Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Behav. Neurosci., 23 October 2017
Sec. Learning and Memory

Brain Interleukin-1 Facilitates Learning of a Water Maze Spatial Memory Task in Young Mice

  • 1Medical Research Institute, Tokyo Women’s Medical University, Tokyo, Japan
  • 2Synaptic Plasticity Project, Tokyo Metropolitan Institute of Medical Science, Tokyo, Japan
  • 3Center for Experimental Animal Models, Institute for Biomedical Sciences, Tokyo University of Science, Chiba, Japan

The proinflammatory cytokine interleukin-1 (IL-1) is produced by many types of cells, including immune cells in the periphery and glia and neurons in the brain. The type I IL-1 receptor (IL-1r1) is primarily responsible for transmitting the inflammatory effects of IL-1 and mediates several biological functions by binding to either IL-1α or IL-1β. IL-1β activation is associated with hippocampus-dependent memory tasks. Although IL-1β impairs spatial memory under certain pathophysiological conditions, IL-1β may be required for the normal physiological regulation of hippocampal plasticity and memory. In addition, brain IL-1β levels are thought to change in the hippocampus in an age-dependent manner. These findings suggest that IL-1β may have a beneficial, temporary effect on learning and memory in young mice, but the matter remains unclear. Therefore, we hypothesized that hippocampal IL-1β has a beneficial effect on spatial learning and memory in young mice via IL-1r1, which is diminished in adults. We investigated the performance of young (3-month-old) and adult (6-month-old) wild-type mice, IL-1β knockout mice (IL-1βko) and IL-1r1 knockout mice (IL-1r1ko) in learning a spatial memory task with a fixed platform in a water maze (WM) and measured the levels of IL-1β and IL-1α in the hippocampus and cortex of adult and young mice by using homogeneous time-resolved fluorescence (HTRF). Learning was significantly impaired in the training trials of the WM spatial memory task in young IL-1βko and IL-1r1ko mice but not in adult IL-1βko and IL-1r1ko mice. Moreover, young IL-1r1ko mice but not IL-1βko mice showed an impairment in long-term memory extinction, suggesting that IL-1α might facilitate memory extinction. In this study, the cytokine assay using HTRF did not indicate a higher expression of hippocampal IL-1 in young mice but cortical IL-1β and IL-1α were significantly increased in adult mice. We need to investigate the role of cortical IL-1 and the local IL-1 expression in the hippocampal neurons in the future.

Introduction

The proinflammatory cytokine interleukin-1 (IL-1) is produced by many types of cells, including immune cells in the periphery and glia and neurons in the brain (Dinarello, 1996). Interleukin-1 receptor (IL-1r) is a cytokine receptor that binds IL-1. The type I receptor (IL-1r1) is primarily responsible for transmitting the inflammatory effects of IL-1, while the type II receptor may act as a suppressor of IL-1 activity by competing for IL-1 binding. IL-1r1 mediates several biological functions by binding to either IL-1α or IL-1β (Labow et al., 1997). An IL-1 receptor antagonist (IL-1ra) blocks the effects of IL-1 (Dinarello, 1996).

Interleukin-1β activation is associated with hippocampus-dependent memory tasks (Depino et al., 2004; Labrousse et al., 2009) and long-term potentiation (LTP; Schneider et al., 1998; Balschun et al., 2003). Intracerebral injection or microinjection of lipopolysaccharide (LPS) into the hippocampus increases hippocampal IL-1β levels, particularly in the CA1 subregion (Tanaka et al., 2006; Wang et al., 2013). Peripheral stimulation with LPS also increases IL-1β levels in the CA1 hippocampus (Bilbo et al., 2008; Richwine et al., 2008). Therefore, many researchers have investigated the effects of IL-1β expressed in the hippocampal CA1 subregion on learning and memory.

As shown in previous studies, IL-1β impairs learning of a spatial memory task under certain pathophysiological conditions, such as acute inflammation caused by IL-1β injection (Oitzl et al., 1993; Gibertini et al., 1995; Rachal Pugh et al., 2001; Scholz et al., 2016) or IL-1β overexpression (Moore et al., 2009; Hein et al., 2010). These effects were studied using the water maze (WM). IL-1β also inhibits LTP in hippocampal slices (Cunningham et al., 1996; Ross et al., 2003; Scholz et al., 2016). Therefore, IL-1β has inhibitory effects on hippocampus-dependent memory processes.

Recent evidence has suggested that IL-1 may be required for the normal physiological regulation of hippocampal plasticity and the learning process. IL-1ra administration impairs memory formation in the WM (Yirmiya et al., 2002) and fear conditioning (Yirmiya et al., 2002; Goshen et al., 2007) and impairs hippocampal LTP (Schneider et al., 1998), whereas relatively low intracerebroventricular (ICV) doses of IL-1β improve learning of a spatial memory task (Yirmiya et al., 2002; Song et al., 2003; Goshen et al., 2007). In addition, learning a spatial memory task is impaired in mice with a targeted deletion of the IL-1r1 [IL-1r1 knockout mice (IL-1r1ko)], and LTP is absent in these mice in both in vivo and in vitro experiments (Avital et al., 2003). Another study has demonstrated that mice with transgenic (TG) overexpression of IL-1ra display impairments in spatial memory and contextual memory (Goshen et al., 2007) and show a low rate of successful trials in a T-maze test (Spulber et al., 2009).

