Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Behav. Neurosci., 29 July 2021
Sec. Pathological Conditions
This article is part of the Research Topic Gene and Environment Interactions in Neurodevelopmental Disorders View all 11 articles

The Interplay Between Prenatal Adversity, Offspring Dopaminergic Genes, and Early Parenting on Toddler Attentional Function

\r\nEszter Szekely,&#x;Eszter Szekely1,2†Alexia Jolicoeur-Martineau,,&#x;Alexia Jolicoeur-Martineau2,3,4†Leslie AtkinsonLeslie Atkinson5Robert D. Levitan,Robert D. Levitan6,7Meir SteinerMeir Steiner8John E. LydonJohn E. Lydon9Alison S. FlemingAlison S. Fleming10James L. KennedyJames L. Kennedy6Ashley Wazana,,*Ashley Wazana1,2,11* on behalf of the MAVAN Team
  • 1Department of Psychiatry, McGill University Faculty of Medicine, Montreal, QC, Canada
  • 2Lady Davis Institute for Medical Research, Jewish General Hospital, Montreal, QC, Canada
  • 3MILA—Quebec Artificial Intelligence Institute, Montreal, QC, Canada
  • 4Department of Computer Sciences, Université de Montréal, Montreal, QC, Canada
  • 5Department of Psychology, Ryerson University, Toronto, ON, Canada
  • 6Centre for Addiction and Mental Health, Toronto, ON, Canada
  • 7Department of Psychiatry, University of Toronto, Toronto, ON, Canada
  • 8Department of Psychiatry and Behavioural Neurosciences, McMaster University, Hamilton, ON, Canada
  • 9Department of Psychology, McGill University, Montreal, QC, Canada
  • 10Department of Psychology, University of Toronto Mississauga, Toronto, ON, Canada
  • 11Centre for Child Development and Mental Health, Jewish General Hospital, Montreal, QC, Canada

Background: Few studies have explored the complex gene-by-prenatal environment-by-early postnatal environment interactions that underlie the development of attentional competence. Here, we examined if variation in dopamine-related genes interacts with prenatal adversity to influence toddler attentional competence and whether this influence is buffered by early positive maternal behavior.

Methods: From the Maternal Adversity, Vulnerability and Neurodevelopment cohort, 134 participants (197 when imputing missing data) had information on prenatal adversity (prenatal stressful life events, prenatal maternal depressive symptoms, and birth weight), five dopamine-related genes (DAT1, DRD4, DRD2, COMT, BDNF), observed maternal parenting behavior at 6 months and parent-rated toddler attentional competence at 18 and 24 months. The Latent Environmental and Genetic Interaction (LEGIT) approach was used to examine genes-by-prenatal environment-by-postnatal environment interactions while controlling for sociodemographic factors and postnatal depression.

Results: Our hypothesis of a three-way interaction between prenatal adversity, dopamine-related genes, and early maternal parenting behavior was not confirmed. However, consistent two-way interactions emerged between prenatal adversity and dopamine-related genes; prenatal adversity and maternal parenting behavior, and dopamine-related genes and maternal parenting behavior in relation to toddler attentional competence. Significant interaction effects were driven by the DAT1, COMT, and BDNF genotypes; prenatal stressful life events; maternal sensitivity, tactile stimulation, vocalization, and infant-related activities.

Conclusions: Multiple dopamine-related genes affected toddler attentional competence and they did so in interaction with prenatal adversity and the early rearing environment, separately. Effects were already visible in young children. Several aspects of early maternal parenting have been identified as potential targets for intervention.

Introduction

There is increasing evidence that an adverse prenatal environment contributes to the risk of developing attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD; Banerjee et al., 2007; Thapar and Rutter, 2009). The most commonly studied prenatal risks of ADHD are maternal lifestyle factors, such as smoking, alcohol consumption, substance use, and severe stress/anxiety experienced during pregnancy (Fleming et al., 1988; Banerjee et al., 2007; Li et al., 2010). Further, low birth weight and prematurity at birth—as indicators of a suboptimal intrauterine environment—have also been implicated in the risk for ADHD, particularly inattention symptoms (Bhutta et al., 2002; Strang-Karlsson et al., 2008). However, much less is known about the role of prenatal maternal depression in the development of offspring ADHD symptoms. This is important given that approximately 40% of mothers of children with ADHD have a history of major depression, making them 2–3 times more likely to be depressed than women in the general population (Chronis-Tuscano et al., 2003; Kessler et al., 2006). Furthermore, prenatal depression is consistently linked to shorter gestation and lower birth weight, which are both common risk factors of ADHD (Field et al., 2006; Field, 2011). The available literature suggests that maternal depressive symptoms during pregnancy can negatively shape the offspring’s attention system and increase the risk of comorbidity in those children who already have a diagnosis of ADHD (Chronis-Tuscano et al., 2010; Van Batenburg Eddes et al., 2013). Based on the above, in the present study we capture prenatal adversity in three important ways: through the number of stressful life events experienced by women during pregnancy, maternal depressive symptoms during pregnancy, and birth weight of children.

The considerable variability in neurodevelopmental outcomes among children who experience prenatal adversity indicates potential differences in children’s vulnerability to the environment. Previous research has highlighted the importance of genetic factors in conferring such vulnerability (Caspi et al., 2002; Rutter, 2006; Laucht et al., 2007). Indeed, gene-environment interactions (G × E) are increasingly recognized as important contributors to the emergence of psychopathology (Caspi and Moffitt, 2006; Rutter, 2006; Belsky et al., 2009). Yet, there have been few published studies examining the contribution of G × E effects to ADHD (Thapar et al., 2007; Nigg, 2012) and even fewer that have specifically focused on the prenatal environment (for a review, see Franke and Buitelaar, 2018). Not surprisingly, these studies have mainly focused on dopaminergic genes, as both pharmacological and genetic research suggest a critical role for dopamine in attentional, motivational, and exploratory neurobehavioral processes (Faraone et al., 2005; Thapar et al., 2007). Regarding attention in particular, animal studies have suggested a direct link between selective lesions of dopaminergic neurons and altered attentional processes in rodents and primates (Nieoullon, 2002; Thiele and Bellgrove, 2018). Based on these studies, the specific attention components that were most affected included selective attention, spatial attention, detection of novelty, and sustained attention (for a review, see Nieoullon, 2002). Notably, the exact result of lesioning dopaminergic neurons in different brain regions depended on the nature of the brain area concerned. As dopamine is mainly present in the frontal cortex and basal ganglia in the brain, it is hypothesized that attention deficits might confer alterations in these subcortical brain structures closely linked to cortical regions rather than simple alterations in dopaminergic transmission (Nieoullon, 2002). Thus, behavioral changes following cortical dopamine depletion have to be interpreted in light of any associated changes in dopaminergic transmission at a subcortical level (Nieoullon, 2002). For instance, methylphenidate, a drug that is most commonly used in the treatment of ADHD by modulating dopaminergic transmission, was found to equally increase frontal cortical activity in both healthy controls and children with ADHD during a response inhibition task, whereas it increased striatal activity only in children with ADHD and decreased it in healthy controls (Vaidya et al., 1998). More directly relevant to our study, genetic variation linked to dopaminergic transmission in both the frontal cortex and related subcortical regions impacted infant attention at age 9 months (Holmboe et al., 2010).

Dopaminergic Genes by Prenatal Adversity Interaction Effects on ADHD

The dopamine transporter DAT1 gene has been a prime candidate for research in this context. The gene codes for a solute carrier protein responsible for the reuptake of dopamine from the synaptic cleft to the presynaptic neuron. This protein is densely present in the striatum and nucleus accumbens and constitutes the primary mechanism of dopamine regulation in these brain regions (Ciliax et al., 1999). The most widely studied DAT1 polymorphism is a variable number tandem repeat (VNTR) sequence in the 3′ untranslated region that is 40 base pairs (bp) in length (Vandenbergh et al., 1992). The most common alleles are the 10 (480-bp; 71.9%) and 9 (440-bp; 23.4%) repeats (Doucette Stamm et al., 1995). This polymorphism is believed to be functional, influencing dopamine transporter availability and binding potential (Gizer et al., 2009) and is associated with sustained attention (Loo et al., 2003). DAT1 has been found to interact with prenatal maternal smoking (Brookes et al., 2006; Neuman et al., 2007), alcohol consumption (Kahn et al., 2003), and family adversity (Laucht et al., 2007) to increase the risk for ADHD. Some studies reported that the DAT1-prenatal maternal smoking interactions were significant only in boys homozygous for the 10-repeat allele and only for hyperactive-impulsive symptoms (Altink et al., 2008; Becker et al., 2008), while another, smaller study found no interaction effect for DAT1 and prenatal maternal smoking on ADHD (Langley et al., 2008).

Another popular candidate for G × E studies on ADHD is the dopamine receptor D4 gene (DRD4), specifically a 48bp VNTR on exon 3. DRD4 is predominantly expressed in the frontal lobe, such as the orbitofrontal cortex and anterior cingulate (Floresco and Maric, 2007). The most common alleles of this polymorphism are the 2-, 4-, and 7-repeat alleles, although this varies significantly across ethnic groups (Chang et al., 1996). This VNTR is likely functional in that the 7-repeat allele slightly differs from the 2- and 4-repeat alleles in secondary messenger activity and in response to clozapine, an antipsychotic medication (Asghari et al., 1994, 1995). The VNTR has further been found to influence sustained attention and information processing from an early age (Auerbach et al., 2001; Fan et al., 2003). In terms of its interaction with the environment, results suggest that the 7-repeat allele of DRD4 exacerbates the effects of prenatal adversity, as reflected in increased risk for ADHD and more severe ADHD symptoms (Grizenko et al., 2012). One study found similar relations but only in the case of teacher-reported inattention symptoms rather than parent-reported ADHD symptoms (Altink et al., 2008). Another, smaller study reported a lack of significant G × E between DRD4 and any measures of prenatal adversity (i.e., maternal smoking, alcohol use, or child’s birth weight (Langley et al., 2008).

An additional dopaminergic gene that has been examined in relation to environmental adversity and ADHD is the dopamine receptor D2 (DRD2) gene (Ficks and Waldman, 2009). DRD2 is expressed in the basal ganglia and prefrontal cortex and is key in regulating the mesolimbic reward system (Usiello et al., 2000). Studies of DRD2 have tended to focus on a TaqIA restriction site (rs1800497), downstream from DRD2 located in an exon of a neighboring gene, ANKK1 (Neville et al., 2004; Grizenko et al., 2012). Nonetheless, this polymorphism is known to influence DRD2 expression levels (Gluskin and Mickey, 2016). DRD2 has been implicated in affecting selective attention in patients with schizophrenia (Nkam et al., 2017). In terms of G × E interactions involving DRD2, ADHD was more prevalent among children whose mothers experienced less stable marital environments (i.e., having had no or multiple marriages) only if they were homozygous for the TaqI-A2 allele (Waldman, 2007).

Another important gene that has been studied in a G × E framework in ADHD is the catechol-O-methyltransferase (COMT), which is involved in the degradation catecholamines, such as dopamine. COMT has a particularly important role in the frontal cortex, where it accounts for approximately 50–60% of the metabolic degradation of dopamine (Karoum et al., 1994). The gene includes a common functional polymorphism with a methionine (“met”) to valine (“val”) substitution at codon 158. The met allele is associated with low enzyme activity, while the val allele is associated with high enzyme activity (Chen et al., 2004). This polymorphism has been implicated in relation to distractibility (Holmboe et al., 2010) and attentional control (Goldberg and Weinberger, 2004; Blasi et al., 2005; Ciampoli et al., 2017). Regarding its interaction with prenatal adversity among children with ADHD, one study found that those who carried the COMT val/val genotype (for rs4680) were more susceptible to the adverse effects of prenatal risks as indexed by lower birth weight to develop early-onset antisocial behavior (Thapar et al., 2005). Furthermore, in a combined analysis of two large cohorts (ALSPAC and PREDO) there was a robust interaction effect of child COMT (val/val rs4680) genotype with maternal prenatal anxiety to predict ADHD symptoms assessed at multiple time points (O’Donnell et al., 2017).

