Skip to main content

REVIEW article

Front. Behav. Neurosci., 08 September 2015
Sec. Individual and Social Behaviors
This article is part of the Research Topic The translational value of non-mammalian in vivo models of executive function and cognition View all 8 articles

Non-mammalian models in behavioral neuroscience: consequences for biological psychiatry

\r\nCaio MaximinoCaio Maximino1Rhayra Xavier do Carmo SilvaRhayra Xavier do Carmo Silva1Sullen de Nazar Santos da SilvaSuéllen de Nazaré Santos da Silva1Laís do Socorro dos Santos RodriguesLaís do Socorro dos Santos Rodrigues1Hellen BarbosaHellen Barbosa1Tayana Silva de Carvalho,Tayana Silva de Carvalho2,3Luana Ketlen dos Reis LeoLuana Ketlen dos Reis Leão3Monica Gomes Lima,Monica Gomes Lima1,3Karen Renata Matos OliveiraKaren Renata Matos Oliveira3Anderson Manoel Herculano*Anderson Manoel Herculano3*
  • 1Laboratório de Neurociências e Comportamento, Departamento de Morfologia e Ciências Fisiológicas, Campus VIII – Marabá, Universidade do Estado do Pará, Marabá, Brazil
  • 2Universität Duisburg-Essen, Essen, Germany
  • 3Laboratório de Neurofarmacologia Experimental, Instituto de Ciências Biológicas, Universidade Federal do Pará, Belém, Brazil

Current models in biological psychiatry focus on a handful of model species, and the majority of work relies on data generated in rodents. However, in the same sense that a comparative approach to neuroanatomy allows for the identification of patterns of brain organization, the inclusion of other species and an adoption of comparative viewpoints in behavioral neuroscience could also lead to increases in knowledge relevant to biological psychiatry. Specifically, this approach could help to identify conserved features of brain structure and behavior, as well as to understand how variation in gene expression or developmental trajectories relates to variation in brain and behavior pertinent to psychiatric disorders. To achieve this goal, the current focus on mammalian species must be expanded to include other species, including non-mammalian taxa. In this article, we review behavioral neuroscientific experiments in non-mammalian species, including traditional “model organisms” (zebrafish and Drosophila) as well as in other species which can be used as “reference.” The application of these domains in biological psychiatry and their translational relevance is considered.

Introduction

The use of model organisms is paramount in the behavioral neurosciences and its ramifications into biological psychiatry. Among these organisms, rats, and mice are still the most widely used, although other fields of the neurosciences use different species. This almost exclusive focus on rodents is problematic from the epistemic point of view, and behavioral neuroscience could profit from the inclusion of more species in its analysis. In fact, since the comparative aspect is a strong argument in favor of using non-human animals in behavioral research, the expansion of species is important to strengthen that argument. Moreover, insights gained from other species—including model organisms and “reference species” (Striedter et al., 2014)—can help understand what is generalizable and what is species-specific. In this Review, we highlight the role of model organisms and reference species in the behavioral neurosciences and discuss some advances, advantages and disadvantages of using a few choice species in behavioral research with consequences for biological psychiatry.

Model Organisms in Behavioral Neuroscience

“the fish is a frog…is a chicken…is a mouse”

(Kimmel, 1989)

Foundational Issues

Before the discussion about model organisms advances, some definitions must be settled. In the life sciences, the term “model organism” refers to a species that is used in an attempt to understand particular biological phenomena (Fields and Johnston, 2005). From an epistemic point of view, a model organism acts as a stand-in for other organisms:

[…] model organisms are always taken to represent a larger group of organisms beyond themselves, and hence rely on very particular types of claims about their (potential) representational scope. […] The actual relationships between the model organism and this larger group often are very ill-articulated in the earliest stages of model organism work, and do not necessarily hinge on particular claims about genetic conservation or precise knowledge of the phylogenetic placement of a particular organisms in relationship to others (Ankeny and Leonelli, 2011, p. 318).

