Skip to main content

HYPOTHESIS AND THEORY article

Front. Behav. Neurosci., 11 April 2019
Sec. Pathological Conditions
This article is part of the Research Topic Impulsive Compulsive Spectrum Disorders View all 21 articles

Compulsivity in Alcohol Use Disorder and Obsessive Compulsive Disorder: Implications for Neuromodulation

  • 1Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Sciences, Albert Einstein College of Medicine, Montefiore Medical Center, Bronx, NY, United States
  • 2Department of Health Sciences, University of Florence, Florence, Italy
  • 3Center for Morphometric Analysis, Massachusetts General Hospital, Harvard Medical School, Boston, MA, United States
  • 4Psychiatry Neuroimaging Laboratory, Brigham and Women’s Hospital, Harvard Medical School, Boston, MA, United States
  • 5Section on Clinical Psychoneuroendocrinology and Neuropsychopharmacology, National Institute on Alcohol Abuse and Alcoholism and National Institute on Drug Abuse, Bethesda, MD, United States
  • 6Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Sciences, Stanford University Medical Center, Stanford, CA, United States

Alcohol use Disorder (AUD) is one of the leading causes of morbidity and mortality worldwide. The progression of the disorder is associated with the development of compulsive alcohol use, which in turn contributes to the high relapse rate and poor longer term functioning reported in most patients, even with treatment. While the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (DSM-5) defines AUD by a cluster of symptoms, parsing its heterogeneous phenotype by domains of behavior such as compulsivity may be a critical step to improve outcomes of this condition. Still, neurobiological underpinnings of compulsivity need to be fully elucidated in AUD in order to better design targeted treatment strategies. In this manuscript, we review and discuss findings supporting common mechanisms between AUD and OCD, dissecting the construct of compulsivity and focusing specifically on characteristic disruptions in habit learning and cognitive control in the two disorders. Finally, neuromodulatory interventions are proposed as a probe to test compulsivity as key pathophysiologic feature of AUD, and as a potential therapy for the subgroup of individuals with compulsive alcohol use, i.e., the more resistant stage of the disorder. This transdiagnostic approach may help to destigmatize the disorder, and suggest potential treatment targets across different conditions.

Introduction

Alcohol use disorder (AUD), a problematic pattern of alcohol use accompanied by clinically significant impairment or distress (American Psychiatric Association, 2013), is one of the leading causes of morbidity and mortality worldwide (GBD 2016 Alcohol Collaborators, 2018). Globally, with 100.4 million estimated cases, AUD was the most prevalent substance use disorder (SUD) in 2016, with 99.2 million and three quarters of all substance use-attributable disability-adjusted life-years (DALYs). In the United States, 12-month and lifetime AUD prevalence is, respectively, estimated to be up to 14 and 30% of the adult population, with 9.8% of all deaths attributable to acute or chronic alcohol use (World Health Organization, 2018).

Despite there being effective treatments available, only an estimated 21.3 and 34.7% of patients with severe (≥6 DSM criteria) 12-month and lifetime AUD seek treatment in the United States (Grant et al., 2016). Further, high rates of relapse and poor longer term functioning are reported in the minority of patients that get some treatment (Maisto et al., 2018). This is particularly true for the subgroup of individuals with severe AUD (Tuithof et al., 2013), that is also characterized by a longer duration of untreated AUD, a lower level of spontaneous recovery as well as higher rates of psychiatric comorbidity (Grant et al., 2016; Saha et al., 2018). In addition to low rates of treatment utilization, AUD is indeed a heterogeneous disorder (Jellinek, 1960; Moss et al., 2007) and current therapeutic approaches are not developed to address this clinical variation. Hence, the magnitude of the therapeutic effect of the available AUD interventions is, overall, relatively modest (Kranzler and Soyka, 2018).

Next to the need to educate the public and policy makers in order to destigmatize the disorder and increase treatment rates (Keyes et al., 2010), the gaps in research and clinical care call for a shared framework to further characterize this heterogeneous disorder and lead to the development of new therapeutics, ultimately giving the clinician a biologically based roadmap to guide assessment and prioritize treatment (National Institute on Alcohol Abuse and Alcoholism, 2017). In this direction, the National Institute on Alcohol Abuse and Alcoholism (NIAAA) has implemented an Alcohol Addiction Research Domain Criteria (AARDoC) in order to identify specific domains relevant for alcohol addiction (Litten et al., 2015). Recently, incentive salience, negative emotionality and executive function have been proposed as key dimensional domains for the assessment of SUDs and AUD, which map onto the three stage cycle of the development of addiction over time (Koob and Le Moal, 1997; Koob and Volkow, 2016; Kwako et al., 2018). The three stage cycle (Koob and Le Moal, 1997) represents a model that provides an understanding of the development of AUD overtime, and may help organize research in AUD complementing the atemporal RDoC framework. The three stage cycle is consistent with the conceptualization of alcohol addiction as an aberrant form of learning, where alcohol exposure leads in time to alteration in the neurocircuitry underlying stress response, reward and cognitive functioning, all of which ultimately leads to compulsive substance use (Wise, 1987; Di Chiara, 1999; Berke and Hyman, 2000; Everitt et al., 2001; Lubman et al., 2004). Hence, compulsive drug seeking has been identified as the central, defining property of alcohol and substance use disorders (Wise and Koob, 2014; NIAAA; National Institute of Drug Abuse). The development of repetitive drug patterns of use, in turn, has been proposed as a main pathophysiologic factor contributing to the high relapse rates characteristic of addiction (Heyman, 2013).

While the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (DSM-5) defines AUD by a cluster of symptoms, parsing its phenotype by domains of behavior, such as compulsivity, may be a critical step to improve outcomes of this condition. On the other hand, neurobiological underpinnings of compulsivity still need to be fully elucidated in addiction -and AUD in particular- in order to design specific targeted treatment strategies.

Compulsivity (Box 1) is a construct that encompasses motor, cognitive, affective and motivational processes. Multiple models have addressed the central question regarding the transition from casual to compulsive drug use over the development of addiction. The traditional hedonic/withdrawal hypothesis of addiction explained compulsive drug use as earlier pleasure seeking and later attempts to avoid unpleasant withdrawal symptoms (Koob and Le Moal, 1997, 2001), but this could not fully explain the high rates of relapse after long period of drug abstinence (Robinson and Berridge, 1993, 2000). More recently this theory has been complemented by models showing how other phenomena take place in the development of addiction, such as incentive sensitization (Robinson and Berridge, 2003), aberrant learning (Robbins and Everitt, 2002), and loss of cognitive control (Jentsch and Taylor, 1999). At the brain connectivity level, these phenomena have demonstrated to map onto limbic-cortico-striatal networks (Ma et al., 2010), with important overlap with neuroimaging findings in OCD (van den Heuvel et al., 2016).

Box 1. Definitions.

Compulsivity The experience of the urge to perform an overt or covert behavior in a stereotyped manner that persists despite lack of goal orientation resulting in not valuable or adverse consequences.
Habit (Stimulus- Response associations) Habitual responses that are directly triggered by stimuli and are defined by insensitivity to their consequences. The S-R associations that mediate habits have been reinforced either by past experiences with reward or by the omission of aversive events.
Goal directed behavior (Response-Outcome associations) Behavior that is mediated by knowledge of the casual relationship between the action and its outcomes and that is performed when the consequences actually constitute a rewarding goal. The goal-directed system exerts control over habits in light of new information.
Inhibitory Control It is a cognitive mechanism that includes exerting control over both goal-directed, reward-seeking (impulsive) and automatic (compulsive) actions.
Reward The subjective salience value of a stimulus that has the potential to induce goal directed behavior.
Incentive salience A form of non-cognitive wanting triggered by reward-related cues and characteristic of the transition from hedonic to habit like compulsive drug seeking; it is explained by conditioned reinforcement of drug-related cues.
Relapse Spontaneous recurrence of a learned behavior (i.e., compulsive drug use) after a given period of extinction.

Following the characterization of the neural basis of compulsivity developed in the field of Obsessive Compulsive Disorder (OCD) research, we discuss the findings supporting common mechanisms between AUD and OCD, focusing on specific aspects of the construct, namely habit learning and cognitive control. Finally, neuromodulatory interventions are proposed as a probe of this hypothesis and as a potential therapy for the subgroup of individuals with AUD who demonstrate compulsive alcohol seeking and use, i.e., the later more severe stage of AUD. In particular, deep transcranial magnetic stimulation (dTMS), recently approved by the Food and Drug Administration to treat compulsivity in OCD, is specifically discussed as a promising non-invasive intervention to treat compulsivity in AUD.

Such a transdiagnostic approach aims to destigmatize AUD, and to help clarify common pathophysiological mechanisms underpinning compulsive behavior across different disorders (Fineberg et al., 2016; Gillan et al., 2016). Consistent with a need for new paths of research in the field of treatments for addictions, the ultimate aim of this manuscript is to highlight and discuss the scientific rationale supporting the development of evidence based treatments for compulsivity in AUD, eventually as a model for other disorders of addiction.

The Habit Model of Compulsivity

Compulsivity has been defined as a behavioral trait in which actions are persistent and repeated despite adverse consequences (Robbins et al., 2012) (Box 1). An initial hypothesis regarding compulsive behavior in OCD -where compulsivity has traditionally found its paradigmatic expression- purported that it represents a goal-directed process associated with a “cognitive bias” or disrupted assignment of value toward available alternative behaviors which are performed to reduce the likelihood that a feared consequence will take place (Salkovskis, 1985; Rachman, 1997). Later, OCD was conceptualized as a disorder of maladaptive habit learning (Graybiel and Rauch, 2000) on the basis of overlap between the frontostriatal circuits underlying repetitive behavioral habits and OCD (Alexander et al., 1986). Data from subsequent preclinical and clinical studies have actually elucidated the neural basis of habit formation in humans or the transition from goal-directed to habitual behavior as a shift away from signaling in ventral associative frontostriatal circuits comprising ventromedial prefrontal cortex (vmPFC), medial orbitofrontal cortex (mOFC) and caudate, toward that in dorsal sensorimotor frontostriatal circuits, including posterior putamen and premotor cortex (Balleine and O’Doherty, 2010; Gillan and Robbins, 2014). In particular, estimated white matter strength in the vmPFC seeded from the caudate have been found to predict flexible goal-directed action, while estimated white matter tract strength in the premotor cortex seeded from the posterior putamen predicts vulnerability to habitual behavioral control in healthy humans (de Wit S. et al., 2012). Patients with OCD consistently exhibit a cognitive bias toward habit learning over goal-directed behavior (Gillan et al., 2011, 2014; Vaghi et al., 2019) associated with relative higher task-related activity in dorsal vs. ventral striatum and weakened resting state caudate-ventrolateral prefrontal cortex (vlPFC) and putamen-dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (dlPFC) connectivity (Banca et al., 2015; Vaghi et al., 2017). In particular, decreased activity in caudate-prefrontal circuits accompanied by hyperactivation of subthalamic nucleus/putaminal regions have been associated to symptom generation in OCD (Banca et al., 2015). Hyperactivity of putamen preceded deactivation during avoidance/relief events indicating a pivotal role of the putamen in regulation of behavior and habit formation in OCD (Banca et al., 2015). Consistently, task dependent functional connectivity during goal-directed planning showed reduced hypoactivation of the right dlPFC coupled with reduced functional connectivity between this latter and the putamen (Vaghi et al., 2017). Other findings indicate a dissociation between actual behavior and subjectively reported action-outcome knowledge, reinforcing the notion that OCD may be driven by a disrupted goal-directed system over habitual response, rather than by dysfunctional beliefs (Vaghi et al., 2019). This diminished outcome sensitivity has also been associated with diminished caudate-parietal connectivity (Harrison et al., 2013).

Based on these studies, evidence favors the habit hypothesis for compulsivity in OCD: this model posits that rather than goal-directed avoidance behaviors, compulsions are a result of excessive habit formation (Gillan and Robbins, 2014; Burguière et al., 2015). Irrational threat beliefs (obsessions) characteristic of OCD may be a consequence, rather than a cause, of compulsive behavior. In other words, obsessions may be an attempt to resolve the discrepancy between patients’ value attribution and their cognitively inexplicable urge to perform compulsive responses to determined stimuli.

Notably, deficits in goal-directed control and over–reliance on habits are a model of compulsivity that is applicable to other psychiatric disorders where compulsions feature prominently (Hollander, 1993; Hollander and Wong, 1995; Fontenelle et al., 2011; Voon et al., 2015) in particular to drug addiction (Robbins and Everitt, 1999; Gerdeman et al., 2003; Everitt and Robbins, 2005; Hogarth et al., 2013).

Habit Hypothesis of Compulsivity in AUD

Compulsivity and Habit Learning in Addiction

Drug addiction has been defined as representing a dysregulation of motivational circuits caused by development of aberrant incentive salience and habit formation, accompanied by compromised executive function over the use of the substance (Koob and Volkow, 2016). Such trajectory correlates with a progression from reward-driven to habit-driven drug-seeking behavior overtime (Koob and Le Moal, 2005; Everitt and Robbins, 2013).