Interleukin-1β is also known to have a dose-dependent effect on fear memory. Although low-dose ICV injection of IL-1β (1 ng IL-1β in 2–4-month-old mice or 15 ng IL-1β in young rats weighing 250–280 g) facilitates fear memory (Song et al., 2003; Goshen et al., 2007), high-dose IL-1β (10 ng IL-1β in 2–4-month-old mice) impairs memory performance in young animals (Goshen et al., 2007). In contrast, the effect of low-dose IL-1β (10 ng) is unclear in adult rats; it either facilitates (Yirmiya et al., 2002) or impairs contextual fear conditioning (Rachal Pugh et al., 2001). In addition, Yirmiya et al. (2002) has shown that low-dose IL-1β has no effect on a spatial memory task using a WM in adult rats (6–8 months old). Moreover, 2–4-month-old IL-1r1ko or IL-1ra TG mice showed an impairment in spatial memory performance in a WM (Avital et al., 2003; Goshen et al., 2007); however, the age-dependent effect of a physiological dose of IL-1β on performance in a spatial memory task is unknown. Studies have compared hippocampal IL-1β expression between young and aged mice (1-month and 12-month-old mice, 3-month and 24-month-old mice) (Badowska-Szalewska et al., 2013, 2014) or between adult and aged mice (5–6-month and 22–25-month-old mice) (Shah et al., 2006). These reports show that basal IL-1β expression is significantly higher in the hippocampus in aged mice than in young mice; however, there has been no comparison between 3-month-old and 6-month-old mice. In contrast, IL-1β mRNA expression in the hippocampal dentate gyrus (DG) under basal conditions was higher and significantly increased after hippocampal tetanic stimulation in young rats (3 months old) compared with that in adult rats (12–16 months old) (Balschun et al., 2003). These findings suggest that physiological IL-1β levels are definitely high in the hippocampus in young animals and lower in middle-aged adult animals but then increase in aged animals.

Therefore, we hypothesized that hippocampal IL-1β upregulation has a beneficial effect via IL-1r1 on spatial learning and memory in young mice, but the effect is diminished in adult mice with a decrease in IL-1β levels. To confirm this hypothesis, we investigated the performance of 3-month-old and 6-month-old wild-type (wt) mice, IL-1β knockout mice (IL-1βko), and IL-1r1ko mice in the WM task and measured the concentration of IL-1β and IL-1α in the hippocampus and cortex using young and adult mice brain tissue.

Materials and Methods

Animals

The subjects were 3-month-old (young) and 6-month-old (adult) male IL-1βko mice (10 young and 8 adult mice; Tokyo University of Science, Noda, Chiba, Japan) and IL-1r1ko mice (Glaccum et al., 1997) (11 young and 10 adult mice; Jackson Laboratories, Bar Harbor, ME, United States), and corresponding age-matched wt controls were used (26 young and 19 adult mice; C57BL/6J). We used separate young mice and adult mice in each experiment. We needed age-matched control mice to pair with the knockout mice and be maintained under the same experimental conditions, including breeding, feeding, and handling, for each experiment. Wt littermates should be used as controls for the knockout mice; however, having at least five age-matched homozygous mice and wt mice for each group at the same time was fundamentally very difficult. Therefore, we produced the knockout mice by mating the homozygous mice, which were backcrossed seven to eight times to C57BL/6J mice, and we used C57BL/6J mice as wt controls. In addition, we carefully arranged the breeding conditions of the mice. Although 3-month-old mice are generally considered to be adults, we called the group of 3-month-old mice young mice, and the group of 6-month-old mice adult mice to distinguish the two groups in this study. IL-1r1ko mice do not express the IL-1 receptor type 1. All known biological functions of IL-1 are mediated by this receptor, and the IL-1r1ko mice show impaired responses to IL-1α and IL-1β (Labow et al., 1997). The mice were housed individually with the conditions, such as handling method and duration, maintained as consistent as possible in a room at 24 ± 2°C with a standard 12-h light:dark cycle, and mice had access to standard chow and water ad libitum. All study protocols were approved by the Animal Care and Use Committee of Tokyo Women’s Medical University (ethics numbers: AE16-51, GE16-42).

Assessment of Spatial Memory Task Learning in the WM

The WM consisted of a 100-cm diameter circular pool filled with water (25 ± 1°C) mixed with white bath salts to make the water opaque. Animals were pre-trained on the task 1 day before the spatial memory experiment. During the pre-training session, the mice were required to locate and climb onto a visible platform (PF) (13 cm in diameter elevated 1 cm above the surface of the water) located in the center of the pool. The maximal trial duration was 15 s. Pre-training consisted of three trials over a 1-day period, with 10-s breaks between trials. The mice performed the spatial memory task the day after pre-training. During the spatial memory task, the PF was placed 0.5 cm below the surface of the water so that it was no longer visible to the mouse, and the submerged PF was placed in a fixed position. For each training trial, the mouse was placed in the tank at a random location and allowed to swim freely. Once the mouse found the PF and climbed onto it, it was allowed to remain on the PF for 10 s. If the mouse did not locate the PF after 60 s, the mouse was guided to the PF and allowed to remain on it for 10 s. The mice performed three trials per block, and we used the average of the three trials, with a 1-min inter-trial interval. Each mouse performed nine blocks of three training trials over 3 days (three blocks per day with a 1.5-h interval between blocks). The illuminated and distal visual cues on the walls and ceiling were kept the same throughout the experiment. We assessed the learning of the WM spatial memory task by measuring the latency to reach the PF across blocks or days. Moreover, we investigated the path length to the PF and the velocity to estimate the locomotor skill in mice.

Probe tests were conducted immediately after training (0 day) and at 1 day, 1 week, and 1 month after training. We omitted the day 0 test in the experiment using young IL-1r1ko mice with wt control mice to investigate the effect of the day 0 probe trial. During the probe test, the PF was removed from the pool, and each mouse was allowed to search freely for 60 s. Each mouse performed three trials, with a 10-s interval between trials. We measured the total duration the mouse spent searching the target quadrant. The test lasted 60 s; therefore, the expected duration spent in the target quadrant was 15 s. We compared the observed total search duration to this chance level. We calculated the ratio of the duration spent in the target quadrant against the chance level as an analysis of the probe test to investigate the effect of IL-1 in the memory extinction process. A video camera above the pool was connected to a computerized tracking system that monitored and automatically measured the latency and the path length for each mouse to reach the PF during the training trials. During the probe test, the total duration the mouse spent searching the target quadrant was monitored and measured. The video data were analyzed by using Top Scan software (Clever Sys., Inc., Reston, VA, United States).