Another important epistemic characteristic of model organisms is that, differently from what Ankeny and Leonelli (Ankeny and Leonelli, 2011; Leonelli and Ankeny, 2013) call “experimental organisms,” they target a wide range of systems and processes occurring in living organisms, including genetics, development, physiology, behavior, evolution, and ecology. In this sense, model organisms are “material analogies” (Hesse, 1963) which, although not faithfully mirroring their target, represent other organisms at the most basic levels (Ankeny and Leonelli, 2011; Leonelli and Ankeny, 2013).

The concept of model organism is not specific to any field of the life sciences; the concept of “animal model,” in contrast, is more common in the behavioral sciences. This term refers not to an organism, but to the conjunction of apparatuses and manipulations used to represent a given behavioral (dys)function in a different species than the target (van der Staay, 2006; Nelson, 2012). Animal models are most widely used in the fields of behavior genetics, biological psychiatry, experimental psychopathology, and neuropsychopharmacology, where they are used to generate biological (physiological or genetic) hypotheses regarding psychiatric disorders, to investigate the psychological aspects of the disorder, or to screen for potential psychiatric drugs (McKinney and Bunney, 1969; Willner, 1991; Wright, 2002).

LaFollette and Shanks (1995) defined two categories of animal models: causal analog models, with which experimenters test causal mechanisms in a model and then extrapolate, by analogy, to the human condition; and hypothetical analogical models, which have the function of generating novel hypotheses. LaFollette and Shanks (1994, 1995) argued that the assumption of interchangeability between non-human animals and humans is weak at best, and therefore animal models are better used to generate novel hypotheses. However, it has been argued that models with predictive validity and construct validity show biological and translational relevance, and therefore can be used as causal analog models (van der Staay, 2006).

While the majority of animal models use model organisms as subjects—especially rats and mice (Griebel and Holmes, 2013) –, that is not a strict requirement to model a given psychiatric disorder. In fact, the definition of an animal model as an experimental preparation is neutral with regard to the extension of what is being modeled and to which species is targeted (Wright, 2002). Nonetheless, most assumptions of model organism research also inform animal modeling (Kalueff et al., 2008; Maximino et al., 2010c; de Mooij-van Malsen et al., 2011; Kas et al., 2011; Stewart and Kalueff, 2014). While the aspects of pharmacological isomorphism, ethological consistency, and symptomatology are central to reasoning with animal models (Willner, 1991; van der Staay, 2006; Kalueff et al., 2007; Belzung and Lemoine, 2011), “[t]he arguments for evolutionary relationships, genetic homologies, and physiological similarities also are part of the epistemic infrastructure that supports the use of animal models” (Nelson, 2012, p. 16). In particular, some authors (Blanchard and Blanchard, 1988; Maximino et al., 2010c; de Mooij-van Malsen et al., 2011; Kas et al., 2011; Stewart and Kalueff, 2014) advocate the use of species-specific behavioral and physiological phenotypes as endpoints for assessing the effects of manipulations across multiple species. This, of course, necessitates the model to be embedded in a theoretical framework which will guide the choice of endpoints to be analyzed and validated (McNaughton and Zangrossi, 2008; Maximino et al., 2010c).

Expanding the Breadth of Species

The choice of species is usually guided by practical advantages—throughput, fertility, developmental speed, availability of genomic and transcriptomic data—and, to a great extent, to the existence of well-established research communities and data availability (Fields and Johnston, 2005; Ankeny and Leonelli, 2011; Leonelli and Ankeny, 2012). In the neurosciences, further criteria are the amenability to genetic manipulations and relative simplicity of the nervous system. These advantages are more extensive in molecular neurosciences, including neurogenetics and developmental neuroscience, and historically gave rise to a handful of model organisms—viz, humans, macaques, rats, mice, zebrafish, Xenopus, Drosophila and C. elegans. Extensive databases of gene expression for flies [http://flybase.org/], frogs [http://www.xenbase.org], humans [http://human.brain-map.org/], mice [http://mouse.brain-map.org/], worms [http://www.wormbase.org/], and zebrafish [http://zfin.org/] are already available, allowing for the comparison of basal expression levels in different brain areas. These data can be used in comparative neuroanatomy to refine homology propositions (Engert, 2014; Mitra, 2014; Striedter et al., 2014), which is essential for circuit approaches in behavioral neuroscience. These data can also be used to mine for the neuroanatomical localization of psychiatric disorder-related genes in different model organisms (de Mooij-van Malsen et al., 2011; Kas et al., 2011). These neuroinformatic approaches exemplify the power of current data tools available for well-established model organisms in the neurosciences.