Borrowing from the habit hypothesis of OCD, the experience of “wanting” the drug, progressively detached from the experience of “liking” it (Berridge and Robinson, 2016), may be understood as post hoc cognitive rationalizations of a goal-insensitive, stimulus-driven behavior (Gillan and Robbins, 2014). While the expectancy of reward may be crucial to initiate drug self-administration in the early stage of the disorder, the drive toward consumption of the drug triggered by the cue becomes quite indipendent from the expectancy of pleasure in the late stages of the disorder. Thanks to conditioned reinforcement, over time the “wanting” for the drug becomes triggered by the incentive motivational properties of drug cues, independently of a cognitive desire for a declarative goal (Berridge and Robinson, 2016). This shift in phenomenology has been associated with a cascade of neuroadaptations that engage the dorsal striatal habit system over the ventral striatal loop of reward and motivation mediated by phasic dopamine release in the dorsolateral striatum (see for review of clinical studies Volkow and Fowler, 2000; Newlin and Strubler, 2007; Dolan and Dayan, 2013; Grant and Kim, 2014; Koob and Volkow, 2016). In particular, translational studies have shown how a gradual progression from hedonic to habitual and compulsive drug use over time (i.e., a gradual progression from a response that is first dependent and then independent from evaluation of the relationship between action and outcomes eventually becomes a response despite adverse consequences) is associated with (i) habit overlearning and (ii) parallel reduction in goal-directed behaviors and inhibitory control that correspond to a shift in recruitment of ventral to dorsal regions of the striatum (Yin et al., 2004; Belin and Everitt, 2008; Balleine et al., 2009; Lesscher and Vanderschuren, 2012; Willuhn et al., 2012). Intermittent drug–induced dopamine (DA) signaling promotes the ability of drug-paired cues to increase DA levels and recruit striatal-globus pallidal-thalamo-cortical loops that engage the dorsal striatum. This shift from a system dedicated to updating predictions of value (ventral) to a system dedicated to the optimization of reward-related responses (dorsal) augments progression through the addiction cycle and helps explain craving and compulsive drug use (Koob and Volkow, 2016).

Compulsivity and Habit Learning in AUD

A confluence of preclinical and clinical data strongly supports that the pathogenesis of AUD involves a shift from associative striatum (caudate) to sensorimotor striatum (posterior putamen) in response to alcohol reward responding, likely driven by simultaneous reductions in goal-directed control over actions and increase in habit associations. Learning about response-outcome (R-O) associations has been investigated using instrumental learning paradigms (Balleine and Dickinson, 1998). In instrumental discrimination tasks, which have been developed to distinguish between goal-directed and habit-based learning (de Wit et al., 2007), stimuli (or cues) are congruent, unrelated or incongruent with subsequent outcomes. Whereas performance (or learning) on congruent and control trials can be supported by both the goal-directed (S-R-O) and habitual system (S-O), performance on the incongruent discrimination (in which each pictures functions as a stimulus and an outcome for opposing responses) relies solely on the habit system. The dominance of habitual control over flexible goal-directed responding has traditionally been assessed using revaluation tests, by manipulating outcome value and observing consequent effects on response (Dickinson, 1985). In a typical outcome devaluation methodology (Adams, 1982), after a training phase, the value of the reinforcer of action (O) is typically reduced (extinction phase) affecting internal or external motivational states, and the experimenter assesses if the behavior (R) appropriately updates, in light of this change, measuring the strength of the R-O association. An alcohol addiction model study conducted in rats showed that instrumental alcohol seeking became insensitive to devaluation after 4 weeks of training, mirrored by a shift in control from the dorsomedial striatum to the dorsolateral striatum (Corbit et al., 2012). Another preclinical study using the instrumental learning paradigm showed how alcohol reinforced behavior was less sensitive to devaluation compared to when behavior was reinforced with food, suggesting that alcohol consumption may be particularly susceptible to habit formation (Dickinson et al., 2002). An fMRI clinical study using a cue-reactivity paradigm showed significantly higher cue-induced activation of the dorsal striatum in heavy drinkers compared to social drinkers and higher cue-induced activation of ventral striatum and prefrontal areas in social drinkers compared to heavy drinkers (Vollstädt-Klein et al., 2010). These findings were interpreted as an indirect indication of a shift from ventral to dorsal striatal involvement in the development of AUD, associated with the increasing role of habit–like drug seeking behavior over the course of the disorder. Another clinical study using neuroimaging and learning tasks showed how early abstinent patients presented greater habit formation compared to controls and a progressive shift toward greater goal-directed behaviors with prolonged abstinence (Voon et al., 2015). Another study conducted in AUD patients was the first providing direct behavioral and neurophysiological evidence for an imbalance between goal-directed and habitual control in humans with a substance use disorder (Sjoerds et al., 2013). Subject underwent fMRI during completion of an instrumental learning task characterized by a discrimination learning phase and an outcome-devaluation test phase designed to study the balance between goal-directed and habit learning: patients with AUD compared to healthy controls showed a strong engagement of the neural habit pathway, comprising dorsolateral/posterior parts of the striatum (posterior putamen, caudate tail/body) and a relatively weak engagement of the goal-directed pathway in the vmPFC and dorsomedial/anterior parts of the striatum (caudate head, anterior putamen) during instrumental learning even in the context of AUD-irrelevant stimuli. Moreover, vmPFC activation was negatively associated with AUD duration. Another study investigating the effects of alcohol on devaluation sensitivity for food reward suggested a general effect of alcohol toward habit-formation (Hogarth, 2012).

All these studies give direct (Sjoerds et al., 2013) and indirect (Dickinson et al., 2002; Vollstädt-Klein et al., 2010; Corbit et al., 2012; Hogarth et al., 2012) demonstration of a shift from ventral to dorsal striatal involvement in the development of AUD, associated with an increasing role of habit–like drug seeking behavior over the course of the disorder. Moreover, alcohol consumption has demonstrated to be particularly susceptible to habit formation over other reinforcers (Dickinson et al., 2002) and it has also shown to exert a general direct effect toward habit-formation attenuating goal directed control over action selection (Hogarth, 2012).

Of note, habit circuitry, including the dorsolateral striatum, is also implicated in punishment resistance (Jonkman et al., 2012), characteristic feature of compulsive behaviors, namely behaviors performed in spite of adverse consequences. Indeed, it is important to notice that the construct of “habit” doesn’t completely overlap with the construct of compulsivity, where habitual alcohol use involves behavior that continues despite outcome devaluation, whereas compulsive alcohol use encompasses continued use despite adverse consequences (Hopf and Lesscher, 2014; Marchant et al., 2018). Studies investigating neural correlates of compulsive alcohol seeking using rodent model of aversion-resistant alcohol seeking showed involvement of mPFC, insula and nucleus accumbens (Seif et al., 2013) and hyperactivation of these areas have been found in heavy drinkers as opposed to light drinkers when viewing threat-paired alcohol cues (Grodin et al., 2018).

Next to extensive preclinical evidence suggesting overlap in the striatal regions involved in habitual and compulsive behavior (Everitt and Robbins, 2005; Belin et al., 2009; Pierce and Vanderschuren, 2010; Jonkman et al., 2012; Seif et al., 2013), a recent study conducted on rats for the first time demonstrated the causal importance of the functional recruitment of the anterior dorsolateral striatum (aDLS), a region strongly associated with the consolidation and performance of stimulus-response habits (Yin et al., 2004; Zapata et al., 2010; Corbit et al., 2012), in the switch from controlled to compulsive alcohol use (Giuliano et al., 2019). This study found that individual differences in the reliance of alcohol seeking habits on aDLS dopamine predict and underlie the emergence of compulsive alcohol seeking, providing first evidence that compulsive alcohol seeking stems from an inability to disengage aDLS control over seeking behavior when faced with negative outcomes. This result shows that the maladaptive nature of alcohol seeking in those individuals that become compulsive lays in the rigidity of those aDLS dopamine-dependent habits.

Compulsivity and Cognitive Control in AUD and OCD

From a neurocognitive point of view, AUD is characterized by an imbalance between overwhelming drive toward alcohol consumption and inability to inhibit alcohol consumption, i.e., a disruption in cognitive control over alcohol use (Baler and Volkow, 2006; Jentsch and Pennington, 2014; Koob and Volkow, 2016). The RdoC domain of cognitive control is defined as a modulatory system that supervises all the activities in the service of goal-directed behavior, when prepotent modes of responding are not adequate to meet the demands of current context. Cognitive (or effortful) control encompasses the ability to select relevant stimuli, inhibit responses influenced by distracting elements, select appropriate responses, monitor the outcome of those responses, and adjust behavior as needed in the face of changing situations.

Despite well documented dysfunctions across multiple cognitive functions (Stavro et al., 2013), deficits in response inhibition have been proposed as the most promising marker of cognitive control impairments measured by behavioral tasks in AUD (Wilcox et al., 2014). Response inhibition, a subdomain of cognitive control and defined as the ability to suppress a pre-potent behavior that is inappropriate or no longer required, has been typically assessed using a range of neuropsychological paradigms including those measuring motor response inhibition (further differentiated into action restraint and action cancelation) and cognitive inhibition (interference control) (Bari and Robbins, 2013). It has been suggested that interference control, action restrain and action cancelation represent early, intermediate and late processes of response inhibition (Sebastian et al., 2013). It has also been proposed that inhibitory control is highest in the action cancelation, making of the Stop Signal Task (SST) the more sensitive task to measure inhibitory control, tapping both impulsivity and compulsivity traits (Smith et al., 2014). In a SST, a “stop” signal appears after the onset of a “go” signal on a subset of trials, requiring the participant to interrupt an ongoing motor response to the go signal that has already been triggered, and the primary dependent measure is the stop-signal reaction time (SSRT). Significant deficits in response inhibition have been observed for all tasks in AUD (Karch et al., 2008; Li et al., 2009) especially using the SST (Smith et al., 2014). Typically, task dependent functional connectivity studies have demonstrated that frontostriatal pathways are critical for response inhibition that is weakened over the progression of AUD: individuals with more severe AUD exhibit impaired connectivity between dorsal striatum and anterior cingulate cortex (ACC), mPFC and mOFC (Courtney et al., 2013; Lee et al., 2013). Studies investigating response inhibition have then further demonstrated the importance of dlPFC-dorsal striatum connectivity for behavioral regulation in AUD especially in the late stage of the disorder (Schulte et al., 2012; Courtney et al., 2013; Müller-Oehring et al., 2013). Moreover, impaired response inhibition in AUD has been related to more intense cue-induced alcohol craving (Papachristou et al., 2013). Importantly, impairments in response inhibition may help differentiate AUD subtypes and predict clinical outcomes (Nigg et al., 2006; Saunders et al., 2008; Schuckit et al., 2012).

In the past several years, deficits in response inhibition have also been described and associated to disrupted functional activations in frontostriatal circuits in OCD involving pre-supplementar motor area (pre-SMA), inferior frontal gyrus, ACC, striatum, thalamus (Maltby et al., 2005; Penadés et al., 2007; Roth et al., 2007; Woolley et al., 2008; Page et al., 2009; de Wit S.J. et al., 2012; van Velzen et al., 2014) and proposed as a neurocognitive endophenotype in OCD (Chamberlain et al., 2007; Menzies et al., 2008; de Wit S.J. et al., 2012). Evidence in both OCD and AUD suggests that disruptions of indirect cortico-striatal-thalamic-cortical (CSTC) pathways may mediate both compulsive behaviors and failure in response inhibition (Table 1) (Aron and Poldrack, 2006; Chambers et al., 2009; Aron, 2011). However, the use of behavioral tasks to dissect cognitive control has some limitations, hampering insights into integrated global brain functioning in non-task related circumstances, which contributes to behavioral variability. Techniques such as a priori defined seed-based connectivity help to elucidate how striatal brain areas are integrated into a broader functional network and how that is related to the duration and severity characteristics of alcohol addiction (Schmaal et al., 2013; Müller-Oehring et al., 2015; Galandra et al., 2019). A very recent study coupling resting-state fMRI with an in-depth neuropsychological assessment of the main cognitive domains (Galandra et al., 2019), gives support to the hypotheses that cognitive impairments in AUD (Stavro et al., 2013; Le Berre et al., 2017) are not explained by specific susceptibility of frontal regions to alcohol neurotoxic effects, but rather to dysfunctional connectivity between the cortical (dlPFC and dACC) and subcortical (basal ganglia) nodes of the networks underlying cognitive control on goal-directed behavior. Indeed, while functional connectivity between the basal ganglia and both the dlPFC and dorsal ACC (dACC) nodes was positively related to executive performance in the whole sample, the strength of the very same connections was significantly reduced in patients reflecting the amount of alcohol consumption. The significance of this decrease in connectivity has been investigated by using a graph-theoretical approach, showing that prolonged alcohol dependence is associated with decreased global brain network efficiency and less anterior striatal segregation (Sjoerds et al., 2017). These studies suggest that executive impairment in AUD patients may reflect altered frontostriatal connectivity which underpins top-down modulation of behavior by mediating the switch between automatic and controlled processing. While reward prediction error signal was found intact suggesting proper behavioral options value decoding in the striatum, impaired dlPFC-striatal connectivity has been associated with abnormal processing of financial gains and losses, suggesting that dysfunctional intrinsic connectivity might also underpin defective behavioral learning during task-performance in AUD (Park et al., 2010). Hence, alterations in frontostriatal connectivity underlining impaired learning and decision making may in turn contribute to characteristic poor treatment outcome in AUD. Relapse in AUD has been associated with pronounced atrophy in bilateral OFC, right mPFC and right ACC, impaired connectivity between dorsal striatum and mOFC, and reduced mPFC activation during goal-directed behavior (Grüsser et al., 2004; Beck et al., 2012; Lee et al., 2013; Durazzo and Meyerhoff, 2017; Sebold et al., 2017). On the other hand, other studies have underlined the association between abstinence and normalization in frontostriatal circuits: mOFC-dorsal striatum functional connectivity, impaired in alcohol-dependent patients, showed to increase and normalize in correlation with the duration of abstinence (Lee et al., 2013), and to be associated with a progressive shift toward goal-directed behavior over habitual control (Voon et al., 2015). With progression from short to long-term abstinence, the synchrony within the reward network (subgenual ACC, caudate, nucleus accumbens, and thalamus) and within the executive control network (dlPFC, ACC, and nucleus accumbens) were found to, respectively, decrease and increase progressively (Camchong et al., 2013).

