Skip to main content

BRIEF RESEARCH REPORT article

Front. Behav. Neurosci., 21 April 2020
Sec. Motivation and Reward
This article is part of the Research Topic Neuroscience of Eating: From Physiology to Pathology View all 16 articles

Neural Correlates of Food Cue Exposure Intervention for Obesity: A Case-Series Approach

  • Department of Clinical Psychological Science, Faculty of Psychology and Neuroscience, Maastricht University, Maastricht, Netherlands

Background: People with overweight have stronger reactivity (e.g., subjective craving) to food cues than lean people, and this reactivity is positively associated with food intake. Cue reactivity is a learned response that can be reduced with food cue exposure therapy.

Objectives: It was hypothesized that participants after food cue exposure therapy would show reduced neural activity in brain regions related to food cue reactivity and increased neural activity in brain regions related to inhibitory-control as compared to participants receiving a control lifestyle intervention.

Method: Neural activity of 10 women with overweight (BMI ≥ 27 kg/m2) in response to individually tailored visually presented palatable high-caloric food stimuli was examined before vs. after a cue exposure intervention (n = 5) or a control lifestyle (n = 5) intervention. Data were analyzed case-by-case.

Results: Neural responses to food stimuli were reduced in food-cue-reactivity-related brain regions after the lifestyle intervention in most participants, and generally not after the cue exposure therapy. Moreover, cue exposure did not lead to increased activity in inhibitory-control-related brain regions. However, decreased neural activity after cue exposure was found in most participants in the lateral occipital complex (LOC), which suggests a decreased visual salience of high-caloric food stimuli.

Conclusion: Receiving a cue exposure therapy did not lead to expected neural responses. As cue exposure relies on inhibitory learning mechanisms, differences in contexts (e.g., environments and food types) between the intervention setting and the scanning sessions may explain the general lack of effect of cue-exposure on neural activity.

Introduction

The prevalence of obesity has reached pandemic proportions (World Health Organization, 2018). Overweight people frequently engage in weight loss attempts, but success in the long-term is rare (Bish et al., 2005; Wing and Phelan, 2005). The main cause for obesity is a long-term energy imbalance, in which the number of consumed calories exceeds the number of expended calories for an extended time (Mitchell et al., 2011; Hall et al., 2012). Therefore, developing effective interventions to change behavior and reduce body weight is important. One possible intervention is food cue exposure therapy (CE) (Jansen et al., 2016; van den Akker et al., 2016).

CE aims to reduce food cue reactivity, which is defined as appetitive responding – like increased salivation and self-reported craving – in response to food-associated cues. Food cue reactivity serves as a physiological and psychological preparation for eating (Jansen et al., 2016). Food cues can be internal, such as hunger, satiety, emotions and thoughts, but also external, such as the smell, sight and taste of food, or environmental contexts (Boswell and Kober, 2016; Jansen et al., 2016). As compared to lean people, overweight people have a stronger food cue reactivity (Ferriday and Brunstrom, 2011), which is related to increased food intake (Boswell and Kober, 2016). Food cues become associated with food intake through classical conditioning (Jansen et al., 2016). As soon as food cues are reliable predictors of intake, they will elicit reactivity (Jansen et al., 2011, 2016), which in turn can lead to food intake. In CE, overweight people are repeatedly exposed to food cues while (over)eating is prevented (Bouton, 2004, 2011; Jansen et al., 2011, 2016; van den Akker et al., 2014). Exposure to food cues first increases food cue reactivity, but after prolonged and repeated non-reinforced exposure sessions, this reactivity decreases (Jansen et al., 2011, 2016). The CE rationale is that a new association between a food cue and intake is formed: the food cue does not predict intake. Importantly, this does not mean that the old association is unlearned (Bouton, 2004; Jansen et al., 2016). As a result of this inhibitory learning, reactivity to food cues diminishes (extinction). To optimize this inhibitory learning, maximizing “expectancy-violation” is a key element of successful therapy (Craske et al., 2014). Expectancy violation is the reduction in a person’s belief in his/her food-related expectancies (e.g., “If I feel exhausted and chocolate is available, then I will lose control and eat all chocolate”). CE has been shown to be an effective method to reduce food desires and overeating (Boutelle and Bouton, 2015; Jansen et al., 2016; Schyns et al., 2016), and it leads to short-term weight loss (Jansen et al., 2011, 2016; Schyns et al., 2016).