Finally, significant G × E effects have also been reported for inattention symptoms involving the brain-derived neurotrophic factor (BDNF) gene, which, besides being a regulator of neuronal development and function, plays a role in dopamine neurotransmission (Guillin et al., 2001; Narita et al., 2003). BDNF exerts influence on the brain’s mesolimbic and corticolimbic reward pathways by modulating their response to dopamine (Guillin et al., 2001). A common polymorphism on the BDNF gene in which a valine is replaced by a methionine at codon 66 (Val66Met; rs6265) has been shown to influence the intracellular trafficking and activity-dependent secretion of BDNF in brain (Chen et al., 2004). The BDNF gene has been associated with general cognitive performance (Dincheva et al., 2012). Within a G × E context, Lasky-Su et al. (2007) found that in lower SES environments children (6–18-year-old) carrying the risk alleles of rs1013442, rs1387144, or Val66Met was associated with having more inattention symptoms.

Parent-Child Interactions and Their Influence on ADHD

Notably, and perhaps more importantly for clinicians, certain environmental factors have the potential to modify the impact of prenatal adversity in genetically susceptible children (Thomas et al., 2015). Parenting, for instance, is a robust environmental predictor of developmental outcomes in children with ADHD (Deault, 2010). While positive parenting can protect against developing comorbidity in children with ADHD—even when exposed to maternal depression (Chronis-Tuscano et al., 2007)—negative parenting has been associated with elevated ADHD symptomatology above and beyond shared genetic effects (Harold et al., 2013). High levels of negativity in parent-child interactions or reciprocal coercive communication are common in families of children with ADHD (Danforth et al., 1991; Pfiffner et al., 2005; Romirowsky and Chronis-Tuscano, 2014). Sensitive parenting may be particularly effective at buffering the negative effects of prenatal adversity on child cognition and behavior (Laucht et al., 2001; Plamondon et al., 2015; Pickles et al., 2017). Randomized clinical control trials found that parent training promoting positive parent-child interactions was effective in ADHD (Young and Amarasinghe, 2010). Although the exact mechanisms are currently unknown, there is an indication that the positive effects of a more sensitive/less intrusive parenting style on ADHD may be indirect, by supporting the development of protective mechanisms, such as inhibitory control mechanisms in children (Miller et al., 2019). Importantly, the general recommendation is that for preschool children showing signs of ADHD parent training should be the first line of treatment, and medication introduced only in case when parent training is not effective (Daley et al., 2009; Pelham et al., 2016). Thus, the literature suggests that parents have a key role in the development of their children’s attention skills (Gauvain, 2001; Davis and Williams, 2011). Attentional competencies develop through dynamic and continuous interactions between the child and their physical and social surroundings (Vygotsky, 1978; Landry et al., 2002). In this process, parents initially regulate their child’s attention through supportive parenting or “scaffolding” until children are able to regulate their own attentional processes (Conner et al., 1997; Gauvain et al., 2001). Failure to develop appropriate attention regulation skills in early childhood can have lasting effects on later development and academic success (Blair, 2002). Although during the preschool years it may be challenging to differentiate delayed regulatory skills from true ADHD, research suggests that, in both cases, parent-child interactions may be key to minimizing later adverse outcomes (Davis and Williams, 2011).

Statistical Issues in Modeling Gene-by-Environment Interaction Effects

To date, most G × E studies on ADHD (and other psychiatric disorders) have considered a single genetic variant and a single environmental exposure at a time, which significantly limits the explanatory value of G × E models for complex phenotypes, such as ADHD. These G × E models often have very small effect sizes and low replication rates (Risch et al., 2009; Lee et al., 2012). One recommended strategy to overcome this limitation is to simultaneously examine multiple candidate genes affecting the same biological pathway (e.g., dopaminergic transmission) as well as multiple relevant environmental factors (Pennington and Bishop, 2009). In a review, Pennington and Bishop (2009) suggested computing composite G and E risk scores across candidate genes and environmental factors and test for G and E main effects and G × E interactions in one omnibus analysis. Then, in case of a significant interaction effect, follow-up analyses should be performed to specify which risk alleles and which environments contribute to the overall effect. However, until now there has been a lack of appropriate statistical methodology to perform such multi-G × E analyses. We recently developed a method for the analysis of complex interactions between multiple genes and environments (Jolicoeur-Martineau et al., 2019, 2020). The Latent Environmental and Genetic InTeraction (LEGIT) approach can be used to construct complex multi-interaction models without the need to estimate an additional parameter for each interaction term, thus improving scalability, especially with higher order interactions. An important limitation of previous G × E models is the lack of information concerning the specific form of the interaction effect (Widaman et al., 2012). For instance, the diathesis-stress model assumes that the differences between individuals with and without the “risk” allele of a given genetic variant will manifest only under adverse circumstances, such that individuals carrying the “risk” allele are affected negatively, while those without the “risk” allele remain relatively unaffected by the environment (Belsky et al., 2009). In comparison, the differential-susceptibility model posits that individuals carrying the “risk” allele are generally more sensitive to the effects of the environment than those without the “risk” allele (Belsky, 1997; Boyce and Ellis, 2005). Accordingly, compared to those with the non-risk allele, individuals with the “risk” allele exhibit poorer outcomes in negative environments, similar outcomes in average environments, and superior outcomes in positive environments. The LEGIT approach allows us to distinguish between these two theoretical frameworks, which may have important consequences for prevention and intervention strategies.

Here, we use a rich longitudinal dataset to examine how dopaminergic candidate genes simultaneously interact with prenatal adversity, and early parenting to influence toddlers’ attentional competence measured longitudinally at two time points. We apply LEGIT—with a G × E1 × E2 design to address this question. Our findings may advance the literature in three important ways. First, we examine the effect of prenatal adversity by including a number of well-established measures of prenatal adversity in one model. Second, we simultaneously consider the modifying effect of multiple dopamine-related genes known to affect the developing human attention system. Third, we complement this by additionally examining important aspects of the early rearing environment that may buffer the negative effects of prenatal adversity in genetically susceptible children. We address these questions using an approach that was specifically designed to deal with the complexity of simultaneously testing multiple interaction effects in relation to an outcome. Due to methodological limitations, few studies to date have attempted to look at the joint contribution of multiple genetic risk variants and multiple environmental exposures (both adverse and protective) to early attention development. This, however, seriously limits our understanding of complex human behavior, which is underlined by the interplay of numerous biological and environmental factors. One novelty of this study is thus the use of LEGIT that enabled the simultaneous testing of a large number of G × E interactions by using latent genetic and environmental features and an alternating optimization algorithm. Another novelty of our study is the inclusion of both macro- and micro-level analytic observations of early maternal behavior. Maternal behaviors included here were analyzed on a second-by-second level within the context of a 20-min mother-infant interaction. Given the time- and labor-intensive nature of collecting such fine-grained data, we are not aware of many G × E studies on early child attention that have used observational measures of early parenting, furthermore both at a macro- and micro-analytic level.

Materials and Methods

Participants

The participants were mother-child dyads from the Maternal Adversity, Vulnerability and Neurodevelopment (MAVAN) project, a Canadian community-based prenatal cohort of 590 women and their children in Montreal (Quebec) and Hamilton (Ontario). Women were recruited in maternity hospitals from 2003 to 2009 during their routine ultrasound examinations. A detailed description of the cohort has been presented elsewhere (O’Donnell et al., 2014). Informed consent was obtained at the time of recruitment and at each time point of data acquisition. Ethics Review Board approval was obtained from the institution of each study site. Retention rates for the MAVAN subjects were 97.4% at 6 months, 84.0% at 18 months, and 80.6% at 36 months. The present study included 134 mother-child dyads with complete data either at 18 or 24 months of child age. The reduction of sample size from 577 to 134 participants is explained by the following: 240 participants had missing genotype data (due to partial funding), 61 participants had missing information on prenatal adversity; seven participants had missing data regarding early parenting; 86 participants had missing information on postnatal maternal depression; 49 participants had missing outcome data at both 18 and 24 months. Thus, the final sample for the complete case analysis included 134 women and their children.

Measures

Genotyping

Child genotype was obtained from buccal swabs. using the TaqMan methods on the ABI-7000 for single nucleotide polymorphism (SNP) markers and ABI-3100 for repeat polymorphisms. To ensure a clear result, any ambiguous genotypes were discarded and the subjects were re-genotyped until the results were unambiguous. Each 20th marker was re-genotyped to check for error rates (0.5%). For the present study, we were interested specifically in genes directly or indirectly related to the dopaminergic system. The five candidate genes included dopamine receptors DRD2, DRD4, dopamine transporter DAT1, the catechol-o-methyltransferase (COMT), and the brain-derived neurotrophic factor (BDNF). DRD2 was captured using SNP rs1800497 (also known as TaqIA) with A as the risk allele (Nyman et al., 2007; Moro et al., 2019). COMT was captured using SNP rs4680 with Met as the risk allele (Holmboe et al., 2010; Soeiro-De-Souza et al., 2013). BDNF was captured using SNP rs6265 with Val as the risk allele. These SNPs were coded as the number of “risk” alleles divided by two (i.e., 0 for no risk allele, 0.5 for one risk allele, 1 for two risk alleles). DAT1 was captured using the 40bp VN TR located in exon 15 coded dichotomously as 1 (10R/10R) and 0 when 9R/9R or 9R/10R (Cornish et al., 2005; Holmboe et al., 2010). DRD4 was captured using the 48bp (VN TR) polymorphism in exon 3 coded dichotomously as 1 (6-8R) and 0 (2-5R), as per Schmidt et al. (2001). Genotype distributions did not deviate from Hardy-Weinberg equilibrium (p > 0.05).

Prenatal Life Events

An adapted version of the Prenatal Life Events Scale (Lobel, 1997; Lobel et al., 2000) was used to assess the occurrence of 17 life events (e.g., being robbed, being involved in a serious accident, having someone close die) that women may have experienced during the pregnancy (24–36 weeks). This adapted version did not include those items from the original version of the scale that had an especially low frequency of occurrence. For each event endorsed, participants reported how undesirable or negative the event was on a scale from 0 (not at all) to 3 (very much). Life events that were evaluated as strongly undesirable (i.e., score of 2 or 3) were coded as 1, everything else was coded as 0. Scores were summed to quantify the number of stressful life events. Total scores ranged from 0 to 17, with higher scores indicating the presence of more stressful life events during pregnancy. Not surprisingly, the internal reliability of this scale was low (α = 0.42) due to the wide range of possibly unrelated life events.

Prenatal Depressive Symptoms

Women rated their depressive symptoms at 24–36 weeks of pregnancy using the Center for Epidemiologic Studies Depression Scale (CES-D; Radloff, 1977) The CES-D includes 20 items capturing mood-, appetite- and sleep-related symptoms in community-based populations. Each item was rated on a scale from 0 (rarely or none of the time) to 3 (most or all of the time) and the items were summed. Total scores ranged from 0 to 60, with higher scores indicating more severe depressive symptoms. Internal reliability of the CES-D in the present sample was high (α = 0.92).

Birth Weight

Children’s weight at birth was assessed at the time of delivery (in grams).