Hippocampal Cytokines

Mice were subjected to three blocks of three training trials, and the next day, they were decapitated to obtain the cytokine data associated with the WM 18-h data. We used six young mice and seven adult mice that did not participate in the WM task and six young mice and seven adult mice that were trained on the WM task 18 h prior (WM 18-h condition). After the animals were deeply anesthetized and decapitated, the bilateral hippocampi and cortexes were removed and homogenized in ice-cold lysis buffer containing 25 mM HEPES, pH 7.43, 0.1% [(3-cholamidopropyl)dimethyl-ammonio]1-propanesulfonate, 5 mM MgCl2, 1.3 mM EDTA, 1 mM EGTA, 10 mg/ml pepstatin, aprotinin, and leupeptin, and 1 mM PMSF. The homogenates were centrifuged (15 min at 50,000 × g) and stored at -80°C. We measured the concentrations of hippocampal IL-1β and IL-1α by using homogeneous time-resolved fluorescence (HTRF) with mouse IL-1β (63ADK010PEB-JP) and mouse IL-1α (63ADK068PEB-JP) cytokine determination kits from Cisbio Japan (Tokyo, Japan). For the protein assay, we used a Coomassie (Bradford) Protein Assay Kit from Thermo Scientific (Rockford, IL, United States).

Statistical Analysis

Data are presented as the average ± standard error (SE). Statistical analyses were performed using Student’s t-test and repeated measures ANOVA. SAS version 9.4 (SAS Institute, Cary, NC, United States) was used for the statistical analyses. Significance was determined at a p-value < 0.05.

Results

Spatial Memory Task Learning in the WM in Young IL-1βko and IL-1r1ko Mice

Beginning with the average of the three trials per block, the latency to the PF was significantly longer in the IL-1βko mice (N = 7) than in the wt mice (N = 7) [Figure 1A; F(1,12) = 9.42, p = 0.0097], and there was no interaction. There was also an effect of time [F(8,96) = 10.52, p < 0.0001]. In Figure 1B, we compared the latency to the PF in the IL-1βko and wt mice using the average of the nine trials per day [Figure 1B; F(1,12) = 9.42, p = 0.0097] and showed an effect of time [F(2,24) = 17.45, p < 0.0001]. The path length to reach the PF was also significantly longer in the IL-1βko mice (N = 7) than in the wt mice (N = 7) [Figure 1C; F(1,12) = 9.56, p = 0.0093], and there was no interaction. There was also an effect of time [F(8,96) = 9.14, p < 0.0001]. Moreover, there was no significant difference in the velocity between IL-1βko and wt mice [F(1,12) = 0.43, p = 0.5266] with no effect of time [F(8,96) = 1.62, p = 0.1287], suggesting that the locomotor skills of the IL-1βko mice and wt mice did not differ. Thus, young IL-1β ko mice exhibited impaired learning in the WM spatial memory task. In the last trial of the 9th block, the average latency to reach the PF was 7.2 ± 1.7 s in the wt group and 19.9 ± 4.9 s in the IL-1βko group, which was over twice the latency observed in the wt group. In the probe tests, we compared the ratio of the total duration spent in the target quadrant against chance between young wt and IL-1βko mice each day. There was a significant difference in the probe test on day 0 [Figure 1D; F(1,12) = 10.64, p = 0.0068]. In contrast, there were no significant differences on day 1 [Figure 1E; F(1,12) = 3.15, p = 0.1012] or at 1 week [Figure 1F; F(1,11) = 2.57, p = 0.1370] or 1 month [Figure 1G; F(1,12) = 0.51, p = 0.4883] after training. The significant difference on day 0 may not be indicative of memory extinction because the low ratio in the first trial of the probe test was affected by the low memory consolidation shown in the last trial of training, and the ratio dropped to chance level in the probe test on day 1 in the IL-1βko mice.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

FIGURE 1. The effect of IL-1β during the WM spatial memory task in young mice. (A) Latency to reach the hidden platform (PF) during the training period in young IL-1β knockout mice (IL-1βko) mice (closed square, n = 7) and age-matched wt mice (open circle, n = 7) showing the average of the three trials in each block. (B) Latency to reach the hidden PF during the training period in young IL-1βko mice (closed square, n = 7) and age-matched wt mice (open circle, n = 7) showing the average of the nine trials on each day. (C) Path length to reach the hidden PF during the training period in young IL-1βko mice (closed square, n = 7) and age-matched wt mice (open circle, n = 7) showing the average of the three trials in each block. (D–G) The ratio of the total duration spent in the target quadrant in the probe trials in young IL-1βko mice (closed square, n = 7) and wt mice (open circle, n = 7) at 0 day (D), 1 day (E), 1 week (F), and 1 month (G) after the final training trial. p < 0.01.

Like the young IL-1βko mice, the young IL-1r1ko mice showed an impairment in learning in the WM spatial memory task. There was a significant difference in the latency to the PF between the young IL-1r1ko mice (N = 7) and the wt mice (N = 7) when the average of the three trials in each block was compared [Figure 2A; F(1,12) = 5.34, p = 0.0394] with no interaction. There was also an effect of time [F(8,96) = 11.91, p < 0.0001]. There was also a difference in the latency across training days when the average of the nine trials each day was compared [Figure 2B; F(1,12) = 5.34, p = 0.0394] with an effect of time [F(2,24) = 14.16, p < 0.0001]. There was no significant difference in the path length to reach the PF between the IL-1r1ko mice (N = 7) and the wt mice (N = 7) [Figure 2C; F(1,12) = 2.79, p = 0.1204] with an effect of time [F(8,96) = 11.73, p < 0.0001] due to the slower velocity of the IL-1r1ko mice; however, there was no significant difference in the velocity between the IL-1r1ko and wt mice [F(1,12) = 4.46, p = 0.0562]. The results suggest that the locomotor skill was slightly but not significantly inferior in the IL-1r1ko mice. Thus, young IL-1r1ko mice also revealed impaired learning of the spatial memory task in the WM. In the probe tests, we compared the ratio of the total duration spent in the target quadrant between young wt and IL-1r1ko mice each day. There were no significant differences in the probe test on day 1 [Figure 2D; F(1,10) = 0.00, p = 0.9862] or 1 week after training [Figure 2E; F(1,12) = 1.52, p = 0.2406], but there was a significant difference 1 month after training [Figure 2F; F(1,9) = 11.03, p = 0.0089] because the ratio of the duration in the target quadrant was maintained at high levels in the IL-1r1ko mice, which reveals that IL-1r1ko mice had an impairment in memory extinction.

