Skip to main content

GENERAL COMMENTARY article

Front. Behav. Neurosci., 02 June 2017
Sec. Pathological Conditions

Commentary: The Brain Basis for Misophonia

\r\nArjan Schrder*Arjan Schröder1*Guido van WingenGuido van Wingen1Nienke C. VulinkNienke C. Vulink1Damiaan Denys,*Damiaan Denys1,2*
  • 1Department of Psychiatry, Academic Medical Center, University of Amsterdam, Amsterdam, Netherlands
  • 2The Netherlands Institute for Neuroscience, An Institute of the Royal Netherlands Academy of Arts and Sciences, Amsterdam, Netherlands

A commentary on
The Brain Basis for Misophonia

by Kumar, S., Tansley-Hancock, O., Sedley, W., Winston, J. S., Callaghan, M. F., Allen, M., et al. (2017). Curr. Biol. 27, 527–533. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2016.12.048

In their recent study Kumar et al. (2017) investigated misophonia using functional and structural MRI. Their results show increased activity and connectivity between brain regions included in the “salience network,” notably the anterior insular cortex (AIC), which is involved in the processing of emotions. Heart rate and galvanic skin responses were increased during misophonic triggers, suggesting increased physical arousal. They postulate having found “the brain basis for misophonia” and that “misophonia is attributed to particular sounds based on abnormal activation and functional connectivity of AIC.” While we congratulate them on carrying out this novel study, we believe the above-quoted sentences are overstated because of important shortcomings.

Firstly, it is unclear whether participants in this study genuinely suffered from misophonia. Even though misophonia is not yet mentioned in the International Classification of Diseases (ICD) or Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (DSM), there is increasing evidence delineating it as a distinct psychiatric disorder with specific and well-defined diagnostic criteria (Edelstein et al., 2013; Johnson et al., 2013; Schröder et al., 2013; Schneider and Arch, 2015). Patients should be (1) obsessed with specific sounds, (2) experience intense anger, and consequently (3) avoid cue-related situations.

In the study of Kumar, subjects were selected based on one single unvalidated questionnaire, no structured psychiatric assessment was made, no involvement of psychiatrist or psychologist is presented, participants were not screened in a face-to-face interview, no information is provided about co-morbidity (subjects may have suffered from hyperacusis or borderline personality disorder, for example), neither use of psychopharmaceuticals or recreational drugs is mentioned. The authors did not make use of the aforementioned diagnostic criteria of misophonia. Hence, it is uncertain whether the brain differences may be attributed to misophonia.

Secondly, it is unclear whether the triggered emotions involve anger, which is essential for the diagnosis of misophonia (Schröder et al., 2013). In this study, participants were only asked to rate their annoyance, not their anger. The observed brain differences may therefore be correlated to general annoyance but not to specific anger. Again, in many other psychiatric disorders patients feel annoyed by sounds, e.g., in autism spectrum disorder and ADHD.

Thirdly, it is unclear because of the design of the study asking participants to visit the lab on two separate occasions, to what extent they have been sensitized to the sounds by repeated exposure. Sensitization implies that repeated exposure to a certain stimulus increases the response.

It cannot be ruled out that participants of the misophonic group were sensitized to the trigger sounds presented in this study.

In conclusion, though the study has triggered interesting discussions about the validity of misophonia, any statements about the “brain basis for misophonia” are, in our opinion, premature since there is lack of evidence that the participants really suffer from misophonia and that the triggered emotions are misophonia related.

Author Contributions

All authors listed, have made substantial, direct and intellectual contribution to the work, and approved it for publication.

Conflict of Interest Statement

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

References

Edelstein, M., Brang, D., Rouw, R., and Ramachandran, V. S. (2013). Misophonia: physiological investigations and case descriptions. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 7:296. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2013.00296

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Johnson, P. L., Webber, T. A., Wu, M. S., Lewin, A. B., Murphy, T. K., and Storch, E. A. (2013). When selective audiovisual stimuli become unbearable: a case series on pediatric misophonia. Neuropsychiatry 3, 569–575. doi: 10.2217/npy.13.70

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kumar, S., Tansley-Hancock, O., Sedley, W., Winston, J. S., Callaghan, M. F., Allen, M., et al. (2017). The brain basis for misophonia. Curr. Biol. 27, 527–533. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2016.12.048

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schneider, R. L., and Arch, J. J. (2015). Letter to the editor: potential treatment targets for misophonia. Gen. Hosp. Psychiatry 37, 370–371. doi: 10.1016/j.genhosppsych.2015.03.020

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schröder, A., Vulink, N., and Denys, D. (2013). Misophonia: diagnostic criteria for a new psychiatric disorder. PLoS ONE 8:e54706. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0054706

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: misophonia, anger, disgust, fMRI, sound

Citation: Schröder A, van Wingen G, Vulink NC and Denys D (2017) Commentary: The Brain Basis for Misophonia. Front. Behav. Neurosci. 11:111. doi: 10.3389/fnbeh.2017.00111

Received: 27 February 2017; Accepted: 22 May 2017;
Published: 02 June 2017.

Edited by:

Pietro Pietrini, IMT School for Advanced Studies Lucca, Italy

Reviewed by:

Claudio Gentili, University of Padua, Italy
Tomaso Vecchi, University of Pavia, Italy

Copyright © 2017 Schröder, van Wingen, Vulink and Denys. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) or licensor are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Arjan Schröder, a.e.schroder@amc.nl
Damiaan Denys, d.denys@amc.nl

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.