In addition to studying well-established model species, behavioral neuroscience could benefit from focusing on other, carefully chosen species to amplify the field of discovery. Striedter et al. (2014) used the term “reference species” to define “carefully selected species from phylogenetically widely spaced vertebrate and invertebrate groups” for comparative neuroanatomy. These species would then serve two purposes: as substrates for broad comparisons across all animals to identify nervous system fundamentals and as anchors for more fine-grained analyses within their particular taxon to assess the meaning of variation in whole brains and functional subsystems (Striedter et al., 2014, p. 5).

While the authors' focus was comparative and evolutionary neuroanatomy, their conclusions and recommendations can be extended to behavioral neuroscience insofar as both areas can profit from comparing taxa to infer how variations in one domain (gene expression, connectivity, activation patterns) relates to variation in behavior (Engert, 2014; Mitra, 2014; Striedter et al., 2014). Striedter et al. (2014) suggest that well-established model organisms be included among these species due to the availability of resources for their study, but point that other reference species should also be selected based on a few criteria. Thus, reference species are not “models for some other species, but […] a basis for comparisons that may reveal both similarities and differences” (Striedter et al., 2014, p. 5) The criteria for choosing a reference species are not established a priori, but might include phylogenetic position (Figure 1) and accumulation of significant data and methodological developments (Hale, 2014; Striedter et al., 2014); the ultimate goal is to allow the emergence of a comprehensive understanding of specific behavioral functions in different species and its specific relationships to brain structure and activity (Striedter et al., 2014; Hale, 2014).

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Phylogenetic context of some “reference species” that can be used in behavioral neurosciences. The figure underlines the position and phylogenetic distances of a few species in relation to each other, and suggest how this information can be used to inform the selection of organisms for research. For example, while most research in the behavioral neurosciences is performed using rodents, selecting a species from an outgroup—for example, chicks or lizards—could inform researchers on evolutionary conservation of biobehavioral traits in mammals. These informations complement the usual criteria for species choice (ease of reproduction, rapid generation time, etc.) and the availability of behavioral and physiological assays.

The assumption of conservation is also best studied in a comparative framework. While this assumption is essential for projecting even rodent data toward humans, it has not been tested for most behavioral domains which are relevant for biological psychiatry (Panksepp et al., 2002; Pollen and Hofmann, 2008). It has been argued that, at least in anxiety research, construct validity is dependent on the assumption of evolutionary conservation (Maximino et al., 2010c); thus, to increase construct validity in animal models, testing the predictions of this assumption is highly desirable (McNaughton and Zangrossi, 2008). In this sense, while a common practice in behavioral neuroscience, using data from a single species (no matter how basal) to infer the ancestral state is unfeasible (Garland, 2001). As we will see along this article, while currently the most basal vertebrate used in biological psychiatry is zebrafish, a comparison with closely-related species (e.g., goldfish or guppy) as well as more basal vertebrates (e.g., sharks or lampreys) is necessary to establish the ancestral state of a given neurobehavioral trait in vertebrates. Studying species phylogenetically located near the origin of vertebrates, such as acorn worms and amphioxus, could also be useful “for comparisons that span both vertebrate and invertebrate nervous systems” (Striedter et al., 2014). As a result, behavioral neuroscience could clearly profit from the adoption of other, non-model, “reference” species in the same way that neuroanatomy had.

