dendrites of dTomato dCA1 neurons (Figure 4G and Supplementary Figures 2A, B).
Finally, we tested whether the ArcCreERT2 system would also be able to visualize synaptic engrams after hippocampal-dependent learning. To this aim, we transduced another group of ArcCreERT2 mice as in the previous experiment, 3 weeks after viral transduction we injected a single dose of TAM intraperitoneally right before TFC training and quantified the number of dTomato-positive cells 7 days later (Figure 4H). We detected dTomato expression in dCA1 7 days after induction (Figure 4I). dTomato expression in dCA1 and mCerulean expression in dCA3 was limited to a very small fraction of cells (Figure 4J), despite a significant recall of the association between context and shock (Figure 4K). In a single instance, we detected GFP reconstitution on a small subset of dendrites of dTomato-expressing dCA1 neurons at 7 days after TFC training (Figure 4L).
Discussion
We used two different genetic schemes—based on the expression of the IEGs cFos and Arc—to express the virally encoded GFP transsynaptic complementation system mGRASP (Kim et al., 2011), with the aim to label structural synaptic engrams in the hippocampal dCA1 of mice. While both schemes are based on activity-dependent transcription, they show important differences. In the cFostTA-based scheme, the promoter of cFos is encoded in a viral construct and controls (virally transduced) mGRASP expression through the tTA-TRE system, while in the ArcCreERT2-based scheme the promoter of Arc is endogenous to the genome and controls (virally transduced) mGRASP expression through the Cre-Lox system. These differences affect the expression kinetics of the reporters and thus (i) the kinetics of mGRASP-positive cells appearance, (ii) the number of mGRASP-positive cells, and (iii) the detection of transsynaptic GFP reconstitution.
cFostTA and ArcCreERT2 systems display different expression kinetics
Appearance of cFostTA-dependent expression peaked at approximately 3 days after induction by exploration of an EE, with the number of dTomato-positive cells 10 days after induction being statistically indistinguishable from baseline in mice transduced with the TRE-post-mGRASP construct. This is because the cFostTA system triggers only transient expression of the reporter as the tTA drives transcription only during the time window where DOX is not present. Thus, after the induction peak, dTomato is degraded and its expression levels fall to baseline. In contrast, in the ArcCreERT2 system the number of tdTomato-positive cells increased and reached plateau at 7 days after induction. This is because the ArcCreERT2 system triggers transcription when TAM is present and expresses the reporter constitutively after that. Thus, after the induction peak, dTomato keeps being replenished and its expression levels plateau rather than falling back to baseline.
The ArcCreERT2 system labels more dCA1 cells than the cFostTA system
At peak expression after EE exploration—3 days for the cFos and 7 days for Arc system—we detected almost three times as many ArcCreERT2- as cFostTA-dTomato positive cells. Although, the IEGs cFos and Arc mark non-fully overlapping neuronal populations, their mRNAs are found in a similar proportion of cells upon activation (Guzowski et al., 2001; Miyashita et al., 2009). Thus, the difference we observe must be due to difference between the genetic systems we employ rather than the IEG promoters. In particular, we think that the higher accumulation of ArcCreERT2-dependent dTomato in combination with the detection threshold of dTomato fluorescence at the single-cell level might explain the difference in numbers between ArcCreERT2- and cFostTA-dTomato positive cells. Arc expression is not uniform, with active cells expressing the IEG at different levels upon induction (Attardo et al., 2018). As such, also the expression of Arc-driven CreERT2 is bound not to be uniform, with some cells expressing higher and some others lower levels of CreERT2. As the mGRASP is virally transduced, each cell contains multiple copies of the DNA encoding for dTomato, hence the limiting factor for dTomato production is likely the amount of CreERT2. Higher-CreERT2-expressing cells will be able to produce amounts of dTomato sufficient to cross the threshold of detectability earlier, while lower-expressing cells will require longer accumulation of dTomato in order to cross the threshold for detectability. Importantly, this buildup is absent in the cFostTA system as tTA-dependent transcription is switched off by DOX administration, thus lower-cFostTA-expressing cells do not have the time to accumulate enough dTomato to cross the detectability threshold. Hence, in the cFostTA system at its expression peak only the subset of higher-cFostTA-expressing cells will be detectable as dTomato-positive. In contrast, a larger number of ArcCreERT2-expressing cells—including lower- and higher-expressing cells—will be detectable as dTomato-positive in the ArcCreERT2 system at its expression peak. In other words, the cFostTA system works as a high-pass filter over the cFos expression range while the ArcCreERT2 system integrates over the Arc expression range.