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Fronto-striatal functional connectivity and cognitive control in AUD and OCD.

Meta-analysis of seed-based resting-state fMRI studies in OCD found consistent hypoconnectivity within frontoparietal (peaking into dlPFC) and salience network (peaking supramarginal gyrus), and between salience, frontoparietal and default-mode network (Gürsel et al., 2018); consistent general dysconnectivity was found within default-mode (peaking in dmPFC and ACC) and frontoparietal network (peaking in the striatum), as well as between frontoparietal, default-mode, and salience networks (Gürsel et al., 2018).

These findings match perfectly with the structures comprising the CSTS circuits, namely OFC, ACC, vmPFC, striatum, and thalamus (Pauls et al., 2014).

While the CSTC loop hypothesis has dominated the OCD literature over the last decades (Alexander et al., 1986), as the nature of integration has become apparent for the processing of information in the striatum, such that information is carried in “spirals,” rather than isolated “loops” (Milad and Rauch, 2012) more attention has been paid to potential “hubs” in the neurocircuitry thought to underlie OCD.

The dorsal ACC has been proposed as a connective hub of cognitive control (Shackman et al., 2011) in a ideal position to receive sensory input and act on that information via downstream motor regulation (Figure 1 and Box 2). Proposing cognitive control as a main domain in OCD pathophysiology, dACC dysfunction and consequent aberrant cognitive control signal specification has been proposed to drive the pursuit of tasks that do not accord to long-term goal, underlying the core pathophysiology of OCD (McGovern and Sheth, 2017).

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Schematic diagram of dACC circuitry implicated in modulation of cognitive control. BA, Brodmann’s area; DLPFC, dosolateral prefrontal cortex; IPL, inferior parietal lobule; LC, locus coeruleus; SMA, supplementary motor area; VTA, ventral tegmental area.

Box 2. Cognitive control and the potentiality of targeting the dACC. Within the RDOC construct of cognitive control and, more specifically the subdomain of response inhibition/response selection, the neural network of the anterior cingulo-insular or salience network (aCIN) seems to be critically relevant (Downar et al., 2016). The dACC stands midway along a hierarchy of medial prefrontal regions that guide goal selection, thought selection and action selection based on internal drives as opposed to external cues. The dACC is active when choosing freely between to different cognitive tasks and during voluntary internally cued modulation of emotional state (Downar et al., 2016). The anterior cingulate cortex (ACC) interfaces attention, executive function, drive, affect and motor control. Convergent data from neuroimaging, neuropsychological, genetics, and neurochemical studies have implicated, bilaterally, dysfunction of dorsolateral prefrontal and anterior cingulate cortical structures, which constitute the cortical arm of the frontostriatal network (caudate and putamen being its subcortical counterpart) driving executive function (Solanto, 1984; Castellanos, 1997; Zametkin and Liotta, 1998; Giedd et al., 2001; Seidman et al., 2006; Makris et al., 2007), in cognitive dyscontrol. The ACC (Brodmann’s area 24 or BA 24) is an important regulator of other cortical and subcortical brain regions as well, and its disconnection appears to be consistent with an executive dysfunction. The ACC is connected with other cortical areas ipsilaterally and controlaterally as well as with subcortical structures. The topographic organization of these axonal connections is precise within the subcortical white matter and architectonically arranged within the overlaying cortical layers of the ACC (Figure 1). This mesh of connections converging to and diverging from the neuronal bodies of the ACC constitutes a central networking node within the overall connectional map of the brain crucial for the interface of drive, emotion, cognition and motricity (Paus, 2001) as well as for the modulation of cognitive control (Cohen et al., 2000). Short (U-fibers) and medium range association fibers connect BA 24 with adjacent cortical areas. The more complex long association fibers belong to three discrete fiber bundles, namely the cingulum bundle (CB) (Mufson and Pandya, 1984), the uncinate fascicle (UF) (Petrides and Pandya, 1988) and the extreme capsule (EmC) (Petrides and Pandya, 1988). Through these three fiber pathways the anterior cingulate cortex is connected in a bidirectional fashion as follows (Schmahmann and Pandya, 2007). The CB connects BA 24 with rostral posterior cingulate BA 23, paracingulate BA 32 and orbital frontal BA 14, 12, and 11 as well as prefrontal BA 8 and 6 (Baleydier and Mauguiere, 1980). Via the UF, BA 24 connects with subcallosal BA 25 and basal forebrain regions as well as the amygdala and the perirhinal cortex. The EmC provides connections with the dysgranular insula and the insular proisocortex. Striatal fibers by virtue of the subcallosal fascicle of Muratoff connect BA 24 to the caudate’s head and, through the external capsule, with the putamen and claustrum. Thalamic connections are with anterior, the medial dorsal as well as the midline thalamic nuclei. Other connections are with the parahippocampus, the hippocampus, the zona incerta and the pons as well as with the locus coeruleus and the ventral tegmental area (Paus, 2001). Motor connections are with the supplementary motor area, the motor cortex and the spinal cord (He et al., 1995). Chronic failure of the ACC network may have profound implications contributing to deficit of cognitive control. Convergent evidence from fMRI (Carter et al., 1998, 1999; Botvinick et al., 1999; Cohen et al., 2000) and evoked potential (Gehring and Knight, 2000) experimentation in humans suggest that ACC is associated with monitoring of conflict and modulation of cognitive control as well as modulation of allocation of attention in real time. As illustrated in Figure 1, interactions between the dorsolateral prefrontal (DLPFC), inferior parietal lobule (IPL), orbital frontal cortex (FOC), amygdala and brainstem centers such as the locus coeruleus or the ventral tegmental area, enable the ACC to integrate sensitive information in real time to monitor conflict of competitive cognitive tasks, modulate cognitive control and produce balanced behavior (Yeterian and Pandya, 1985; Cohen et al., 2000). It appears that alterations in the ACC and its associated frontostriatal network (primarily the caudate nucleus and putamen being its subcortical counterpart), which are driving executive function, are critical in executive dysfunction. Therefore, neuromodulatory interventions seem to be an important therapeutic means to modulate ACC function and restore balance in behavior when cognitive control is lost in such conditions as AUD.

Functional connectivity studies have suggested that cognitive and behavioral alterations observed in AUD might reflect functional imbalances within a CTST loop involving the key nodes of the reward (Park et al., 2010; Camchong et al., 2013; Müller-Oehring et al., 2013), salience (Sullivan et al., 2013; Zhu et al., 2017) and executive networks (Weiland et al., 2014; Zhu et al., 2017; Galandra et al., 2019), namely striatum, dlPFC, and dACC. Activity of the cingulate cortex is also emerging as a marker of subsequent alcohol relapse (De Ridder et al., 2011; Zakiniaeiz et al., 2016).

Neuromodulatory Strategies to Target Compulsivity in AUD

State of Art in the Treatment of AUD

A variety of therapeutical approaches are currently available for AUD, such as behavioral treatments, medications and mutual-support groups (NIAAA treatment Navigator page). While, due to the anonymous nature of mutual-support groups, it is difficult to determine their success rates compared with those led by health professionals, cognitive behavioral therapy (CBT), motivational enhancement therapy (MET), marital and family counseling and other brief interventions have demonstrated to be beneficial in AUD. In addition to behavioral treatments, there are currently three FDA approved medications for the post withdrawal maintenance of alcohol abstinence, namely disulfiram, naltrexone, and acamprosate (Kranzler and Soyka, 2018). Disulfiram works negatively reinforcing aversion toward alcohol by inhibiting the enzyme acetaldehyde dehydrogenase and resulting in unpleasant effects when combined with alcohol (Mutschler et al., 2016). Naltrexone is an opiate antagonist that is hypothesized to work buffering the endorphin-mediated rewarding effects of alcohol (Mason et al., 2002). Acamprosate was thought to reduce craving for alcohol by acting as GABA agonist and modulator of glutamate NMDA activity, both disrupted by chronic alcohol use (Mason et al., 2002) and recent evidence suggests that its anti-relapse effects may act via calcium (Spanagel et al., 2014). However, given the broad distribution of neurotransmitter systems in the brain, it is particularly difficult to have a targeted action on neural circuits using pharmacotherapeutics.

Although a review of pharmacotherapy is out of the scope of this paper and mechanisms of action have to be fully elucidated (Koob and Mason, 2016), the available medications for AUD mainly try to reduce the positive reinforcing properties of alcohol or the negative reinforcing aspects of chronic alcohol use by relieving craving (Heilig et al., 2010).

Research has provided evidence that the propensity to engage in drug/alcohol-seeking is determined by the expected value and probability of getting the drug (Hogarth, 2012). Whereas the overall propensity to engage in a goal-directed choice is determined by the expected outcome value of the outcome, in habit learning the capacity of cues to elicit this choice is determined by the expected probability of getting the outcome. Hence, in habit learning, devaluing the outcome does not affect the response. In other words, the actual treatments may have partial efficacy because they may work on tonic (expected alcohol value), but not on cue-elicited phasic DA signaling (Hogarth et al., 2014). In order to improve treatment outcome, new interventions should be developed to directly target inflexible, habitual cue-elicited drug-seeking behavior.

Neuromodulation to Treat Compulsivity in AUD

Following the conceptual framework outlined above, neuromodulatory interventions able to selectively target frontostriatal circuitries may held therapeutic promise for the treatment of compulsive alcohol seeking in AUD. Promisingly, effective neuromodulatory interventions for compulsive symptoms have been associated with normalization in connectivity and restored behavioral control from the striatum to prefrontal cortical regions (Mian et al., 2010; Figee et al., 2013; Dunlop et al., 2016).

Whereas transcranial direct current stimulation (tDCS) needs more investigation for the treatment of compulsivity given the lack of sham controlled studies, repetitive Transcranial Magnetic Stimulation (rTMS) (Box 2) as non-invasive and relatively site specific, has been the most studied neuromodulation technique in the treatment of compulsive behavior in both AUD and OCD (Campbell et al., 2018; Shivakumar et al., 2019). As the time of writing, there have been 10 studies probing rTMS as a tool to change alcohol consumption and explore the associated neurocircuit changes in AUD (Table 2). Different outcome measures were used to assess the impact of different stimulation protocols on relapse, wanting of the drug (“craving”), cognitive control and associated funtional connectivity (Table 2). Traditional rTMS studies targeting the right dlPFC had some success at reducing alcohol craving (Mishra et al., 2010, 2015; Herremans et al., 2015) in accordance with the results obtained by a sham controlled tDCS study applied to the same area (Boggio et al., 2008). To date, the majority of interventional rTMS studies in OCD have been conducted stimulating the dlPFC as well, with mixed results. On the other hand, the most recent meta-analysis to assess whether the effectiveness of rTMS in improving OCD symptoms is moderated by its application over different cortical targets revealed that rTMS applied over the SMA yields greater improvements than rTMS applied over the dlPFC or OFC (Rehn et al., 2018). This therapeutic effect has been attributed to the normalization of hyperactive orbito-fronto-striatal circuits induced by low frequency-rTMS (LF-rTMS) (Mantovani et al., 2010). The SMA plays a central role in motor planning and response-inhibition (de Wit S.J. et al., 2012) and has extensive connections to regions involved in cognitive and emotional processes. Studies suggest that hyperactivity in this area may be associated with deficient inhibitory control over repetitive behaviors (Oliveri et al., 2003), thus making it an attractive target for the inhibitory effects of LF-rTMS. The efficacy of neurosurgical treatments for the treatment of resistant OCD patients suggests other promising deep targets for treating compulsivity. Stereotactic lesions in the dACC (cingulotomy) have shown long term efficay in the treatment of refractory OCD with average full response of 41% (Sheth et al., 2013; Brown et al., 2016). Based on good results with stereotactic ablation, Deep Brain Stimulation (DBS) has been explored and DBS of anterior and ventral capsule, ventral striatum or subthalamic nucleus (STN) has shown a global percentage of responders of 60.0% with long-term efficacy (Bais et al., 2014). Stimulation of STN has been reported to decrease OFC and mPFC metabolism, as well as ACC activity, while stimulation of striatal areas has been associated with decrease in OFC and subgenual ACC activity (Alonso et al., 2015).