Maternal Sensitivity and Parenting Behaviors

When children were 6 months old, maternal sensitivity and maternal parenting behaviors were observed during a 20-min free-play session, which took place in the participant’s home and was videotaped for coding purposes. We assessed maternal sensitivity using the Ainsworth Maternal Sensitivity Scales (Ainsworth et al., 1978). This is a validated gold standard, macro-analytic-level measure of maternal sensitivity, focusing on four aspects of early care: sensitivity to infant signals, cooperation vs. interference with ongoing behavior, psychological and physical availability, and acceptance vs. rejection of infant’s needs. Scores ranged from 1 to 9, with higher scores indicating more highly involved mothers. Mean inter-rater reliability (intra-class correlation) for the Ainsworth scale ratings was 0.88 (n = 28). The four scales were very highly correlated (r > 0.94). As such, we used only the sensitivity scale. Maternal parenting behaviors were assessed using the Behavioral Evaluation Strategies and Taxonomies (BEST; Educational Consulting, Inc. Florida, USA; S and K NorPark Computer Design, Toronto). The BEST consists of second-by-second micro-analytic-level frequency ratings and duration measures of maternal and child behaviors (Fleming et al., 1988). Two trained raters scored the duration and frequency of specific behaviors. Inter-rater reliabilities (intra-class correlation) ranged 0.74–0.90 (n = 18). Maternal sensitivity and BEST behaviors were coded independently, with coders of one scheme blind to codes on the other. Parental behaviors included maternal attention towards the child, tactile contact between mother and child, maternal vocalization, and mother-child activities. These measures have been used in our past research (Krpan et al., 2005; Giardino et al., 2008; Wazana et al., 2015; Graffi et al., 2018). For the purpose of this study, the duration of the respective maternal behaviors was first transformed into percentages of the total duration the mother spent interacting with her child, which excluded the time spent feeding, talking to someone else, or where the dyad was obscured. Percentages were subsequently z-standardized and averaged to form a score on the following dimensions:

1. Attention: focused (i.e., concentrated) looking at the infant, unfocused (i.e., unconcentrated) looking at infant or focused looking at an infant-related object (i.e., joint attention), “mother and infant are focusing on the same object”

2. Tactile stimulation: kissing, poking/tickling, mouthing/raspberries, stroking/patting

3. Vocalization: humming/singing, talking, laughing/smiling

4. Infant-related activities: social games, showing toy, play with a toy, play without a toy, rocking/jiggling, grooming the infant

Child Attentional Competency

Attentional competency was assessed using the Attention subscale from the Competence domain of the Infant-Toddler Social and Emotional Assessment (ITSEA) at 18 and 24 months (Briggs-Gowan and Carter, 1998, 2007). The ITSEA is a developmentally and clinically sensitive parent-rated questionnaire of social-emotional problems and competencies in 1–3 year-olds (Briggs-Gowan and Carter, 1998). The Attention subscale is formed by summing five items assessing attentional function, such as “plays with toys for 5 min or more,” “looks at things for a minute or longer.” Internal consistency of the Attention subscale in the present sample was good (Cronbach’s alpha = 0.76 at 18 months and 0.74 at 24 months). Scores were distributed evenly across the range of possible values (0–2) at both time points, values were higher at 24 months (M = 1.43, SD = 0.45) than 18 months (M = 1.29, SD = 0.51), with moderate consistency over time (ICC(3,1) = 0.57, ICC(3,k) = 0.73). Outcome scores were divided by 2 to rescale them between 0 and 1. Using a linear model (LM) with a constrained outcome variable is problematic as model predictions could go beyond the observed range. Therefore, we used a generalized linear model (GLM) with a Quasi-binomial family, which ensures that the outcome is constrained to the range [0, 1] instead of being unconstrained, such as when using a Gaussian family.

Covariates

Covariates included child sex, maternal age at delivery, and maternal education (“high school or less,” “some college, completed college, or some university,” and “university graduate or more”). We additionally included a covariate that indicated whether the child had available data on attentional competency at 24 months to adjust for the fact that baseline attentional competency was significantly better at 24 months (β = 0.18, S = 9,862.5, p < 0.0001). The intercept β0 of the model represents attentional competency at 18 months, while β0+ β24M together represents attention competency at 24 months. All continuous variables were standardized except for maternal age.

Statistical Analysis

Descriptives

Hardy-Weinberg equilibrium of genotype distribution was tested using exact tests (Engels, 2009). Since most continuous variables used in this study are non-normally distributed, we used non-parametric tests to describe the characteristics of our sample. We used chi-square tests for categorical-by-categorical, Wilcoxon rank-sum tests for binary-by-continuous, and Wilcoxon signed-rank tests for paired comparisons. We examined correlations between variables used in analyses using Kendall’s tau coefficients.

Main Analyses

Data were analyzed using the LEGIT R (Jolicoeur-Martineau et al., 2019) with a repeated measures design to predict attentional competency at 18 and 24 months. We fitted a 3-way G × E1 × E2 interaction model where G is a weighted sum (i.e., latent score) of the five dopamine-related candidate genes (i.e., DRD2, DRD4, BNDF, COMT, DAT1), E1 is a weighted sum of our three prenatal adversity variables (i.e., prenatal maternal stressful life events, prenatal maternal depressive symptoms, and birth weight), and E2 is a weighted sum of all early maternal parenting behaviors (i.e., Ainsworth sensitivity, maternal attention, tactile stimulation, vocalization, and infant-related activities). A schematic representation of the proposed three-way interaction model is shown in the Supplementary Figure 1. Further information on how the latent sum of G, E1, E2, and their interactions were calculated is provided as Supplemental Material.

Treatment of Missing Data

Missing information was imputed for participants that had at least one measure available for each latent score (i.e., G, E1, and E2), and the outcome variable at either 18 or 24 months (N = 197). All analyses were performed on both the complete cases (N = 134) and the imputed dataset (N = 197). Given that our model included interaction terms, traditional imputation methods which do not account for non-linearities are bound to be biased (Seaman et al., 2012). Thus, we used missForest (Stekhoven and Buhlmann, 2012), which has been shown to outperform the popular multiple imputation method by chained equations (mice) with predictive mean matching (pmm; van Buuren and Groothuis-Oudshoorn, 2010). Furthermore, the imputation accuracy of MissForest has been shown to approach state-of-the-art modern imputation techniques (Yoon et al., 2018; Payrovnaziri et al., 2020).

Similar to many complex, non-linear methods, it is not possible to pool estimates from multiple imputations using LEGIT. As the signs of the parameters inside the latent scores may differ randomly (models with the same parameters, but with different signs can be equivalent), pooling across multiple LEGIT models would lead to a regression of the parameters towards zero. Moreover, given the various parameters involved, it is difficult to know which sign is the correct one. All these can make pooling highly inconsistent, if not impossible. In addition, performing variable selection is unfeasible using multiple imputations. For the above reasons, we used a single imputation method called missForest. Contrary to other methods, such as mice, MissForest produces similar imputations when using different random seeds.

Variable Selection

To be more parsimonious, we can apply variable selection to retain only the most important elements in each latent score (G, E1, E2). Unfortunately, quality-of-fit measures like the Akaike information criterion (AIC; Akaike, 1998), corrected Akaike information criterion (AICc; Hurvich and Tsai, 1989), and Bayesian information criterion (BIC; Schwarz, 1978) are not defined in GLMs of quasi-binomial family. This means that we cannot use variable selection with these fit measures. Consequently, the variable selection was performed in the LM models, and the retained variables were included in the GLM models of the quasi-binomial family. Variables selected in the LM models generally remained significant in the GLM models, and their relative contribution did not change meaningfully. Variable selection was performed using “parallel natural evolutionary variable selection” available within LEGIT. Models with the lowest AICc value were considered as best fitting the data. Results from the models both with and without variable selection are presented.

In-Sample and Out-of-Sample Effect Sizes

To further assess model fit, we also examined the in-sample effect size and out-of-sample effect size. In-sample effect size was estimated using the regular R2, out-of-sample effect size (which measures how well the model generalizes to new observations) was estimated using the leave-one-out cross-validated (LOOCV) R2. The LOOCV was calculated in the same way as the R2 with the exception that the predictions for a given participant were obtained from a model that did not include the participant in question.

Data analysis was carried out in version 9.4 of the SAS System for Windows (Copyright © 2002–2012, SAS Institute Inc. SAS and all other SAS Institute Inc. product or service names are registered trademarks or trademarks of SAS Institute Inc., Cary, NC, USA). Graphical outputs and imputations were generated using R version 3.2.5 (R Development Core Team., 2016).

Results

Descriptive Analyses

Sample characteristics are shown in Table 1; correlations between the predictors, outcomes and covariates are shown in Table 2. Attentional competence at 18 and 24 months were highly correlated (r(103) = 0.50, p < 0.0001). Attentional competence at 18 months was positively associated with maternal sensitivity (r(120) = 0.14, p = 0.04) and negatively associated with postnatal depressive symptoms (r(120) = −0.13, p = 0.04). Attentional competence at 24 months was negatively associated with birth weight (r(117) = −0.14, p < 0.05), prenatal depressive symptoms (r(117) = −0.22, p = 0.01), and postnatal depressive symptoms (r(117) = −0.28, p = 0.0002). Prenatal depressive symptoms were positively associated with prenatal life events (r(134) = 0.24, p < 0.0001) and postnatal depressive symptoms (r(134) = 0.41 p < 0.0001). Maternal parenting measures were not significantly associated with each other, except for vocalization, which was positively related to infant-related activities (r(134) = 0.16, p = 0.006) and maternal sensitivity (r(134) = 0.19, p = 0.003). Significant gene-environment correlations were observed between DRD2 and birth weight (r(134) = −0.16, p = 0.02) and between DRD4 and prenatal depressive symptoms (r(134) = −0.15, p = 0.04) and vocalization (r(134) = 0.15, p = 0.03).

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Demographic characteristics of MAVAN participants.

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Kendall-Tau correlation matrix of all variables included in analyses.

Three-Way Interaction Models

In the complete-case analysis, the G × E1 × E2 interaction effect emerged significant (β = −17.17, SE = 3.50, p < 0.0001). However, this was not replicated in the imputed analysis (β = −0.87, SE = 1.31, p = 0.51). Although both the complete-case and imputed data analyses had relatively large in-sample effect sizes (R2 = 0.31 and 0.26, respectively), their out-of-sample effect sizes were very low (LOOCV R2 = −0.16 and 0.07, respectively), indicating poor generalization. The negative LOOCV R2 and the fact that the three-way interaction effect was only significant in the non-imputed analysis are strongly suggestive of model overfitting. For these reasons, we reran all analyses without the three-way interaction term but retaining all two-way (i.e., G × E1, G × E2, E1 × E2) interaction terms. Results of the three-way interaction models are shown in Table 3.

TABLE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. Predicting toddler attentional competence at 18 and 24 months based on the three-way interaction of prenatal adversity, dopamine-related genes, and early maternal parenting behaviors with/without imputation.

Two-Way Interaction Models

Results of the two-way interactions models are shown in Table 4. All two-way interaction effects were significant in both the complete-case and imputed analyses (p < 0.0001). In the complete-case full model, DAT1 (β = 0.15, SE = 0.05, p = 0.002), BDNF (β = 0.25, SE = 0.12, p = 0.04), and COMT (β = −0.52, SE = 0.09, p < 0.0001) seemed to be the most important genetic drivers of the observed interaction effects. Among the prenatal adversity factors, maternal stressful life events emerged as most important for the interaction (β = 0.87, SE = 0.15, p < 0.0001). Regarding early maternal parenting, tactile stimulation (β = 0.22, SE = 0.08, p = 0.01), vocalization (β = 0.32, SE = 0.09, p = 0.0003), and infant-related activities (β = 0.40, SE = 0.11, p = 0.0003) seemed to be the most relevant in interacting with dopamine-related genes or with prenatal adversity. The model had moderate effect size (in-sample R2 = 0.32, LOOCV R2 = 0.03), interaction effects are visualized in Figure 1.

TABLE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Table 4. Predicting toddler attentional competence at 18 and 24 months based on two-way interactions between prenatal adversity, dopamine-related genes, and early maternal parenting behaviors with/without imputation and with/without variable selection.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. The prediction of toddler attentional competence at 18–24 months based on the two-way interaction model (Table 4) without missing data imputation and without variable selection (column 1). (A) When prenatal adversity is low, attentional competence of young children with low dopaminergic genetic scores increases from low to high with increasingly positive early maternal parenting behavior. Meanwhile, children with moderate or high dopaminergic genetic scores start at a relatively high level of attentional competence, which seems to be unaffected by an increase in early positive maternal parenting behaviors. (B) When prenatal adversity is moderate, young children with low dopaminergic genetic scores start at moderate levels of attentional competence, which rapidly increases as positive early maternal parenting behavior increases. Meanwhile, children with moderate or high dopaminergic genetic scores start at a high level of attentional competence, which seems to be unaffected by an increase in early positive maternal parenting behaviors. (C) When prenatal adversity is high, young children with low dopaminergic genetic scores start at a high level of attentional competence, which seems relatively unaffected by increasingly positive early maternal parenting behaviors. Children with moderate dopaminergic genetic scores initially have moderate levels of attentional competence, which increases linearly as positive early maternal parenting behavior increases. Children with high dopaminergic genetic scores initially have low levels of attentional competence, which also increases linearly as early positive maternal parenting behaviors increase. Note. Figures for the two-way interaction models with/without imputation and with/without variable selection all look very similar. Note 2. Despite the absence of a three-way interaction effect, results must be graphically represented similarly to a three-way interaction model, since all three main components (i.e., G, E1, and E2) interact with one another in two-way interactions within the same model.