Transsynaptic GFP reconstitution in the ArcCreERT2 but not in the cFostTA system
As a consequence of the integrative process in the ArcCreERT2 system, higher-ArcCreERT2-expressing cells at 7 days will also accumulate a greater amount of GFP in comparison to higher-cFostTA-expressing cells at 3 days. This explains why we detected GFP reconstitution only in the ArcCreERT2—but not the cFostTA—system and only at 7 days—but not at 3 days—after induction. Further protein buildup in the ArcCreERT2 system might be necessary to achieve an amount of GFP sufficient to cross the threshold for detectability or even possibly for transsynaptic reconstitution. Recent work did, however, report GFP reconstitution in the dual eGRASP system under the control of cFostTA (Choi et al., 2018, 2021). This discrepancy is most likely due to the fact that in the aforementioned study the split GFP was engineered with an additional S72A mutation and to take advantage of the stronger interaction between peptide p40 and the SH3 domain in the post-eGRASP construct to enhance fluorescence (Choi et al., 2018), thus using the eGRASP system might lead to a brighter labeling.
The level of ArcCreERT2-driven accumulation of mGRASP proteins in single cells did not depend on the behavioral stimulus that triggered cellular activity, as we detected GFP reconstitution in dCA1 after exploration of EE as well as after TFC training. However, TFC training marked a smaller subset of cells than exploration of an EE in dCA1 and dCA3 of ArcCreERT2 mice [this work Figures 4E, J, and (Castello-Waldow et al., 2020)] and higher sparseness of pre- and postsynaptic mGRASP-expressing cells makes detection of GFP puncta much less likely. This explains why we detected GFP reconstitution after TFC training only very sporadically.
Enduring structural connectivity between dCA3 and CA1 Arc-expressing cells upon learning
The observation of mGRASP trans synaptic reconstitution between dCA3 and dCA1 Arc-positive cells indicates that structural connectivity between Arc-expressing neuronal ensembles is present 1 week after memory encoding. This result is consistent with the hypothesis that enduring synaptic connectivity can re-entrain a memory state days after memory acquisition (Josselyn and Tonegawa, 2020). However, as hippocampal CA1 excitatory synaptic network is highly dynamic (Attardo et al., 2015; Pfeiffer et al., 2018) and the Arc-based system is always driving transcription of mGRASP after induction, we cannot distinguish between synapses that were present at memory acquisition and synapses that were added at a later time point. Importantly, as mGRASP expression in the ArcCreERT2-dependent system provides long term labeling of synaptic engrams, it could be combined with longitudinal intravital optical imaging to tackle this issue. In fact, this combination would enable not only to detect the presence but also to track the persistence of synaptic engrams and thus to follow the temporal evolution of synaptic engrams in live mice over several weeks; thereby, enabling to investigate the function of synaptic engrams in the face of continuous learning.
Data availability statement
The raw data supporting the conclusions of this article will be made available by the authors, without undue reservation.
Ethics statement
This animal study was reviewed and approved by Regierung von Oberbayern – Veterinärwesen.
Author contributions
BM and SS performed the majority of the experiments and analyzed the data. AU, TC-W, and RH performed a subset of the experiments. AC and AA procured funding. AU and AA designed the experiments. AU, HK, NH, and AA supervised the project. AA wrote the manuscript. All authors contributed to the article and approved the submitted version.
