Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Behav. Neurosci., 08 December 2022
Sec. Learning and Memory
This article is part of the Research Topic Early Life Events: Shedding Light on Neurobiological Mechanisms View all 6 articles

Altered cognition and anxiety in adolescent offspring whose mothers underwent different-pattern maternal sleep deprivation, and cognition link to hippocampal expressions of Bdnf and Syt-1

\nRu-Meng Wei&#x;Ru-Meng Wei1Yue-Ming Zhang&#x;Yue-Ming Zhang1Yun LiYun Li1Qi-Tao WuQi-Tao Wu1Ya-Tao WangYa-Tao Wang1Xue-Yan LiXue-Yan Li1Xue-Wei Li
Xue-Wei Li2*Gui-Hai Chen
Gui-Hai Chen1*
  • 1Department of Neurology (Sleep Disorders), The Affiliated Chaohu Hospital of Anhui Medical University, Hefei, Anhui, China
  • 2Department of Neurology, The First Affiliated Hospital, Hengyang Medical School, University of South China, Hengyang, Hunan, China

Background: Inadequate sleep during pregnancy negatively affects the neural development of offspring. Previous studies have focused on the continuous sleep deprivation (CSD) paradigm, but the sleep pattern during late pregnancy is usually fragmented.

Objective: To compare the effects of CSD and fragmented sleep deprivation (FSD) in late pregnancy on emotion, cognition, and expression of synaptic plasticity-related proteins in offspring mice.

Methods: Pregnant CD-1 mice were either subjected to 3/6 h of CSD/FSD during gestation days 15–21, while those in the control group were left untreated. After delivery, the offspring were divided into five groups, i.e., control (CON), short or long CSD (CSD3h, CSD6h), and short or long FSD (FSD3h, FSD6h). When the offspring were 2 months old, the anxiety-like behavior level was tested using the open field (OF) and elevated plus maze (EPM) test, and spatial learning and memory were evaluated using the Morris water maze (MWM) test. The expression of hippocampal of brain-derived neurotrophic factor (Bdnf) and synaptotagmin-1 (Syt-1) was determined using RT-PCR and western blotting.

Results: The CSD6h, FSD3h, and FSD6h had longer latency, fewer center times in the OF test, less open arms time and fewer numbers of entries in the open arms of the EPM, longer learning distance swam and lower memory percentage of distance swam in the target quadrant in the MWM test, and decreased BDNF and increased Syt-1 mRNA and protein levels in the hippocampus. Compared to the CSD6h, the FSD3h and FSD6h had longer distance swam, a lower percentage of distance swam in the target quadrant, decreased BDNF, and increased Syt-1 mRNA and protein levels in the hippocampus.

Conclusion: The results suggested that maternal sleep deprivation during late pregnancy impairs emotion and cognition in offspring, and FSD worsened the cognitive performance to a higher extent than CSD. The observed cognitive impairment could be associated with the expression of altered hippocampal of Bdnf and Syt-1 genes.

Introduction

Inadequate sleep during pregnancy is a common phenomenon, and epidemiological studies have demonstrated that approximately half of the population of pregnant women is dissatisfied with the quality of sleep (Sedov et al., 2018). Owing to the influence of estrogen and progesterone levels, psychological state, and fetal development, pregnant women are susceptible to sleep disorders, including insomnia, sleep-disordered breathing, and restless leg syndrome, resulting in inadequate sleep (Mindell et al., 2015). Irregular contractions, increased fetal movements, leg cramps, frequent urination, and dyspnea in late pregnancy lead to frequent awakenings, decreased slow-wave sleep, and decreased rapid eye movement (REM) sleep (Wilson et al., 2011; Izci-Balserak et al., 2018). Therefore, sleep patterns in pregnant women usually exhibit fragmentation.

To date, various sleep deprivation (SD) paradigms have been applied in basic research, including the gentle stimulation method (Longordo et al., 2009), the treadmill method (Ward et al., 2009), and the modified multiple platform method (Machado et al., 2004). These SD paradigms are divided into total SD and selective SD. For instance, the modified multiple-platform method selectively deprives REM sleep, based on the principle that muscle tension declines when the sleep stage changes from non-REM to REM sleep (Kim et al., 2021). In contrast, the gentle stimulation method is a continuous SD (CSD) paradigm that completely deprives all sleep stages by physically touching or knocking on the cage (Vecsey et al., 2013). However, none of these SD paradigms can properly simulate the sleep-deprived patterns in late pregnancy, as they merely deprive specific sleep stages, while the most frequent sleep disturbance during pregnancy is sleep fragmentation (Khalyfa et al., 2014). It is, therefore, necessary to use an appropriate SD paradigm for exploring the effects of maternal SD (MSD) on the behaviors of offspring and underlying mechanisms.

There have been some reports of fragmented SD (FSD) paradigms, which randomly interrupt sleep by arousing the animals at various specific time intervals, resulting in fractured sleep. The FSD paradigm designed for rats includes a 2-min cycle of 30 s-on and 90 s-off for 24 or 72 h on a treadmill, ensuring that the animals awaken during the 30 s-on intervals (Tartar et al., 2006). A relatively mild protocol is designed for mice, which includes 2-min repetitive cycles of 10 s-on and 110 s-off during the light-on phase that lasts for 2 months, by using an electronically controlled running rotor (Xie et al., 2020). After undergoing these procedures, the rodents exhibit impaired spatial learning and memory and increased anxiety-like behaviors. It seems that these paradigms are applicable to simulate the characteristics of fragmented sleep in clinical settings.

Insufficient sleep during pregnancy is thought to play an adverse role in the growth and development of the offspring (Chang et al., 2010; Zhao et al., 2014). Experimental studies on rodents have demonstrated that offspring whose mothers experienced SD during pregnancy exhibit impaired cognition, altered anxiety-like behaviors, decreased male sexual behaviors, and dysfunctional metabolism (Alvarenga et al., 2013; Radhakrishnan et al., 2015; Peng et al., 2016; Trzepizur et al., 2017). As paradigm heterogeneity can result in different emotional and cognitive outcomes, it is necessary to compare the effects of SD during late pregnancy on the emotional and cognitive behaviors of offspring using different procedures (CSD/FSD).

The hippocampus is a key region of the brain that is involved in processing cognitive and emotional information (Anacker and Hen, 2017), whose functions can be impaired by adverse stresses in early life (Grigoryan and Segal, 2016). Indeed, SD during pregnancy has adverse effects on the hippocampus of the offspring, including reduced neurogenesis and altered synaptic plasticity, which results in emotional and cognitive deficits (Zhao et al., 2014; Yu et al., 2018). The impaired synaptic plasticity could be related to the altered levels of synaptic plasticity-associated proteins in the hippocampus. However, the alterations in the synaptic proteins of mice whose mothers suffered from different SD during pregnancy are poorly studied.

Brain-derived neurotrophic factor (BDNF) is an important regulator of synaptic plasticity in the central nervous system and plays a key role in the formation of learning and memory (Autry and Monteggia, 2012). After binding to tropomyosin-related kinase B (TrkB) receptors, BDNF affects the downstream signaling pathways to regulate long-term potentiation (LTP) and synaptic transmission, which affects synaptic plasticity (Leal et al., 2014). Previous studies have demonstrated that exposure to stress during early life, including maternal separation and prenatal inflammation, can decrease the hippocampal levels of BDNF in adulthood (Schaafsma et al., 2017; Menezes et al., 2020). Synaptotagmin-1 (Syt-1), an important presynaptic vesicle protein, is the main fast-phase Ca2+ receptor during the exocytosis of synaptic vesicles and plays an important role in synaptic plasticity (Chapman, 2002). Previous studies have demonstrated that adult CD-1 mice exposure to inflammation during pregnancy exacerbates age-related learning and memory decline accompanied by increased levels of Syt-1 (Li et al., 2016). These findings suggest that the two synaptic plasticity-associated proteins, BDNF and Syt-1, play an important role in learning and memory. However, it remains to be understood whether the effects of SD during pregnancy on emotion and cognitive function in offspring are mediated via BDNF and Syt-1.