In the variable selection model, DAT1 (β = 0.30, SE = 0.07, p < 0.0001), and COMT (β = −0.70, SE = 0.11, p < 0.0001) were retained for the genetic component; prenatal stressful life events (β = 1) for the adversity component; and maternal vocalization (β = 0.40, SE = 0.12, p = 0.0008), maternal infant-related activities (β = 0.50, SE = 0.15, p = 0.001), and maternal sensitivity (β = 0.09, SE = 0.05, p = 0.10) for the early maternal parenting component. The effect size of the model with variable selection was moderate (in-sample R2 = 0.41 and LOOCV R2 = 0.17).

A very similar picture emerged in the imputed models. In the full model without variable selection, maternal sensitivity emerged as an additional important early parenting behavior for the observed interactions (β = 0.07, SE = 0.03, p = 0.03). In the variable selection model, DAT1 (β = 0.12, SE = 0.04 p = 0.005), BNDF (β = 0.37, SE = 0.10, p = 0.0002) and COMT (β = 0.50, SE = 0.07, p < 0.0001) were retained for the genetic component; maternal stressful life events for the adversity component (β = 1); and maternal sensitivity (β = 0.07, SE = 0.03, p = 0.03), tactile stimulation (β = 0.22, SE = 0.06, p = 0.0006), vocalization (β = 0.30, SE = 0.07, p < 0.0001), and infant-related activities (β = 0.41, SE = 0.10, p < 0.0001) for early maternal parenting. Effect sizes of both models were moderate (in-sample R2 = 0.25, LOOCV R2 = 0.10 for the full model; in-sample R2 = 0.25, LOOCV R2 = 0.14 for the model with variable selection).

Discussion

In a prospectively followed prenatal cohort, we examined the complex interplay between three important forces of attention development: 1) genetic variations in the dopaminergic pathway (using a genetic score composed of five dopamine-related genes), prenatal adversity (captured through children’s birth weight, the presence of prenatal maternal depressive symptoms and stressful life events), and the earliest rearing environment (captured through a range of observed maternal parenting behaviors). Our study benefitted from a sample with rich measures on prenatal adversity, dopamine-related gene variants, observational measures of maternal parenting behavior, and repeated assessments of toddlers’ attentional competence. A further strength of our study was the use of a statistical approach (LEGIT) to simultaneously analyze complex G × E interactions, which provides greatly enhanced power over traditional models that analyze a single G × E effect at a time.

Our hypothesis of finding a three-way interaction effect for prenatal adversity, dopamine-related genes, and early maternal behavior on toddlers’ attentional competency was not confirmed. Although the complete case analysis indicated the presence of such an interaction effect, the model did not generalize, and when imputing missing observations, the interaction effect was not significant anymore. These observations point to possible model overfitting, especially in smaller samples. In line with this, when we reran the analysis without the three-way interaction term, a more consistent picture emerged. Significant two-way interaction effects emerged for prenatal adversity by dopamine-related genes; prenatal adversity by early maternal behavior; and dopamine-related genes by early maternal behavior on toddler attentional competence in both the complete case analysis and analysis with imputation for missing data. Furthermore, the in- and out-of-sample effect sizes also indicated that the model generalizes.

Our findings suggest that multiple dopamine-related genes interact with prenatal adversity to predict toddler attentional competence. Based on our models, DAT1, COMT, and BDNF emerged as the most significant among the genes tested. Previously, DAT1 has been one of the most consistently implicated candidate genes in relation to ADHD by linkage, association, and meta-analytic studies (Sharp et al., 2009). Importantly, DAT1 genotype has been linked to variation in both cognitive and neurobiological measures of attention (Gizer et al., 2009; Sharp et al., 2009). Moreover, a number of environmental factors have been hypothesized to moderate the effect of DAT1 on ADHD-related phenotypes (Franke and Buitelaar, 2018). Some of these include prenatal factors, such as maternal smoking and alcohol use during pregnancy, prenatal maternal stress, and birth weight (for a review, see Franke and Buitelaar, 2018). Contrary to our findings, the single study that looked at DAT1 by prenatal stress interaction effect on ADHD reported a lack of such effect (Grizenko et al., 2012). However, that study used a retrospective design to collect information on prenatal maternal stress when children were 6–12 years old. The two studies that assessed interactions of DAT1 with birth weight reported nominally significant effects on the occurrence of conduct problems in children with ADHD in a case-only study (Langley et al., 2008) and significant G × E effects for a genetic index including DAT1, DRD4, and DRD2 and birth weight on ADHD symptoms in a sibling sample (Jackson and Beaver, 2015). Importantly, certain aspects of parenting were also shown to interact with DAT1 in relation to ADHD-related phenotypes. These include parental expressed emotions, negative and positive parenting practices, and maternal warmth (for a review, see Franke and Buitelaar, 2018). In summary, our study supports prior evidence for the involvement of DAT1 in ADHD-related phenotypes in interaction with either the prenatal or postnatal environment.

Although a recent meta-analysis did not confirm the main effect of COMT gene variants on ADHD, it could not rule out the importance of COMT in combination with other factors (Sun et al., 2014). Indeed, in a combined analysis of two large cohorts (ALSPAC and PREDO), prenatal anxiety and child COMT genotype predicted ADHD symptoms at multiple time points (O’Donnell et al., 2017). In addition, COMT genotype also seemed to interact with prenatal maternal smoking to predict aggressive behavior and autistic symptoms in children with ADHD (Nijmeijer et al., 2010; Brennan et al., 2011) and interact with birth weight to predict antisocial behavior in children with ADHD (Thapar et al., 2005). Interactions of the COMT gene with parenting behavior have not been investigated to our knowledge in relation to ADHD. In summary, our findings are in line with previous literature suggesting an interplay between COMT and the prenatal environment to shape ADHD-related phenotypes. Furthermore, we add to the existing literature by showing that a genetic index including COMT interacts with maternal parenting behavior to affect the attentional competence of young children.

Variants in the BDNF gene have also been implicated in ADHD-related phenotypes both as exerting a main effect (Langley et al., 2008; Li et al., 2014; Luo et al., 2020) and in interaction with environmental stressors, such as early deprivation or family SES (Lasky-Su et al., 2007; Gunnar et al., 2012) in both European and Asian populations. G × E studies of ADHD-related phenotypes involving the BDNF gene, however, are still rare. One interesting study examined the interaction of the BDNF Val66Met polymorphism and parenting in children (aged 6–15 years) diagnosed with ADHD and found a significant interaction effect for child BDNF by mothers’ positive feelings about caring in relation to the development of internalizing comorbidities (Park et al., 2014).

We further found that prenatal adversity interacted with both dopamine-related genes and maternal parenting behavior in affecting toddler attentional functioning. There is growing evidence for the involvement of prenatal adversity in the risk for developing ADHD-related phenotypes (Glover, 2011; Graignic-Philippe et al., 2014), although there is currently insufficient support for a causal relationship (Sciberras et al., 2017). The most commonly researched adversities in relation to ADHD include maternal prenatal smoking, alcohol and substance use, maternal stress, and offspring birth weight (Morgan et al., 2018). Unfortunately, we were unable to investigate the effects of prenatal smoking, alcohol, and substance use as these variables had an extremely large proportion of missing data in our cohort. Nevertheless, we did examine the effects of prenatal maternal stress and birth weight, as well as maternal prenatal depressive symptoms, which have also been consistently implicated in the development of maladaptive child outcomes (Madigan et al., 2018). Of all prenatal adversities considered here, maternal prenatal stress seemed to be the most relevant component when considering offspring dopamine-related genes. This finding is partly in line with a recent study that reported significant G × E effects for prenatal maternal stress and children’s DRD4 genotype but not DAT1 on ADHD symptoms (Grizenko et al., 2012). However, as both this and our study show, not all children exposed to prenatal adversity will experience later difficulties. Constitutional characteristics, such as genetic variation may be key in determining who will be more susceptible to the deleterious effects of the environment, as is contended by the diathesis-stress or differential susceptibility hypotheses (Belsky, 1997, 2005; Ingram and Luxton, 2005).

Another noteworthy finding of this study is that early positive maternal behavior seemed to buffer the effect of both prenatal adversity and genetic susceptibility although not their joint effect on toddler attentional competence. The observation that positive maternal behavior may attenuate both environmental and genetic risks is in line with previous literature (Sonuga-Barke and Harold, 2018) and has important consequences for guiding interventions such as behavioral parent training programs for families with ADHD. Despite the strong evidence in support of a biological basis for ADHD symptoms, researchers have speculated that the child’s environment may play a particularly salient role in determining outcomes for children with ADHD, even if environmental factors may not be the primary cause of their core symptoms (Barkley, 2006). During infancy, the caregiver provides much of the child’s attention regulation through orienting (Posner et al., 2014). This external control eventually becomes internalized as toddlers gradually gain control over their own emotional and cognitive states through self-regulation (Posner et al., 2014). Therefore, understanding the ways in which parents can help their children better regulate their attention, emotions, and behavior is going to be invaluable for the success of behavioral parent training programs, for parents typically play a major role in changing their child’s behavioral symptoms (e.g., through parent training and behavior therapy programs; Johnston and Mash, 2001; Deault, 2010). A newly emerging field of “therapy genetics” has produced some promising results to this end. In one study among a large group of toddlers with externalizing problems, the largest effect for a video-feedback-based intervention promoting positive parenting and sensitive discipline was found in children carrying the DRD4 7R allele (Bakermans-Kranenburg et al., 2008). In another smaller pilot study of children with ADHD, the largest effects of a behavioral parent training program were seen in children not homozygous for the DAT1 10R allele (van den Hoofdakker et al., 2012).

Our study also pinpointed a number of specific maternal behaviors that were linked with improved attentional competence in toddlers, such as maternal sensitivity, tactile stimulation, vocalization, and activities including play and grooming. These behaviors emerged from coded observations of mother-child interactions rather than maternal self-reports. The over-reliance on self-report questionnaires for assessing parenting behavior may limit both the validity and reliability of the parenting behaviors being assessed. In addition, most prior studies tended to isolate one or two parenting behaviors, rather than examining several parenting measures simultaneously to explore if more robust associations exist that go beyond specific measures of parenting (Deault, 2010).

ADHD has classically been viewed as a primarily fixed cognitive “deficit,” mainly underlined by genetic and neurobiological mechanisms (Barkley, 1990; Weiss and Hechtman, 1993; Hinshaw, 1994). However, this view falls short in accounting for the way environmental and biological risk factors seem to interact to produce the diverse developmental pathways, clinical outcomes, and frequent comorbidities observed in ADHD (Mannuzza et al., 2004; Castellanos et al., 2005; Halperin et al., 2008). As a result, researchers have recently turned to the biopsychosocial framework to better explain the complex developmental processes underlying the pathophysiology of ADHD (Singh, 2008). Contrary to the fixed deficit model, the biopsychosocial theory posits that ADHD is caused by the interplay of genetic and environmental influences that occur throughout development in underlying neurobiological systems (Sonuga-Barke, 1998, 2009). Accordingly, the original risk for developing the disorder can be moderated by later factors that alter the trajectory of development for better or worse (Taylor, 1999; Singh, 2008). Understanding these moderating influences—both protective and harmful—is essential for predicting key features of the disorder, such as its emergence, persistence, offset, and the frequent development of comorbidities (Sonuga-Barke, 2009). In line with this thinking, here we reported that certain positive aspects of the early maternal behavior moderated the negative impact of both prenatal adversity and genetic susceptibility on toddlers’ attentional competence, albeit not their joint effect.