To gain insight in the mechanism of change, examining neural correlates of food cue reactivity may be valuable. A recent review described the following food-cue-reactivity-related brain regions: ventral striatum with nucleus accumbens (NAcc), midbrain, orbitofrontal cortex (OFC), anterior insula (INS), gustatory cortex (GC), lateral occipital cortex (LOC), and somatosensory cortex (SSC) (Giuliani et al., 2018). An increased activity was found in these brain regions when participants viewed high-caloric foods as compared to low-caloric foods or non-food images, and this was also predictive of the amount of food consumed (van der Laan et al., 2011; Smeets et al., 2012; Frankort et al., 2014; Giuliani et al., 2018; Hermann et al., 2019). However, a meta-analysis showed that these effects were quite inconsistent: the concurrence was moderate between studies in the activated clusters to food vs. non-food visual stimuli in healthy-weight participants (van der Laan et al., 2011). As CE intends to reduce food cue reactivity (Jansen et al., 2016), a decreased activity in food-cue-reactivity-related brain regions is expected in the current study.

Additionally, as a candidate-mechanism behind effective CE is inhibitory learning (Boutelle and Bouton, 2015), increased neural activity in inhibitory-control brain regions when processing palatable food stimuli is expected. Inhibitory-control-related brain regions include: dorso- and ventrolateral prefrontal cortex (dlPFC, vlPFC), parietal posterior cortex (PPC), dorsal anterior cingulate cortex (dACC), caudate, pre supplementary motor area (preSMA), and the globus pallidus (GP) (Kober et al., 2010; Giuliani et al., 2018).

In the current study, neural responses in food-cue-reactivity and in inhibitory-control brain regions to individually tailored high-caloric palatable food stimuli were examined pre- and post-CE or a healthy lifestyle (LS) intervention on subject-level (i.e., for each participant separately). During a functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) session, participants were instructed to actively evaluate the taste of the visually presented food stimuli (hedonic focus) or to evaluate the colors of these food stimuli (neutral focus). We hypothesized that CE, as compared to LS, would lead to reduced neural activity in food-cue-reactivity-related brain regions and increased neural activity in inhibitory-control-related brain regions when viewing high-caloric food stimuli, mostly in the hedonic focus condition as this focus is aligned with the experience of craving (Roefs et al., 2018).

Materials and Methods

Participants

Ten female overweight participants (BMI: M = 32.32 SD = 4.43 kg/m2, age: M = 38.40 SD = 10.76 years) from a larger trial (n = 45) participated in this study, and were randomly assigned to CE (n = 5) or LS (n = 5) (van den Akker et al., 2016; Schyns et al., 2019). To overcome the problem of high heterogeneity in neural responses due to individual differences that could occur in small sample sizes (Roiser et al., 2016), data were analyzed on subject-level, as separate cases (for a similar approach see: Hubacher et al., 2015). All participants, except one, were right-handed. Participants were scanned within 2 weeks before and within 2 weeks after intervention.1

Inclusion criteria included: female, age between 18 and 60 years, BMI of at least 27 kg/m2, no MRI contra-indications and no history of psychiatric or neurological illnesses. The study was approved by the local Ethical Committee. The participants gave written informed consent and were compensated for participation (€ 45).

Interventions

Interventions were provided by trained students, using a strict protocol, supervised by co-author GS. Both CE and LS consisted of eight individual sessions, scheduled twice per week, during ≈1 month.

During CE, participants performed several food cue exposures with a therapist. The exposure sessions were done in various overeating contexts (e.g., at the laboratory, at home watching television or work). Additionally, participants performed daily exposure exercises on their own at home or at other overeating-associated environmental contexts. LS consisted of four face-to-face sessions alternated with four telephone sessions. LS participants received healthy lifestyle advice, performed mindfulness and power posing exercises, and obtained psychoeducation on body image. For this intervention, daily homework exercises were given on mindfulness and on previous therapy session content. For a detailed description of both interventions see (van den Akker et al., 2016).

Behavioral Assessments

BMI Measurement

Weight and height were measured pre and post-intervention to compute BMI in kg/m2.