Funding
AC was supported by an FP7 Grant from the ERC, the ERANET and I-CORE programs, the Israeli Ministry of Health, the BMBF, the Nella and Leon Benoziyo Center for Neurological Diseases, the Henry Chanoch Krenter Institute for Biomedical Imaging and Genomics, The ISF, the Perlman Family, the Adelis, Marc Besen, Pratt, and Irving I. Moskowitz foundations and by Roberto and Renata Ruhman, and Bruno and Simone Lich. AA was supported by the Leibniz Institute for Neurobiology, the Deutsche Forschungsgemeinschaft (DFG, grants: #AT205/1-1, #AT205/7-1, #AT205/9-1, and #AT205/10-1) and the Schram Foundation (#T287/29575/2017). The funders were not involved in the study design, collection, analysis, interpretation of data, the writing of this article, or the decision to submit it for publication.
Acknowledgments
We thank Jihuyn Kim and Jinhuyn Kim (Brain Science Institute, Korea Institute of Science and Technology) for early access to the TREmGRASP constructs and for constructive comments to the manuscript, Dr. Jan Deussing and the GEMM core facility for mouse genotyping and support, and Albin Varga and the animal caretakers for mouse colonies maintenance.
Conflict of interest
The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.
Publisher’s note
All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.
Supplementary material
The Supplementary Material for this article can be found online at: https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fnbeh.2022.1072571/full#supplementary-material
Supplementary Figure 1 | cFostTA-dependent expression of mCerulean in dCA3 and of dTomato in dCA1 without GFP reconstitution. (A) Schematic description of the viral constructs injected. (B) Confocal picture, single Z-plane, of dCA3 3 days after induction of c-FOS-dependent expression of mCerulean upon exposure to EE. Red, Syto60; Cyan, mCerulean. White triangles indicate mCerulean-positive cells. L, Lateral, M, Medial. Scale bar = 10 μm. (C) Confocal picture, Maximum Intensity Projection of 5 Z-plane, of dCA1 3 days after induction of c-FOS-dependent expression of mCerulean upon exposure to EE. Red, dTomato; Green, GFP. Scale bar = 7 μm.
Supplementary Figure 2 | A subset of dTomato + dCA1 dendrites show GFP reconstitution. (A) Schematic description of the viral constructs injected. (B) The percentage of apical and basal dTomato-expressing dCA1 dendrites showing GFP reconstitution was not significantly different (p = 0.45; nApical = 7, nBasal = 5, image stacks; unpaired t-test).
References
Albarran, E., Raissi, A., Jáidar, O., Shatz, C. J., and Ding, J. B. (2021). Enhancing motor learning by increasing the stability of newly formed dendritic spines in the motor cortex. Neuron 109, 3298.e–3311.e. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2021.07.030
Attardo, A., Fitzgerald, J. E., and Schnitzer, M. J. (2015). Impermanence of dendritic spines in live adult CA1 hippocampus. Nature 523, 1–17. doi: 10.1038/nature14467
Attardo, A., Lu, J., Kawashima, T., Okuno, H., Fitzgerald, J. E., Bito, H., et al. (2018). Long-term consolidation of ensemble neural plasticity patterns in hippocampal area CA1. Cell Rep. 25, 640.e–650.e. doi: 10.1016/j.celrep.2018.09.064
Castello-Waldow, T. P., Weston, G., Ulivi, A. F., Chenani, A., Loewenstein, Y., Chen, A., et al. (2020). Hippocampal neurons with stable excitatory connectivity become part of neuronal representations. PLoS Biol. 18:e3000928. doi: 10.1371/journal.pbio.3000928