Although our findings were mainly interpreted in relation to the pathophysiology of ADHD, it was done so, since the overwhelming majority of available G × E studies that examined interactions between the very environmental exposures and genetic variants we considered in this study, focused on ADHD-related deficits in attention. However, it is important to note that attention deficits are present in numerous other psychiatric disorders, such as schizophrenia, bipolar disorder, mood disorders, and autism spectrum disorder to name a few (Burack et al., 2017). Furthermore, dysfunctions in the dopamine system that are related to the gene variants we considered here are also implicated, amongst others, in schizophrenia, bipolar disorder, Parkinson’s disease, phenylketonuria, and autism spectrum disorder (Diamond et al., 1997; DiCarlo et al., 2019; Nieoullon, 2002; Hayden and Nurnberger Jr, 2006; Scheggia et al., 2012; Mandolini et al., 2019; Pigoni et al., 2019). We plan on following our participants to see if lower attentional competence early in life will evolve into cognitive and psychopathological problems later on.

Inevitably, we were faced with a number of limitations. First, obtaining rich measures and detailed coding of maternal behavior meant that we had to compromise regarding the sample size. However, as Jolicoeur-Martineau et al. (2019) previously demonstrated, LEGIT performs well with sample sizes similar to that of the current study. Second, we assessed maternal prenatal depressive symptoms at a single time point. Consequently, this prevented us from examining the effect of timing and chronicity, the latter of which is a known modifier of the effect of maternal depressive symptoms on child outcomes (Brennan et al., 2000; Hammen and Brennan, 2003; Lahti et al., 2017; Tuovinen et al., 2018). Third, toddlers’ attentional competency was rated by the mothers. This can be problematic when mothers are also reporting on their own mood symptoms. Nevertheless, our study benefitted from using observational measures of early maternal parenting behaviors, which were rated by trained coders blind to the mothers’ prenatal depressive symptoms and offspring attentional competency. Furthermore, the ITSEA used to assess toddlers’ attentional competency is a valid parent-report measure (Carter et al., 2003), which is less prone to measurement error. Parents are asked to report on what is present, i.e., their child’s everyday activities that are indicative of the level of their attentional functioning (e.g., “Plays with toys for 5 min or more.”), rather than what is absent, i.e., deficits in their children’s attentional functioning. The assessment of ADHD can be challenging in the early years, thus recognition of important developmental processes, such as attentional competence can be a useful guide to the types of processes that are likely precursors to the disorder (Deault, 2010). As our young participants become older and increasingly capable of understanding verbal task instructions, we aim to repeat these analyses using laboratory-based assessments of child attention.

Implications

As, we have seen here, prenatal adversity can render genetically susceptible children to exhibit lower attentional competence already in toddlerhood, while a positive early rearing environment facilitates the development of children’s attentional competence. Therefore, standard prenatal care should include components that target women’s psychological well-being during pregnancy. At the same time, interventions for children with a high susceptibility for developing attentional problems might benefit from promoting positive parenting practices. In addition, these findings underscore the importance of including measurement of the psychosocial environment of the child in line with the biopsychosocial formulation of mental disorders, even when studying neurodevelopmental disorders or related processes (White et al., in press). Furthermore, future research should combine longitudinal developmental cohorts with similar available measures to investigate the complex interplay between the various genetic and environmental components that act to produce complex phenotypes. The computational tools necessary to investigate such complex interactions are now readily available to researchers.

Data Availability Statement

The data for this study will be shared upon request to the corresponding author.

Ethics Statement

The studies involving human participants were reviewed and approved by REB, Douglas Institute of Health, Montreal, Canada. Written informed consent to participate in this study was provided by the participants’ legal guardian/next of kin.

Author Contributions

LA, RL, MS, JL, AF, and JK were responsible for study design, data collection, and revision of manuscript. ES, AJ-M, and AW were responsible for study design, analysis, drafting, and revision of manuscript. All authors contributed to the article and approved the submitted version.

Funding

This research was made possible by the Canadian Institutes of Health Research (CIHR grants: 359912, 365309, 231614), the Fonds de la recherche en santé du Québec (FRSQ grant: 22418), and the March of Dimes Foundation (grant: 12-FY12-198). The MAVAN project has been supported by funding from the McGill Faculty of Medicine, the Blema and Arnold Steinberg Family Foundation, the Ludmer Centre for Neuroinformatics and Mental Health, the Sackler Foundation, and the JPB Foundation.

Conflict of Interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher’s Note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

Acknowledgments

We thank all members and participants of the MAVAN project for their time and commitment to this research.

Supplementary Material

The Supplementary Material for this article can be found online at: https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fnbeh.2021.701971/full#supplementary-material.

References

Ainsworth, M. D., Blehar, M., Waters, E., and Wall, S. (1978). Patterns of Attachment. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

Google Scholar

Akaike, H. (1998). “Information theory and an extension of the maximum likelihood principle,” in Selected Papers of Hirotugu Akaike, (Berlin, Germany: Springer), 199–213.

Google Scholar

Altink, M. E., Arias Vásquez, A., Franke, B., Slaats Willemse, D. I., Buschgens, C. J., Rommelse, N. N., et al. (2008). The dopamine receptor D47 repeat allele and prenatal smoking in ADHD-affected children and their unaffected siblings: no gene environment interaction. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry 49, 1053–1060. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2008.01998.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Asghari, V., Sanyal, S., Buchwaldt, S., Paterson, A., Jovanovic, V., Van Tol, H. H., et al. (1995). Modulation of intracellular cyclic AMP levels by different human dopamine D4 receptor variants. J. Neurochem. 65, 1157–1165. doi: 10.1046/j.1471-4159.1995.65031157.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Asghari, V., Schoots, O., van Kats, S., Ohara, K., Jovanovic, V., Guan, H.-C., et al. (1994). Dopamine D4 receptor repeat: analysis of different native and mutant forms of the human and rat genes. Mol. Pharmacol. 46, 364–373.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Auerbach, J. G., Benjamin, J., Faroy, M., Geller, V., and Ebstein, R. (2001). DRD4 related to infant attention and information processing: a developmental link to ADHD? Psychiatr. Genet. 11, 31–35. doi: 10.1097/00041444-200103000-00006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bakermans-Kranenburg, M. J., Van, I. M. H., Pijlman, F. T., Mesman, J., and Juffer, F. (2008). Experimental evidence for differential susceptibility: dopamine D4 receptor polymorphism (DRD4 VNTR) moderates intervention effects on toddlers’ externalizing behavior in a randomized controlled trial. Dev. Psychol. 44, 293–300. doi: 10.1037/0012-1649.44.1.293

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Banerjee, T. D., Middleton, F., and Faraone, S. V. (2007). Environmental risk factors for attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder. Acta Paediatr. 96, 1269–1274. doi: 10.1111/j.1651-2227.2007.00430.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Barkley, R. A. (2006). “A theory of ADHD,” in Attention-Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder: A Handbook for Diagnosis and Treatment, 3rd edition (New York, NY: The Guilford Press), 297–394.

Google Scholar

Barkley, R. A. (1990). Attention-Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder: A Handbook for Diagnosis and Treatment. New York, NY, USA: Guilford Press.

Google Scholar

Becker, K., El-Faddagh, M., Schmidt, M. H., Esser, G., and Laucht, M. (2008). Interaction of dopamine transporter genotype with prenatal smoke exposure on ADHD symptoms. J. Pediatr. 152, 263–269.e261. doi: 10.1016/j.jpeds.2007.07.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Belsky, J. (1997). Theory testing, effect-size evaluation and differential susceptibility to rearing influence: the case of mothering and attachment. Child Dev. 68, 598–600.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Belsky, J. (2005). “Differential susceptibility to rearing influence: an evolutionary hypothesis and some evidence,” in Origins of the Social Mind: Evolutionary Psychology and Child Development, eds B. J. Ellis and D. F. Bjorklund (New York, NY: Guilford Press), 139–163.

Google Scholar

Belsky, J., Jonassaint, C., Pluess, M., Stanton, M., Brummett, B., Williams, R., et al. (2009). Vulnerability genes or plasticity genes? Mol. Psychiatry 14, 746–754. doi: 10.1038/mp.2009.44.

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bhutta, A. T., Cleves, M. A., Casey, P. H., Cradock, M. M., and Anand, K. J. (2002). Cognitive and behavioral outcomes of school-aged children who were born preterm: a meta-analysis. JAMA 288, 728–737. doi: 10.1001/jama.288.6.728

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Blair, C. (2002). School readiness: Integrating cognition and emotion in a neurobiological conceptualization of children’s functioning at school entry. Am. Psychol. 57:111. doi: 10.1037//0003-066x.57.2.111

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Blasi, G., Mattay, V. S., Bertolino, A., Elvevåg, B., Callicott, J. H., Das, S., et al. (2005). Effect of catechol-O-methyltransferase val158met genotype on attentional control. J. Neurosci. 25, 5038–5045. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0476-05.2005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Boyce, W. T., and Ellis, B. J. (2005). Biological sensitivity to context: I. An evolutionary-developmental theory of the origins and functions of stress reactivity. Dev. Psychopathol. 17, 271–301. doi: 10.1017/s0954579405050145

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Brennan, P. A., Hammen, C., Andersen, M. J., Bor, W., Najman, J. M., and Williams, G. M. (2000). Chronicity, severity and timing of maternal depressive symptoms: relationships with child outcomes at age 5. Dev. Psychol. 36, 759–766. doi: 10.1037//0012-1649.36.6.759

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Brennan, P. A., Hammen, C., Sylvers, P., Bor, W., Najman, J., Lind, P., et al. (2011). Interactions between the COMT Val108/158Met polymorphism and maternal prenatal smoking predict aggressive behavior outcomes. Biol. Psychol. 87, 99–105. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2011.02.013

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Briggs-Gowan, M. J., and Carter, A. S. (1998). Preliminary acceptability and psychometrics of the infant-toddler social and emotional assessment (ITSEA): a new adult-report questionnaire. Infant Mental Health J. 19, 422–445. doi: 10.1002/(SICI)1097-0355(199824)19:4<422::AID-IMHJ5>3.0.CO;2-U

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Briggs-Gowan, M. J., and Carter, A. S. (2007). Applying the Infant-Toddler Social and Emotional Assessment (ITSEA) and Brief-ITSEA in early intervention. Infant Mental Health J. 28, 564–583. doi: 10.1002/imhj.20154

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Brookes, K.-J., Mill, J., Guindalini, C., Curran, S., Xu, X., Knight, J., et al. (2006). A common haplotype of the dopamine transporter gene associated with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder and interacting with maternal use of alcohol during pregnancy. Arch. Gen. Psychiatry 63, 74–81. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.63.1.74

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Burack, J. A., Jefferies, L. N., Ringo, J., and Landry, O. (2017). Attention. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.

Google Scholar

Carter, A. S., Briggs-Gowan, M. J., Jones, S. M., and Little, T. D. (2003). The infant-toddler social and emotional assessment (ITSEA): factor structure, reliability and validity. J. Abnorm Child Psychol. 31, 495–514. doi: 10.1023/a:1025449031360

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Caspi, A., and Moffitt, T. E. (2006). Gene-environment interactions in psychiatry: joining forces with neuroscience. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 7, 583–590. doi: 10.1038/nrn1925

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Caspi, A., McClay, J., Moffitt, T. E., Mill, J., Martin, J., Craig, I. W., et al. (2002). Role of genotype in the cycle of violence in maltreated children. Science 297, 851–854. doi: 10.1126/science.1072290

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Castellanos, F. X., Sonuga-Barke, E. J., Scheres, A., Di Martino, A., Hyde, C., Walters, J. R., et al. (2005). Varieties of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder-related intra-individual variability. Biol. Psychiatry 57, 1416–1423. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2004.12.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chang, F.-M., Kidd, J. R., Livak, K. J., Pakstis, A. J., and Kidd, K. K. (1996). The world-wide distribution of allele frequencies at the human dopamine D4 receptor locus. Hum. Genet. 98, 91–101.