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Studies using rTMS as a tool to change alcohol consumption.

Despite the limited number of cases examining DBS in patients with AUD, DBS of the nucleus accumbens has shown long term treatment benefit, reducing alcohol craving for up to 8 years associated with modulation of anterior mid-cingulate cortex functioning and cognitive control (Kuhn et al., 2011; Müller et al., 2016). In light of the striking results obtained with DBS of striatal areas and the evidence of aDLS dopamine dependent alcohol seeking (Giuliano et al., 2019), the recent development of new coils designed to target deep cerebral areas with rTMS, may be particularly interesting, avoiding the complications and adverse events related to invasive techniques and possibly gaining some of their therapeutic advantages. Indeed, there is already evidence that cortical stimulation with deep TMS (dTMS) may modulate sub-cortical striatal activity in AUD (Addolorato et al., 2017). While rTMS proved to indirectly modulate the insula and the ACC by stimulating the dlPFC (Nahas et al., 2001) or the frontal pole (Hanlon et al., 2017), H coil design series now promise to target these deeper structures directly, with greater effectiveness. A recent big randomized study investigated the efficacy of dTMS in OCD (Carmi et al., 2018): after provocation of symptoms, 99 treatment resistant OCD patients were treated with either high frequency (HF) or sham dTMS over the mPFC and ACC for 6 weeks and 29 sessions. Despite the lack of neuroimaging, this study conveys relevant results in terms of response rate (38,1%) and maintenance (4 weeks follow up) and to date it is the largest TMS controlled study ever conducted in OCD which led to FDA approval to market the dTMS system for treatment of OCD.

In addition, the mPFC and ACC have been associated with initiation of compulsive behavior in OCD (Rauch et al., 1994; Adler et al., 2000; Viol et al., 2019), they have also been individuated as core regions activating during alcohol cue processing (Heinz et al., 2009; Zakiniaeiz et al., 2016;Grodin et al., 2018; Hanlon et al., 2018) and may be considered as promising new targets for dTMS in AUD (Ceccanti et al., 2015; Herremans et al., 2016).

Summary and Perspective

Findings suggest that later stages of AUD may be better conceptualized as a disorder characterized by compulsive features, namely overreliance on stimulus-driven habits at the expense of flexible, goal-directed action, leading to frequent and persistent substance use despite serious negative consequences (Belin and Everitt, 2008; Fontenelle et al., 2011). In other words, although alcohol seeking is initially a goal-directed behavior consolidated by operant conditioning, in which alcohol is sought for its rewarding effect, it becomes ultimately a maladaptive optimized response elicited by alcohol-associated stimuli, characterized by over-active striatal habit forming circuitries coupled with lack of sufficient inhibitory control (Chamberlain et al., 2005; Vanderschuren and Everitt, 2005; Menzies et al., 2008; Smith et al., 2014). As seen in OCD, there is a lack of extinction of obsessions (Lovibond et al., 2009), in AUD there is disruption in extinction learning of ethanol-seeking behavior with persistency of the behavior overtime despite adverse consequences (Gass et al., 2017; Grodin et al., 2018). Development of compulsivity may thus explain part of the treatment resistance and relapse in AUD (Sinha et al., 2011; Courtney et al., 2013; Lee et al., 2013). Despite the residual presence of a reward component in driving the behavior, craving for alcohol in the late stages of the disorder is comparable to a compulsion, in phenomenology – in the emergence of urges in response to alcohol-related cues and the inability to resist them- and in neurocircuitry.

The field of cognitive neuroscience can provide measures that are a reflection of the underlying neurobiology, and may eventually inform treatment selection. Disruption in inhibitory control has been proposed as endophenotype and pathophysiologic factor in the development of OCD and AUD (Chamberlain et al., 2007; Yücel and Lubman, 2007; Menzies et al., 2008; de Wit S.J. et al., 2012; Jentsch and Pennington, 2014; Leeman et al., 2014; Wilcox et al., 2014; Gillan et al., 2016). In particular, impaired response inhibition is associated with severity, duration of illness, impaired goal-directed planning and reduced frontostriatal connectivity in AUD. These findings suggest that in AUD response inhibition may be used as a useful marker of cognitive impairment, disruption in connectivity between frontal (dlPFC and dACC) and striatal nodes, as well as a potential treatment target. Recent studies have demonstrated how brain stimulation techniques may affect response inhibition, suggesting how non-invasive neuromulation may be particularly promising in order to develop treatments whose effect in drinking outcome are mediated by improvement in cognitive control (Nakamura-Palacios et al., 2012; Nardone et al., 2012). Future work is still needed to clearly identify the most reliable and valid markers for the deficits, and the degree to which deficits or changes in cognitive control moderate or mediate response to particular treatments in AUD. Recent findings point to the hypotheses that cognitive impairments in AUD are related to dysfunctional connectivity between cortical (dlPFC and dACC) and subcortical nodes (basal ganglia). Thus, studies aimed to characterize instrinsic resting state connectivity and hubs in the reward, salience and executive networks in AUD patients may be particularly useful for the development of new treatment strategies.

Treatment with rTMS is still in its infancy but is a promising tool for developing effective and viable circuit-specific treatment strategies in AUD. The current evidence suggests that targeting inflexible seeking responses may offer a therapeutic strategy to promote abstinence and prevent relapse in AUD. The results of the available studies, in accordance with recent network models of addiction (Dunlop et al., 2017) seems to point to the potential of mPFC and dACC (Box 3 and Figure 1) as TMS targets for the treatment of compulsive alcohol seeking in AUD. The available findings suggest that those areas may be pivotal in order to enhance cortico-striatal-thalamic connectivity and capacity for response selection/inhibition, with the potential to act on the two core aspects of incentive salience and habit learning (Box 3 and Figure 1). Future efforts to improve outcome for rTMS in AUD will likely benefit from rigorous manipulation of the cognitive state during neuromodulation.

Box 3. Repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation as a tool for personalized psychiatry. In rTMS, single TMS pulses are delivered at various frequency (typically 1–20 Hz) in either a fixed or bursting pattern from 600 to 4000 pulses per session. There is general agreement that low frequency (LF) stimulation (e.g., 1 Hz) causes long term depression of cortical excitability, whereas higher frequency (HF) stimulation (e.g., 10–20 HZ) induces long term potentiation of cortical excitability. These effects can be achieved through teta burst stimulation (TBS). With continuous TBS (cTBS), three pulse bursts at 50 Hz are applied at a frequency of 5 Hz. In most protocols, this cycle continues until 600 pulses have been delivered (20 s). For intermittent TBS (iTBS), bursts are applied at the same rate (five groups of three pulse bursts per second) for 2 s, followed by an 8-s pause. In most protocols this 10-s cycle occurs until 600 pulses have been delivered (190 s). When performed over the primary motor cortex, 600 pulses of cTBS inhibit cortical excitability, whereas 600 pulses of iTBS amplify cortical excitability (Huang et al., 2005). The advantage of TBS protocols is that effect sizes comparable to fixed frequency protocols can be achieved significantly faster (1–2 min versus 20–30 min). The spatial resolution and penetration depth of a TMS pulse depend on the coil. Typical figure-of-eight coil affects approximately 10 cm2 of cortical surface, while H shape coil design approximately 100 cm2. Most flat coil designs have penetration depths from 1 to 2 cm, whereas the H-coil designs has higer depths of 2–3 cm (Deng et al., 2014). The introduction of H-coils has offered the opportunity of non-invasively modulate activity in brain targets that previously were accessible only by neurosurgial procedures. The combined use of neuronavigation and neuroimaging with rTMS, makes of this latter a feasible therapeutic tool for personalized psychiatry.

Conclusion

Despite effective treatments that are available in AUD, there remain high rates of relapse and poor long term functioning even in those patients who get therapy. We describe evidence supporting the role of compulsivity (persistent use despite adverse consequences), in the development of AUD resistant to approved interventions. Following a transdiagnostical approach and using OCD as comparison, we highlight features of compulsivity in AUD, showing how phenotypical similarities between the two disorders involve overlapping pathophysiological mechanisms. After identifying compulsivity as a promising and neglected target domain for new treatment approaches in AUD, we discuss neuromodulatory interventions in order to improve recovery in AUD.

For disorders, such as AUD, whose development relies on learning, rTMS gives the unique opportunity to non-invasively act on target neurocircuits with the best space-time resolution. The possibility to couple rTMS with specific tasks designed to activate specific associated circuits allows unique opportunity for personalized therapy. The current evidence seems to point to the potential of mPFC and dACC as TMS targets for the treatment of compulsive alcohol seeking in AUD. Further longitudinal studies combining neuromodulation with neuroimaging and neurocognitive measures are needed to shed light on additional mechanisms underlying rTMS effects in AUD patients, and to test the hypotheses that targeting compulsivity, the habit system and inhibitory control, normalizing fronto-striatal function may convey treatment benefit in AUD.

In conclusion, the conceptualization of AUD within a dimensional and learning framework opens new opportunities for research and clinical management:

• It underlines the importance of reducing the duration of untreated illness in order to prevent development of compulsivity, associated to resistance to treatment and relapse.

• It guides the implementation of a stepped care approach, that considers different diagnostic and treatment strategies in relation to the stages of AUD, underlying the importance of assessing and treating compulsivity.

• It supports recovery as a realistic goal, based on the opportunity of modulating neuroplasticity.

• It supports the implementation of studies aimed to investigate neuromodulation as a promising treatment strategy.

Author Contributions

EB and EH contributed to the conceptualization of the manuscript. EB wrote the manuscript. NM wrote Box 3. All authors contributed to manuscript revisions, and read and approved the submitted version.

Conflict of Interest Statement

EH received a research grant to study dTMS in OCD from BrainsWay.

The remaining authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

References

Adams, C. D. (1982). Variations in the sensitivity of instrumental responding to reinforcer devaluation. Q. J. Exp. Psychol. 34B, 77–98. doi: 10.1080/14640748208400878

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Addolorato, G., Antonelli, M., Cocciolillo, F., Vassallo, G. A., Tarli, C., Sestito, L., et al. (2017). Deep transcranial magnetic stimulation of the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex in alcohol use disorder patients: effects on dopamine transporter availability and alcohol intake. Eur. Neuropsychopharmacol. 27, 450–461. doi: 10.1016/j.euroneuro.2017.03.008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Adler, C. M., McDonough-Ryan, P., Sax, K. W., Holland, S. K., Arndt, S., and Strakowski, S. M. (2000). fMRIb of neuronal activation with symptom provocation in unmedicated patients with obsessive compulsive disorder. J. Psychiatr. Res. 34, 317–324. doi: 10.1016/S0022-3956(00)00022-4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Alexander, G. E., DeLong, M. R., and Strick, P. L. (1986). Parallel organization of functionally segregated circuits linking basal ganglia and cortex. Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 9, 357–381. doi: 10.1146/annurev.ne.09.030186.002041

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Alonso, P., Cuadras, D., Gabriëls, L., Denys, D., Goodman, W., Greenberg, B. D., et al. (2015). Deep brain stimulation for obsessive-compulsive disorder: a meta-analysis of treatment outcome and predictors of response. PLoS One 10:e0133591. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0133591

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

American Psychiatric Association (2013). Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (DSM-5®). Arlington, TX: American Psychiatric Pub. doi: 10.1176/appi.books.9780890425596

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Aron, A. R. (2011). From reactive to proactive and selective control: developing a richer model for stopping inappropriate responses. Biol. Psychiatry 69, e55–e68. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2010.07.024

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Aron, A. R., and Poldrack, R. A. (2006). Cortical and subcortical contributions to stop signal response inhibition: role of the subthalamic nucleus. J. Neurosci. 26, 2424–2433. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4682-05.2006

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bais, M., Figee, M., and Denys, D. (2014). Neuromodulation in obsessive-compulsive disorder. Psychiatr. Clin. North Am. 37, 393–413. doi: 10.1016/j.psc.2014.06.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Baler, R. D., and Volkow, N. D. (2006). Drug addiction: the neurobiology of disrupted self-control. Trends Mol. Med. 12, 559–566. doi: 10.1016/j.molmed.2006.10.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Baleydier, C., and Mauguiere, F. (1980). The duality of the cingulate gyrus in monkey. Neuroanatomical study and functional hypothesis. Brain 103, 525–554. doi: 10.1093/brain/103.3.525

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Balleine, B. W., and Dickinson, A. (1998). Goal-directed instrumental action: contingency and incentive learning and their cortical substrates. Neuropharmacology 37, 407–419. doi: 10.1016/S0028-3908(98)00033-1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Balleine, B. W., Liljeholm, M., and Ostlund, S. B. (2009). The integrative function of the basal ganglia in instrumental conditioning. Behav. Brain Res. 199, 43–52. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2008.10.034