Hunger Assessment

Participants were asked to refrain from eating or drinking (except water) for at least 1 h before the scan-sessions. To check compliance and have an indicative for subjective hunger, self-reported hunger was measured using a 100 mm visual analog scale (VAS), with the question: “How hungry do you feel at this moment?” ranging from 0 (not hungry at all) to 100 (very hungry) at the start of each session. Additionally, participants registered what and at what time they had eaten last.

Expectancy Violation

Eight food-cue-associated eating beliefs were rated on perceived expectancy if an associated cue would be followed by eating. Expectancies were measured pre- and post-intervention using 100 mm VAS, with a higher score reflecting a greater perceived expectancy of eating (see methodology paper for details: van den Akker et al., 2016).

Stimuli

Individual Stimulus Selection

Food stimuli used in the fMRI experiment were individually tailored. Each participant selected their five most palatable food items from a list of 33 high-caloric food items in an online questionnaire that was completed ≈1 week before the first scanning session. She then rated the selected stimuli on 10-point scales ranging from 1 (not palatable at all) to 10 (very palatable).

Stimulus Presentation

For each of the five chosen palatable food items, two different pictures were included in the fMRI stimulation protocol, to avoid visual adaptation by seeing the same picture too often. Pictures were presented as pop-out high-resolution colored images on a light gray background (RGB: 191 191 191; CKYM: 25 20 20 0) in the center of a black screen covering a visual angle of ≈12°.

Experimental Task

Attentional Focus Manipulation

The participant performed a fast-paced 1-back task in each functional run to induce an attentional focus (hedonic vs. neutral). During the 1-back task, the participant compared each presented food picture (starting from the 2nd presented picture) to the previously presented picture, and indicated whether the presented food was more or less palatable than the previous one (hedonic focus), or whether the picture contained more or fewer colors than the previous one (neutral focus). Each food stimulus was presented for 500 ms, with an inter-stimulus interval (ISI) as a response window of 1,500 ms. The participant’s responses were registered using a buttonbox, with a right index finger press for “fewer” and a right middle finger press for “more.”

fMRI Stimulation Protocol

The fMRI task consisted of four runs. In each run, six different conditions were presented, but for the current study only two conditions were relevant and included in the analyses: blocks with palatable high-caloric food stimuli – neutral focus (PAL-NEU) and blocks with palatable high-caloric food stimuli – hedonic focus (PAL-HED). Ten blocks were presented seven times in a randomized order with 12 stimuli each, across the four functional runs. Prior to each block, a cue-word “taste” or “color” was presented for 1 s to inform the participant which attentional focus to apply. Blocks lasted 24 s and were always followed by a 20 s rest block (fixation cross). Total functional scanning time was ≈35 min.

MRI Data Acquisition

Images were acquired on a 3 Tesla MRI scanner (Magnetom Prisma Fit, Siemens Medical Systems) using a 64-channel head/neck coil. Functional (T2-weighted) images were acquired using multiband gradient echo-planar imaging in an axial interleaved order (Feinberg et al., 2010) with the following settings: TR = 2,000 ms, TE = 30 ms, flip angle = 77°, FOV = 208 × 124 mm2, and voxel size of 2 × 2 × 2 mm3. To ensure whole brain coverage, slices were acquired in a backward tilted direction of ≈15 degrees to the transversal – coronal line. As anatomical scan, a high-resolution, three-dimensional (3D) T1-weighted MPRAGE scan was acquired, with the following settings: TR = 2,250 ms, TE = 2.21 ms, flip angle = 9°, FOV = 256 × 192 mm2, and voxel size 1 × 1 × 1 mmł and had a duration of ± 5 min.

fMRI Data Analysis

Preprocessing

Analyses were performed using SPM12 (Statistical Parametric Mapping, London, United Kingdom) and Matlab version 8.3.0.532 (R2014a). Functional images were slice-time corrected, realigned, co-registered, normalized using unified brain segmentation, and spatially smoothed using a Gaussian kernel of 6 mm full width at half-maximum (FWHM). Preprocessed functional volume time series were used for statistical analysis.

Statistical Analysis

To compare the session differences on subject-level, a general linear model (GLM) design matrix was created including the two scan-sessions (pre- and post-intervention) as eight consecutive runs. Each experimental task condition was set as a predictor, which resulted in six predictors of interest per run (with two of interest for the current study). Additionally, six motion and eight run mean intensity predictors of no interest were added to the model as confound regressors. Predictor time courses were obtained using condition box-car shaped waves convolved with a two-gamma ideal hemodynamic response function (HRF).