Therefore, the present study aimed to explore whether different SD paradigms during late pregnancy affect the emotion and hippocampus-dependent spatial learning and memory in adolescent CD-1 mice offspring, and if so, whether the effects are mediated via alterations in the expression levels of Syt-1 and BDNF in the hippocampus of offspring mice.

Methods

Animals

CD-1 mice (7–8 weeks) were purchased from Beijing Vital River Laboratory Animal Company (SPF grade). Adaptive feeding was performed for 1 week before experimentation. Mating was performed in the cage at 21:00 h at a male:female ratio of 1:2. The vaginal plug was checked for the following days at 7:00 h, and the day the vaginal plug was observed was designated as day 0 of gestation (gd 0). Pregnant females were housed individually in cages in a feeding environment under a 12-h light/dark cycle (lights on at 8:00 h and off at 20:00 h), at a temperature of 22–25°C, relative humidity of 50 ± 5%, and had ad libitum access to food and water. The pregnant mice were randomly divided into five groups during late gestation (gd 15–21), with 10 pregnancy mice in each group, and subjected to short or long CSD (3/6 h) or FSD (3/6 h), while the control group did not receive any intervention. The delivery day was designated as postnatal day 0 (PND0). The offspring of these pregnant mice were used as the study subjects and were noted accordingly: long CSD (CSD6h), long FSD (FSD6h), short CSD (CSD3h), short FSD (FSD3h), and control (CON) groups. The offspring were reared up to adolescence (2-month-old) after birth (Spear, 2000; Kota et al., 2011; Zhang Z. Z. et al., 2022). Finally, a total of 16 mice, including 8 males and 8 females, were included in each group for behavioral analyses, western blotting, and RT-PCR experiments. The experimental procedures were approved by the Laboratory Animal Committee of Anhui Medical University.

Sleep deprivation

Sleep deprivation was performed using a special apparatus (BW-NSD404, Shanghai Bio-will Co, Ltd) for 3 h (12:00 h−15:00 h) or 6 h (12:00 h−18:00 h) per day. In the FSD, the repetitive cycle of the SD apparatus was set to 10 s-on and 110 s-off periods with 30 interruptions/h (Xie et al., 2020), while the device moved uninterruptedly at 0.5 m/min in the CSD (Zhang Y. M. et al., 2022). Food and water were made available throughout the period of SD (Figure 1).

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Timeline of the experiment (refer to experimental protocols for details).

Vaginal smear

The vaginal smear method was used to assess the estrous cycle of female mice, which is divided into four phases: proestrus, estrus, metaestrus, and diestrus (Ng et al., 2010). The female mice were selected to start behavioral experiments in the diestrus due to the lesser effect of estrogen during this period (Wessels et al., 2015). The vagina of mice was gently rinsed 3–4 times with 20 μl of stroke physiological saline solution in a 200-μl pipette, the vaginal rinsing solution was evenly applied to the slides, and the slides were dried naturally and fixed with anhydrous ethanol at room temperature for 10 min. The slides were gently rinsed with running water, gently shaken off, and stained with hematoxylin stain for 5 min; the floating color was washed off in the water. The unbound hematoxylin was eluted with 1% hydrochloric acid ethanol treatment for 5 s and then the slides were gently rinsed in water. The slides were re-stained with ammonia for 30 s to return to blue, gently rinsed in water, and then shaken off. The slides were stained with 0.5% eosin for 30 s; the slides were gently rinsed in water and then air-dried, sealed, and photographed with a confocal microscope (Olympus FV3000, Japan) to observe the cell composition of the vaginal smears (Supplementary Figure 3).

Open field test

The open field (OF) test (Tong et al., 2015) was used for evaluating locomotor activity and anxiety-like behaviors and was performed at PND61. The experimental device consisted of a wooden box without a lid and a black inner wall (internal dimensions of 81 × 81 × 28 cm3). The bottom of the box was equally divided into 16 squares of dimensions 20 × 20 cm. A colored toy was placed in the center to stimulate the curiosity and exploratory activity of the mice. The mice were individually placed in a corner square facing the wall and were permitted to freely explore the environment for 5 min. After each test, the urine and feces of each mouse were removed, and 75% alcohol was used for avoiding interference in the subsequent experiments. The ANY-Maze software (Stoeling, USA) was used for automatic calculations and analyses of latency (time spent to leave the first square) and center time (time spent in the four center squares).

Elevated plus maze test

The elevated plus maze (EPM) test (Chen et al., 2022) was used for studying the anxiety-like behaviors of the offspring and was performed at PND62. The equipment appeared like a cross and consisted of two open arms and two closed arms (30 cm in length and 5 cm in width each) with a center square (5 × 5 cm2). The equipment was elevated to a height of 80 cm above the floor. The mice were individually placed in the center square area, facing one of the open arms. The maze was cleaned with 75% ethanol after the test was complete for each mouse. The ANY-Maze software (Stoeling, USA) was used to analyze the time spent in the open arms and the number of entries into the open arms within 6 min.

Morris water maze test

The spatial learning and memory abilities of the 2-month-old mice offspring were assessed by the Morris water maze (MWM) test over a period of 7 days, with 16 mice per group. The tests were carried out from PND63 to P69. The protocol used in this study has been previously reported (Wu et al., 2020). The water maze consisted of an underwater platform (10 cm in diameter and 24 cm in height) and a circular black tank (150 cm in diameter and 30 cm in height) filled with water (20–22°C, 25 cm in depth). The periphery of the tank was surrounded by a white curtain from the ceiling to the floor, in which three black cardboard shapes (square, triangle, and circle) served as spatial cues. The formal experiment was divided into two parts: the learning phase (the navigation task) and the memory phase (the probe trial task). In the learning phase, the mice were randomly placed in the water from the four quadrants with their heads facing the wall of the pool for each test and were allowed to rest on the platform for 30 s regardless of whether they climbed onto the platform within the specified time (60 s) or were guided by the experimenter to the platform. A total of four tests were conducted each day with a 15 min interval, and the experiment lasted for 7 days. In the memory phase, the experiment was performed on the last day of the learning period, after 2 h of the last trial. Briefly, the platform was removed, and the mice were placed in the water from the opposite quadrant of the target quadrant and allowed to explore freely for 60 s. Swimming distance, speed of finding the platform in the learning phase, and the percentage of distance swam in the target quadrant in the memory phase were analyzed using the ANY-Maze software (Stoeling, USA).

Tissue preparation

The CD-1 mice were euthanized after giving the proper anesthesia (2% Sodium pentobarbital) at PND71. The brain tissues were rapidly removed from the skull and cut along with the midsagittal plane on ice. The cerebral cortex was carefully dissected to expose the hippocampus and separate the hippocampus from the cerebral cortex and the surrounding brain tissue. Finally, the hippocampal tissue was carefully collected and snap-frozen in liquid nitrogen followed by transfer to −80°C for various biochemical analyses.