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Balleine, B. W., and O’Doherty, J. P. (2010). Human and rodent homologies in action control:corticostriatal determinants of goal-directed and habitual action. Neuropsychopharmacology 35, 48–69. doi: 10.1038/npp.2009.131

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Banca, P., Voon, V., Vestergaard, M. D., Philipiak, G., Almeida, I., Pocinho, F., et al. (2015). Imbalance in habitual versus goal directed neural systems during symptom provocation in obsessive-compulsive disorder. Brain 138(Pt 3), 798–811. doi: 10.1093/brain/awu379

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bari, A., and Robbins, T. W. (2013). Inhibition and impulsivity: behavioral and neural basis of response control. Prog. Neurobiol. 108, 44–79. doi: 10.1016/j.pneurobio.2013.06.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Beck, A., Wüstenberg, T., Genauck, A., Wrase, J., Schlagenhauf, F., Smolka, M. N., et al. (2012). Effect of brain structure, brain function, and brain connectivity on relapse in alcohol-dependent patients. Arch. Gen. Psychiatry 69, 842–852. doi: 10.1001/archgenpsychiatry.2011.2026

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Belin, D., and Everitt, B. J. (2008). Cocaine seeking habits depend upon dopamine-dependent serial connectivity linking the ventral with the dorsal striatum. Neuron 57, 432–441. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2007.12.019

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Belin, D., Jonkman, S., Dickinson, A., Robbins, T. W., and Everitt, B. J. (2009). Parallel and interactive learning processes within the basal ganglia: relevance for the understanding of addiction. Behav. Brain Res. 199, 89–102. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2008.09.027

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Berke, J. D., and Hyman, S. E. (2000). Addiction, dopamine, and the molecular mechanisms of memory. Neuron 25, 515–532. doi: 10.1016/S0896-6273(00)81056-9

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Berridge, K. C., and Robinson, T. E. (2016). Liking, wanting, and the incentive-sensitization theory of addiction. Am. Psychol. 71, 670–679. doi: 10.1037/amp0000059

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Boggio, P. S., Sultani, N., Fecteau, S., Merabet, L., Mecca, T., Pascual-Leone, A., et al. (2008). Prefrontal cortex modulation using transcranial DC stimulation reduces alcohol craving: a double-blind, sham-controlled study. Drug Alcohol Depend. 92, 55–60. doi: 10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2007.06.011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Botvinick, M., Nystrom, L. E., Fissell, K., Carter, C. S., and Cohen, J. D. (1999). Conflict monitoring versus selection-for-action in anterior cingulate cortex. Nature 402, 179–181. doi: 10.1038/46035

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Brown, L. T., Mikell, C. B., Youngerman, B. E., Zhang, Y., McKhann, G. M. II, and Sheth, S. A. (2016). Dorsal anterior cingulotomy and anterior capsulotomy for severe, refractory obsessive-compulsive disorder: a systematic review of observational studies. J. Neurosurg. 124, 77–89. doi: 10.3171/2015.1.JNS14681

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Burguière, E., Monteiro, P., Mallet, L., Feng, G., and Graybiel, A. M. (2015). Striatal circuits, habits, and implications for obsessive-compulsive disorder. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 30, 59–65. doi: 10.1016/j.conb.2014.08.008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Camchong, J., Stenger, A., and Fein, G. (2013). Resting-state synchrony during early alcohol abstinence can predict subsequent relapse. Cereb. Cortex 23, 2086–2099. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhs190

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Campbell, E. J., Lawrence, A. J., and Perry, C. J. (2018). New steps for treating alcohol use disorder. Psychopharmacology 235, 1759–1773. doi: 10.1007/s00213-018-4887-7

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Carmi, L., Alyagon, U., Barnea-Ygael, N., Zohar, J., Dar, R., and Zangen, A. (2018). Clinical and electrophysiological outcomes of deep TMS over the medial prefrontal and anterior cingulate cortices in OCD patients. Brain Stimul. 11, 158–165. doi: 10.1016/j.brs.2017.09.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Carter, C. S., Botvinick, M. M., and Cohen, J. D. (1999). The contribution of the anterior cingulate cortex to executive processes in cognition. Rev. Neurosci. 10, 49–57. doi: 10.1515/REVNEURO.1999.10.1.49

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Carter, C. S., Braver, T. S., Barch, D. M., Botvinick, M. M., Noll, D., and Cohen, J. D. (1998). Anterior cingulate cortex, error detection, and the online monitoring of performance. Science 280, 747–749. doi: 10.1126/science.280.5364.747

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Castellanos, F. X. (1997). Toward a pathophysiology of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Clin. Pediatr. 36, 381–393. doi: 10.1177/000992289703600702

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ceccanti, M., Inghilleri, M., Attilia, M. L., Raccah, R., Fiore, M., Zangen, A., et al. (2015). Deep TMS on alcoholics: effects on cortisolemia and dopamine pathway modulation. A pilot study. Can. J. Physiol. Pharmacol. 93, 283–290. doi: 10.1139/cjpp-2014-0188

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chamberlain, S. R., Blackwell, A. D., Fineberg, N. A., Robbins, T. W., and Sahakian, B. J. (2005). The neuropsychology of obsessive compulsive disorder: the importance of failures in cognitive and behavioural inhibition as candidate endophenotypic markers. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 29, 399–419. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2004.11.006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chamberlain, S. R., Fineberg, N. A., Menzies, L. A., Blackwell, A. D., Bullmore, E. T., Robbins, T. W., et al. (2007). Impaired cognitive flexibility and motor inhibition in unaffected first-degree relatives of patients with obsessive-compulsive disorder. Am. J. Psychiatry 164, 335–338. doi: 10.1176/ajp.2007.164.2.335

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chambers, C. D., Garavan, H., and Bellgrove, M. A. (2009). Insights into the neural basis of response inhibition from cognitive and clinical neuroscience. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 33, 631–646. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2008.08.016

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chanraud, S., Pitel, A. L., Pfefferbaum, A., and Sullivan, E. V. (2011). Disruption of functional connectivity of the default-mode network in alcoholism. Cereb. Cortex 21, 2272–2281. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhq297

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cohen, J. D., Botvinick, M., and Carter, C. S. (2000). Anterior cingulate and prefrontal cortex: who’s in control? Nat. Neurosci. 3, 421–423. doi: 10.1038/74783

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Corbit, L. H., Nie, H., and Janak, P. H. (2012). Habitual alcohol seeking: time course and the contribution of subregions of the dorsal striatum. Biol. Psychiatry 72, 389–395. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2012.02.024

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Courtney, K. E., Ghahremani, D. G., and Ray, L. A. (2013). Fronto-striatal functional connectivity during response inhibition in alcohol dependence. Addict. Biol. 18, 593–604. doi: 10.1111/adb.12013

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

De Ridder, D., Vanneste, S., Kovacs, S., Sunaert, S., and Dom, G. (2011). Transient alcohol craving suppression by rTMS of dorsal anterior cingulate: an fMRI and LORETA EEG study. Neurosci. Lett. 496, 5–10. doi: 10.1016/j.neulet.2011.03.074

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

de Wit, S., Niry, D., Wariyar, R., Aitken, M. R., and Dickinson, A. (2007). Stimulus-outcome interactions during instrumental discrimination learning by rats and humans. J. Exp. Psychol. Anim. Behav. Process. 33, 1–11. doi: 10.1037/0097-7403.33.1.1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

de Wit, S., Watson, P., Harsay, H. A., Cohen, M. X., van de Vijver, I., and Ridderinkhof, K. R. (2012). Corticostriatal connectivity underlies individual differences in the balance between habitual and goal-directed action control. J. Neurosci. 32, 12066–12075. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1088-12.2012

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

de Wit, S. J., de Vries, F. E., van der Werf, Y. D., Cath, D. C., Heslenfeld, D. J., Veltman, E. M., et al. (2012). Presupplementary motor area hyperactivity during response inhibition: a candidate endophenotype of obsessive-compulsive disorder. Am. J. Psychiatry 169, 1100–1108. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2012.12010073

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Deng, Z. D., Lisanby, S. H., and Peterchev, A. V. (2014). Coil design considerations for deeptranscranial magnetic stimulation. Clin. Neurophysiol. 125, 1202–1212. doi: 10.1016/j.clinph.2013.11.038

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Di Chiara, G. (1999). Drug addiction as dopamine-dependent associative learning disorder. Eur. J. Pharmacol. 375, 13–30. doi: 10.1016/S0014-2999(99)00372-6

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dickinson, A. (1985). Actions and habits: the development of behavioural autonomy. Philos. Trans. R. Soc B. 308, 67–78. doi: 10.1098/rstb.1985.0010

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dickinson, A., Wood, N., and Smith, J. W. (2002). Alcohol seeking by rats: action or habit? Q. J. Exp. Psychol. B. 55, 331–348. doi: 10.1080/0272499024400016

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dolan, R. J., and Dayan, P. (2013). Goals and habits in the brain. Neuron 80, 312–325. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2013.09.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Downar, J., Blumberger, D. M., and Daskalakis, Z. J. (2016). The neural crossroads of psychiatric illness: an emerging target for brain stimulation. Trends Cogn. Sci. 20, 107–120. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2015.10.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dunlop, K., Hanlon, C. A., and Downar, J. (2017). Noninvasive brain stimulation treatments for addiction and major depression. Ann. N Y Acad. Sci. 1394, 31–54. doi: 10.1111/nyas.12985

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dunlop, K., Woodside, B., Olmsted, M., Colton, P., Giacobbe, P., and Downar, J. (2016). Reductions in cortico-striatal hyperconnectivity accompany successful treatment of obsessive-compulsive disorder with dorsomedial prefrontal rTMS. Neuropsychopharmacology 41, 1395–1403. doi: 10.1038/npp.2015.292

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Durazzo, T. C., and Meyerhoff, D. J. (2017). Psychiatric, demographic, and brain morphological predictors of relapse after treatment for an alcohol use disorder. Alcohol. Clin. Exp. Res. 41, 107–116. doi: 10.1111/acer.13267

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Everitt, B. J., Dickinson, A., and Robbins, T. W. (2001). The neuropsychological basis of addictive behaviour. Brain Res. Brain Res. Rev. 36, 129–138. doi: 10.1016/S0165-0173(01)00088-1

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Everitt, B. J., and Robbins, T. W. (2005). Neural systems of reinforcement for drug addiction: from actions to habits to compulsion. Nat. Neurosci. 8, 1481–1489. doi: 10.1038/nn1579

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Everitt, B. J., and Robbins, T. W. (2013). From the ventral to the dorsal striatum: devolving views of their roles in drug addiction. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 37, 1946–1954. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2013.02.010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Figee, M., Wielaard, I., Mazaheri, A., and Denys, D. (2013). Neurosurgical targets for compulsivity: what can we learn from acquired brain lesions? Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 37, 328–339. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2013.01.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fineberg, N. A., Menchon, J. M., Zohar, J., and Veltman, D. J. (2016). Compulsivity-a new trans-diagnostic research domain for the roadmap for mental health research in Europe (ROAMER) and research domain criteria (RDoC) initiatives. Eur. Neuropsychopharmacol. 26, 797–799. doi: 10.1016/j.euroneuro.2016.04.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fontenelle, L. F., Oostermeijer, S., Harrison, B. J., Pantelis, C., and Yücel, M. (2011). Obsessive-compulsive disorder, impulse control disorders and drug addiction: common features and potential treatments. Drugs 71, 827–840. doi: 10.2165/11591790-000000000-00000

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Galandra, C., Basso, G., Manera, M., Crespi, C., Giorgi, I., Vittadini, G., et al. (2019). Abnormal fronto-striatal intrinsic connectivity reflects executive dysfunction in alcohol use disorders. Cortex 115, 27–42. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2019.01.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gass, J. T., McGonigal, J. T., and Chandler, L. J. (2017). Deficits in the extinction of ethanol-seeking behavior following chronic intermittent ethanol exposure are attenuated with positive allosteric modulation of mGlu5. Neuropharmacology 113(Pt A), 198–205. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropharm.2016.10.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

GBD 2016 Alcohol Collaborators (2018). Alcohol use and burden for 195 countries and territories, 1990-2016: a systematic analysis for the Global Burden of Disease Study. Lancet 392, 1015–1035.