Chenani, A., Weston, G., Ulivi, A. F., Castello-Waldow, T. P., Huettl, R.-E., Chen, A., et al. (2022). Repeated stress exposure leads to structural synaptic instability prior to disorganization of hippocampal coding and impairments in learning. Transl. Psychiatry 12:381. doi: 10.1038/s41398-022-02107-5
Choi, D. I., and Kaang, B.-K. (2022). Interrogating structural plasticity among synaptic engrams. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 75:102552. doi: 10.1016/j.conb.2022.102552
Choi, D. I., Kim, J., Lee, H., Kim, J., Sung, Y., Choi, J. E., et al. (2021). Synaptic correlates of associative fear memory in the lateral amygdala. Neuron 109, 2717.e–2726.e. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2021.07.003
Choi, J.-H., Sim, S.-E., Kim, J., Choi, D. I., Oh, J., Ye, S., et al. (2018). Interregional synaptic maps among engram cells underlie memory formation. Science 360, 430–435. doi: 10.1126/science.aas9204
DeNardo, L. A., Liu, C. D., Allen, W. E., Adams, E. L., Friedmann, D., Fu, L., et al. (2019). Temporal evolution of cortical ensembles promoting remote memory retrieval. Nat. Neurosci. 22, 460–469. doi: 10.1038/s41593-018-0318-7
Denny, C. A., Kheirbek, M. A., Alba, E. L., Tanaka, K. F., Brachman, R. A., Laughman, K. B., et al. (2014). Hippocampal memory traces are differentially modulated by experience, time, and adult neurogenesis. Neuron 83, 189–201. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2014.05.018
Domi, E., Xu, L., Toivainen, S., Nordeman, A., Gobbo, F., Venniro, M., et al. (2021). A neural substrate of compulsive alcohol use. Sci. Adv. 7:eabg9045. doi: 10.1126/sciadv.abg9045
Druckmann, S., Feng, L., Lee, B., Yook, C., Zhao, T., Magee, J. C., et al. (2014). structured synaptic connectivity between hippocampal regions. Neuron 81, 629–640. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2013.11.026
Feinberg, E. H., Vanhoven, M. K., Bendesky, A., Wang, G., Fetter, R. D., Shen, K., et al. (2008). GFP Reconstitution Across Synaptic Partners (GRASP) defines cell contacts and synapses in living nervous systems. Neuron 57, 353–363. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2007.11.030
Fu, M., Yu, X., Lu, J., and Zuo, Y. (2012). Repetitive motor learning induces coordinated formation of clustered dendritic spines in vivo. Nature 483, 1–5. doi: 10.1038/nature10844
Gallinaro, J. V., Gašparović, N., and Rotter, S. (2022). Homeostatic control of synaptic rewiring in recurrent networks induces the formation of stable memory engrams. PLoS Comput. Biol. 18:e1009836. doi: 10.1371/journal.pcbi.1009836
Guenthner, C. J., Miyamichi, K., Yang, H. H., Heller, H. C., and Luo, L. (2013). Permanent genetic access to transiently active neurons via TRAP: Targeted recombination in active populations. Neuron 78, 773–784. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2013.03.025
Guzowski, J. F., Setlow, B., Wagner, E. K., and McGaugh, J. L. (2001). Experience-dependent gene expression in the rat hippocampus after spatial learning: A comparison of the immediate-early genes Arc, c-fos, and zif268. J. Neurosci. 21, 5089–5098. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.21-14-05089.2001
Hayashi-Takagi, A., Yagishita, S., Nakamura, M., Shirai, F., Wu, Y. I., Loshbaugh, A. L., et al. (2015). Labelling and optical erasure of synaptic memory traces in the motor cortex. Nature 525, 1–21. doi: 10.1038/nature15257
Hebb, D. O. (1949). “The organization of behavior,” in A neuropsychological theory, (Wiley: New York).