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

FIGURE 2. The effect of IL-1r1 during the WM spatial memory task in young mice. (A) Latency to reach the hidden PF during the training period in young IL-1r1ko mice (closed triangle, n = 7) and age-matched wt mice (open circle, n = 7) showing the average of the three trials in each block. (B) Latency to reach the hidden PF during the training period in young IL-1r1ko mice (closed triangle, n = 7) and age-matched wt mice (open circle, n = 7) showing the average of the nine trials on each day. (C) Path length to reach the hidden PF during the training period in young IL-1r1ko mice (closed triangle, n = 7) and age-matched wt mice (open circle, n = 7) showing the average of the three trials in each block. (D–F) The ratio of the total duration spent in the target quadrant in the probe trials in young IL-1r1ko mice (closed triangle, n = 7) and wt mice (open circle, n = 7) at 1 day (D), 1 week (E), and 1 month (F) after the final training trial. p < 0.05, ∗∗p < 0.01.

Spatial Memory Task Learning in the WM in Adult IL-1βko and IL-1r1ko Mice

There were no significant differences in the latency to the PF between adult IL-1βko mice (N = 5) and wt mice (N = 5) when the average of the three trials in each block was compared [Figure 3A; F(1,8) = 1.26, p = 0.2938], but there was an effect of time [F(8,64) = 4.25, p = 0.0004]. There was also no difference in the average of the nine trials per day [Figure 3B; F(1,8) = 1.26, p = 0.2938], but there was an effect of time [F(2,16) = 6.44, p = 0.0089]. There was no significant difference in the path length to reach the PF between the IL-1βko mice and the wt mice [Figure 3C; F(1,8) = 2.57, p = 0.1475], but there was an effect of time [F(8,64) = 4.96, p < 0.0001]. Additionally, there was no significant difference in the velocity between the IL-1βko mice and the wt mice [F(1,8) = 0.02, p = 0.8807]. During the probe test, there was no significant difference in the ratio of the total duration spent in the target quadrant between adult wt and IL-1βko mice on day 0 [Figure 3D; F(1,7) = 2.27, p = 0.1758] and day 1 [Figure 3E; F(1,8) = 0.00, p = 0.9855] as well as 1 week [Figure 3F; F(1,6) = 0.35, p = 0.5733] and 1 month [Figure 3G; F(1,7) = 0.00, p = 0.9868] after training.

FIGURE 3
www.frontiersin.org

FIGURE 3. The effect of IL-1β during the WM spatial memory task in adult mice. (A) Latency to reach the hidden PF during the training period in adult IL-1βko mice (open square, n = 5) and age-matched wt mice (closed circle, n = 5) showing the average of the three trials in each block. (B) Latency to reach the hidden PF during the training period in adult IL-1βko mice (open square, n = 5) and age-matched wt mice (closed circle, n = 5) showing the average of the nine trials on each day. (C) Path length to reach the hidden PF during the training period in adult IL-1βko mice (open square, n = 5) and age-matched wt mice (closed circle, n = 5) showing the average of the three trials in each block. (D–G) The ratio of the total duration spent in the target quadrant in the probe trials in adult IL-1βko mice (open square, n = 5) and wt mice (closed circle, n = 5) at 0 day (D), 1 day (E), 1 week (F), and 1 month (G) after the final training trial.

Similar to the adult IL-1βko mice, there were no significant differences in the latency to the PF between the adult IL-1r1ko (N = 5) and the wt (N = 5) mice when the average of the three trials in each block was compared [Figure 4A; F(1,8) = 1.34, p = 0.2799], but there was an effect of time [F(8,64) = 3.80, p = 0.0011]. Figure 4B shows that there was no difference between the two groups in the average of the nine trials per day [Figure 4B; F(1,8) = 1.34, p = 0.2799], but there was an effect of time [F(2,16) = 5.13, p = 0.0190]. There was no significant difference in the path length to reach the PF between the IL-1r1ko mice and the wt mice [Figure 4C; F(1,8) = 3.10, p = 0.1162], but there was an effect of time [F(8,64) = 4.18, p = 0.0005]. Additionally, there was no significant difference in the velocity between the IL-1r1ko mice and the wt mice [F(1,8) = 0.01, p = 0.9144]. During the probe test, there was no significant difference in the ratio of the total duration spent in the target quadrant between the adult wt and IL-1r1ko mice on day 0 [Figure 4D; F(1,6) = 3.74, p = 0.1012] and day 1 [Figure 4E; F(1,7) = 0.14, p = 0.7199] as well as 1 week [Figure 4F; F(1,7) = 0.84, p = 0.3912] and 1 month [Figure 4G; F(1,6) = 3.78, p = 0.0999] after training.