Google Scholar

Gehring, W. J., and Knight, R. T. (2000). Prefrontal-cingulate interactions in action monitoring. Nat. Neurosci. 3, 516–520. doi: 10.1038/74899

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gerdeman, G. L., Partridge, J. G., Lupica, C. R., and Lovinger, D. M. (2003). It could be habit forming: drugs of abuse and striatal synaptic plasticity. Trends Neurosci. 26, 184–192. doi: 10.1016/S0166-2236(03)00065-1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Giedd, J. N., Blumenthal, J., Molloy, E., and Castellanos, F. X. (2001). Brain imaging of attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci. 931, 33–49. doi: 10.1111/j.1749-6632.2001.tb05772.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gillan, C. M., Morein-Zamir, S., Urcelay, G. P., Sule, A., Voon, V., Apergis-Schoute, A. M., et al. (2014). Enhanced avoidance habits in obsessive-compulsive disorder. Biol. Psychiatry 75, 631–638. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2013.02.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gillan, C. M., Papmeyer, M., Morein-Zamir, S., Sahakian, B. J., Fineberg, N. A., Robbins, T. W., et al. (2011). Disruption in the balance between goal-directed behavior and habit learning in obsessive-compulsive disorder. Am. J. Psychiatry 168, 718–726. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2011.10071062

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gillan, C. M., and Robbins, T. W. (2014). Goal-directed learning and obsessive-compulsive disorder. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. B Biol. Sci. 369:20130475. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2013.0475

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gillan, C. M., Robbins, T. W., Sahakian, B. J., van den Heuvel, O. A., and van Wingen, G. (2016). The role of habit in compulsivity. Eur. Neuropsychopharmacol. 26, 828–840. doi: 10.1016/j.euroneuro.2015.12.033

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Giuliano, C., Belin, D., and Everitt, B. J. (2019). Compulsive alcohol seeking results from a failure to disengage dorsolateral striatal control over behavior. J. Neurosci. 39, 1744–1754. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2615-18.2018

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Grant, B. F., Saha, T. D., Ruan, W. J., Goldstein, R. B., Chou, S. P., Jung, J., et al. (2016). Epidemiology of DSM-5 drug use disorder: results from the national epidemiologic survey on alcohol and related conditions-III. JAMA Psychiatry 73, 39–47. doi: 10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2015.2132

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Grant, J. E., and Kim, S. W. (2014). Brain circuitry of compulsivity and impulsivity. CNS Spectr. 19, 21–27. doi: 10.1017/S109285291300028X

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Graybiel, A. M., and Rauch, S. L. (2000). Toward a neurobiology of obsessive-compulsive disorder. Neuron 28, 343–347. doi: 10.1016/S0896-6273(00)00113-6

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Grodin, E. N., Sussman, L., Sundby, K., Brennan, G. M., Diazgranados, N., Heilig, M., et al. (2018). Neural correlates of compulsive alcohol seeking in heavy drinkers. Biol. Psychiatry Cogn. Neurosci. Neuroimaging 3, 1022–1031. doi: 10.1016/j.bpsc.2018.06.009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Grüsser, S. M., Wrase, J., Klein, S., Hermann, D., Smolka, M. N., Ruf, M., et al. (2004). Cue-induced activation of the striatum and medial prefrontal cortex is associated with subsequent relapse in abstinent alcoholics. Psychopharmacology 175, 296–302. doi: 10.1007/s00213-004-1828-4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gürsel, D. A., Avram, M., Sorg, C., Brandl, F., and Koch, K. (2018). Frontoparietal areas linkimpairments of large-scale intrinsic brain networks with aberrant fronto-striatal interactions in OCD: a meta-analysis of resting-state functional connectivity. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 87, 151–160. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2018.01.016

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hanlon, C. A., Dowdle, L. T., Correia, B., Mithoefer, O., Kearney-Ramos, T., Lench, D., et al. (2017). Left frontal pole theta burst stimulationdecreases orbitofrontal and insula activity in cocaine users and alcohol users. Drug Alcohol Depend. 178, 310–317. doi: 10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2017.03.039

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hanlon, C. A., Dowdle, L. T., and Henderson, J. S. (2018). Modulating neural circuits with transcranial magnetic stimulation: implications for addiction treatment development. Pharmacol. Rev. 70, 661–683. doi: 10.1124/pr.116.013649

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Harrison, B. J., Pujol, J., Cardoner, N., Deus, J., Alonso, P., López-Solà, M., et al. (2013). Brain corticostriatal systems and the major clinical symptom dimensions of obsessive-compulsive disorder. Biol. Psychiatry 73, 321–328. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2012.10.006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

He, S. Q., Dum, R. P., and Strick, P. L. (1995). Topographic organization of corticospinal projections from the frontal lobe: motor areas on the medial surface of the hemisphere. J. Neurosci. 15(5 Pt 1), 3284–3306. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.15-05-03284.1995

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Heilig, M., Thorsell, A., Sommer, W. H., Hansson, A. C., Ramchandani, V. A., George, D. T., et al. (2010). Translating the neuroscience of alcoholism into clinical treatments: from blocking the buzz to curing the blues. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 35, 334–344. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2009.11.018

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Heinz, A., Beck, A., Grüsser, S. M., Grace, A. A., and Wrase, J. (2009). Identifying the neural circuitry of alcohol craving and relapse vulnerability. Addict. Biol. 14, 108–118. doi: 10.1111/j.1369-1600.2008.00136.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Herremans, S. C., De Raedt, R., Van Schuerbeek, P., Marinazzo, D., Matthys, F., De Mey, J., et al. (2016). Accelerated HF-rTMS protocol has a rate-dependent effect on dACC activation in alcohol-dependent patients: an open-label feasibility study. Alcohol. Clin. Exp. Res. 40, 196–205. doi: 10.1111/acer.12937

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Herremans, S. C., Van Schuerbeek, P., De Raedt, R., Matthys, F., Buyl, R., De Mey, J., et al. (2015). The impact of accelerated right prefrontal high-frequency repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation (rTMS) on cue-reactivity: an fMRI study on craving in recently detoxified alcohol-dependent patients. PLoS One 10:e0136182. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0136182

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Herremans, S. C., Vanderhasselt, M. A., De Raedt, R., and Baeken, C. (2013). Reduced intra-individual reaction time variability during a Go-NoGo task in detoxified alcohol-dependent patients after one right-sided dorsolateral prefrontal HF-rTMS session. Alcohol Alcohol. 48, 552–557. doi: 10.1093/alcalc/agt054

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Heyman, G. M. (2013). Addiction and choice: theory and new data. Front. Psychiatry 4:31. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2013.00031

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hogarth, L. (2012). Goal-directed and transfer-cue-elicited drug-seeking are dissociated by pharmacotherapy: evidence for independent additive controllers. J. Exp. Psychol. Anim. Behav. Process. 38, 266–278. doi: 10.1037/a0028914

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hogarth, L., Attwood, A. S., Bate, H. A., and Munafò, M. R. (2012). Acute alcohol impairs human goal-directed action. Biol. Psychol. 90, 154–160. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2012.02.016

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hogarth, L., Field, M., and Rose, A. K. (2013). Phasic transition from goal-directed to habitual control over drug-seeking produced by conflicting reinforcer expectancy. Addict. Biol. 18, 88–97. doi: 10.1111/adb.12009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hogarth, L., Retzler, C., Munafò, M. R., Tran, D. M., Troisi, J. R. II, Rose, A. K., et al. (2014). Extinction of cue-evoked drug-seeking relies on degrading hierarchicalinstrumental expectancies. Behav. Res. Ther. 59, 61–70. doi: 10.1016/j.brat.2014.06.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hollander, E. (ed.) (1993). Obsessive-Compulsive Related Disorders. Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Pub.

Google Scholar

Hollander, E., and Wong, C. M. (1995). Obsessive-compulsive spectrum disorders. J. Clin. Psychiatry 56(Suppl. 4), 3–6; discussion 53–55.

Google Scholar

Hopf, F. W., and Lesscher, H. M. (2014). Rodent models for compulsive alcohol intake. Alcohol 48, 253–264. doi: 10.1016/j.alcohol.2014.03.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Höppner, J., Broese, T., Wendler, L., Berger, C., and Thome, J. (2011). Repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation (rTMS) for treatment of alcohol dependence. World J. Biol. Psychiatry 12(Suppl. 1), 57–62. doi: 10.3109/15622975.2011.598383

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Huang, Y. Z., Edwards, M. J., Rounis, E., Bhatia, K. P., and Rothwell, J. C. (2005). Theta burststimulation of the human motor cortex. Neuron 45, 201–206. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2004.12.033

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jellinek, E. M. (1960). Alcoholism, a genus and some of its species. Can. Med. Assoc. J. 83, 1341–1345.

Google Scholar

Jentsch, J. D., and Pennington, Z. T. (2014). Reward, interrupted: inhibitory control and its relevance to addictions. Neuropharmacology 76(Pt B), 479–486. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropharm.2013.05.022

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jentsch, J. D., and Taylor, J. R. (1999). Impulsivity resulting from frontostriatal dysfunction in drug abuse: implications for the control of behavior by reward-related stimuli. Psychopharmacology 146, 373–390. doi: 10.1007/PL00005483

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jonkman, S., Pelloux, Y., and Everitt, B. J. (2012). Differential roles of the dorsolateral and midlateral striatum in punished cocaine seeking. J. Neurosci. 32, 4645–4650. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0348-12.2012

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Karch, S., Jäger, L., Karamatskos, E., Graz, C., Stammel, A., Flatz, W., et al. (2008). Influence of trait anxiety on inhibitory control in alcohol-dependent patients: simultaneous acquisition of ERPs and BOLD responses. J. Psychiatr. Res. 42, 734–745. doi: 10.1016/j.jpsychires.2007.07.016

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keyes, K. M., Hatzenbuehler, M. L., McLaughlin, K. A., Link, B., Olfson, M., Grant, B. F., et al. (2010). Stigma and treatment for alcohol disorders in the United States. Am. J. Epidemiol. 172, 1364–1372. doi: 10.1093/aje/kwq304

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Koob, G. F., and Le Moal, M. (1997). Drug abuse: hedonic homeostatic dysregulation. Science 278, 52–58. doi: 10.1126/science.278.5335.52

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Koob, G. F., and Le Moal, M. (2001). Drug addiction, dysregulation of reward, and allostasis. Neuropsychopharmacology 24, 97–129. doi: 10.1016/S0893-133X(00)00195-0

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Koob, G. F., and Le Moal, M. (2005). Plasticity of reward neurocircuitry and the ‘dark side’ of drug addiction. Nat. Neurosci. 8, 1442–1444. doi: 10.1038/nn1105-1442

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Koob, G. F., and Mason, B. J. (2016). Existing and future drugs for the treatment of the dark side of addiction. Annu. Rev. Pharmacol. Toxicol. 56, 299–322. doi: 10.1146/annurev-pharmtox-010715-103143

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Koob, G. F., and Volkow, N. D. (2016). Neurobiology of addiction: a neurocircuitry analysis. Lancet Psychiatry 3, 760–773. doi: 10.1016/S2215-0366(16)00104-8

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kranzler, H. R., and Soyka, M. (2018). Diagnosis and pharmacotherapy of alcohol use disorder: a review. JAMA 320, 815–824. doi: 10.1001/jama.2018.11406

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kuhn, J., Gründler, T. O., Bauer, R., Huff, W., Fischer, A. G., Lenartz, D., et al. (2011). Successful deep brain stimulation of the nucleus accumbens in severe alcohol dependence is associated with changed performance monitoring. Addict. Biol. 16, 620–623. doi: 10.1111/j.1369-1600.2011.00337.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kwako, L. E., Bickel, W. K., and Goldman, D. (2018). Addiction biomarkers: dimensional approaches to understanding addiction. Trends Mol. Med. 24, 121–128. doi: 10.1016/j.molmed.2017.12.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Le Berre, A. P., Fama, R., and Sullivan, E. V. (2017). Executive functions, memory, and social cognitive deficits and recovery in chronic alcoholism: a critical review to inform future research. Alcohol. Clin. Exp. Res. 41, 1432–1443. doi: 10.1111/acer.13431

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lee, S., Lee, E., Ku, J., Yoon, K. J., Namkoong, K., and Jung, Y. C. (2013). Disruption of orbitofronto-striatal functional connectivity underlies maladaptive persistent behaviors in alcohol-dependent patients. Psychiatry Investig. 10, 266–272. doi: 10.4306/pi.2013.10.3.266

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Leeman, R. F., Beseler, C. L., Helms, C. M., Patock-Peckham, J. A., Wakeling, V. A., and Kahler, C. W. (2014). A brief, critical review of research on impaired control over alcohol use and suggestions for future studies. Alcohol. Clin. Exp. Res. 38, 301–308. doi: 10.1111/acer.12269

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lesscher, H. M., and Vanderschuren, L. J. (2012). Compulsive drug use and its neural substrates. Rev. Neurosci. 23, 731–745. doi: 10.1515/revneuro-2012-0066

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Li, C. S., Luo, X., Yan, P., Bergquist, K., and Sinha, R. (2009). Altered impulse control in alcohol dependence: neural measures of stop signal performance. Alcohol. Clin. Exp. Res. 33, 740–750. doi: 10.1111/j.1530-0277.2008.00891.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Litten, R. Z., Ryan, M. L., Falk, D. E., Reilly, M., Fertig, J. B., and Koob, G. F. (2015). Heterogeneity of alcohol use disorder: understanding mechanisms to advance personalized treatment. Alcohol. Clin. Exp. Res. 39, 579–584. doi: 10.1111/acer.12669

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lovibond, P. F., Mitchell, C. J., Minard, E., Brady, A., and Menzies, R. G. (2009). Safety behaviours preserve threat beliefs: protection from extinction of human fear conditioning by an avoidance response. Behav. Res. Ther. 47, 716–720. doi: 10.1016/j.brat.2009.04.013