Hsiang, H.-L. L., Epp, J. R., van den Oever, M. C., Yan, C., Rashid, A. J., Insel, N., et al. (2014). Manipulating a “cocaine engram” in mice. J. Neurosci. 34, 14115–14127. doi: 10.1523/jneurosci.3327-14.2014
Hwang, F.-J., Roth, R. H., Wu, Y.-W., Sun, Y., Kwon, D. K., Liu, Y., et al. (2022). Motor learning selectively strengthens cortical and striatal synapses of motor engram neurons. Neuron 110, 2790.e–2801.e. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2022.06.006
Josselyn, S. A., and Tonegawa, S. (2020). Memory engrams: Recalling the past and imagining the future. Science 367:eaaw4325. doi: 10.1126/science.aaw4325
Kawashima, T., Kitamura, K., Suzuki, K., Nonaka, M., Kamijo, S., Takemoto-Kimura, S., et al. (2013). Functional labeling of neurons and their projections using the synthetic activity-dependent promoter E-SARE. Nat. Methods 10, 889–895. doi: 10.1038/nmeth.2559
Kawashima, T., Kitamura, K., Suzuki, K., Nonaka, M., Kamijo, S., Takemoto-Kimura, S., et al. (2014). Functional labeling of neurons and their projections using the synthetic activity-dependent promoter E-SARE (vol 10, pg 889, 2013). Nat. Pub. Group 11, 210–210. doi: 10.1038/nmeth0214-210a
Kawashima, T., Okuno, H., Nonaka, M., Adachi-Morishima, A., Kyo, N., Okamura, M., et al. (2009). Synaptic activity-responsive element in the Arc/Arg3.1 promoter essential for synapse-to-nucleus signaling in activated neurons. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 106, 316–321. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0806518106
Kim, J., Zhao, T., Petralia, R. S., Yu, Y., Peng, H., Myers, E., et al. (2011). mGRASP enables mapping mammalian synaptic connectivity with light microscopy. Nat. Methods 9, 96–102. doi: 10.1038/nmeth.1784
Kitamura, T., Ogawa, S. K., Roy, D. S., Okuyama, T., Morrissey, M. D., Smith, L. M., et al. (2017). Engrams and circuits crucial for systems consolidation of a memory. Science 356, 73–78. doi: 10.1126/science.aam6808
Liu, X., Ramirez, S., Pang, P. T., Puryear, C. B., Govindarajan, A., Deisseroth, K., et al. (2012). Optogenetic stimulation of a hippocampal engram activates fear memory recall. Nature 484, 381–385. doi: 10.1038/nature11028
Miyashita, T., Kubik, S., Haghighi, N., Steward, O., and Guzowski, J. F. (2009). Rapid activation of plasticity-associated gene transcription in hippocampal neurons provides a mechanism for encoding of one-trial experience. J. Neurosci. 29, 898–906. doi: 10.1523/jneurosci.4588-08.2009
Pfeiffer, T., Poll, S., Bancelin, S., Angibaud, J., Inavalli, V. K., Keppler, K., et al. (2018). Chronic 2P-STED imaging reveals high turnover of dendritic spines in the hippocampus in vivo. Elife 7, e34700. doi: 10.7554/elife.34700
Ramirez, S., Liu, X., Lin, P.-A., Suh, J., Pignatelli, M., Redondo, R. L., et al. (2013). Creating a false memory in the hippocampus. Science 341, 387–391. doi: 10.1126/science.1239073
Ramirez, S., Liu, X., Macdonald, C. J., Moffa, A., Zhou, J., Redondo, R. L., et al. (2015). Activating positive memory engrams suppresses depression-like behaviour. Nature 522, 335–339. doi: 10.1038/nature14514
Rao-Ruiz, P., Couey, J. J., Marcelo, I. M., Bouwkamp, C. G., Slump, D. E., Matos, M. R., et al. (2019). Engram-specific transcriptome profiling of contextual memory consolidation. Nat. Commun. 10:2232. doi: 10.1038/s41467-019-09960-x
Rashid, A. J., Yan, C., Mercaldo, V., Hsiang, H.-L. L., Park, S., Cole, C. J., et al. (2016). Competition between engrams influences fear memory formation and recall. Science 353, 383–387. doi: 10.1126/science.aaf0594
Redondo, R. L., Kim, J., Arons, A. L., Ramirez, S., Liu, X., and Tonegawa, S. (2014). Bidirectional switch of the valence associated with a hippocampal contextual memory engram. Nature 513, 1–11. doi: 10.1038/nature13725