FIGURE 4
www.frontiersin.org

FIGURE 4. The effect of IL-1r1 during the WM spatial memory task in adult mice. (A) Latency to reach the hidden PF during the training period in adult IL-1r1ko mice (open triangle, n = 5) and age-matched wt mice (closed circle, n = 5) showing the average of the three trials in each block. (B) Latency to reach the hidden PF during the training period in adult IL-1r1ko mice (open triangle, n = 5) and age-matched wt mice (closed circle, n = 5) showing the average of the nine trials on each day. (C) Path length to reach the hidden PF during the training period in adult IL-1r1ko mice (open triangle, n = 5) and age-matched wt mice (closed circle, n = 5) showing the average of the three trials in each block. (D–G) The ratio of the total duration spent in the target quadrant in the probe trials in adult IL-1r1ko mice (open triangle, n = 5) and wt mice (closed circle, n = 5) at 0 day (D), 1 day (E), 1 week (F), and 1 month (G) after the final training trial.

The Basal Concentrations of IL-1β and IL-1α in the Hippocampus and Cortex

The hippocampal concentration of basal IL-1α was not significantly different between the young (N = 6) and adult (N = 7) wt mice (Figure 5A, p = 0.3944, t = 0.274) or between the young and adult IL-1βko mice (N = 3, Figure 5A, p = 0.2085, t = 0.939). In addition, there was also no significant difference in the basal levels of hippocampal IL-1β between the young (N = 6) and adult (N = 7) wt mice (Figure 5A, p = 0.4035, t = 0.250). The hippocampal concentration of IL-1β was high in young IL-1r1ko mice (N = 4) compared with adult IL-1r1ko mice (N = 5) (Figure 5A, p = 0.01644, t = 2.6513). Interestingly, basal levels of IL-1α and IL-1β in the cortex were significantly higher in the adult wt mice (N = 6) than in the young wt mice (N = 6) (Figure 5B, p < 0.0001, t = 11.77; Figure 5B, p = 0.0115, t = 2.68). Moreover, cortical IL-1α levels were significantly higher in adult IL-1βko mice than in young IL-1βko mice (N = 3, Figure 5B, p = 0.02875, t = 2.27).

FIGURE 5
www.frontiersin.org

FIGURE 5. Basal hippocampal and cortical concentrations of IL-1α and IL-1β in young and adult wt mice, IL-1r1ko mice, and IL-1βko mice. (A) Concentrations of basal levels of hippocampal IL-1β (black bar) and IL-1α (gray bar). (B) Concentrations of basal levels of cortical IL-1β (black bar) and IL-1α (gray bar). wt, wild-type mice; r1ko, IL-1r1ko mice; b1ko, IL-1βko mice; (Y), young; (A), adult. p < 0.05, ∗∗p < 0.0001.

Hippocampal Concentrations of IL-1β and IL-1α 18 h after the WM Task

We measured the concentrations of hippocampal IL-1β and IL-1α 18 h after the WM task in young and adult wt mice to investigate whether IL-1 signaling changed after the WM task. The concentrations of IL-1β decreased after the WM task but not significantly in young (Figure 6; p = 0.0980, t = 1.39, N = 6 in each group) and adult mice (Figure 6; p = 0.0594, t = 1.68, N = 7 in each group). Additionally, IL-1α was significantly decreased in young mice after the WM task (Figure 6; p = 0.0266, t = 2.19, N = 6 in each group), but no difference was observed in adults (Figure 6; p = 0.2076, t = 0.844, N = 7 in each group).

FIGURE 6
www.frontiersin.org

FIGURE 6. Changes in the hippocampal concentrations of IL-1β and IL-1α in young and adult wt mice after the WM task. Concentrations of hippocampal IL-1β (black bar) and IL-1α (gray bar) under basal conditions (BC) and 18 h after the WM task (WM 18 h). wt, wild-type mice; (Y), young; (A), adult. p < 0.05.

Discussion

In this study, we hypothesized that physiological IL-1β levels and their effects vary with age in the brain. Our hypothesis is that IL-1β has a beneficial effect via IL-1r1 on hippocampal spatial memory in young mice, whereas the effect is diminished in adult mice. Therefore, we expected that there would be a significant impairment in learning a WM spatial memory task in young IL-1βko mice and IL-1r1ko mice but not in adult IL-1βko mice and IL-1r1ko mice. In addition, we studied whether physiological IL-1 levels were high in the basal hippocampus and further increased 18 h after WM test in young wt mice compared to that in adult mice.

The results show that young IL-1β ko and IL-1r1ko mice showed an impairment in spatial memory task learning in the WM, suggesting that brain IL-1β activates IL-1r1 to facilitate hippocampal spatial learning and memory in young mice (Figures 1A,B, 2A,B). Because the IL-1α and IL-1β genes have been reported to be highly expressed in association with memory recall (Scholz et al., 2016), IL-1 might play an important role in hippocampal spatial memory in young mice. Furthermore, there was no significant difference in the velocity between IL-1β and wt mice or between IL-1r1ko and wt mice, suggesting that locomotor behavior was not impaired in either IL-1βko or IL-1r1ko mice. Thus, young IL-1βko mice and IL-1r1ko mice specifically exhibited an impairment in spatial memory task learning in the WM. Therefore, brain IL-1 facilitates the learning of the spatial memory task in the WM in young mice.

In probe trials, we estimated the memory extinction process by examining the gradual decline in the ratio of the duration of time spent in the target quadrant. We omitted the day-0 WM probe test in the experiment using the young IL-1r1ko mice; therefore, the decline in the ratio was delayed until 1 week after training in the wt mice (Figure 2E). However, the ratio was close to chance level in the WM probe test 1 month after training (Figure 2F). In contrast, the ratio was maintained at a high level 1 month after training in the IL-1r1ko mice (Figure 2F), suggesting that the memory extinction process is impaired in IL-1r1ko mice. This extended memory was not observed in IL-1βko mice (Figure 1G); therefore, IL-1α might be related to the spatial memory extinction process. Because IL-1α mRNA is known to be expressed after learning training (Depino et al., 2004), inducible IL-1α might be related to memory extinction. Moreover, IL-1α expression is further increased under physiological temperatures (34–36°C) (Ross et al., 2003); therefore, IL-1α might be increased in the brain after the training trials. Furthermore, clinical research has shown that genetic variants in IL-1 genes are involved in lower cognition associated with inflammation (Benke et al., 2011). Therefore, IL-1α plays a role in cognitive function.