Western blotting

The hippocampus was stripped and grounded thoroughly, following which 600 μl of RIPA cell lysis buffer containing 0.6 mM PMSF (Beyotime, P0013B) was added for lysis, and centrifuged at 12,000 rpm for 15 min. The supernatant was collected, and 5X SDS-PAGE protein loading buffer (Solarbio, S8010) was added to the collected protein samples at a ratio of 1:4. The samples were then heated in a boiling water bath for 15 min to fully denature the proteins. After the samples had cooled down to room temperature, 5–10 μl of the protein sample was directly loaded into each of the loading wells of the gel used for SDS-PAGE. Electrophoresis was performed at a constant pressure, at 80 volts, for ~1 h. The gels were then placed over pre-cut filter papers and a PVDF membrane (Millipore, IPVH00010) of the same size as the adhesive strip that had been previously soaked in methanol for 2–3 min and immersed in a transfer buffer for 5 min. Constant flow transfer membranes with a transfer time of 20 and 45 min were used for BDNF and Syt-1, respectively. The protein membrane was then immediately placed in a pre-prepared western wash solution and rinsed for 5 min for washing off the transfer solution from the membrane. The western blotting solution containing 5% skimmed milk powder was added, and the membrane was slowly shaken on a shaker and blocked for 2 h at room temperature. The membrane was subsequently incubated with the rabbit anti-BDNF (1:1000; Abcam, Cambridge, UK) and rabbit anti-Syt-1 (1:1000; Bioss, Beijing, China) primary antibodies, and the horseradish peroxidase (HRP) labeled goat anti-rabbit IgG (Zsbio, ZB-2301) secondary antibody, according to the manufacturer's instructions. The protein bands were analyzed using Image J software (Media Cybernetics, USA) for calculating the relative expression of BDNF and Syt-1.

Real-time fluorescence-based quantitative PCR

The RNA was reverse transcribed into cDNA using a reverse transcription kit (TaKaRa, RR047A), and the cDNA was used as a template for fluorescence quantification. The PCR mixture comprised 5 μl of 2 × SYBR Green Mixture, 1 μl of forward primer, 1 μl of reverse primer, 1 μl of cDNA, and 2 μl of RNase-Free water. The reaction conditions included a single cycle of pre-denaturation at 95°C for 1 min, denaturation at 95°C for 20 s, and extension at 60°C for 1 min for a total of 40 cycles. The relative content of the mRNA was calculated using the 2−ΔΔCt method. The sequences of the primers used for PCR are enlisted in Table 1.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Primer sequences used in PCR.

Statistical analyses

Statistical analysis was performed using GraphPad Prism version 8.0. The data followed a normal distribution and was expressed as mean ± standard error of the mean (SEM), and normality was examined with the Shapiro–Wilk test. The main effect of treatment was analyzed using two-way ANOVA with treatment and sex as independent variables in all groups. The swimming distance and velocity in the MWM test were analyzed by repeated measures analysis of variance (rm-ANOVA). Two-way ANOVA was used for analyzing other data, including the percentage of distance swam in the target quadrant of the MWM, time spent in the center and latency in the OF, time spent in the open arms and the number of entries into the open arms in the EPM, and the mRNA and protein expression of BDNF and Syt-1. The correlation between all the data was determined using Pearson's correlation test. Tukey's test post-hoc was performed to compare the results among the groups. P < 0.05 was considered to be statistically significant.

Results

Anxiety-based tasks

The open-field test

The results revealed a treatment effect in the latency [F(4, 70) = 17.102, P < 0.01] and center time [F(4, 70) = 12.477, P < 0.01] among the five groups while there is no sex effect [latency: F(1, 70) = 2.136, P = 0.148; center time: F(1, 70) = 0.023, P = 0.879] and no interaction between sex × treatment [latency: F(4, 70) = 0.349, P = 0.844; center time: F(4, 70) = 0.043, P = 0.996]. The Tukey's test post-hoc analysis revealed that the mice in the CSD6h, FSD3h, and FSD6h groups exhibited longer latency (Ps < 0.05) and shorter center time (Ps < 0.05) than the mice in the CON group; however, the latency (Ps > 0.05) and center time (Ps > 0.05) were similar among these three groups. Notably, there were no significant differences in the latency and center time between the CSD3h and CON groups (Ps > 0.05, Figures 2A,B).

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Results of the open field tests (16 mice were tested per treatment group, including 8 males and 8 females). The latency and time spent in the center in the open field are depicted graphically. A longer latency period and a shorter center time were regarded as increased anxiety-like behaviors. (A) Latency in the open field. (B) Time spent in the center. *P < 0.05 and **P < 0.01 vs. Male CON group; #P < 0.05 and ##P < 0.01 vs. Male CSD3h group; aP < 0.05 and aaP < 0.01 vs. Female CON group; bP < 0.05 and bbP < 0.01 vs. Female CSD3h group.

The elevated plus maze test

The results revealed that the SD treatments had a significant effect on time spent in the open arms [F(4, 70) = 12.473, P < 0.01] and the number of open arms entries [F(4, 70) = 21.281, P < 0.01], while the sex had no effect on those (time spent in open arms: P = 0.995; the number of open arms entries: P = 0.673). The interaction of sex × treatment had no significant effect on those parameters (time spent in open arms: P = 0.973, the number of open arms entries: P = 0.917). The Tukey's test post-hoc analysis revealed that the mice in the CSD6h, FSD3h, and FSD6h groups exhibited a shorter duration in the open arms (Ps < 0.05) and fewer number of entries in the open arms (Ps < 0.01) than the mice in the CON group, and there was a similarity in the three measures among the three groups (Ps > 0.05). There were no significant differences in time spent in the open arms and the number of entries into the open arms between the CSD3h and CON groups (Ps > 0.05, Figures 3A,B).

FIGURE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 3. Results of the elevated plus maze test (16 mice were tested per treatment group, including 8 males and 8 females). The time spent in the center and the number of open arms entries of the elevated plus maze are depicted graphically. A shorter duration in the open arms and fewer entries into the open arms were regarded as increased anxiety-like behaviors. (A) Number of entries into the open arms. (B) Time spent in the open arms. *P < 0.05 and **P < 0.01 vs. Male CON group; #P < 0.05 and ##P < 0.01 vs. Male CSD3h group; aP < 0.05 and aaP < 0.01 vs. Female CON group; bP < 0.05 and bbP < 0.01 vs. Female CSD3h group.

Morris water maze test

Learning phase

The results revealed no sex differences in swimming velocity and distance swam for the CON [velocity: F(1, 14) = 0.535, P = 0.477; distance swam: F(1, 14) = 0.017, P = 0.898; Figures 4A,D] and other treatment groups (CSD3h, CSD6h, FSD3h, FSD6h) (Ps > 0.05; Supplementary Figures 1, 2A–D). The swimming velocity was not affected by the number of days trained [Male: F(6, 210) = 1.634, P = 0.139; Female: F(6, 210) = 1.494, P = 0.182] and the SD treatment [Male: F(4, 35) = 0.746, P = 0.568; Female: F(4, 35) = 0.484, P = 0.747] (Figures 4B,C). The distance swam gradually decreased in all groups as training progressed [Male: F(6, 210) = 199.551, P < 0.01; Female: F(6, 210) = 278.352, P < 0.01], and the SD treatments had significant effects on the distance swam [Male: F(4, 35) = 20.368, P < 0.01; Female: F(4, 35) = 20.542, P < 0.01] (Figures 4E,F). A significant interaction of time × treatment on the distance swam was observed [Male: F(24, 210) = 1.771, P = 0.018; Female: F(24, 210) = 2.882, P < 0.01]. Tukey's test post-hoc analysis revealed that the mice in the CSD6h, FSD3h, and FSD6h groups swam longer distances (Ps < 0.05) to find a hidden platform than those in the CON and CSD3h groups. Furthermore, the FSD3h and FSD6h groups had longer distances swam (Ps < 0.05) than the CSD6h group, while there were no differences between the FSD3h and FSD6h groups, and between the CON and CSD3h groups (Ps > 0.05), in terms of distance swam.