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lubman, D. I., Yücel, M., and Pantelis, C. (2004). Addiction, a condition of compulsive behaviour? Neuroimaging and neuropsychological evidence of inhibitory dysregulation. Addiction 99, 1491–1502. doi: 10.1111/j.1360-0443.2004.00808.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ma, N., Liu, Y., Li, N., Wang, C. X., Zhang, H., Jiang, X. F., et al. (2010). Addiction related alteration in resting-state brain connectivity. Neuroimage 49, 738–744. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.08.037

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Maisto, S. A., Hallgren, K. A., Roos, C. R., and Witkiewitz, K. (2018). Course of remission from and relapse to heavy drinking following outpatient treatment of alcohol use disorder. Drug Alcohol Depend. 187, 319–326. doi: 10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2018.03.011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Makris, N., Biederman, J., Valera, E. M., Bush, G., Kaiser, J., Kennedy, D. N., et al. (2007). Cortical thinning of the attention and executive function networks in adults with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Cereb. Cortex 17, 1364–1375. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhl047

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Maltby, N., Tolin, D. F., Worhunsky, P., O’Keefe, T. M., and Kiehl, K. A. (2005). Dysfunctional action monitoring hyperactivates frontal-striatal circuits in obsessive-compulsive disorder: an event-related fMRI study. Neuroimage 24, 495–503. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2004.08.041

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mantovani, A., Simpson, H. B., Fallon, B. A., Rossi, S., and Lisanby, S. H. (2010). Randomized sham-controlled trial of repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation in treatment-resistant obsessive-compulsive disorder. Int. J. Neuropsychopharmacol. 13, 217–227. doi: 10.1017/S1461145709990435

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Marchant, N. J., Campbell, E. J., and Kaganovsky, K. (2018). Punishment of alcohol-reinforced responding in alcohol preferring P rats reveals a bimodal population: implications for models of compulsive drug seeking. Prog. Neuropsychopharmacol. Biol. Psychiatry 87(Pt A), 68–77. doi: 10.1016/j.pnpbp.2017.07.020

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mason, B. J., Goodman, A. M., Dixon, R. M., Hameed, M. H., Hulot, T., Wesnes, K., et al. (2002). A pharmacokinetic and pharmacodynamic drug interaction study of acamprosate and naltrexone. Neuropsychopharmacology 27, 596–606. doi: 10.1016/S0893-133X(02)00368-8

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

McGovern, R. A., and Sheth, S. A. (2017). Role of the dorsal anterior cingulate cortex in obsessive-compulsive disorder: converging evidence from cognitive neuroscience and psychiatric neurosurgery. J. Neurosurg. 126, 132–147. doi: 10.3171/2016.1.JNS15601

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Menzies, L., Chamberlain, S. R., Laird, A. R., Thelen, S. M., Sahakian, B. J., and Bullmore, E. T. (2008). Integrating evidence from neuroimaging and neuropsychological studies of obsessive-compulsive disorder: the orbitofronto-striatal model revisited. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 32, 525–549. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2007.09.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mian, M. K., Campos, M., Sheth, S. A., and Eskandar, E. N. (2010). Deep brain stimulation for obsessive-compulsive disorder: past, present, and future. Neurosurg. Focus 29:E10. doi: 10.3171/2010.4.FOCUS10107

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Milad, M. R., and Rauch, S. L. (2012). Obsessive-compulsive disorder: beyond segregated cortico-striatal pathways. Trends Cogn. Sci. 16, 43–51. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2011.11.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mishra, B. R., Nizamie, S. H., Das, B., and Praharaj, S. K. (2010). Efficacy of repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation in alcohol dependence: a sham-controlled study. Addiction 105, 49–55. doi: 10.1111/j.1360-0443.2009.02777

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mishra, B. R., Praharaj, S. K., Katshu, M. Z., Sarkar, S., and Nizamie, S. H. (2015). Comparison of anticraving efficacy of right and left repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation in alcohol dependence: a randomized double-blind study. J. Neuropsychiatry Clin. Neurosci. 27, e54–e59. doi: 10.1176/appi.neuropsych.13010013

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Moss, H. B., Chen, C. M., and Yi, H. Y. (2007). Subtypes of alcohol dependence in a nationally representative sample. Drug Alcohol Depend. 91, 149–158. doi: 10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2007.05.016

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mufson, E. J., and Pandya, D. N. (1984). Some observations on the course and composition of the cingulum bundle in the rhesus monkey. J. Comp. Neurol. 225, 31–43. doi: 10.1002/cne.902250105

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Müller, U. J., Sturm, V., Voges, J., Heinze, H.-J., Galazky, I., Büntjen, L., et al. (2016). Nucleus accumbens deep brain stimulation for alcohol addiction–safety and clinical long-term results of a pilot trial. Pharmacopsychiatry 49, 170–173. doi: 10.1055/s-0042-104507

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Müller-Oehring, E. M., Jung, Y. C., Pfefferbaum, A., Sullivan, E. V., and Schulte, T. (2015). The resting brain of alcoholics. Cereb. Cortex. 25, 4155–4168. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhu134

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Müller-Oehring, E. M., Jung, Y. C., Sullivan, E. V., Hawkes, W. C., Pfefferbaum, A., and Schulte, T. (2013). Midbrain-driven emotion and reward processing in alcoholism. Neuropsychopharmacology 38, 1844–1853. doi: 10.1038/npp.2013.102

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mutschler, J., Grosshans, M., Soyka, M., and Rösner, S. (2016). Current findings and mechanisms of action of disulfiram in the treatment of alcohol dependence. Pharmacopsychiatry 49, 137–141. doi: 10.1055/s-0042-103592

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nahas, Z., Teneback, C. C., Kozel, A., Speer, A. M., DeBrux, C., Molloy, M., et al. (2001). Brain effects of TMS delivered over prefrontal cortex in depressed adults: role of stimulation frequency and coil-cortex distance. J. Neuropsychiatry Clin. Neurosci. 13, 459–470. doi: 10.1176/jnp.13.4.459

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nakamura-Palacios, E. M., de Almeida Benevides, M. C., da Penha Zago-Gomes, M., de Oliveira, R. W., de Vasconcellos, V. F., de Castro, L. N., et al. (2012). Auditory event-related potentials (P3) and cognitive changes induced by frontal direct current stimulation in alcoholics according to Lesch alcoholism typology. Int. J. Neuropsychopharmacol. 15, 601–616. doi: 10.1017/S1461145711001040

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nardone, R., Bergmann, J., Christova, M., Lochner, P., Tezzon, F., Golaszewski, S., et al. (2012). Non-invasive brain stimulation in the functional evaluation of alcohol effects and in the treatment of alcohol craving: a review. Neurosci. Res. 74, 169–176. doi: 10.1016/j.neures.2012.08.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

National Institute on Alcohol Abuse and Alcoholism (2017). NIAAA Strategic Plan 2017–2021. Bethesda, MD: National Institute on Alcohol Abuse and Alcoholism.

Newlin, D. B., and Strubler, K. A. (2007). The habitual brain: an ”adapted habit” theory of substance use disorders. Subst. Use Misuse 42, 503–526. doi: 10.1080/10826080601144606

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nigg, J. T., Wong, M. M., Martel, M. M., Jester, J. M., Puttler, L. I., Glass, J. M., et al. (2006). Poor response inhibition as a predictor of problem drinking and illicit drug use in adolescents at risk for alcoholism and other substance use disorders. J. Am. Acad. Child Adolesc. Psychiatry 45, 468–475. doi: 10.1097/01.chi.0000199028.76452.a9

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Oliveri, M., Babiloni, C., Filippi, M. M., Caltagirone, C., Babiloni, F., Cicinelli, P., et al. (2003). Influence of the supplementary motor area on primary motor cortex excitability during movements triggered by neutral or emotionally unpleasant visual cues. Exp. Brain Res. 149, 214–221. doi: 10.1007/s00221-002-1346-8

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Page, L. A., Rubia, K., Deeley, Q., Daly, E., Toal, F., Mataix-Cols, D., et al. (2009). A functional magnetic resonance imaging study of inhibitory control in obsessive-compulsive disorder. Psychiatry Res. Neuroimaging 174, 202–209. doi: 10.1016/j.pscychresns.2009.05.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Papachristou, H., Nederkoorn, C., Havermans, R., Bongers, P., Beunen, S., and Jansen, A. (2013). Higher levels of trait impulsiveness and a less effective response inhibition are linked to more intense cue-elicited craving for alcohol in alcohol-dependent patients. Psychopharmacology 228, 641–649. doi: 10.1007/s00213-013-3063-3

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Park, S. Q., Kahnt, T., Beck, A., Cohen, M. X., Dolan, R. J., Wrase, J., et al. (2010). Prefrontal cortex fails to learn from reward prediction errors in alcohol dependence. J. Neurosci. 30, 7749–7753. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5587-09.2010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pauls, D. L., Abramovitch, A., Rauch, S. L., and Geller, D. A. (2014). Obsessive-compulsive disorder: an integrative genetic and neurobiological perspective. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 15, 410–424. doi: 10.1038/nrn3746

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Paus, T. (2001). Primate anterior cingulate cortex: where motor control, drive and cognition interface. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 2, 417–424. doi: 10.1038/35077500

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Penadés, R., Catalán, R., Rubia, K., Andrés, S., Salamero, M., and Gastó, C. (2007). Impairedresponse inhibition in obsessive compulsive disorder. Eur. Psychiatry 22, 404–410. doi: 10.1016/j.eurpsy.2006.05.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Petrides, M., and Pandya, D. N. (1988). Association fiber pathways to the frontal cortex from the superior temporal region in the rhesus monkey. J. Comp. Neurol. 273, 52–66. doi: 10.1002/cne.902730106

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pierce, R. C., and Vanderschuren, L. J. (2010). Kicking the habit: the neural basis of ingrained behaviors in cocaine addiction. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 35, 212–219. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2010.01.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rachman, S. (1997). A cognitive theory of obsessions. Behav. Res. Ther. 35, 793–802. doi: 10.1016/S0005-7967(97)00040-5

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rauch, S. L., Jenike, M. A., Alpert, N. M., Baer, L., Breiter, H. C., Savage, C. R., et al. (1994). Regional cerebral blood flow measured during symptom provocation in obsessive-compulsive disorder using oxygen 15-labeled carbon dioxide and positron emission tomography. Arch. Gen. Psychiatry 51, 62–70. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.1994.03950010062008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rehn, S., Eslick, G. D., and Brakoulias, V. (2018). A meta-analysis of the effectiveness of different cortical targets used in repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation (rTMS) for the treatment of obsessive-compulsive disorder (OCD). Psychiatr. Q. 89, 645–665. doi: 10.1007/s11126-018-9566-7

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Robbins, T. W., and Everitt, B. J. (1999). Drug addiction: bad habits add up. Nature 398, 567–570. doi: 10.1038/19208

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Robbins, T. W., and Everitt, B. J. (2002). Limbic-striatal memory systems and drug addiction. Neurobiol. Learn. Mem. 78, 625–636. doi: 10.1006/nlme.2002.4103

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Robbins, T. W., Gillan, C. M., Smith, D. G., de Wit, S., and Ersche, K. D. (2012). Neurocognitive endophenotypes of impulsivity and compulsivity: towards dimensional psychiatry. Trends Cogn. Sci. 16, 81–91. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2011.11.009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Robinson, T. E., and Berridge, K. C. (1993). The neural basis of drug craving: an incentive-sensitization theory of addiction. Brain Res. Brain Res. Rev. 18, 247–291. doi: 10.1016/0165-0173(93)90013-P

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Robinson, T. E., and Berridge, K. C. (2000). The psychology and neurobiology of addiction: an incentive-sensitization view. Addiction 95(Suppl. 2), S91–S117.