Behavioral Research in Non-mammalian Species: Relevance to Biological Psychiatry

Important as it may be for comparative and evolutionary neuroscience, cross-species research in the behavioral neuroscience is still incipient. Some approaches have been proposed which include comparative research in order to clarify genotype-phenotype relationships, thereby increasing the translational value of animal behavior in relation to human neuropsychiatric disorders (Kalueff et al., 2008; LaPorte et al., 2008; de Mooij-van Malsen et al., 2011; Kas et al., 2011). The main rationale is based on the utility of model organisms in each specific domain. For example, rodent stress responses are driven by corticosterone, while zebrafish and humans use cortisol (Steenbergen et al., 2011). In addition to this critical but utilitarian aspect, the expansion toward “reference species”—especially non-mammalian species—could allow for uncovering conserved and divergent mechanisms underlying pathogenesis (Kalueff and Stewart, 2014; Stewart and Kalueff, 2014). An example of conservation at the genomic and functional levels is the regulation of neurosteroidogenesis in relation to the anxiolytic effect of fluoxetine in both zebrafish (Wong et al., 2013) and mice (Pinna, 2010); an example of divergence is the serotonergic system, which show both duplicated genes and nuclei other than the raphe a multiplication of nuclei in fish in relation to rodents but is equally involved in anxiety and fear (Lillesaar, 2011; Herculano and Maximino, 2014). In both cases, a derivative endophenotype (Kalueff and Stewart, 2014) results from domain interaction that is conserved or divergent.

In what follows, we will exemplify conserved (or at least convergent) phenotypes that can be found in non-mammalian model organisms and in proposed reference species, as well divergent phenotypes which are found in model and reference species which could be useful for neuroscientific research. While not attempting to be a comprehensive review of every possible use of non-mammalian species in biological psychiatry, some specific examples were chosen based on phylogenetic position, data availability, and ease of use in laboratory settings. In most of the cases, pharmacological correlates are the strongest argument for the use of that species and behavioral model, which underlines the need to reinforce construct validity (the theoretical network behind the model) and face validity (the neurobehavioral isomorphism between the model and the target pathology).

Teleost Fish Behavior

Anxiety-like Behavior in Cyprinids

Human psychiatric disorders associated with anxiety, stress and phobic states result from abnormalities in neurobiological processes, inducing characteristic behavioral responses (Panksepp, 2004, 2006; LeDoux, 2012). Recently, diverse behavioral tests for anxiety, fear, and stress were proposed using teleost fish, among which zebrafish and goldfish stand out (Maximino et al., 2010d; Stewart et al., 2011a; Steenbergen et al., 2011). The potential of these species as model organisms for the analysis of genetic and biological mechanisms of fear and anxiety in vertebrates is beginning to be realized (Kalueff et al., 2014a; Stewart et al., 2014b). Moreover, other fish species—including goldfish and guppies—are being used in behavioral neuroscience, expanding the comparative breadth of teleost fish in this field (Hall et al., 2014).

Zebrafish is a small cyprinid that has long been used as a model organism in developmental biology and genetics (Signore et al., 2009). Its physiology is comparatively simple, making the species adequate for high-throughput investigation in pharmacology, toxicology, behavioral genetics and pharmacogenomics (Kokel and Peterson, 2008; Gerlai, 2010, 2015; Kalueff et al., 2014b). Zebrafish also presents neuroanatomical landmarks and neurotransmitter systems which are very similar to those observed in mammals (Rinkwitz et al., 2011; Kalueff et al., 2014b).

Due to the many inherent advantages of zebrafish as a model organism—including low cost, easy manipulation and upkeep in relation to other vertebrate models, and 70–80% genetic homology with humans (Table 1)—zebrafish are increasingly useful in fields such as behavior genetics (Gerlai, 2003; Norton and Bally-Cuif, 2010). Although the degree of genetic homology with humans is not as high as in rodents, it is favorable in comparison with other genetically tractable organisms such as Drosophila melanogaster and Caernohabditis elegans (Kokel and Peterson, 2008). Despite the rising popularity of zebrafish in biological psychiatry (Griebel and Holmes, 2013; Stewart et al., 2014a), behavioral analyses still lack a more torough study (Gerlai, 2003, 2010, 2014, 2015).