Google Scholar

Chen, J., Lipska, B. K., Halim, N., Ma, Q. D., Matsumoto, M., Melhem, S., et al. (2004). Functional analysis of genetic variation in catechol-O-methyltransferase (COMT): effects on mRNA, protein and enzyme activity in postmortem human brain. Am. J. Hum. Genet. 75, 807–821. doi: 10.1086/425589

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chronis-Tuscano, A. M., Lahey, B. B., Pelham Jr, W. E., Kipp, H. L., Baumann, B. L., Lee, S. S., et al. (2003). Psychopathology and substance abuse in parents of young children with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. J. Am. Acad. Child Adolesc. Psychiatry 42, 1424–1432. doi: 10.1097/00004583-200312000-00009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chronis-Tuscano, A. M., Lahey, B. B., Pelham, W. E. Jr., Williams, S. H., Baumann, B. L., Kipp, H., et al. (2007). Maternal depression and early positive parenting predict future conduct problems in young children with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Dev. Psychol. 43, 70–82. doi: 10.1037/0012-1649.43.1.70

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chronis-Tuscano, A. M., Molina, B. S., Pelham, W. E., Applegate, B., Dahlke, A., Overmyer, M., et al. (2010). Very early predictors of adolescent depression and suicide attempts in children with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Arch. Genet. Psychiatry 67, 1044–1051. doi: 10.1001/archgenpsychiatry.2010.127

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ciampoli, M., Contarini, G., Mereu, M., and Papaleo, F. (2017). Attentional control in adolescent mice assessed with a modified five choice serial reaction time task. Sci. Rep. 7, 1–16. doi: 10.1038/s41598-017-10112-8

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ciliax, B. J., Drash, G. W., Staley, J. K., Haber, S., Mobley, C. J., Miller, G. W., et al. (1999). Immunocytochemical localization of the dopamine transporter in human brain. J. Comp. Neurol. 409, 38–56. doi: 10.1002/(sici)1096-9861(19990621)409:1<38::aid-cne4>3.0.co;2-1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Conner, D. B., Knight, D. K., and Cross, D. R. (1997). Mothers’ and fathers’ scaffolding of their 2-year-olds during problem-solving and literacy interactions. Br. J. Dev. Psychol. 15, 323–338.

Google Scholar

Cornish, K. M., Manly, T., Savage, R., Swanson, J., Morisano, D., Butler, N., et al. (2005). Association of the dopamine transporter (DAT1) 10/10-repeat genotype with ADHD symptoms and response inhibition in a general population sample. Mol. Psychiatry 10:686. doi: 10.1038/sj.mp.4001641

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Daley, D., Jones, K., Hutchings, J., and Thompson, M. (2009). Attention deficit hyperactivity disorder in pre-school children: current findings, recommended interventions and future directions. Child Care Health Dev. 35, 754–766. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2214.2009.00938.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Danforth, J. S., Anderson, L., Barkley, R. A., and Stokes, T. F. (1991). Observations of parent-child interactions with hyperactive children: Research and clinical implications. Clin. Psychol. Rev. 11, 703–727.

Google Scholar

Davis, D. W., and Williams, P. G. (2011). Attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder in preschool-age children: issues and concerns. Clin. Pediatr. (Phila) 50, 144–152. doi: 10.1177/0009922810384722

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Deault, L. C. (2010). A systematic review of parenting in relation to the development of comorbidities and functional impairments in children with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD). Child Psychiatry Hum. Dev. 41, 168–192. doi: 10.1007/s10578-009-0159-4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Diamond, A., Prevor, M. B., Callender, G., and Druin, D. P. (1997). Prefrontal cortex cognitive deficits in children treated early and continuously for PKU. Monogr. Soc. Res. Child Dev. 62, 1–206.

Google Scholar

DiCarlo, G. E., Aguilar, J. I., Matthies, H. J., Harrison, F. E., Bundschuh, K. E., West, A., et al. (2019). Autism-linked dopamine transporter mutation alters striatal dopamine neurotransmission and dopamine-dependent behaviors. J. Clin. Invest. 129, 3407–3419. doi: 10.1172/JCI127411

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dincheva, I., Glatt, C. E., and Lee, F. S. (2012). Impact of the BDNF Val66Met polymorphism on cognition: implications for behavioral genetics. Neuroscientist 18, 439–451. doi: 10.1177/1073858411431646

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Doucette Stamm, L. A., Blakely, D. J., Tian, J., Mockus, S., and Mao, J. I. (1995). Population genetic study of the human dopamine transporter gene (DAT1). Genet. Epidemiol. 12, 303–308.

Google Scholar

Engels, W. R. (2009). Exact tests for Hardy-Weinberg proportions. Genetics 183, 1431–1441. doi: 10.1534/genetics.109.108977

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fan, J., Fossella, J., Sommer, T., Wu, Y., and Posner, M. I. (2003). Mapping the genetic variation of executive attention onto brain activity. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U S A 100, 7406–7411. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0732088100

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Faraone, S. V., Perlis, R. H., Doyle, A. E., Smoller, J. W., Goralnick, J. J., Holmgren, M. A., et al. (2005). Molecular genetics of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Biol. Psychiatry 57, 1313–1323. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2004.11.024

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ficks, C. A., and Waldman, I. D. (2009). Gene-environment interactions in attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Curr. Psychiatry Rep. 11, 387–392. doi: 10.1007/s11920-009-0058-1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Field, T. (2011). Prenatal depression effects on early development: a review. Infant Behav. Dev. 34, 1–14. doi: 10.1016/j.infbeh.2010.09.008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Field, T., Diego, M., and Hernandez-Reif, M. (2006). Prenatal depression effects on the fetus and newborn: a review. Infant Behav. Dev. 29, 445–455. doi: 10.1016/j.infbeh.2006.03.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fleming, A. S., Ruble, D. N., Flett, G. L., and Shaul, D. L. (1988). Postpartum adjustment in first-time mothers: Relations between mood, maternal attitudes and mother-infant interactions. Dev. Psychol. 24, 71–81.

Google Scholar

Floresco, S. B., and Maric, T. T. (2007). Dopaminergic regulation of inhibitory and excitatory transmission in the basolateral amygdala-prefrontal cortical pathway. J. Neurosci. 27, 2045–2057. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5474-06.2007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Franke, B., and Buitelaar, J. K. (2018). “Gene-environment interactions,” in Oxford Textbook of Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder, eds T. Banaschewski, D. Coghill, and A. Zuddas (New York, NY, USA: Oxford University Press), 35–56.

Google Scholar

Gauvain, M. (2001). Cultural tools, social interaction and the development of thinking. Hum. Dev. 44, 126–143. doi: 10.1159/000057052

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gauvain, M., de la Ossa, J. L., and Hurtado-Ortiz, M. T. (2001). Parental guidance as children learn to use cultural tools: the case of pictorial plans. Cogn. Dev. 16, 551–575. doi: 10.1016/S0885-2014(01)00049-1

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Giardino, J., Gonzalez, A., Steiner, M., and Fleming, A. S. (2008). Effects of motherhood on physiological and subjective responses to infant cries in teenage mothers: a comparison with non-mothers and adult mothers. Horm. Behav. 53, 149–158. doi: 10.1016/j.yhbeh.2007.09.010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gizer, I. R., Ficks, C., and Waldman, I. D. (2009). Candidate gene studies of ADHD: a meta-analytic review. Hum. Genet. 126, 51–90. doi: 10.1007/s00439-009-0694-x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Glover, V. (2011). Annual Research Review: Prenatal stress and the origins of psychopathology: an evolutionary perspective. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry 52, 356–367. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2011.02371.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gluskin, B., and Mickey, B. (2016). Genetic variation and dopamine D2 receptor availability: a systematic review and meta-analysis of human in vivo molecular imaging studies. Transl. Psychiatry 6:e747. doi: 10.1038/tp.2016.22

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Goldberg, T. E., and Weinberger, D. R. (2004). Genes and the parsing of cognitive processes. Trends Cogn. Sci. 8, 325–335. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2004.05.011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Graffi, J., Moss, E., Jolicoeur-Martineau, A., Moss, G., Lecompte, V., Pascuzzo, K., et al. (2018). The dopamine D4 receptor gene, birth weight, maternal depression, maternal attention and the prediction of disorganized attachment at 36 months of age: a prospective gene × environment analysis. Infant Behav. Dev. 50, 64–77. doi: 10.1016/j.infbeh.2017.11.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Graignic-Philippe, R., Dayan, J., Chokron, S., Jacquet, A. Y., and Tordjman, S. (2014). Effects of prenatal stress on fetal and child development: a critical literature review. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 43, 137–162. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2014.03.022

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Grizenko, N., Fortier, M.-E., Zadorozny, C., Thakur, G., Schmitz, N., Duval, R., et al. (2012). Maternal stress during pregnancy, ADHD symptomatology in children and genotype: gene-environment interaction. J. Can. Acad. Child Adolesc. Psychiatry 21, 9–15.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Guillin, O., Diaz, J., Carroll, P., Griffon, N., Schwartz, J.-C., Sokoloff, P., et al. (2001). BDNF controls dopamine D 3 receptor expression and triggers behavioural sensitization. Nature 411, 86–89. doi: 10.1038/35075076.

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gunnar, M. R., Jennifer, A. W., Thomas, K. M., Glatt, C. E., McKenna, M. C., Clark, A. G., et al. (2012). The BDNF Val66Met polymorphism moderates early deprivation effects on attention problems. Dev. Psychopathol. 24:1215. doi: 10.1017/S095457941200065X

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Halperin, J. M., Trampush, J. W., Miller, C. J., Marks, D. J., and Newcorn, J. H. (2008). Neuropsychological outcome in adolescents/young adults with childhood ADHD: profiles of persisters, remitters and controls. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry 49, 958–966. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2008.01926.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hammen, C., and Brennan, P. A. (2003). Severity, chronicity and timing of maternal depression and risk for adolescent offspring diagnoses in a community sample. Arch. Genet. Psychiatry 60, 253–258. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.60.3.253

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Harold, G. T., Leve, L. D., Barrett, D., Elam, K., Neiderhiser, J. M., Natsuaki, M. N., et al. (2013). Biological and rearing mother influences on child ADHD symptoms: revisiting the developmental interface between nature and nurture. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry 54, 1038–1046. doi: 10.1111/jcpp.12100

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hayden, E. P., and Nurnberger Jr, J. (2006). Molecular genetics of bipolar disorder. Genes Brain Behav. 5, 85–95. doi: 10.1111/j.1601-183X.2005.00138.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hinshaw, S. P. (1994). Attention Deficits and Hyperactivity in Children. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage Publications, Inc.

Google Scholar

Holmboe, K., Nemoda, Z., Fearon, R. M. P., Csibra, G., Sasvari-Szekely, M., Johnson, M. H., et al. (2010). Polymorphisms in dopamine system genes are associated with individual differences in attention in infancy. Dev. Psychol. 46, 404–416. doi: 10.1037/a0018180

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hurvich, C. M., and Tsai, C.-L. (1989). Regression and time series model selection in small samples. Biometrika 76, 297–307.

Google Scholar

Ingram, R. E., and Luxton, D. D. (2005). “Vulnerability-stress models,” in Development of Psychopathology: A Vulnerability Stress Perspective, eds B. L. Hankin, and J. R. Z. Abela (Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage Publications Inc), 32–46).