Google Scholar

Robinson, T. E., and Berridge, K. C. (2003). Addiction. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 54, 25–53. doi: 10.1146/annurev.psych.54.101601.145237

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Roth, R. M., Saykin, A. J., Flashman, L. A., Pixley, H. S., West, J. D., and Mamourian, A. C. (2007). Event-related functional magnetic resonance imaging of response inhibition in obsessive-compulsive disorder. Biol. Psychiatry 62, 901–909. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2006.12.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Saha, T. D., Grant, B. F., Chou, S. P., Kerridge, B. T., Pickering, R. P., and Ruan, W. J. (2018). Concurrent use of alcohol with other drugs and DSM-5 alcohol use disorder comorbid with other drug use disorders: sociodemographic characteristics, severity, and psychopathology. Drug Alcohol Depend. 187, 261–269. doi: 10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2018.03.006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Salkovskis, P. M. (1985). Obsessional-compulsive problems: a cognitive-behavioural analysis. Behav. Res. Ther. 23, 571–583. doi: 10.1016/0005-7967(85)90105-6

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Saunders, B., Farag, N., Vincent, A. S., Collins, F. L. Jr., Sorocco, K. H., and Lovallo, W. R. (2008). Impulsive errors on a Go-NoGo reaction time task: disinhibitory traits in relation to a family history of alcoholism. Alcohol. Clin. Exp. Res. 32, 888–894. doi: 10.1111/j.1530-0277.2008.00648

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schmaal, L., Goudriaan, A. E., Joos, L., Krüse, A. M., Dom, G., van den Brink, W., et al. (2013). Modafinil modulates resting-state functional network connectivity and cognitive control in alcohol-dependent patients. Biol. Psychiatry 73, 789–795. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2012.12.025

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schmahmann, J. D., and Pandya, D. N. (2007). The complex history of the fronto-occipital fasciculus. J. Hist. Neurosci. 16, 362–377. doi: 10.1080/09647040600620468

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schuckit, M. A., Tapert, S., Matthews, S. C., Paulus, M. P., Tolentino, N. J., Smith, T. L., et al. (2012). fMRI differences between subjects with low and high responses to alcohol during a stop signal task. Alcohol. Clin. Exp. Res. 36, 130–140. doi: 10.1111/j.1530-0277.2011.01590.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schulte, T., Müller-Oehring, E. M., Sullivan, E. V., and Pfefferbaum, A. (2012). Synchrony of corticostriatal-midbrain activation enables normal inhibitory control and conflict processing in recovering alcoholic men. Biol. Psychiatry 71, 269–278. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2011.10.022

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sebastian, A., Pohl, M. F., Klöppel, S., Feige, B., Lange, T., Stahl, C., et al. (2013). Disentangling common and specific neural subprocesses of response inhibition. Neuroimage 64, 601–615. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2012.09.020

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sebold, M., Nebe, S., Garbusow, M., Guggenmos, M., Schad, D. J., Beck, A., et al. (2017). When habits are dangerous: alcohol expectancies and habitual decision making predict relapse in alcohol dependence. Biol. Psychiatry 82, 847–856. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2017.04.019

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Seidman, L. J., Valera, E. M., Makris, N., Monuteaux, M. C., Boriel, D. L., Kelkar, K., et al. (2006). Dorsolateral prefrontal and anterior cingulate cortex volumetric abnormalities in adults with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder identified by magnetic resonance imaging. Biol. Psychiatry 60, 1071–1080. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2006.04.031

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Seif, T., Chang, S. J., Simms, J. A., Gibb, S. L., Dadgar, J., Chen, B. T., et al. (2013). Cortical activation of accumbens hyperpolarization-active NMDARs mediates aversion-resistant alcohol intake. Nat. Neurosci. 16, 1094–1100. doi: 10.1038/nn.3445

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Shackman, A. J., Salomons, T. V., Slagter, H. A., Fox, A. S., Winter, J. J., and Davidson, R. J. (2011). The integration of negative affect, pain and cognitive control in the cingulate cortex. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 12, 154–167. doi: 10.1038/nrn2994

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sheth, S. A., Neal, J., Tangherlini, F., Mian, M. K., Gentil, A., Cosgrove, G. R., et al. (2013). Limbic system surgery for treatment-refractory obsessive-compulsive disorder: a prospective long-term follow-up of 64 patients. J. Neurosurg. 118, 491–497. doi: 10.3171/2012.11.JNS12389

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Shivakumar, V., Dinakaran, D., Narayanaswamy, J. C., and Venkatasubramanian, G. (2019). Noninvasive brain stimulation in obsessive-compulsive disorder. Indian J. Psychiatry 61(Suppl. 1), S66–S76. doi: 10.4103/psychiatry.IndianJPsychiatry_522_18

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sinha, R., Fox, H. C., Hong, K. I., Hansen, J., Tuit, K., and Kreek, M. J. (2011). Effects of adrenal sensitivity, stress- and cue-induced craving, and anxiety on subsequent alcohol relapse and treatment outcomes. Arch. Gen. Psychiatry 68, 942–952. doi: 10.1001/archgenpsychiatry.2011.49

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sjoerds, Z., de Wit, S., van den Brink, W., Robbins, T. W., Beekman, A. T., Penninx, B. W., et al. (2013). Behavioral and neuroimaging evidence for overreliance on habit learning in alcohol-dependent patients. Transl. Psychiatry 3:e337. doi: 10.1038/tp.2013.107

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sjoerds, Z., Stufflebeam, S. M., Veltman, D. J., Van den Brink, W., Penninx, B. W., and Douw, L. (2017). Loss of brain graph network efficiency in alcohol dependence. Addict. Biol. 22, 523–534. doi: 10.1111/adb.12346

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Smith, J. L., Mattick, R. P., Jamadar, S. D., and Iredale, J. M. (2014). Deficits in behavioural inhibition in substance abuse and addiction: a meta-analysis. Drug Alcohol Depend. 145, 1–33. doi: 10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2014.08.009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Solanto, M. V. (1984). Neuropharmacological basis of stimulant drug action in attentiondeficit disorder with hyperactivity: a review and synthesis. Psychol. Bull. 95, 387–409. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.95.3.387

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Spanagel, R., Vengeliene, V., Jandeleit, B., Fischer, W. N., Grindstaff, K., Zhang, X., et al. (2014). Acamprosate produces its anti-relapse effects via calcium. Neuropsychopharmacology 39, 783–791. doi: 10.1038/npp.2013.264

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Stavro, K., Pelletier, J., and Potvin, S. (2013). Widespread and sustained cognitive deficits in alcoholism: a meta-analysis. Addict. Biol. 18, 203–213. doi: 10.1111/j.1369-1600.2011.00418.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sullivan, E. V., Müller-Oehring, E., Pitel, A. L., Chanraud, S., Shankaranarayanan, A., Alsop, D. C., et al. (2013). A selective insular perfusion deficit contributes to compromised salience network connectivity in recovering alcoholic men. Biol. Psychiatry 74, 547–555. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2013.02.026

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tuithof, M., Ten Have, M., van den Brink, W., Vollebergh, W., and de Graaf, R. (2013). Predicting persistency of DSM-5 alcohol use disorder and examining drinking patterns of recently remitted individuals: a prospective general population study. Addiction 108, 2091–2099. doi: 10.1111/add.12309

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vaghi, M. M., Cardinal, R. N., Apergis-Schoute, A. M., Fineberg, N. A., Sule, A., and Robbins, T. W. (2019). Action-outcome knowledge dissociates from behavior in obsessive-compulsive disorder following contingency degradation. Biol. Psychiatry Cogn. Neurosci. Neuroimaging 4, 200–209. doi: 10.1016/j.bpsc.2018.09.014

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vaghi, M. M., Hampshire, A., Fineberg, N. A., Kaser, M., Brühl, A. B., Sahakian, B. J., et al. (2017). Hypoactivation and dysconnectivity of a frontostriatal circuit during goal-directed planning as an endophenotype for obsessive-compulsive disorder. Biol. Psychiatry Cogn. Neurosci. Neuroimaging 2, 655–663. doi: 10.1016/j.bpsc.2017.05.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

van den Heuvel, O. A., van Wingen, G., Soriano-Mas, C., Alonso, P., Chamberlain, S. R., Nakamae, T., et al. (2016). Brain circuitry of compulsivity. Eur. Neuropsychopharmacol. 26, 810–827. doi: 10.1016/j.euroneuro.2015.12.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

van Velzen, L. S., Vriend, C., de Wit, S. J., and van den Heuvel, O. A. (2014). Response inhibition and interference control in obsessive-compulsive spectrum disorders. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 8:419. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2014.00419

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vanderschuren, L. J., and Everitt, B. J. (2005). Behavioral and neural mechanisms of compulsive drug seeking. Eur. J. Pharmacol. 526, 77–88. doi: 10.1016/j.ejphar.2005.09.037

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Viol, K., Aas, B., Kastinger, A., Kronbichler, M., Schöller, H., Reiter, E. M., et al. (2019). Individual OCD-provoking stimuli activate disorder-related and self-related neuronal networks in fMRI. Psychiatry Res. Neuroimaging 283, 135–144. doi: 10.1016/j.pscychresns.2018.12.008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Volkow, N. D., and Fowler, J. S. (2000). Addiction, a disease of compulsion and drive: involvement of the orbitofrontal cortex. Cereb. Cortex 10, 318–325. doi: 10.1093/cercor/10.3.318

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vollstädt-Klein, S., Wichert, S., Rabinstein, J., Bühler, M., Klein, O., Ende, G., et al. (2010). Initial, habitual and compulsive alcohol use is characterized by a shift of cue processing from ventral to dorsal striatum. Addiction 105, 1741–1749. doi: 10.1111/j.1360-0443.2010.03022.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Voon, V., Derbyshire, K., Rück, C., Irvine, M. A., Worbe, Y., Enander, J., et al. (2015). Disorders of compulsivity: a common bias towards learning habits. Mol. Psychiatry 20, 345–352. doi: 10.1038/mp.2014.44

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Weiland, B. J., Sabbineni, A., Calhoun, V. D., Welsh, R. C., Bryan, A. D., Jung, R. E., et al. (2014). Reduced left executive control network functional connectivity is associated with alcohol use disorders. Alcohol. Clin. Exp. Res. 38, 2445–2453. doi: 10.1111/acer.12505

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wilcox, C. E., Dekonenko, C. J., Mayer, A. R., Bogenschutz, M. P., and Turner, J. A. (2014). Cognitive control in alcohol use disorder: deficits and clinical relevance. Rev. Neurosci. 25, 1–24. doi: 10.1515/revneuro-2013-0054

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Willuhn, I., Burgeno, L. M., Everitt, B. J., and Phillips, P. E. (2012). Hierarchical recruitment of phasic dopamine signaling in the striatum during the progression of cocaine use. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 109, 20703–20708. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1213460109

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wise, R. A. (1987). The role of reward pathways in the development of drug dependence. Pharmacol. Ther. 35, 227–263. doi: 10.1016/0163-7258(87)90108-2

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wise, R. A., and Koob, G. F. (2014). The development and maintenance of drug addiction. Neuropsychopharmacology 39, 254–262. doi: 10.1038/npp.2013.261

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Woolley, J., Heyman, I., Brammer, M., Frampton, I., McGuire, P. K., and Rubia, K. (2008). Brain activation in paediatric obsessive compulsive disorder during tasks of inhibitory control. Br. J. Psychiatry 192, 25–31. doi: 10.1192/bjp.bp.107.036558

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

World Health Organization (2018). Global Status Report on Alcohol and Health. Geneva: World Health Organization.

Google Scholar

Yeterian, E. H., and Pandya, D. N. (1985). Corticothalamic connections of the posterior parietal cortex in the rhesus monkey. J. Comp. Neurol. 237, 408–426. doi: 10.1002/cne.902370309

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Yin, H. H., Knowlton, B. J., and Balleine, B. W. (2004). Lesions of dorsolateral striatum preserve outcome expectancy but disrupt habit formation in instrumental learning. Eur. J. Neurosci. 19, 181–189. doi: 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2004.03095.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Yücel, M., and Lubman, D. I. (2007). Neurocognitive and neuroimaging evidence of behavioural dysregulation in human drug addiction: implications for diagnosis, treatment and prevention. Drug Alcohol Rev. 26, 33–39. doi: 10.1080/09595230601036978

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zakiniaeiz, Y., Scheinost, D., Seo, D., Sinha, R., and Constable, R. T. (2016). Cingulate cortex functional connectivity predicts future relapse in alcohol dependent individuals. Neuroimage Clin. 13, 181–187. doi: 10.1016/j.nicl.2016.10.019

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zametkin, A. J., and Liotta, W. (1998). The neurobiology of attention-deficit/hyperactivitydisorder. J. Clin. Psychiatry 59(Suppl. 7), 17–23.

Google Scholar

Zapata, A., Minney, V. L., and Shippenberg, T. S. (2010). Shift from goal-directed to habitual cocaine seeking after prolonged experience in rats. J. Neurosci. 30, 15457–15463. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4072-10.2010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zhu, X., Cortes, C. R., Mathur, K., Tomasi, D., and Momenan, R. (2017). Model-free functional connectivity and impulsivity correlates of alcohol dependence: a resting-state study. Addict. Biol. 22, 206–217. doi: 10.1111/adb.12272

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: alcohol use disorder, compulsivity, obsessive compulsive disorder, habit learning, cognitive control, neuromodulation, transcranial magnetic stimulation

Citation: Burchi E, Makris N, Lee MR, Pallanti S and Hollander E (2019) Compulsivity in Alcohol Use Disorder and Obsessive Compulsive Disorder: Implications for Neuromodulation. Front. Behav. Neurosci. 13:70. doi: 10.3389/fnbeh.2019.00070

Received: 30 January 2019; Accepted: 22 March 2019;
Published: 11 April 2019.

Edited by:

Walter Adriani, Istituto Superiore di Sanità (ISS), Italy

Reviewed by:

Erin Jane Campbell, The Florey Institute of Neuroscience and Mental Health, Australia
Francesca Pacitti, University of L’Aquila, Italy

Copyright © 2019 Burchi, Makris, Lee, Pallanti and Hollander. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Elisabetta Burchi, YmV0dGluYS5idXJjaGlAZ21haWwuY29t

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.