Case Series Approach: First-Level Analysis

To investigate the effects of interest, we computed the following contrasts on subject-level for the high-caloric palatable food conditions: (1) main effect of session (t-contrasts A: pre-intervention > post-intervention and B: post-intervention > pre-intervention) and (2) session (pre-intervention vs. post-intervention) attentional focus (neutral vs. hedonic) interaction F-contrasts. To extract beta values, each condition of interest was also contrasted against baseline.2

Region of Interest (ROI) Analysis

We defined a-priori ROI masks for food cue reactivity based on the review of Giuliani and colleagues (Giuliani et al., 2018), including: ventral striatum with NAcc, midbrain, OFC, anterior INS, GC, LOC, and SSC and for inhibitory control, including: dlPFC, vlPFC, PPC, dACC, caudate, preSMA and the GP. The ROI masks were manually generated by using the WFU Pickatlas tool (version 3.0.5) in SPM12.

To correct for multiple comparisons, family-wise error (FWE) correction based on Gaussian random field theory was applied to control for false positives at α = 0.05 on subject-level (Eklund et al., 2016). This method was applied for the statistical maps of the main effects of session, and was combined with a clustersize threshold (k) of three contiguous voxels to only include more robust clusters. For the subtler session attentional focus interaction, uncorrected statistical maps with p < 0.001 with k = 3 voxels were reported. The MarsBar toolbox3 was used to extract beta values in SPM12. For localization and clustersize information of activated clusters, XJview4 was used. Figures were created using the MNI brain template from MRIcroGL software package5.

Results

Behavioral Assessments

An overview of behavioral assessments is provided in Table 1. The time between first and second scan session ranged for CE: 34–43 days and for LS: 36–58 days. Three CE and four LS participants lost weight after the intervention. Hunger-ratings were higher post-intervention than pre-intervention in all CE participants, whereas this was only true for two LS-participants (LS1 and LS5). However, 7 of the 10 participants reported relatively low hunger at post-intervention (scores ≤ 44 on a 0–100 VAS). All participants rated their selected foods as highly palatable (average scores ≥ 8.8 on 10-point scale). Expectancy violation changes (post-intervention – pre-intervention) differed between CE and LS participants. All CE participants showed a higher reduction of eating expectancies after intervention as compared to the LS participants.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Participant characteristics and pre-post intervention behavioral measures and post – pre differences.

Neural Responses

In Figures 1, 2, main effects of session are displayed, as well as the session attentional focus interaction per participant in the food-cue-reactivity-ROIs. Details of each significant cluster can be found in Tables 2, 3.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Results from univariate analyses per participant for cue exposure intervention in food-cue-reactivity-ROIs. t-maps of significant main effects of session are shown in food-cue-reactivity-ROIs: pre > post-intervention in red, post > pre-intervention in blue (p < 0.05 FWE cor) and F-map of session * attentional focus interaction (p < 0.001 unc.) in green. Bar plots represents mean extracted beta values from the contributing clusters per condition per comparison. N1, neutral attentional focus pre-intervention; H1, hedonic attentional focus pre-intervention; N2, neutral attentional focus post-intervention; H2, hedonic attentional focus post-intervention; LOC, Lateral Occipital Complex; SSC, somatosensory cortex; OFC, orbitofrontal cortex, INS, Insula.

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Results from univariate analyses per participant for healthy lifestyle intervention in food-cue-reactivity-ROIs. t-maps of significant main effects of session are shown in food-cue-reactivity-ROIs: pre > post-intervention in red, post > pre-intervention in blue (p < 0.05 FWE cor) and F-map of session * attentional focus interaction (p < 0.001 unc.) in green. Bar plots represents mean extracted beta values from the contributing clusters per condition per comparison. N1, neutral attentional focus pre-intervention; H1, hedonic attentional focus pre-intervention; N2, neutral attentional focus post-intervention; H2, hedonic attentional focus post-intervention; LOC, Lateral Occipital Complex; SSC, somatosensory cortex; OFC, orbitofrontal cortex; INS, Insula.

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Significant clusters from univariate analyses per participant for cue exposure intervention in food-cue-reactivity-ROIs.