FIGURE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 4. Results of the Morris water maze test (16 mice were tested per treatment group, including 8 males and 8 females). (A–G) The velocity and distance swam covered during the 7 days of training, and the distance percentage in the target quadrant for space exploration are depicted graphically. Longer distance swam was regarded as a reduction in learning ability, while lower percentages of distance swam were regarded as a reduction in memory ability. (A) CON speed, (B) Male speed, (C) Female speed, (D) CON distance swam, (E) Male distance swam, (F) Female distance swam, and (G) distance percentage. *P < 0.05 and **P < 0.01 vs. Male CON group; #P < 0.05 and ##P < 0.01 vs. Male CSD3h group; $P < 0.05 vs. Male CSD6h group; aP < 0.05 and aaP < 0.01 vs. Female CON group; bP < 0.05 and bbP < 0.01 vs. Female CSD3h group; cP < 0.05 and ccP < 0.01 vs. Female CSD6h group.

Memory phase

The SD treatments [F(4, 70) = 37.139, P < 0.01] had a significant effect on the percentages of distance swam in the target quadrant, while sex [F(1, 70) = 1.301, P =0.258] and the interaction of sex × treatment [F(4, 70) = 0.012, P = 0.999] had no effect on it. Furthermore, the percentage of distance swam in the target quadrant in the CSD6h, FSD3h, and FSD6h groups was lower than that of the CON and CSD3h groups (Ps < 0.05). Also, the FSD3h and FSD6h groups showed a lower percentage of distance swam in the target quadrant than the CSD6h group (Ps < 0.05), while there were no differences between the FSD3h and FSD6h groups, and between the CON and CSD3h groups (Ps > 0.05), in terms of percentage of distance swam in the target quadrant (Figure 4G).

Effects of SD on the hippocampal expression of Bdnf and Syt-1

The SD treatments had a significant effect on the hippocampal protein levels of BDNF and Syt-1 [BDNF: F(4, 50) = 161.963, P < 0.01; Syt-1: F(4, 50) = 113.377, P < 0.01], while the sex had no effect on those [BDNF: F(1, 50) = 2.842, P = 0.098; Syt-1: F(1, 50) = 3.665, P = 0.061]. No significant interaction of treatment × sex was observed on the levels of BDNF and Syt-1 protein [BDNF: F(4, 50) = 1.495, P = 0.218; Syt-1: F(4, 50) = 0.280, P = 0.890]. Compared to those of the CON and CSD3h groups, the CSD6h, FSD3h, and FSD6h groups had lower protein levels of BDNF (Ps < 0.01) and higher protein levels of Syt-1 (Ps < 0.01). The FSD3h and FSD6h groups also had lower protein levels of BDNF and higher protein levels of Syt-1 compared to those of the CSD6h group (Ps < 0.05). There were no significant differences in the protein levels of BDNF and Syt-1 between the FSD3h and FSD6h groups and between the CON and CSD3h groups (Ps > 0.05; Figures 5A–C).

FIGURE 5
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 5. Effects of MSD on the expression of hippocampal of BDNF and Syt-1 proteins in the offspring. (A) The expression levels of BDNF (n = 12, 6 females and 6 males in each treatment group) and Syt-1 (n = 12, 6 females and 6 males in each treatment group) proteins in the hippocampus at 2 months. The results of protein quantification are depicted in (B,C). **P < 0.01 vs. Male CON group; ##P < 0.01 vs. Male CSD3h group; $$P < 0.01 vs. Male CSD6h group; aaP < 0.01 vs. Female CON group; bbP < 0.01 vs. Female CSD3h group; ccP < 0.01 vs. Female CSD6h group.

The SD treatments had a significant effect on the hippocampal mRNA levels of BDNF and Syt-1 [BDNF: F(4, 70) = 48.143, P < 0.01; Syt-1: F(4, 70) = 57.205, P < 0.01], while the sex had no effect on those Syt-1 [BDNF: F(1, 70) = 0.078, P = 0.782; Syt-1: F(1, 70) = 0.287, P = 0.594]. No significant interaction of treatment × sex was observed on the levels of BDNF and Syt-1 mRNA [BDNF: F(4, 70) = 0.459, P = 0.766; Syt-1: F(4, 70) = 0.569, P = 0.686]. Similar to the expression of the protein, the CSD6h, FSD3h, and FSD6h groups had significantly lower levels of BDNF mRNA (Ps < 0.05) and higher levels of Syt-1 mRNA (Ps < 0.05) compared to those of the CON and CSD3h groups. The FSD3h and FSD6h groups also had significantly lower levels of BDNF mRNA and higher levels of Syt-1 mRNA (Ps < 0.05) compared to those of the CSD6h group. There were no significant differences in the levels of BDNF and Syt-1 mRNA between the FSD3h and FSD6h groups, and between the CON and CSD3h groups (Ps > 0.05; Figures 6A,B).

FIGURE 6
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 6. The effects of MSD on the expression of hippocampal of BDNF and Syt-1 mRNA in the offspring. The mRNA expression levels of BDNF (n = 16, 8 females and 8 males in each treatment group) and Syt1 (n = 16, 8 females and 8 males in each treatment group) in the hippocampus at 2 months. The results of mRNA quantification are depicted in (A,B). **P < 0.01 vs. Male CON group; #P < 0.05 and ##P < 0.01 vs. Male CSD3h group; $P < 0.05 and $$P < 0.01 vs. Male CSD6h group; aP < 0.05 and aaP < 0.01 vs. Female CON group; bP < 0.05 and bbP < 0.01 vs. Female CSD3h group; cP < 0.05 and ccP < 0.01 vs. Female CSD6h group.

Correlations between anxious/cognitive performance and the expression levels of BDNF and Syt-1

Correlation with protein levels of BDNF and Syt-1

Linear correlation analyses revealed that the hippocampal protein levels of BDNF were negatively correlated with the distance swam in the learning phase in all groups and positively correlated with the percentage of distance swam in the target quadrant in the memory phase in the CON, CSD6h, FSD3h, and FSD6h in the MWM test (Ps < 0.05). Additionally, the hippocampal protein levels of Syt-1 in all groups were positively correlated with the distance swam and negatively correlated with the percentage of distance swam (Ps < 0.05; Table 2). None of the anxiety-related indicators correlated with the hippocampal protein levels of BDNF and Syt-1 in the OF and EPM test (P < 0.05; Table 2).

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Correlation between the performance in the anxiety/cognition-related tasks and levels of hippocampal synaptic proteins [r (p)].

Correlation with the mRNA levels of BDNF and Syt-1

The results revealed the hippocampal levels of BDNF mRNA in all groups were negatively correlated with the distance swam in the learning phase and positively correlated with the percentage of distance swam in the memory phase in the MWM test (Ps < 0.05). Furthermore, the hippocampal mRNA levels of Syt-1 in all groups were positively correlated with the distance swam and negatively correlated with the percentage of distance swam in the target quadrant (Ps < 0.05; Table 3). Most of the anxiety-related indicators were not correlated with the hippocampal mRNA levels of BFNF and Syt-1 in the OF and EPM tests (Table 3).