Google Scholar

Jackson, D. B., and Beaver, K. M. (2015). Sibling differences in low birth weight, dopaminergic polymorphisms and ADHD symptomatology: evidence of G × E. Psychiatry Res. 226, 467–473. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2015.01.025

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Johnston, C., and Mash, E. J. (2001). Families of children with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: review and recommendations for future research. Clin. Child Fam. Psychol. Rev. 4, 183–207. doi: 10.1023/a:1017592030434

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jolicoeur-Martineau, A., Belsky, J., Szekely, E., Widaman, K. F., Pluess, M., Greenwood, C., et al. (2020). Distinguishing differential susceptibility, diathesis-stress and vantage sensitivity: beyond the single gene and environment model. Dev. Psychopathol. 32, 73–83. doi: 10.1017/S0954579418001438

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jolicoeur-Martineau, A., Wazana, A., Szekely, E., Steiner, M., Fleming, A. S., Kennedy, J. L., et al. (2019). Alternating optimization for G × E modelling with weighted genetic and environmental scores: examples from the MAVAN study. Psychol. Methods 24:196. doi: 10.1037/met0000175

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kahn, R. S., Khoury, J., Nichols, W. C., and Lanphear, B. P. (2003). Role of dopamine transporter genotype and maternal prenatal smoking in childhood hyperactive-impulsive, inattentive and oppositional behaviors. J. Pediatr. 143, 104–110. doi: 10.1016/S0022-3476(03)00208-7

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Karoum, F., Chrapusta, S. J., and Egan, M. F. (1994). 3 Methoxytyramine is the major metabolite of released dopamine in the rat frontal cortex: reassessment of the effects of antipsychotics on the dynamics of dopamine release and metabolism in the frontal cortex, nucleus accumbens and striatum by a simple two pool model. J. Neurochem. 63, 972–979. doi: 10.1046/j.1471-4159.1994.63030972.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kessler, R. C., Adler, L., Barkley, R., Biederman, J., Conners, C. K., Demler, O., et al. (2006). The prevalence and correlates of adult ADHD in the United States: results from the national comorbidity survey replication. Am. J. Psychiatry 163, 716–723. doi: 10.1176/ajp.2006.163.4.716

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Krpan, K. M., Coombs, R., Zinga, D., Steiner, M., and Fleming, A. S. (2005). Experiential and hormonal correlates of maternal behavior in teen and adult mothers. Horm. Behav. 47, 112–122. doi: 10.1016/j.yhbeh.2004.08.006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lahti, M., Savolainen, K., Tuovinen, S., Pesonen, A. K., Lahti, J., Heinonen, K., et al. (2017). Maternal depressive symptoms during and after pregnancy and psychiatric problems in children. J. Am. Acad. Child Adolesc. Psychiatry 56, 30–39.e37. doi: 10.1016/j.jaac.2016.10.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Landry, S. H., Miller-Loncar, C. L., Smith, K. E., and Swank, P. R. (2002). The role of early parenting in children’s development of executive processes. Dev. Neuropsychol. 21, 15–41. doi: 10.1207/S15326942DN2101_2

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Langley, K., Turic, D., Rice, F., Holmans, P., Van Den Bree, M., Craddock, N., et al. (2008). Testing for gene × environment interaction effects in attention deficit hyperactivity disorder and associated antisocial behavior. Am. J. Med. Genet. B Neuropsychiatr. Genet. 147, 49–53. doi: 10.1002/ajmg.b.30571

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lasky-Su, J., Faraone, S. V., Lange, C., Tsuang, M., Doyle, A., Smoller, J., et al. (2007). A study of how socioeconomic status moderates the relationship between SNPs encompassing BDNF and ADHD symptom counts in ADHD families. Behav. Genet. 37, 487–497. doi: 10.1007/s10519-006-9136-x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Laucht, M., Esser, G., and Schmidt, M. H. (2001). Differential development of infants at risk for psychopathology: the moderating role of early maternal responsivity. Dev. Med. Child Neurol. 43, 292–300. doi: 10.1017/s0012162201000561

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Laucht, M., Skowronek, M. H., Becker, K., Schmidt, M. H., Esser, G., Schulze, T. G., et al. (2007). Interacting effects of the dopamine transporter gene and psychosocial adversity on attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder symptoms among 15-year-olds from a high-risk community sample. Arch. Gen. Psychiatry 64, 585–590. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.64.5.585

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lee, P. H., Perlis, R. H., Jung, J.-Y., Byrne, E. M., Rueckert, E., Siburian, R., et al. (2012). Multi-locus genome-wide association analysis supports the role of glutamatergic synaptic transmission in the etiology of major depressive disorder. Transl. Psychiatry 2:e184. doi: 10.1038/tp.2012.95

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Li, H., Liu, L., Tang, Y., Ji, N., Yang, L., Qian, Q., et al. (2014). Sex-specific association of brain-derived neurotrophic factor (BDNF) Val66Met polymorphism and plasma BDNF with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder in a drug-naive Han Chinese sample. Psychiatry Res. 217, 191–197. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2014.03.011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Li, J., Olsen, J., Vestergaard, M., and Obel, C. (2010). Attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder in the offspring following prenatal maternal bereavement: a nationwide follow-up study in Denmark. Eur. Child Adolesc. Psychiatry 19, 747–753. doi: 10.1007/s00787-010-0113-9

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lobel, M. (1997). The Prenatal Life Event Scale (PLES). Stony Brook, NY: State University of New York at Stony Brook.

Lobel, M., DeVincent, C. J., Kaminer, A., and Meyer, B. A. (2000). The impact of prenatal maternal stress and optimistic disposition on birth outcomes in medically high-risk women. Health Psychol. 19, 544–553. doi: 10.1037//0278-6133.19.6.544

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Loo, S. K., Specter, E., Smolen, A., Hopfer, C., Teale, P. D., Reite, M. L., et al. (2003). Functional effects of the DAT1 polymorphism on EEG measures in ADHD. J. Am. Acad. Child Adolesc. Psychiatry 42, 986–993. doi: 10.1097/01.CHI.0000046890.27264.88

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Luo, L., Jiang, X., Cao, G., Xiong, P., Yang, R., Zhang, J., et al. (2020). Association between BDNF gene polymorphisms and attention deficit hyperactivity disorder in school-aged children in Wuhan, China. J. Affect. Disord. 264, 304–309. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2020.01.017

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Madigan, S., Oatley, H., Racine, N., Fearon, R. M. P., Schumacher, L., Akbari, E., et al. (2018). A meta-analysis of maternal prenatal depression and anxiety on child socioemotional development. J. Am. Acad. Child Adolesc Psychiatry 57, 645–657.e8. doi: 10.1016/j.jaac.2018.06.012

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mandolini, G., Lazzaretti, M., Pigoni, A., Delvecchio, G., Soares, J., Brambilla, P., et al. (2019). The impact of BDNF Val66Met polymorphism on cognition in bipolar disorder: a review: special section on “translational and neuroscience studies in affective disorders” Section Editor, Maria Nobile MD, PhD. This Section of JAD focuses on the relevance of translational and neuroscience studies in providing a better understanding of the neural basis of affective disorders. The main aim is to briefly summaries relevant research findings in clinical neuroscience with particular regards to specific innovative topics in mood and anxiety disorders. J. Affect. Disor. 243, 552–558. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2018.07.054

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mannuzza, S., Klein, R. G., Abikoff, H., and Moulton Iii, J. L. (2004). Significance of childhood conduct problems to later development of conduct disorder among children with ADHD: a prospective follow-up study. J. Abnorm. Child Psychol. 32, 565–573. doi: 10.1023/b:jacp.0000037784.80885.1a

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Miller, N. V., Hane, A. A., Degnan, K. A., Fox, N. A., and Chronis Tuscano, A. (2019). Investigation of a developmental pathway from infant anger reactivity to childhood inhibitory control and ADHD symptoms: interactive effects of early maternal caregiving. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry 60, 762–772. doi: 10.1111/jcpp.13047

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Morgan, J. E., Loo, S. K., and Lee, S. S. (2018). Neurocognitive functioning mediates the prospective association of birth weight with youth ADHD symptoms. J. Clin. Child Adoles. Psychol. 47, 727–736. doi: 10.1080/15374416.2016.1183498

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Moro, M. M., El-Gebaly, H. H., Zaky, E. A., and Kamal, T. M. (2019). A study of dopamine D2 receptor Taq1 a alleles in children with attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder. Curr. Pediatr. Res. 23.

Google Scholar

Narita, M., Aoki, K., Takagi, M., Yajima, Y., and Suzuki, T. (2003). Implication of brain-derived neurotrophic factor in the release of dopamine and dopamine-related behaviors induced by methamphetamine. Neuroscience 119, 767–775. doi: 10.1016/s0306-4522(03)00099-x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Neuman, R. J., Lobos, E., Reich, W., Henderson, C. A., Sun, L.-W., Todd, R. D., et al. (2007). Prenatal smoking exposure and dopaminergic genotypes interact to cause a severe ADHD subtype. Biol. Psychiatry 61, 1320–1328. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2006.08.049

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Neville, M. J., Johnstone, E. C., and Walton, R. T. (2004). Identification and characterization of ANKK1: a novel kinase gene closely linked to DRD2 on chromosome band 11q23. 1. Hum. Mutation 23, 540–545. doi: 10.1002/humu.20039

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nieoullon, A. (2002). Dopamine and the regulation of cognition and attention. Prog. Neurobiol. 67, 53–83. doi: 10.1016/s0301-0082(02)00011-4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nigg, J. T. (2012). Future directions in ADHD etiology research. J. Clin. Child Adoles. Psychol. 41, 524–533. doi: 10.1080/15374416.2012.686870

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nijmeijer, J. S., Hartman, C. A., Rommelse, N. N., Altink, M. E., Buschgens, C. J., Fliers, E. A., et al. (2010). Perinatal risk factors interacting with catechol O methyltransferase and the serotonin transporter gene predict ASD symptoms in children with ADHD. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry 51, 1242–1250. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2010.02277.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nkam, I., Ramoz, N., Breton, F., Mallet, J., Gorwood, P., and Dubertret, C. (2017). Impact of DRD2/ANKK1 and COMT polymorphisms on attention and cognitive functions in schizophrenia. PloS One 12:e0170147. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0170147

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nyman, E. S., Ogdie, M. N., Loukola, A., Varilo, T., Taanila, A., Hurtig, T., et al. (2007). ADHD candidate gene study in a population-based birth cohort: association with DBH and DRD2. J. Am. Acad. Child 46, 1614–1621. doi: 10.1097/chi.0b013e3181579682

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

O’Donnell, K. J., Glover, V., Lahti, J., Lahti, M., Edgar, R. D., Räikkönen, K., et al. (2017). Maternal prenatal anxiety and child COMT genotype predict working memory and symptoms of ADHD. PloS One 12:e0177506. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0177506

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

O’Donnell, K. A., Gaudreau, H., Colalillo, S., Steiner, M., Atkinson, L., Moss, E., et al. (2014). The maternal adversity, vulnerability and neurodevelopment project: theory and methodology. Can. J. Psychiatry 59, 497–508. doi: 10.1177/070674371405900906

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Park, S., Kim, B. N., Kim, J. W., Jung, Y. K., Lee, J., Shin, M. S., et al. (2014). The role of the brain-derived neurotrophic factor genotype and parenting in early life in predicting externalizing and internalizing symptoms in children with attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder. Behav. Brain Funct. 10:43. doi: 10.1186/1744-9081-10-43

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Payrovnaziri, S. N., Xing, A., Shaeke, S., Liu, X., Bian, J., He, Z., et al. (2020). The impact of missing value imputation on the interpretations of predictive models: a case study on one-year mortality prediction in ICU patients with acute myocardial infarction. MedRxiv [Preprint]. doi: 10.1101/2020.06.06.20124347

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pelham, W. E.Jr., Fabiano, G. A., Waxmonsky, J. G., Greiner, A. R., Gnagy, E. M., Pelham, W. E., et al. (2016). Treatment sequencing for childhood ADHD: a multiple-randomization study of adaptive medication and behavioral interventions. J. Clin. Child Adolesc. Psychol. 45, 396–415. doi: 10.1080/15374416.2015.1105138

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pennington, B. F., and Bishop, D. V. (2009). Relations among speech, language and reading disorders. Ann. Rev. Psychol. 60, 283–306. doi: 10.1146/annurev.psych.60.110707.163548

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pfiffner, L. J., McBurnett, K., Rathouz, P. J., and Judice, S. (2005). Family correlates of oppositional and conduct disorders in children with attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder. J. Abnorm. Child Psychol. 33, 551–563. doi: 10.1007/s10802-005-6737-4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pickles, A., Sharp, H., Hellier, J., and Hill, J. (2017). Prenatal anxiety, maternal stroking in infancy and symptoms of emotional and behavioral disorders at 3.5 years. Eur. Child Adolesc. Psychiatry 26, 325–334. doi: 10.1007/s00787-016-0886-6

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pigoni, A., Lazzaretti, M., Mandolini, G., Delvecchio, G., Altamura, A., Soares, J., et al. (2019). The impact of COMT polymorphisms on cognition in bipolar disorder: a review: special section on “translational and neuroscience studies in affective disorders” Section Editor, Maria Nobile MD, PhD. This Section of JAD focuses on the relevance of translational and neuroscience studies in providing a better understanding of the neural basis of affective disorders. The main aim is to briefly summaries relevant research findings in clinical neuroscience with particular regards to specific innovative topics in mood and anxiety disorders. J. Affect. Disord. 243, 545–551. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2018.08.009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Plamondon, A., Akbari, E., Atkinson, L., Steiner, M., Meaney, M. J., Fleming, A. S., et al. (2015). Spatial working memory and attention skills are predicted by maternal stress during pregnancy. Early Hum. Dev. 91, 23–29. doi: 10.1016/j.earlhumdev.2014.11.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Posner, M. I., Rothbart, M. K., Sheese, B. E., and Voelker, P. (2014). Developing attention: behavioral and brain mechanisms. Adv. Neurosci. (Hindawi) 2014:405094. doi: 10.1155/2014/405094

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

R Development Core Team. (2016). R: a language and environment for statistical computing. Vienna, Austria. Available online at: http://www.R-project.org/.