TABLE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. Significant clusters from univariate analyses per participant for healthy lifestyle intervention in food-cue-reactivity-ROIs.

Main Effects Session

Contrary to our hypothesis, food-cue-reactivity-related activity was not substantially reduced after intervention for CE participants. In fact, LS participants showed reductions in more ROIs (e.g., SSC, INS, LOC, and OFC; see participants LS1 and LS3), and involved clusters were larger. However, reduction in beta values was larger in those clusters that changed significantly from pre- to post-intervention in CE participants. Note that these clusters were substantially smaller and localized in the LOC solely (see participants: CE1, CE3, CE4, and CE5). To examine opposite effects, we also compared post-intervention > pre-intervention contrasts. Here, unexpectedly, the CE participants showed also increased activation in small clusters in the LOC (see: CE1 and CE5) and in the OFC (CE2) after intervention. LS participants showed an increase in activity in the SSC (LS2, LS4 and LS5) and in the LOC (LS3).

Session Attentional Focus Interaction

As with the main effect of session, the interaction effect was also mainly observed in LS participants. Analyses of the interaction yielded significant clusters for all LS participants. Here, four LS participants (LS1, LS3, LS4, and LS5) showed a larger reduction from pre to post with the hedonic focus than with the neutral focus in the right INS and OFC and bilaterally in the SSC and LOC. For participant LS2 this was reversed, activity in food-cue-reactivity-ROIs (right OFC and left SSC) was increased from pre to post with the hedonic focus. Two CE participants showed an interaction effect, where participant CE4 showed a more reduced activation pre to post in the hedonic focus than in the neutral focus in a very small cluster in the right SCC. Participant CE3 showed more robust clusters, involving the INS and the SCC with an unexpectedly larger reduction in activation from pre to post in the neutral focus condition than the hedonic focus.

We also compared neural activity per participant in inhibitory-control-ROIs. Tests of the main- and interaction-effects did not lead to any meaningful results for the CE intervention either. The clusters of neural activity per participant can be found in Supplementary Tables S1, S2.

Discussion

Contrary to our hypothesis, the results showed that for these cases a cue exposure intervention did not lead to a significantly stronger reduction in neural activity in brain regions related to food-cue-reactivity, in response to visual high-caloric palatable food stimuli, as compared to the participants that received a lifestyle intervention. In fact, most participants’ reductions in neural activity in food-cue-reactivity-related brain regions were more pronounced and more widespread after a lifestyle intervention and mostly with a hedonic attentional focus. When comparing activity in inhibitory-control-related brain regions on subject-level, no meaningful results were observed.

Surprisingly, the expected reduced activity was more apparent in LS participants (in e.g., SSC, INS, OFC, and LOC). During the intervention, LS participants received education on dieting and healthy weight loss and on nutrients and energy balance (van den Akker et al., 2016). This could have raised awareness of negative health aspects of high caloric foods, which may have contributed to participants’ reduced neural responses to high-caloric foods. This interpretation aligns well with previous studies, showing that focusing on negative health aspects can control reward-related activity to visually presented high-caloric food stimuli (Hollmann et al., 2012; Siep et al., 2012). Although participants were instructed during scanning to attend to the hedonic aspects of the foods presented, this lifestyle training may have interfered with this hedonic focus during the post-intervention scanning-session by increasing awareness of negative health aspects.

Unexpectedly, CE did not lead to a significant reduction of neural activity in most cases in food-cue-reactivity-related brain regions. Behavioral outcomes showed that self-reported expectancy violations did improve specifically for the CE participants. Also, hunger was higher for all CE participants at post-intervention measurement. However, these CE-related behavioral effects could not be meaningfully related to post-pre intervention patterns of neural activity. These neural pre-post intervention findings could be the consequence of participants learning a new inhibitory association (the cue does not predict intake) during food cue exposure, which then exists next to the original disinhibiting association (the cue does predicts intake) (Jansen et al., 2016). That is, the food-cue-intake association is not erased, and therefore food cues might still trigger neural activity in food-cue-reactivity-ROIs. However, also in inhibitory-control-related brain regions, no strong increased neural activity in CE participants was found after intervention in these inhibitory-control regions.