TABLE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. Correlation between the performance in the anxiety/cognition-related tasks and hippocampal mRNA levels of BDNF and Syt-1 [r (p)].

Discussion

Sleep disturbance is a common phenomenon in women during pregnancy (Pien and Schwab, 2004; Ross et al., 2005), and it is widely accepted that this may have deleterious effects on anxiety and cognition in offspring (Peng et al., 2016). In this study, pregnant CD-1 mice were used for studying the different patterns and durations of SD during late pregnancy for evaluating the anxious- and cognitive-related behaviors and alterations in the expression of synaptic plasticity-related proteins in offspring. Our results demonstrated that 6 h of CSD and 3/6 h of FSD in mothers can lead to increased anxiety-like behaviors and cognitive impairment in the offspring, while FSD may induce more severe cognitive impairment. Furthermore, the impaired cognitive performance in adolescent offspring may be attributed to a reduction in the expression of the Bdnf gene and an increase in the expression of the Syt-1 gene in the hippocampus.

The effects of SD during pregnancy on anxiety-like behaviors in offspring

In this study, the adolescent mice from CSD6h, FSD3h, and FSD6h groups exhibited anxiety-like behaviors in the OF and EMP tests. The findings were consistent with a previous study that showed that 2-month-old Sprague–Dawley rats, whose mothers underwent 6 h SD per day (12:00–18:00 h) during late pregnancy by gently tapping or rattling the cage, displayed anxiety-like behavior in the EPM test and novelty-suppressed feeding test (Peng et al., 2016). A previous study reporting similar findings demonstrated that the number of ultrasonic vocalizations (USVs) reduced during peak vocalization days (postnatal days 9–11) in the offspring of Wistar rats who experienced REM SD in late pregnancy. USVs in rat pups are considered to be homologous to the cries of human infants and are regarded as an expression of anxiety (Gulia et al., 2014). However, contrasting studies have demonstrated that 5 h of SD per day (9:00–14:00 h) in Wistar rats through continually enforced locomotion in late pregnancy can lead to reduced anxiety-like behaviors in pre-adolescent, adolescent, and post-adolescent offspring rats during the EPM test (Radhakrishnan et al., 2015). The discrepancy in these observations could be attributed to the different strains of rodents and variations in the duration and protocol used for SD. The present study further compared the effects of 3 and 6 h of FSD and CSD on anxiety-related behaviors, and the results demonstrated that the anxiety levels of the mice in the FSD3h, FSD6h, and CSD6h groups were similar, suggesting that continuous and fragmented SD may affect the development of the emotion-related regions of the brain to a similar extent. Interestingly, the effects of FSD induced at different durations did not have a “dose–effect relationship” on the anxiety levels. The anxiety levels of the mice in the CSD3h group were not significantly different from those in the CON group and could be attributed to the fact that the intensity of stress in the CSD3h did not reach the threshold.

Effects of SD during pregnancy on the cognition-related behaviors of offspring

The results of this study demonstrated that the mice in the CSD6h, FSD3h, and FSD6h groups had impaired spatial learning and memory, and the FSD3h and FSD6h groups were more severe than that in the CSD6h group. Previous studies have similarly demonstrated that 6 h of SD per day (12:00–6:00 h) in Sprague–Dawley rats during early/middle/late pregnancy leads to impaired spatial learning and memory in 2-month-old offspring (Peng et al., 2016). In this study, we considered that SD in late pregnancy could be prenatal stress. The impact of prenatal stress on brain function has been associated with alterations in the regulation of the hypothalamic–pituitary–adrenal (HPA) axis of mothers, resulting in fetal exposure to high levels of corticosterone (Weinstock, 1997). Studies have demonstrated that prenatal exposure to high levels of corticosterone causes hyperactivation of the HPA axis with a concomitant reduction in the density of corticosteroid receptors in the hippocampus of adult offspring (Barbazanges et al., 1996) and mediates cognitive behavioral changes induced by prenatal stress in mice (Abdul Aziz et al., 2012). The previous study showed that prenatal stress leads to increased serum corticosterone and reduced hippocampal glucocorticoid receptors in adult offspring rats, and offspring impaired learning and memory may be related to the HPA axis (Zheng et al., 2019). Based on previous studies, we speculate that the impaired cognition of the mice in the CSD6h, FSD3h, and FSD6h groups may be attributed to the hyperactivation of the HPA axis and the noxious effects of corticosterone, which may be a potential mechanism. Some studies showed that chronic intermittent hypoxia and chronic intermittent cold stress may lead to HPA sensitization and elevated reactivity of the HPA axis (Ma et al., 2008; Girotti et al., 2011). We, therefore, speculate that the cognitive impairment observed in the FSD3h and FSD6h groups was more severe than that in the CSD6h group and might be attributed to the intermittent activation of the HPA axis induced by the repetitive awakenings of the pregnant mice. There were no differences between the CSD3h and CON groups, which could be possibly attributed to the fact that the threshold for the activation of the HPA axis was not reached. Unfortunately, we did not measure the activity of the HPA axis in this experiment, which will merit further study in our future experiments.

Effects of SD on the hippocampal expression of Bdnf and Syt-1 in offspring

Numerous studies have suggested that the hippocampal BDNF and Syt-1 are closely related to hippocampus-dependent learning and memory (Amidfar et al., 2020; Shi et al., 2020). The hippocampal levels of BDNF have been reported to be downregulated under conditions of cognitive impairment induced by prenatal restraint stress (Shang et al., 2019), perinatal ethanol exposure (Mahdinia et al., 2021), and chronic social isolation stress (Bagheri et al., 2021) in rodents. Previous studies demonstrated that prenatal restraint stress enhances the expression of Syt-1 in the hippocampus of offspring, which leads to the dendritic atrophy of pyramidal neurons by increasing the release of glutamate (Jia et al., 2010). Consistently, in this study, the hippocampal protein and mRNA of BDNF decreased and Syt-1 increased in the mice of prenatal experienced CSD6h, FSD3h, and FSD6h, and that exposure to FSD has greater effects on the hippocampal levels of both BDNF and Syt-1 than CSD. Although the effects of MSD on the hippocampal levels of BDNF and Syt-1 in the offspring were different, the mechanism underlying this difference remains to be elucidated.

Correlation between anxiety-/cognition-related performance and expression of Bdnf and Syt-1 genes

A reduction in hippocampal levels of BDNF is associated with cognitive decline, which has been reported in numerous rodent models of cognitive declines, such as Sprague–Dawley rats subjected to 48 h of SD (Wadhwa et al., 2017), Wistar rat offspring whose mothers experienced chronic mild stress during pregnancy (Guan et al., 2016), and Sprague–Dawley rats who experienced maternal separation (Ohta et al., 2017). Consistent with these results, we observed that the hippocampal levels of BDNF were correlated with the indicators in the MWM test. Syt-1 may be a stress-responsive gene that alters synaptic plasticity and is abundant in the hippocampus (Thome et al., 2001; Fox and Sanes, 2007). Our previous studies have reported that the increased hippocampal expression of Syt-1 protein and mRNA is associated with impaired learning and memory during “pathological” aging resulting from prenatal inflammatory insult (Zhang et al., 2020). Similarly, the results of the present study demonstrated that the expression of increased hippocampal Syt-1 might be associated with impaired spatial learning and memory in adolescent mice resulting from prenatal exposure to SD.