Unlike in the young IL-1βko and IL-1r1ko mice, spatial memory task learning in the WM was not impaired in the adult IL-1βko and IL-1r1ko mice relative to the wt mice (Figures 3A,B, 4A,B). Moreover, the impairment in the memory extinction task in the 1-month WM probe test found in young IL-1r1ko mice was diminished in the adult IL-1r1ko mice (Figure 4G). Our current results suggest that IL-1β and IL-1α have a beneficial effect on hippocampal spatial learning and memory that is limited to young mice.

Unexpectedly, there were no significant differences in the basal concentrations of hippocampal IL-1β and IL-1α between young and adult wt mice (Figure 5A). In addition, hippocampal IL-1β and IL-1α did not increase 18 h after WM in young or adult mice in our study (Figure 6). As IL-1β mRNA was increased during LTP in the DG just 8 h after tetanic stimulation in 3-month-old young rats (Balschun et al., 2003), we need to investigate the concentration of IL-1β and IL-1α in the hippocampus at an earlier timepoint. It is unclear whether IL-1β in hippocampal neurons locally facilitates learning of the hippocampal WM spatial memory task in young mice. In addition, IL-1α and IL-1β levels were significantly higher in the cortex in adult wt mice than in young wt mice (Figure 5B). The RSC plays an integral role in spatial memory (Czajkowski et al., 2014; Miller et al., 2014; Todd and Bucci, 2015). In addition, several lines of evidence indicate that the prefrontal cortex is reported to have a crucial function in spatial memory (Euston et al., 2012; Sapiurka et al., 2016), in particular in spatial memory extinction (Mendez-Couz et al., 2014). Therefore, cortical IL-1 may play a role in hippocampal spatial learning and memory. We also need to investigate the role of cortical IL-1 in the future.

We demonstrated an age-related change in IL-1β function. Viviani and Boraso (2011) suggest that IL-1β plays a role in synaptic plasticity and contributes to the functional deficit that characterizes the aged brain. For example, IL-1β increases GABAergic inhibition to impair LTP (Hellstrom et al., 2005). If these functions increase with age, the beneficial effect of IL-1β to facilitate memory in young mice might disappear. Another researcher has suggested a potential role for neuroinflammation in the aged brain (Sparkman and Johnson, 2008). The aging brain shows a shift in the homeostatic balance of inflammatory mediators to a proinflammatory state. In addition, inflammation impairs synaptic plasticity and cognition by activating one or more members of the mitogen activated protein (MAP) kinase family, such as p38 MAP kinase. Neuroinflammatory mediators also modulate neuronal Ca2+ signaling and homeostasis, which affect synapses (Sama and Norris, 2013). Sensitivity to IL-1β might be augmented in aged hippocampal synapses, and the increased synaptic sensitivity could be due to the reconfiguration of the IL-1 receptor subunit (Prieto et al., 2015).

Finally, we investigated the age-related production of hippocampal IL-1β and the effect of the WM task as a physiological stressor. A previous study showed that basal IL-1β levels in the hippocampus are higher in 22- to 25-month-old mice than in 5- to 6-month-old mice (Shah et al., 2006). In addition, peripheral stimulation with LPS also increases IL-1β levels in the hippocampus in 22- to 24-month-old mice more than in 3- to 6-month-old mice (Bilbo et al., 2008; Richwine et al., 2008). These findings suggest that the basal IL-1β levels may be high in old mice and may increase further after physiological stress such as neuroinflammation. We also measured the concentrations of hippocampal IL-1β and IL-1α 18 h after the WM task in young and adult wt mice to investigate whether basal levels of IL-1 were increased after the WM task. Our data show that the hippocampal IL-1 levels were decreased 18 h after the WM task in adult and young mice (Figure 6). Previous reports have shown that exposure to a forced swim test decreases the concentration of IL-1 in the cortex and hippocampus (Deak et al., 2003) and that running decreases the levels of IL-1β in the hippocampus (Chennaoui et al., 2008). Therefore, the brain condition 18 h after the WM task might be similar to that after a swimming exercise, which decreases IL-1β.

In this study, we observed a beneficial effect of IL-1 on spatial memory task learning in the WM in young mice, and the effect was diminished in adult mice. In conclusion, brain IL-1 regulates learning in a WM spatial memory task in young mice.

Author Contributions

TT, KF, KY, YI, and MK made substantial contribution to the design, acquisition, analysis, and interpretation of the experimental work, and TT wrote the manuscript.

Funding

This work was supported by KAKENHI (17K10064).

Conflict of Interest Statement

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

References

Avital, A., Goshen, I., Kamsler, A., Segal, M., Iverfeldt, K., Richter-Levin, G., et al. (2003). Impaired interleukin-1 signaling is associated with deficits in hippocampal memory processes and neural plasticity. Hippocampus 13, 826–834. doi: 10.1002/hipo.10135

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Badowska-Szalewska, E., Ludkiewicz, B., Sidor-Kaczmarek, J., Lietzau, G., Spodnik, J. H., Swietlik, D., et al. (2013). Hippocampal interleukin-1beta in the juvenile and middle-aged rat: response to chronic forced swim or high-light open-field stress stimulation. Acta Neurobiol. Exp. (Wars.) 73, 364–378.