Important to realize is that inhibitory learning during extinction is context-dependent and food-specific (Bouton, 2004, 2011; Jansen et al., 2016). Both the context (fMRI scanner vs. a laboratory room, participants’ home and other relevant contexts) and the food stimuli differed between the current fMRI measurement and the intervention setting. Furthermore, a CE intervention only led to reduced consumption of the exposed foods, but not of other foods (Schyns et al., 2016, 2018, 2019). So, there was no generalization to other foods. In an earlier study (Frankort et al., 2014), we did observe a reduction in neural activity in food-cue-reactivity-related brain regions after cue exposure. Importantly, here, the cue exposure and measurement of neural activity both took place in the scanner while using the same food stimuli throughout (i.e., chocolate) (Frankort et al., 2014). Taken together, these findings underline the importance of considering context and the food-specificity of cue exposure while examining neural responses.

In line with our hypothesis, the current study showed reduced activation in the lateral occipital complex (LOC) in four participants after CE. The LOC was identified, in a meta-analysis comparing visual food to non-food stimuli, as one of the main brain regions involved in visual food cue processing (van der Laan et al., 2011). The decreased LOC activity may reflect a decrease in visual saliency of the palatable high-caloric foods as a result of CE. As this decreased LOC activation was specifically found for the CE participants, it therefore might be a precursor for extinction.

A limitation of this study is that due to the inclusion of only female participants and the case-series analyses approach, it is hard to translate the current results to a group-intervention effect, or to a broader population (i.e., males). Results should be interpreted with caution. Data was analyzed with a-priori defined ROI masks, which reduces the between-subjects variability of activation locations (Ziauddeen et al., 2012) and makes interpreting and comparing findings more reliable. The scanning-protocol pre- and post-intervention was kept exactly the same, which made it a strong within-subject design, and therefore the current study might give interesting leads for conducting a group-level future study. Future research needs to replicate these findings, and investigate whether neural changes induced by a lifestyle intervention are related to concurrent and future weight change.

Data Availability Statement

All datasets generated for this study are included in the article/Supplementary Material.

Ethics Statement

The studies involving human participants were reviewed and approved by the local Ethical Committee of the faculty of Psychology and Neuroscience, Maastricht University. The patients/participants provided their written informed consent to participate in this study.

Author Contributions

SF, AR, and AJ designed the study. SF collected the data and analyzed the data. GS and KA designed and coordinated the interventions. SF and AR wrote the manuscript. AJ, GS, and KA gave feedback on the manuscript. All authors approved the final version.

Funding

This study was partly financed by the Netherlands Organization for Scientific Research (NWO) vici-grant (453.10.006) and the Maastricht University Interfaculty Program “Eatwell” awarded to AJ, and NWO vidi-grant (452.16.007) awarded to AR.

Conflict of Interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Supplementary Material

The Supplementary Material for this article can be found online at: https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fnbeh.2020.00046/full#supplementary-material

Footnotes

  1. ^ Note that these pre-intervention fMRI data was also included in a previous study (Franssen et al., 2020).
  2. ^ For participant 2 of the LS group only three runs were acquired in the pre-intervention session due to set-up problems. Therefore, also only three runs of the post-intervention session were included for this participant. This was done to be able to balance the conditions in the GLM for defining contrasts. For the post-intervention session, we selected the three runs with best data quality (i.e., least movement of participant during scanning).
  3. ^ http://marsbar.sourceforge.net/
  4. ^ http://www.alivelearn.net/xjview/
  5. ^ http://www.mccauslandcenter.sc.edu/mricro/mricrogl/

References

Bish, C. L., Blanck, H. M., Serdula, M. K., Marcus, M., Kohl, H. W., and Khan, L. K. (2005). Diet and physical activity behaviors among americans trying to lose weight: 2000 behavioral risk factor surveillance system. Obes. Res. 13, 596–607. doi: 10.1038/oby.2005.64

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Boswell, R. G., and Kober, H. (2016). Food cue reactivity and craving predict eating and weight gain: a meta-analytic review. Obes. Rev. 17, 159–177. doi: 10.1111/obr.12354

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Boutelle, K. N., and Bouton, M. E. (2015). Implications of learning theory for developing programs to decrease overeating. Appetite 93, 62–74. doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2015.05.013

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bouton, M. E. (2004). Context and behavioral processes in extinction. Learn. Mem. 11, 485–494. doi: 10.1101/lm.78804