Accumulating evidence demonstrates that apart from its role in learning and memory, the hippocampus is also the central target of the mesolimbic system that is related to the modulation of anxiety behaviors (Bannerman et al., 2004; Rezvanfard et al., 2009; Solati, 2011; Tang et al., 2019). The results of the present study demonstrated that there was no correlation between anxious behavioral indices in the OF and EPM tests and the expression of hippocampal of Bdnf and Syt-1 genes in CD-1 mice whose mothers experienced SD during late pregnancy.

The present study has certain limitations, as described hereafter. First, immunohistochemistry analyses were not performed for quantitative analysis of the effect of MSD on the expression levels of BDNF and Syt-1 in the different subregions of the hippocampus. Second, the changes in the markers of the HPA axis of mothers and offspring mice were not measured, including the expression of corticosterone and glucocorticoid receptors. Third, RNA interference technologies were not used for reducing the expression of BDNF and increasing the expression of Syt-1 for verifying the targets of SD.

Summary

In conclusion, this experiment is the first to use fragmented sleep deprivation patterns that simulate abnormal sleep patterns of pregnant women to explore its effects on offspring anxiety and cognition and the underlying molecular mechanisms, the present study demonstrated that FSD and CSD in CD-1 mice in late pregnancy can lead to increased anxiety-like behavior and cognitive impairment in adolescent offspring mice, and FSD induced worse cognitive performance than CSD. The study also demonstrated a possible link between impaired cognition and the decreased expression of the Bdnf gene and the increased expression of the Syt1 gene in the hippocampus.

Data availability statement

The original contributions presented in the study are included in the article/Supplementary material, further inquiries can be directed to the corresponding authors.

Ethics statement

The animal study was reviewed and approved by the Association of Laboratory Animal Sciences and the Center for Laboratory Animal Sciences at Anhui Medical University (approval number: LLSC20190710).

Author contributions

R-MW and Y-MZ designed the study, performed the behavioral tests, and prepared the manuscript. YL and Q-TW performed western blotting and real-time fluorescence-based quantitative polymerase chain reaction. Y-TW and X-YL analyzed the data and prepared the graphs. G-HC and X-WL revised the manuscript and were responsible for the completeness and accuracy of the data. All authors have read and approved the final manuscript.

Funding

This work was financially supported by the National Natural Science Foundation of China (Grant Number: 81671316), the Scientific Research Fund Project of Hunan Provincial Health Commission (Grant Number: 20200497), and the Natural Science Foundation of Hunan Province of China (Grant Number: 2021JJ70040).

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher's note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

Supplementary material

The Supplementary Material for this article can be found online at: https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fnbeh.2022.1066725/full#supplementary-material

Supplementary Figure 1. The learning swimming velocity and distance of the Morris water maze (MWM) test in the CSD3h and CSD6h groups. The speed and swimming distance between male and female mice were not statistically different in the CSD3h (A,C) and CSD6h (B,D) groups. Error bars = SEM. There are 8 male and 8 female mice in each treatment group.

Supplementary Figure 2. The learning swimming velocity and distance of the Morris water maze (MWM) test in the FSD3h and FSD6h groups. The speed and swimming distance between male and female mice were not statistically different in the FSD3h (A,C) and FSD6h (B,D) groups. Error bars = SEM. There are 8 male and 8 female mice in each treatment group.

Supplementary Figure 3. The vaginal smears in the diestrus phase.

References

Abdul Aziz, N. H., Kendall, D. A., and Pardon, M. C. (2012). Prenatal exposure to chronic mild stress increases corticosterone levels in the amniotic fluid and induces cognitive deficits in female offspring, improved by treatment with the antidepressant drug amitriptyline. Behav. Brain Res. 231, 29–39. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2012.02.040

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Alvarenga, T. A., Aguiar, M. F., Mazaro-Costa, R., Tufik, S., and Andersen, M. L. (2013). Effects of sleep deprivation during pregnancy on the reproductive capability of the offspring. Fertil. Steril. 100, 1752–1757. doi: 10.1016/j.fertnstert.2013.08.014

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Amidfar, M., de Oliveira, J., Kucharska, E., Budni, J., and Kim, Y. K. (2020). The role of CREB and BDNF in neurobiology and treatment of Alzheimer's disease. Life Sci. 257, 118020. doi: 10.1016/j.lfs.2020.118020

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Anacker, C., and Hen, R. (2017). Adult hippocampal neurogenesis and cognitive flexibility - linking memory and mood. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 18, 335–346. doi: 10.1038/nrn.2017.45

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Autry, A. E., and Monteggia, L. M. (2012). Brain-derived neurotrophic factor and neuropsychiatric disorders. Pharmacol. Rev. 64, 238–258. doi: 10.1124/pr.111.005108

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bagheri, S., Moradi, K., Ehghaghi, E., Badripour, A., Keykhaei, M., Ashraf-Ganjouei, A., et al. (2021). Melatonin improves learning and memory of mice with chronic social isolation stress via an interaction between microglia polarization and BDNF/TrkB/CREB signaling pathway. Eur. J. Pharmacol. 908, 174358. doi: 10.1016/j.ejphar.2021.174358

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bannerman, D. M., Rawlins, J. N., McHugh, S. B., Deacon, R. M., Yee, B. K., Bast, T., et al. (2004). Regional dissociations within the hippocampus–memory and anxiety. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 28, 273–283. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2004.03.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Barbazanges, A., Piazza, P. V., Le Moal, M., and Maccari, S. (1996). Maternal glucocorticoid secretion mediates long-term effects of prenatal stress. J. Neurosci. 16, 3943–3949. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.16-12-03943.1996

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chang, J. J., Pien, G. W., Duntley, S. P., and Macones, G. A. (2010). Sleep deprivation during pregnancy and maternal and fetal outcomes: is there a relationship?. Sleep Med. Rev. 14, 107–114. doi: 10.1016/j.smrv.2009.05.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chapman, E. R. (2002). Synaptotagmin: A Ca(2+) sensor that triggers exocytosis? Nat. Rev. Mol. Cell Biol. 3, 498–508. doi: 10.1038/nrm855

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chen, J., Zhang, Z. Z., Luo, B. L., Yang, Q. G., Ni, M. Z., Wu, Q. T., et al. (2022). Prenatal exposure to inflammation increases anxiety-like behaviors in F1 and F2 generations: possible links to decreased FABP7 in hippocampus. Front. Behav. Neurosci. 16, 973069. doi: 10.3389/fnbeh.2022.973069

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fox, M. A., and Sanes, J. R. (2007). Synaptotagmin I and II are present in distinct subsets of central synapses. J. Comp. Neurol. 503, 280–296. doi: 10.1002/cne.21381

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Girotti, M., Donegan, J. J., and Morilak, D. A. (2011). Chronic intermittent cold stress sensitizes neuro-immune reactivity in the rat brain. Psychoneuroendocrinology. 36, 1164–1174. doi: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2011.02.008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Grigoryan, G., and Segal, M. (2016). Lasting differential effects on plasticity induced by prenatal stress in dorsal and ventral hippocampus. Neural Plast. 2016, 2540462. doi: 10.1155/2016/2540462

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Guan, S. Z., Ning, L., Tao, N., Lian, Y. L., Liu, J. W., and Ng, T. B. (2016). Effects of maternal stress during pregnancy on learning and memory via hippocampal BDNF, Arc (Arg3.1) expression in offspring. Environ. Toxicol. Pharmacol. 46, 158–167. doi: 10.1016/j.etap.2016.04.012