Reijmers, L. G., Perkins, B. L., Matsuo, N., and Mayford, M. (2007). Localization of a stable neural correlate of associative memory. Science 317, 1230–1233. doi: 10.1126/science.1143839
Sørensen, A. T., Cooper, Y. A., Baratta, M. V., Weng, F.-J., Zhang, Y., Ramamoorthi, K., et al. (2016). A robust activity marking system for exploring active neuronal ensembles. Elife 5:e13918. doi: 10.7554/elife.13918
Tanaka, K. Z., Pevzner, A., Hamidi, A. B., Nakazawa, Y., Graham, J., and Wiltgen, B. J. (2014a). Cortical representations are reinstated by the hippocampus during memory retrieval. Neuron 84, 347–354. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2014.09.037
Tanaka, K. Z., Pevzner, A., Hamidi, A. B., Nakazawa, Y., Graham, J., and Wiltgen, B. J. (2014b). Cortical representations are reinstated by the hippocampus during memory retrieval. Neuron 84, 347–354. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2014.09.037
Trachtenberg, J. T., Chen, B., Knott, G., Feng, G., Sanes, J., Welker, E., et al. (2002). Long-term in vivo imaging of experience-dependent synaptic plasticity in adult cortex. Nature 420, 788–794. doi: 10.1038/nature01273
Vetere, G., Kenney, J. W., Tran, L. M., Xia, F., Steadman, P. E., Parkinson, J., et al. (2017). Chemogenetic interrogation of a brain-wide fear memory network in mice. Neuron 94, 363.e–374.e. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2017.03.037
Visser, E., Matos, M. R., van der Loo, R. J., Marchant, N. J., Vries, T. J., and de, et al. (2020). A persistent alcohol cue memory trace drives relapse to alcohol seeking after prolonged abstinence. Sci. Adv. 6:eaax7060. doi: 10.1126/sciadv.aax7060
Xu, T., Yu, X., Perlik, A. J., Tobin, W. F., Zweig, J. A., Tennant, K., et al. (2009). Rapid formation and selective stabilization of synapses for enduring motor memories. Nature 462, 915–919. doi: 10.1038/nature08389
Yang, G., Pan, F., and Gan, W.-B. (2009). Stably maintained dendritic spines are associated with lifelong memories. Nature 462, 920–924. doi: 10.1038/nature08577
Yang, Y., Liu, D., Huang, W., Deng, J., Sun, Y., Zuo, Y., et al. (2016). Selective synaptic remodeling of amygdalocortical connections associated with fear memory. Nat. Neurosci. 19, 1348–1355. doi: 10.1038/nn.4370
Zhang, Z., Ferretti, V., Güntan, İ, Moro, A., Steinberg, E. A., Ye, Z., et al. (2015). Neuronal ensembles sufficient for recovery sleep and the sedative actions of α2 adrenergic agonists. Nat. Neurosci. 18, 553–561. doi: 10.1038/nn.3957
Zhou, Y., Won, J., Karlsson, M. G., Zhou, M., Rogerson, T., Balaji, J., et al. (2009). CREB regulates excitability and the allocation of memory to subsets of neurons in the amygdala. Nat. Neurosci. 12, 1438–1443. doi: 10.1038/nn.2405
Zuo, Y., Lin, A., Chang, P., and Gan, W.-B. (2005a). Development of long-term dendritic spine stability in diverse regions of cerebral cortex. Neuron 46, 181–189. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2005.04.001
Keywords: synaptic engram, dorsal hippocampus, fear conditioning, mGRASP, cellular engram
Citation: Murthy BKB, Somatakis S, Ulivi AF, Klimmt H, Castello-Waldow TP, Haynes N, Huettl RE, Chen A and Attardo A (2023) Arc-driven mGRASP highlights CA1 to CA3 synaptic engrams. Front. Behav. Neurosci. 16:1072571. doi: 10.3389/fnbeh.2022.1072571
Received: 17 October 2022; Accepted: 29 December 2022;
Published: 30 January 2023.
Edited by:
Michel C. Van Den Oever, Vrije Universiteit Amsterdam, NetherlandsReviewed by:
Ana M. M. Oliveira, Heidelberg University, GermanySylvie Lisa Lesuis, University of Amsterdam, Netherlands
Copyright © 2023 Murthy, Somatakis, Ulivi, Klimmt, Castello-Waldow, Haynes, Huettl, Chen and Attardo. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.
*Correspondence: Alessio Attardo, alessio.attardo@lin-magdeburg.de
†These authors have contributed equally to this work and share first authorship