Google Scholar

Badowska-Szalewska, E., Ludkiewicz, B., Spodnik, J. H., and Morys, J. (2014). Interleukin-1beta-immunoreactive neurons in the hippocampus and paraventricular nucleus of the hypothalamus after stress stimulation in aged versus adult rats. J. Neurosci. Res. 92, 1446–1456. doi: 10.1002/jnr.23428

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Balschun, D., Randolf, A., Pitossi, F., Schneider, H., Del Rey, A., and Besedovsky, H. O. (2003). Hippocampal interleukin-1 beta gene expression during long-term potentiation decays with age. Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci. 992, 1–8. doi: 10.1111/j.1749-6632.2003.tb03132.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Benke, K. S., Carlson, M. C., Doan, B. Q., Walston, J. D., Xue, Q. L., Reiner, A. P., et al. (2011). The association of genetic variants in interleukin-1 genes with cognition: findings from the cardiovascular health study. Exp. Gerontol. 46, 1010–1019. doi: 10.1016/j.exger.2011.09.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bilbo, S. D., Barrientos, R. M., Eads, A. S., Northcutt, A., Watkins, L. R., Rudy, J. W., et al. (2008). Early-life infection leads to altered BDNF and IL-1beta mRNA expression in rat hippocampus following learning in adulthood. Brain Behav. Immun. 22, 451–455. doi: 10.1016/j.bbi.2007.10.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chennaoui, M., Drogou, C., and Gomez-Merino, D. (2008). Effects of physical training on IL-1beta, IL-6 and IL-1ra concentrations in various brain areas of the rat. Eur. Cytokine Netw. 19, 8–14. doi: 10.1684/ecn.2008.0115

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cunningham, A. J., Murray, C. A., O’Neill, L. A., Lynch, M. A., and O’Connor, J. J. (1996). Interleukin-1 beta (IL-1 beta) and tumour necrosis factor (TNF) inhibit long-term potentiation in the rat dentate gyrus in vitro. Neurosci. Lett. 203, 17–20. doi: 10.1016/0304-3940(95)12252-4

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Czajkowski, R., Jayaprakash, B., Wiltgen, B., Rogerson, T., Guzman-Karlsson, M. C., Barth, A. L., et al. (2014). Encoding and storage of spatial information in the retrosplenial cortex. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 111, 8661–8666. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1313222111

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Deak, T., Bellamy, C., and D’Agostino, L. G. (2003). Exposure to forced swim stress does not alter central production of IL-1. Brain Res. 972, 53–63. doi: 10.1016/S0006-8993(03)02485-5

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Depino, A. M., Alonso, M., Ferrari, C., del Rey, A., Anthony, D., Besedovsky, H., et al. (2004). Learning modulation by endogenous hippocampal IL-1: blockade of endogenous IL-1 facilitates memory formation. Hippocampus 14, 526–535. doi: 10.1002/hipo.10164

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dinarello, C. A. (1996). Biologic basis for interleukin-1 in disease. Blood 87, 2095–2147.

Google Scholar

Euston, D. R., Gruber, A. J., and McNaughton, B. L. (2012). The role of medial prefrontal cortex in memory and decision making. Neuron 76, 1057–1070. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2012.12.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gibertini, M., Newton, C., Friedman, H., and Klein, T. W. (1995). Spatial learning impairment in mice infected with Legionella pneumophila or administered exogenous interleukin-1-beta. Brain Behav. Immun. 9, 113–128. doi: 10.1006/brbi.1995.1012

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Glaccum, M. B., Stocking, K. L., Charrier, K., Smith, J. L., Willis, C. R., Maliszewski, C., et al. (1997). Phenotypic and functional characterization of mice that lack the type I receptor for IL-1. J. Immunol. 159, 3364–3371.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Goshen, I., Kreisel, T., Ounallah-Saad, H., Renbaum, P., Zalzstein, Y., Ben-Hur, T., et al. (2007). A dual role for interleukin-1 in hippocampal-dependent memory processes. Psychoneuroendocrinology 32, 1106–1115. doi: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2007.09.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hein, A. M., Stasko, M. R., Matousek, S. B., Scott-McKean, J. J., Maier, S. F., Olschowka, J. A., et al. (2010). Sustained hippocampal IL-1beta overexpression impairs contextual and spatial memory in transgenic mice. Brain Behav. Immun. 24, 243–253. doi: 10.1016/j.bbi.2009.10.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hellstrom, I. C., Danik, M., Luheshi, G. N., and Williams, S. (2005). Chronic LPS exposure produces changes in intrinsic membrane properties and a sustained IL-beta-dependent increase in GABAergic inhibition in hippocampal CA1 pyramidal neurons. Hippocampus 15, 656–664. doi: 10.1002/hipo.20086

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Labow, M., Shuster, D., Zetterstrom, M., Nunes, P., Terry, R., Cullinan, E. B., et al. (1997). Absence of IL-1 signaling and reduced inflammatory response in IL-1 type I receptor-deficient mice. J. Immunol. 159, 2452–2461.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Labrousse, V. F., Costes, L., Aubert, A., Darnaudery, M., Ferreira, G., Amedee, T., et al. (2009). Impaired interleukin-1beta and c-Fos expression in the hippocampus is associated with a spatial memory deficit in P2X(7) receptor-deficient mice. PLOS ONE 4:e6006. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0006006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mendez-Couz, M., Conejo, N. M., Vallejo, G., and Arias, J. L. (2014). Spatial memory extinction: a c-Fos protein mapping study. Behav. Brain Res. 260, 101–110. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2013.11.032

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Miller, A. M., Vedder, L. C., Law, L. M., and Smith, D. M. (2014). Cues, context, and long-term memory: the role of the retrosplenial cortex in spatial cognition. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 8:586. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2014.00586

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Moore, A. H., Wu, M., Shaftel, S. S., Graham, K. A., and O’Banion, M. K. (2009). Sustained expression of interleukin-1beta in mouse hippocampus impairs spatial memory. Neuroscience 164, 1484–1495. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2009.08.073

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Oitzl, M. S., van Oers, H., Schobitz, B., and de Kloet, E. R. (1993). Interleukin-1 beta, but not interleukin-6, impairs spatial navigation learning. Brain Res. 613, 160–163. doi: 10.1016/0006-8993(93)90468-3