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bouton, M. E. (2011). Learning and the persistence of appetite: extinction and the motivation to eat and overeat. Physiol. Behav. 103, 51–58. doi: 10.1016/j.physbeh.2010.11.025

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Craske, M. G., Treanor, M., Conway, C. C., Zbozinek, T., and Vervliet, B. (2014). Maximizing exposure therapy: an inhibitory learning approach. Behav. Res. Ther. 58, 10–23. doi: 10.1016/j.brat.2014.04.006

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Eklund, A., Nichols, T. E., and Knutsson, H. (2016). Cluster failure: Why fMRI inferences for spatial extent have inflated false-positive rates. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 113, 7900–7905. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1602413113

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Feinberg, D. A., Moeller, S., Smith, S. M., Auerbach, E., Ramanna, S., Glasser, M. F., et al. (2010). Multiplexed echo planar imaging for sub-second whole brain fmri and fast diffusion imaging. PLoS One 5:e15710. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0015710

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ferriday, D., and Brunstrom, J. M. (2011). ‘I just can’t help myself’: effects of food-cue exposure in overweight and lean individuals. Int. J. Obes. 35, 142–149. doi: 10.1038/ijo.2010.117

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Frankort, A., Roefs, A., Siep, N., Roebroeck, A., Havermans, R., and Jansen, A. (2014). The craving stops before you feel it: neural correlates of chocolate craving during cue exposure with response prevention. Cereb. Cortex 24, 1589–1600. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bht016

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Franssen, S., Jansen, A., den Hurk, J., Roebroeck, A., and Roefs, A. (2020). Power of mind: attentional focus rather than food palatability dominates neural responding to visual food stimuli in females with overweight. Appetite 148:104609. doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2020.104609

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Giuliani, N. R., Merchant, J. S., Cosme, D., and Berkman, E. T. (2018). Neural predictors of eating behavior and dietary change. Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci. 00, 1–13. doi: 10.1111/nyas.13637

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hall, K. D., Heymsfield, S. B., Kemnitz, J. W., Klein, S., Schoeller, D. A., and Speakman, J. R. (2012). Energy balance and its components: implications for body weight regulation. Am. J. Clin. Nutr. 95, 989–994. doi: 10.3945/ajcn.112.036350

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hermann, P., Gál, V., Kóbor, I., Kirwan, C. B., Kovács, P., Kitka, T., et al. (2019). Efficacy of weight loss intervention can be predicted based on early alterations of fMRI food cue reactivity in the striatum. Neuroimage Clin. 23:101803. doi: 10.1016/j.nicl.2019.101803

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hollmann, M., Hellrung, L., Pleger, B., Schlögl, H., Kabisch, S., Stumvoll, M., et al. (2012). Neural correlates of the volitional regulation of the desire for food. Int. J. Obes. 36, 648–655. doi: 10.1038/ijo.2011.125

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hubacher, M., Kappos, L., Weier, K., Stöcklin, M., Opwis, K., and Penner, I.-K. (2015). Case-based fMRI analysis after cognitive rehabilitation in MS: a novel approach. Front. Neurol. 6:78. doi: 10.3389/fneur.2015.00078

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jansen, A., Havermans, R. C., and Nederkoorn, C. (2011). “Cued Overeating,” in Handbook of Behavior, Food and Nutrition, eds V. R. Preedy, R. R. Watson, and C. R. Martin (New York, NY: Springer), 1431–1443. doi: 10.1007/978-0-387-92271-3

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jansen, A., Schyns, G., Bongers, P., and van den Akker, K. (2016). From lab to clinic: extinction of cued cravings to reduce overeating. Physiol. Behav. 162, 174–180. doi: 10.1016/j.physbeh.2016.03.018

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kober, H., Mende-Siedlecki, P., Kross, E. F., Weber, J., Mischel, W., Hart, C. L., et al. (2010). Prefrontal-striatal pathway underlies cognitive regulation of craving. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 107, 14811–14816. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1007779107

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mitchell, N. S., Catenacci, V. A., Wyatt, H. R., and Hill, J. O. (2011). Obesity: overview of an epidemic. Psychiatr. Clin. North Am. 34, 717–732. doi: 10.1016/j.psc.2011.08.005