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gulia, K. K., Patel, N., Radhakrishnan, A., and Kumar, V. M. (2014). Reduction in ultrasonic vocalizations in pups born to rapid eye movement sleep restricted mothers in rat model. PLoS ONE. 9, e84948. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0084948

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Izci-Balserak, B., Keenan, B. T., Corbitt, C., Staley, B., Perlis, M., and Pien, G. W. (2018). Changes in sleep characteristics and breathing parameters during sleep in early and late pregnancy. J. Clin. Sleep Med. 14, 1161–1168. doi: 10.5664/jcsm.7216

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jia, N., Yang, K., Sun, Q., Cai, Q., Li, H., Cheng, D., et al. (2010). Prenatal stress causes dendritic atrophy of pyramidal neurons in hippocampal CA3 region by glutamate in offspring rats. Dev. Neurobiol. 70, 114–125. doi: 10.1002/dneu.20766

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Khalyfa, A., Mutskov, V., Carreras, A., Khalyfa, A. A., Hakim, F., and Gozal, D. (2014). Sleep fragmentation during late gestation induces metabolic perturbations and epigenetic changes in adiponectin gene expression in male adult offspring mice. Diabetes 63, 3230–3241. doi: 10.2337/db14-0202

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kim, S. M., Zhang, S., Park, J., Sung, H. J., Tran, T. T., Chung, C., et al. (2021). REM sleep deprivation impairs learning and memory by decreasing brain O-GlcNAc cycling in mouse. Neurotherapeutics 18, 2504–2517. doi: 10.1007/s13311-021-01094-7

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kota, D., Sanjakdar, S., Marks, M. J., Khabour, O., Alzoubi, K., and Damaj, M. I. (2011). Exploring behavioral and molecular mechanisms of nicotine reward in adolescent mice. Biochem. Pharmacol. 82, 1008–1014. doi: 10.1016/j.bcp.2011.06.019

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Leal, G., Comprido, D., and Duarte, C. B. (2014). BDNF-induced local protein synthesis and synaptic plasticity. Neuropharmacology 76, 639–656. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropharm.2013.04.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Li, X. Y., Wang, F., Chen, G. H., Li, X. W., Yang, Q. G., Cao, L., et al. (2016). Inflammatory insult during pregnancy accelerates age-related behavioral and neurobiochemical changes in CD-1 mice. Age 38, 59. doi: 10.1007/s11357-016-9920-3

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Longordo, F., Kopp, C., and Lüthi, A. (2009). Consequences of sleep deprivation on neurotransmitter receptor expression and function. Eur. J. Neurosci. 29, 1810–1819. doi: 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2009.06719.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ma, S., Mifflin, S. W., Cunningham, J. T., and Morilak, D. A. (2008). Chronic intermittent hypoxia sensitizes acute hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal stress reactivity and Fos induction in the rat locus coeruleus in response to subsequent immobilization stress. Neuroscience 154, 1639–1647. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2008.04.068

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Machado, R. B., Hipólide, D. C., Benedito-Silva, A. A., and Tufik, S. (2004). Sleep deprivation induced by the modified multiple platform technique: quantification of sleep loss and recovery. Brain Res. 1004, 45–51. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2004.01.019

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mahdinia, R., Goudarzi, I., Lashkarbolouki, T., and Salmani, M. E. (2021). Vitamin E attenuates alterations in learning, memory and BDNF levels caused by perinatal ethanol exposure. Nutr. Neurosci. 24, 747–761. doi: 10.1080/1028415X.2019.1674523

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Menezes, J., Souto das Neves, B. H., Gonçalves, R., Benetti, F., and Mello-Carpes, P. B. (2020). Maternal deprivation impairs memory and cognitive flexibility, effect that is avoided by environmental enrichment. Behav. Brain Res. 381, 112468. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2020.112468

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mindell, J. A., Cook, R. A., and Nikolovski, J. (2015). Sleep patterns and sleep disturbances across pregnancy. Sleep Med. 16, 483–488. doi: 10.1016/j.sleep.2014.12.006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ng, K. Y., Yong, J., and Chakraborty, T. R. (2010). Estrous cycle in ob/ob and ovariectomized female mice and its relation with estrogen and leptin. Physiol. Behav. 99, 125–130. doi: 10.1016/j.physbeh.2009.11.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ohta, K. I., Suzuki, S., Warita, K., Kaji, T., Kusaka, T., and Miki, T. (2017). Prolonged maternal separation attenuates BDNF-ERK signaling correlated with spine formation in the hippocampus during early brain development. J. Neurochem. 141, 179–194. doi: 10.1111/jnc.13977

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Peng, Y., Wang, W., Tan, T., He, W., Dong, Z., Wang, Y. T., et al. (2016). Maternal sleep deprivation at different stages of pregnancy impairs the emotional and cognitive functions, and suppresses hippocampal long-term potentiation in the offspring rats. Mol. Brain 9, 17. doi: 10.1186/s13041-016-0197-3

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pien, G. W., and Schwab, R. J. (2004). Sleep disorders during pregnancy. Sleep 27, 1405–1417. doi: 10.1093/sleep/27.7.1405

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Radhakrishnan, A., Aswathy, B. S., Kumar, V. M., and Gulia, K. K. (2015). Sleep deprivation during late pregnancy produces hyperactivity and increased risk-taking behavior in offspring. Brain Res. 1596, 88–98. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2014.11.021

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rezvanfard, M., Zarrindast, M. R., and Bina, P. (2009). Role of ventral hippocampal GABA(A) and NMDA receptors in the anxiolytic effect of carbamazepine in rats using the elevated plus maze test. Pharmacology 84, 356–366. doi: 10.1159/000256666

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ross, L. E., Murray, B. J., and Steiner, M. (2005). Sleep and perinatal mood disorders: a critical review. J. Psychiatry Neurosci. 30, 247–256.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Schaafsma, W., Basterra, L. B., Jacobs, S., Brouwer, N., Meerlo, P., Schaafsma, A., et al. (2017). Maternal inflammation induces immune activation of fetal microglia and leads to disrupted microglia immune responses, behavior, and learning performance in adulthood. Neurobiol. Dis. 106, 291–300. doi: 10.1016/j.nbd.2017.07.017

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sedov, I. D., Cameron, E. E., Madigan, S., and Tomfohr-Madsen, L. M. (2018). Sleep quality during pregnancy: a meta-analysis. Sleep Med. Rev. 38, 168–176. doi: 10.1016/j.smrv.2017.06.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Shang, Y., Wang, X., Li, F., Yin, T., Zhang, J., and Zhang, T. (2019). rTMS ameliorates prenatal stress-induced cognitive deficits in male-offspring rats associated with BDNF/TrkB signaling pathway. Neurorehabil. Neural Repair. 33, 271–283. doi: 10.1177/1545968319834898

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Shi, Z., Zhang, K., Zhou, H., Jiang, L., Xie, B., Wang, R., et al. (2020). Increased miR-34c mediates synaptic deficits by targeting synaptotagmin 1 through ROS-JNK-p53 pathway in Alzheimer's disease. Aging Cell 19, e13125. doi: 10.1111/acel.13125