Google Scholar

Radloff, L. S. (1977). The CES-D scale: a self-report depression scale for research in the general population. Appl. Psychol. Meas. 1, 385–401.

Google Scholar

Risch, N., Herrell, R., Lehner, T., Liang, K.-Y., Eaves, L., Hoh, J., et al. (2009). Interaction between the serotonin transporter gene (5-HTTLPR), stressful life events and risk of depression: a meta-analysis. JAMA 301, 2462–2471. doi: 10.1001/jama.2009.878

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Romirowsky, A. M., and Chronis-Tuscano, A. (2014). Paternal ADHD symptoms and child conduct problems: is father involvement always beneficial? Child Care Health Dev. 40, 706–714. doi: 10.1111/cch.12092

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rutter, M. (2006). Implications of resilience concepts for scientific understanding. Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci. 1094, 1–12. doi: 10.1196/annals.1376.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Scheggia, D., Sannino, S., Luisa Scattoni, M., and Papaleo, F. (2012). COMT as a drug target for cognitive functions and dysfunctions. CNS Neurol. Disord. Drug Targets 11, 209–221. doi: 10.2174/187152712800672481

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schmidt, L. A., Fox, N. A., Perez-Edgar, K., Hu, S., and Hamer, D. H. (2001). Association of DRD4 with attention problems in normal childhood development. Psychiatr. Genet. 11, 25–29. doi: 10.1097/00041444-200103000-00005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schwarz, G. J. T. A. O. S. (1978). Estimating the dimension of a model. Ann. Stat. 6, 461–464.

Google Scholar

Sciberras, E., Mulraney, M., Silva, D., and Coghill, D. (2017). Prenatal risk factors and the etiology of ADHD-review of existing evidence. Curr. Psychiatry Rep. 19:1. doi: 10.1007/s11920-017-0753-2

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Seaman, S. R., Bartlett, J. W., and White, I. R. (2012). Multiple imputation of missing covariates with non-linear effects and interactions: an evaluation of statistical methods. BMC Med. Res. Methodol. 12:46. doi: 10.1186/1471-2288-12-46

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sharp, S. I., McQuillin, A., and Gurling, H. M. (2009). Genetics of attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD). Neuropharmacology 57, 590–600. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropharm.2009.08.011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Singh, I. (2008). Beyond polemics: science and ethics of ADHD. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 9, 957–964. doi: 10.1038/nrn2514

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Soeiro-De-Souza, M. G., Stanford, M. S., Bio, D. S., Machado-Vieira, R., and Moreno, R. A. (2013). Association of the COMT Met158 allele with trait impulsivity in healthy young adults. Mol. Med. Rep. 7, 1067–1072. doi: 10.3892/mmr.2013.1336

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sonuga-Barke, E. J. (1998). Categorical models of childhood disorder: a conceptual and empirical analysis. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry 39, 115–133.

Google Scholar

Sonuga-Barke, E. J. S. (2009). “Attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder: towards a development synthesis,” in Oxford Handbook of Developmental Psychology, ed P. Zelazo (New York, NY, USA: Oxford University Press).

Google Scholar

Sonuga-Barke, E. J., and Harold, G. T. (2018). “Conceptualizing and investigating the role of the environment in ADHD: Correlate, cause, consequence, context and treatment,” in Oxford Textbook of Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder, eds T. Banaschewski, D. Coghill, and A. Zuddas (New York, NY: Oxford University Press), 25–34.

Google Scholar

Stekhoven, D. J., and Buhlmann, P. (2012). MissForest–non-parametric missing value imputation for mixed-type data. Bioinformatics 28, 112–118. doi: 10.1093/bioinformatics/btr597

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Strang-Karlsson, S., Räikkönen, K., Pesonen, A.-K., Kajantie, E., Paavonen, E. J., Lahti, J., et al. (2008). Very low birth weight and behavioral symptoms of attention deficit hyperactivity disorder in young adulthood: the Helsinki study of very-low-birth-weight adults. Am. J. Psychiatry 165, 1345–1353. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2008.08010085

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sun, H., Yuan, F., Shen, X., Xiong, G., and Wu, J. (2014). Role of COMT in ADHD: a systematic meta-analysis. Mol. Neurobiol. 49, 251–261. doi: 10.1007/s12035-013-8516-5

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Taylor, E. (1999). Developmental neuropsychopathology of attention deficit and impulsiveness. Dev. Psychopathol. 11, 607–628. doi: 10.1017/s0954579499002230

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Thapar, A., and Rutter, M. (2009). Do prenatal risk factors cause psychiatric disorder? be wary of causal claims. Br. J. Psychiatry 195, 100–101. doi: 10.1192/bjp.bp.109.062828

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Thapar, A., Langley, K., Asherson, P., and Gill, M. (2007). Gene-environment interplay in attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder and the importance of a developmental perspective. Br. J. Psychiatry 190, 1–3. doi: 10.1192/bjp.bp.106.027003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Thapar, A., Langley, K., Fowler, T., Rice, F., Turic, D., Whittinger, N., et al. (2005). Catechol O-methyltransferase gene variant and birth weight predict early-onset antisocial behavior in children with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Arch. Gen. Psychiatry 62, 1275–1278. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.62.11.1275

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Thiele, A., and Bellgrove, M. A. (2018). Neuromodulation of attention. Neuron 97, 769–785. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2018.01.008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Thomas, S. R., O’Brien, K. A., Clarke, T. L., Liu, Y., and Chronis-Tuscano, A. (2015). Maternal depression history moderates parenting responses to compliant and noncompliant behaviors of children with ADHD. J. Abnorm. Child Psychol. 43, 1257–1269. doi: 10.1007/s10802-014-9957-7

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tuovinen, S., Lahti-Pulkkinen, M., Girchenko, P., Lipsanen, J., Lahti, J., Heinonen, K., et al. (2018). Maternal depressive symptoms during and after pregnancy and child developmental milestones. Depress. Anxiety 35, 732–741. doi: 10.1002/da.22756

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Usiello, A., Baik, J.-H., Rougé-Pont, F., Picetti, R., Dierich, A., LeMeur, M., et al. (2000). Distinct functions of the two isoforms of dopamine D2 receptors. Nature 408, 199–203. doi: 10.1038/35041572

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vaidya, C. J., Austin, G., Kirkorian, G., Ridlehuber, H. W., Desmond, J. E., Glover, G. H., et al. (1998). Selective effects of methylphenidate in attention deficit hyperactivity disorder: a functional magnetic resonance study. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U S A 95, 14494–14499. doi: 10.1073/pnas.95.24.14494

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Van Batenburg Eddes, T., Brion, M., Henrichs, J., Jaddoe, V., Hofman, A., Verhulst, F., et al. (2013). Parental depressive and anxiety symptoms during pregnancy and attention problems in children: a cross cohort consistency study. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry 54, 591–600. doi: 10.1111/jcpp.12023

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

van Buuren, S., and Groothuis-Oudshoorn, K. (2010). Mice: multivariate imputation by chained equations in R. J. Stat. Software 45, 1–68. doi: 10.18637/jss.v045.i03

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

van den Hoofdakker, B. J., Nauta, M. H., Dijck-Brouwer, D. A., van der Veen-Mulders, L., Sytema, S., Emmelkamp, P. M., et al. (2012). Dopamine transporter gene moderates response to behavioral parent training in children with ADHD: a pilot study. Dev. Psychol. 48, 567–574. doi: 10.1037/a0026564

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vandenbergh, D. J., Persico, A. M., Hawkins, A. L., Griffin, C. A., Li, X., Jabs, E. W., et al. (1992). Human dopamine transporter gene (DAT1) maps to chromosome 5p15. 3 and displays a VNTR. Genomics 14, 1104–1106. doi: 10.1016/s0888-7543(05)80138-7

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vygotsky, L. (1978). Interaction between learning and development. Read. Dev. Child. 23, 34–41.

Waldman, I. D. (2007). Gene environment interactions reexamined: does mother’s marital stability interact with the dopamine receptor D2 gene in the etiology of childhood attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder? Dev. Psychopathol. 19, 1117–1128. doi: 10.1017/S0954579407000570

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wazana, A., Moss, E., Jolicoeur-Martineau, A., Graffi, J., Tsabari, G., Lecompte, V., et al. (2015). The interplay of birth weight, dopamine receptor D4 gene (DRD4) and early maternal care in the prediction of disorganized attachment at 36 months of age. Dev. Psychopathol. 27, 1145–1161. doi: 10.1017/S0954579415000735

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Weiss, G., and Hechtman, L. T. (1993). Hyperactive Children Grown Up: ADHD in Children, Adolescents and Adults. 2 Edn. New York, NY: Guilford Press.

Google Scholar

White, M., Lafaille-Magnan, M., Roche, C., Jolicoeur-Martineau, A., Wazana, A., Connor, K. L., et al. (in press). “Developmental origins, differential susceptibility and resilience in neurodevelopmental disorders,” in Neurodevelopmental Pediatrics: Genetic and Environmental Influences, eds E. Goldowitz, and O. Yager (Switzerland: Springer Nature).

Widaman, K. F., Helm, J. L., Castro-Schilo, L., Pluess, M., Stallings, M. C., Belsky, J., et al. (2012). Distinguishing ordinal and disordinal interactions. Psychol. Methods 17:615. doi: 10.1037/a0030003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Yoon, J., Jordon, J., and van der Schaar, M. (2018). “GAIN: missing data imputation using generative adversarial nets,” Proceedings of the 35th International Conference on Machine Learning, PMLR, vol. 80, 5689–5698. Available online at: http://proceedings.mlr.press/v80/yoon18a.html.

Google Scholar

Young, S., and Amarasinghe, J. M. (2010). Practitioner review: non-pharmacological treatments for ADHD: a lifespan approach. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry 51, 116–133. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2009.02191.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: attention, child, prenatal adversity, dopamine, genes, maternal sensitivity to infant cues, parenting (MeSH)

Citation: Szekely E, Jolicoeur-Martineau A, Atkinson L, Levitan RD, Steiner M, Lydon JE, Fleming AS, Kennedy JL and Wazana A (2021) The Interplay Between Prenatal Adversity, Offspring Dopaminergic Genes, and Early Parenting on Toddler Attentional Function. Front. Behav. Neurosci. 15:701971. doi: 10.3389/fnbeh.2021.701971

Received: 28 April 2021; Accepted: 12 July 2021;
Published: 29 July 2021.

Edited by:

Lorenzo More, University of Central Lancashire, United Kingdom

Reviewed by:

Gabriella Contarini, University of Catania, Italy
Ekaterina Kovsh, Southern Federal University, Russia

Copyright © 2021 Szekely, Jolicoeur-Martineau, Atkinson, Levitan, Steiner, Lydon, Fleming, Kennedy and Wazana. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Ashley Wazana, ashley.wazana@mcgill.ca

These authors have contributed equally to this work and share first authorship

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.