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Roefs, A., Franssen, S., and Jansen, A. (2018). The dynamic nature of food reward processing in the brain. Curr. Opin. Clin. Nutr. Metab. Care 21, 444–448. doi: 10.1097/MCO.0000000000000504

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Roiser, J. P., Linden, D. E., Gorno-Tempinin, M. L., Moran, R. J., Dickerson, B. C., and Grafton, S. T. (2016). Minimum statistical standards for submissions to neuroimage: clinical. Neuroimage Clin. 12, 1045–1047. doi: 10.1016/j.nicl.2016.08.002

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schyns, G., Roefs, A., Mulkens, S., and Jansen, A. (2016). Expectancy violation, reduction of food cue reactivity and less eating in the absence of hunger after one food cue exposure session for overweight and obese women. Behav. Res. Ther. 76, 57–64. doi: 10.1016/j.brat.2015.11.007

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schyns, G., van den Akker, K., Roefs, A., Hilberath, R., and Jansen, A. (2018). What works better? Food cue exposure aiming at the habituation of eating desires or food cue exposure aiming at the violation of overeating expectancies? Behav. Res. Ther. 102, 1–7. doi: 10.1016/j.brat.2017.12.001

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schyns, G., van den Akker, K., Roefs, A., Houben, K., and Jansen, A. (2019). Exposure therapy vs lifestyle intervention to reduce food cue reactivity and binge eating in obesity: a pilot study. J. Behav. Ther. Exp. Psychiatry 67:101453. doi: 10.1016/j.jbtep.2019.01.005

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Siep, N., Roefs, A., Roebroeck, A., Havermans, R., Bonte, M., and Jansen, A. (2012). Fighting food temptations: The modulating effects of short-term cognitive reappraisal, suppression and up-regulation on mesocorticolimbic activity related to appetitive motivation. Neuroimage 60, 213–220. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.12.067

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Smeets, P. A. M., Charbonnier, L., van Meer, F., van der Laan, L. N., and Spetter, M. S. (2012). Food-induced brain responses and eating behaviour. Proc. Nutr. Soc. 71, 511–520. doi: 10.1017/S0029665112000808

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

van den Akker, K., Havermans, R. C., Bouton, M. E., and Jansen, A. (2014). How partial reinforcement of food cues affects the extinction and reacquisition of appetitive responses. New model for dieting success? Appetite 81, 242–252. doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2014.06.024

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

van den Akker, K., Schyns, G., and Jansen, A. (2016). Enhancing inhibitory learning to reduce overeating: design and rationale of a cue exposure therapy trial in overweight and obese women. Contemp. Clin. Trials 49, 85–91. doi: 10.1016/j.cct.2016.06.008

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

van der Laan, L. N., de Ridder, D. T. D., Viergever, M. A., and Smeets, P. A. M. (2011). The first taste is always with the eyes: a meta-analysis on the neural correlates of processing visual food cues. Neuroimage 55, 296–303. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.11.055

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wing, R. R., and Phelan, S. (2005). Long-term weight loss maintenance. Am. J. Clin. Nutr 82, 222–225. doi: 10.1093/ajcn/82.1.222S.

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

World Health Organization (2018). Available online at: http://www.who.int/en/news-room/fact-sheets/detail/obesity-and-overweight Geneva. World Health Organization.

Google Scholar

Ziauddeen, H., Farooqi, I. S., and Fletcher, P. C. (2012). Obesity and the brain: how convincing is the addiction model? Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 13, 279–286. doi: 10.1038/nrn3212

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: obesity, exposure therapy, functional MRI, case-series, cue reactivity

Citation: Franssen S, Jansen A, Schyns G, van den Akker K and Roefs A (2020) Neural Correlates of Food Cue Exposure Intervention for Obesity: A Case-Series Approach. Front. Behav. Neurosci. 14:46. doi: 10.3389/fnbeh.2020.00046

Received: 18 December 2019; Accepted: 16 March 2020;
Published: 21 April 2020.

Edited by:

Liana Fattore, Italian National Research Council, Italy

Reviewed by:

Claire Jennifer Foldi, Monash University, Australia
Jamie Peters, University of Colorado, United States

Copyright © 2020 Franssen, Jansen, Schyns, van den Akker and Roefs. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Sieske Franssen, sieske.franssen@maastrichtuniversity.nl

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.