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Prieto, G. A., Snigdha, S., Baglietto-Vargas, D., Smith, E. D., Berchtold, N. C., Tong, L., et al. (2015). Synapse-specific IL-1 receptor subunit reconfiguration augments vulnerability to IL-1beta in the aged hippocampus. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 112, E5078–E5087. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1514486112

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rachal Pugh, C., Fleshner, M., Watkins, L. R., Maier, S. F., and Rudy, J. W. (2001). The immune system and memory consolidation: a role for the cytokine IL-1beta. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 25, 29–41. doi: 10.1016/S0149-7634(00)00048-8

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Richwine, A. F., Parkin, A. O., Buchanan, J. B., Chen, J., Markham, J. A., Juraska, J. M., et al. (2008). Architectural changes to CA1 pyramidal neurons in adult and aged mice after peripheral immune stimulation. Psychoneuroendocrinology 33, 1369–1377. doi: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2008.08.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ross, F. M., Allan, S. M., Rothwell, N. J., and Verkhratsky, A. (2003). A dual role for interleukin-1 in LTP in mouse hippocampal slices. J. Neuroimmunol. 144, 61–67. doi: 10.1016/j.jneuroim.2003.08.030

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sama, D. M., and Norris, C. M. (2013). Calcium dysregulation and neuroinflammation: discrete and integrated mechanisms for age-related synaptic dysfunction. Ageing Res. Rev. 12, 982–995. doi: 10.1016/j.arr.2013.05.008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sapiurka, M., Squire, L. R., and Clark, R. E. (2016). Distinct roles of hippocampus and medial prefrontal cortex in spatial and nonspatial memory. Hippocampus 26, 1515–1524. doi: 10.1002/hipo.22652

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schneider, H., Pitossi, F., Balschun, D., Wagner, A., del Rey, A., and Besedovsky, H. O. (1998). A neuromodulatory role of interleukin-1beta in the hippocampus. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 95, 7778–7783. doi: 10.1073/pnas.95.13.7778

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Scholz, B., Doidge, A. N., Barnes, P., Hall, J., Wilkinson, L. S., and Thomas, K. L. (2016). The regulation of cytokine networks in Hippocampal CA1 differentiates extinction from those required for the maintenance of contextual fear memory after recall. PLOS ONE 11:e0153102. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0153102

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Shah, S. A., Prough, D. S., Garcia, J. M., DeWitt, D. S., and Hellmich, H. L. (2006). Molecular correlates of age-specific responses to traumatic brain injury in mice. Exp. Gerontol. 41, 1201–1205. doi: 10.1016/j.exger.2006.07.006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Song, C., Phillips, A. G., and Leonard, B. (2003). Interleukin 1 beta enhances conditioned fear memory in rats: possible involvement of glucocorticoids. Eur. J. Neurosci. 18, 1739–1743. doi: 10.1046/j.1460-9568.2003.02886.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sparkman, N. L., and Johnson, R. W. (2008). Neuroinflammation associated with aging sensitizes the brain to the effects of infection or stress. Neuroimmunomodulation 15, 323–330. doi: 10.1159/000156474

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Spulber, S., Mateos, L., Oprica, M., Cedazo-Minguez, A., Bartfai, T., Winblad, B., et al. (2009). Impaired long term memory consolidation in transgenic mice overexpressing the human soluble form of IL-1ra in the brain. J. Neuroimmunol. 208, 46–53. doi: 10.1016/j.jneuroim.2009.01.010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tanaka, S., Ide, M., Shibutani, T., Ohtaki, H., Numazawa, S., Shioda, S., et al. (2006). Lipopolysaccharide-induced microglial activation induces learning and memory deficits without neuronal cell death in rats. J. Neurosci. Res. 83, 557–566. doi: 10.1002/jnr.20752

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Todd, T. P., and Bucci, D. J. (2015). Retrosplenial cortex and long-term memory: molecules to behavior. Neural Plast. 2015:414173. doi: 10.1155/2015/414173

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Viviani, B., and Boraso, M. (2011). Cytokines and neuronal channels: a molecular basis for age-related decline of neuronal function? Exp. Gerontol. 46, 199–206. doi: 10.1016/j.exger.2010.09.008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wang, K. C., Fan, L. W., Kaizaki, A., Pang, Y., Cai, Z., and Tien, L. T. (2013). Neonatal lipopolysaccharide exposure induces long-lasting learning impairment, less anxiety-like response and hippocampal injury in adult rats. Neuroscience 234, 146–157. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2012.12.049

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Yirmiya, R., Winocur, G., and Goshen, I. (2002). Brain interleukin-1 is involved in spatial memory and passive avoidance conditioning. Neurobiol. Learn. Mem. 78, 379–389. doi: 10.1006/nlme.2002.4072

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: interleukin-1β (IL-1β), interleukin-1α (IL-1α), IL-1β knockout mice (IL-1βko), IL-1 receptor 1 (IL-1r1), IL-1r1 knockout mice (IL-1r1ko), water maze (WM), young and adult mice, spatial memory

Citation: Takemiya T, Fumizawa K, Yamagata K, Iwakura Y and Kawakami M (2017) Brain Interleukin-1 Facilitates Learning of a Water Maze Spatial Memory Task in Young Mice. Front. Behav. Neurosci. 11:202. doi: 10.3389/fnbeh.2017.00202

Received: 02 February 2017; Accepted: 06 October 2017;
Published: 23 October 2017.

Edited by:

Djoher Nora Abrous, Université de Bordeaux, France

Reviewed by:

Stéphanie Daumas, Neuroscience Paris Seine, Sorbonne Universities, France
Agnes Nadjar, Laboratoire NutriNeurO (INRA), France

Copyright © 2017 Takemiya, Fumizawa, Yamagata, Iwakura and Kawakami. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) or licensor are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Takako Takemiya, takemiya.takako@twmu.ac.jp

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.