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Solati, J. (2011). Dorsal hippocampal N-methyl-D-aspartate glutamatergic and δ-opioidergic systems modulate anxiety behaviors in rats in a noninteractive manner. Kaohsiung J. Med. Sci. 27, 485–493. doi: 10.1016/j.kjms.2011.06.011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Spear, L. P. (2000). The adolescent brain and age-related behavioral manifestations. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 24, 417–463. doi: 10.1016/S0149-7634(00)00014-2

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tang, M., Huang, H., Li, S., Zhou, M., Liu, Z., Huang, R., et al. (2019). Hippocampal proteomic changes of susceptibility and resilience to depression or anxiety in a rat model of chronic mild stress. Transl. Psychiatry 9, 260. doi: 10.1038/s41398-019-0605-4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tartar, J. L., Ward, C. P., McKenna, J. T., Thakkar, M., Arrigoni, E., McCarley, R. W., et al. (2006). Hippocampal synaptic plasticity and spatial learning are impaired in a rat model of sleep fragmentation. Eur. J. Neurosci. 23, 2739–2748. doi: 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2006.04808.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Thome, J., Pesold, B., Baader, M., Hu, M., Gewirtz, J. C., Duman, R. S., et al. (2001). Stress differentially regulates synaptophysin and synaptotagmin expression in hippocampus. Biol. Psychiatry 50, 809–812. doi: 10.1016/S0006-3223(01)01229-X

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tong, J. J., Chen, G. H., Wang, F., Li, X. W., Cao, L., Sui, X., et al. (2015). Chronic acarbose treatment alleviates age-related behavioral and biochemical changes in SAMP8 mice. Behav. Brain Res. 284, 138–152. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2015.01.052

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Trzepizur, W., Khalyfa, A., Qiao, Z., Popko, B., and Gozal, D. (2017). Integrated stress response activation by sleep fragmentation during late gestation in mice leads to emergence of adverse metabolic phenotype in offspring. Metab. Clin. Exp. 69, 188–198. doi: 10.1016/j.metabol.2017.01.026

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vecsey, C. G., Wimmer, M. E., Havekes, R., Park, A. J., Perron, I. J., Meerlo, P., et al. (2013). Daily acclimation handling does not affect hippocampal long-term potentiation or cause chronic sleep deprivation in mice. Sleep 36, 601–607. doi: 10.5665/sleep.2556

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wadhwa, M., Prabhakar, A., Ray, K., Roy, K., Kumari, P., Jha, P. K., et al. (2017). Inhibiting the microglia activation improves the spatial memory and adult neurogenesis in rat hippocampus during 48 h of sleep deprivation. J. Neuroinflamm. 14, 222. doi: 10.1186/s12974-017-0998-z

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ward, C. P., McCarley, R. W., and Strecker, R. E. (2009). Experimental sleep fragmentation impairs spatial reference but not working memory in Fischer/Brown Norway rats. J. Sleep Res. 18, 238–244. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2869.2008.00714.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Weinstock, M. (1997). Does prenatal stress impair coping and regulation of hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal axis?. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 21, 1–10. doi: 10.1016/S0149-7634(96)00014-0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wessels, J. M., Leyland, N. A., Agarwal, S. K., and Foster, W. G. (2015). Estrogen induced changes in uterine brain-derived neurotrophic factor and its receptors. Human Reprod. 30, 925–936. doi: 10.1093/humrep/dev018

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wilson, D. L., Barnes, M., Ellett, L., Permezel, M., Jackson, M., and Crowe, S. F. (2011). Decreased sleep efficiency, increased wake after sleep onset and increased cortical arousals in late pregnancy. Aust. N. Z. J. Obstet. Gynaecol. 51, 38–46. doi: 10.1111/j.1479-828X.2010.01252.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wu, Y. F., Zhang, Y. M., Ge, H. H., Ren, C. Y., Zhang, Z. Z., Cao, L., et al. (2020). Effects of embryonic inflammation and adolescent psychosocial environment on cognition and hippocampal Staufen in middle-aged mice. Front. Aging Neurosci. 12, 578719. doi: 10.3389/fnagi.2020.578719

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Xie, Y., Deng, S. Y., Chen, S. M., Chen, X. J., Lai, W. W., Huang, L. F., et al. (2020). A chronic sleep fragmentation model using vibrating orbital rotor to induce cognitive deficit and anxiety-like behavior in young wild-type mice. J. Visual. Exp. 163, e61531. doi: 10.3791/61531

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Yu, Y., Huang, Z., Dai, C., Du, Y., Han, H., Wang, Y. T., et al. (2018). Facilitated AMPAR endocytosis causally contributes to the maternal sleep deprivation-induced impairments of synaptic plasticity and cognition in the offspring rats. Neuropharmacology 133, 155–162. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropharm.2018.01.030

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zhang, Y. M., Cheng, Y. Z., Wang, Y. T., Wei, R. M., Ge, Y. J., Kong, X. Y., et al. (2022). Environmental enrichment reverses maternal sleep deprivation-induced anxiety-like behavior and cognitive impairment in CD-1 mice. Front. Behav. Neurosci. 16, 943900. doi: 10.3389/fnbeh.2022.943900

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zhang, Z. Z., Zeng, L. P., Chen, J., Wu, Y. F., Wang, Y. T., Xia, L., et al. (2022). Long-term environmental enrichment relieves dysfunctional cognition and synaptic protein levels induced by prenatal inflammation in older CD-1 mice. Neural Plast. 2022, 1483101. doi: 10.1155/2022/1483101

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zhang, Z. Z., Zhuang, Z. Q., Sun, S. Y., Ge, H. H., Wu, Y. F., Cao, L., et al. (2020). Effects of prenatal exposure to inflammation coupled with stress exposure during adolescence on cognition and synaptic protein levels in aged CD-1 mice. Front. Aging Neurosci. 12, 157. doi: 10.3389/fnagi.2020.00157

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zhao, Q., Peng, C., Wu, X., Chen, Y., Wang, C., and You, Z. (2014). Maternal sleep deprivation inhibits hippocampal neurogenesis associated with inflammatory response in young offspring rats. Neurobiol. Dis. 68, 57–65. doi: 10.1016/j.nbd.2014.04.008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zheng, X. X., Chen, Y. W., Yue, Y. S., Li, Y. C., Xia, S. Z., Li, Y., et al. (2019). Icariin ameliorates learning and memory impairments through ERK/CaMKIIα/CREB signaling and HPA axis in prenatally stressed female offspring. Biomed. Pharmacother. 117, 109077. doi: 10.1016/j.biopha.2019.109077

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: sleep deprivation, spatial learning and memory, anxiety, BDNF, synaptotagmin-1

Citation: Wei R-M, Zhang Y-M, Li Y, Wu Q-T, Wang Y-T, Li X-Y, Li X-W and Chen G-H (2022) Altered cognition and anxiety in adolescent offspring whose mothers underwent different-pattern maternal sleep deprivation, and cognition link to hippocampal expressions of Bdnf and Syt-1. Front. Behav. Neurosci. 16:1066725. doi: 10.3389/fnbeh.2022.1066725

Received: 11 October 2022; Accepted: 15 November 2022;
Published: 08 December 2022.

Edited by:

Danusa Mar Arcego, McGill University, Canada

Reviewed by:

Hossein Salmani, Jiroft University of Medical Sciences, Iran
Giuseppe Talani, National Research Council (CNR), Italy

Copyright © 2022 Wei, Zhang, Li, Wu, Wang, Li, Li and Chen. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Gui-Hai Chen, doctorcgh@163.com; Xue-Wei Li, lixuewei2003@163.com

These authors have contributed equally to this work

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.