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Advantages and disadvantages of zebrafish and goldfish as models in behavioral neuroscience.

Experiments using zebrafish larvae have been widely used, given that complex behavior appears in this species from 4 to 5 days post fertilization (dpf); these behaviors include prey capture, avoidance, phototaxis, and thigmotaxis, which are readily quantifiable in automated setups (Ahmad et al., 2012). Many studies suggest that avoidance and thigmotaxis can be used as measures of anxiety in zebrafish, and stimulus control of avoidance and thigmotaxis in zebrafish larvae is similar to that of anxiety in humans (Richendrfer et al., 2012).

Zebrafish possess all the “classic” neurotransmitters found in vertebrates (Rinkwitz et al., 2011), and its neuroendocrine system allows for different physiological stress responses (Steenbergen et al., 2011; Pavlidis et al., 2015). In adult animals, two important methods—the novel tank test (Cachat et al., 2010) and the light/dark test (Maximino et al., 2010b)—have been proposed to analyze anxiety-like behavior. In the first case, the animal is introduced to a novel environment, typically adjusting its spatial distribution in a “diving” response that tends to habituate with decreasing novelty and is accompanied by freezing and erratic swimming (Egan et al., 2009; Cachat et al., 2010, 2011; Wong et al., 2010). In the light/dark test, the apparatus is composed of a preferred black compartment and a non-preferred white compartment, and the preference for the black compartment (which does not habituate; Maximino et al., 2010a) is accompanied by risk assessment and, when the animal enters the white compartment, erratic swimming, thigmotaxis and freezing (Maximino et al., 2014d). Both tests show considerable pharmacological isomorphism (Cachat et al., 2011; Stewart et al., 2011b,c; Maximino et al., 2014d) and have been successfully adapted to other species, including goldfish (Maximino et al., 2007, 2010b; Kang et al., 2011c; Nakamachi et al., 2014).

The GABAergic system, a key regulatory element in experimental and clinical anxiety, is well-documented in zebrafish; its inhibition produces anxiogenic-like effects in zebrafish (López-Patiño et al., 2008; Ellis et al., 2012), and positively modulating GABAergic signaling with benzodiazepines or pentobarbital leads to sedation (Kokel et al., 2010; Stewart et al., 2011c; Gupta et al., 2014) and/or anxiolysis (Bencan et al., 2009; Maximino et al., 2011b; Stewart et al., 2011b,c; Vada et al., 2015). In goldfish, a similar pattern is also observed (Matsuda et al., 2011b; Nakamachi et al., 2014).

Serotonin (5-HT) mechanisms were strongly implicated in anxiety in humans and non-human animals (Maximino, 2012). Although the serotonergic system is not anatomically or genetically conserved (Lillesaar, 2011; Maximino et al., 2013a; Herculano and Maximino, 2014), there is some evidence for functional conservation. Extracellular 5-HT levels are positively correlated with anxiety-like behavior in the light/dark test and negatively correlated in the novel tank test (Maximino et al., 2013c). Moreover, the anxiolytic-like effect of drugs targeting different systems is associated with their ability to decrease serotonin turnover in the zebrafish brain (Maximino et al., 2014d). Drugs which increase serotonin levels increase anxiety-like behavior in the light/dark test (Maximino et al., 2013c; Herculano and Maximino, 2014) and decrease it in the novel tank test (Maximino et al., 2013c; Stewart et al., 2013). In the light/dark test, exposure to an aversive olfactory stimulus (alarm substance) greatly increases anxiety-like behavior, an effect which is blocked by acute fluoxetine treatment but not by the 5-HT1A antagonist WAY 100,635 (Maximino et al., 2014c); WAY 100,635 was able to block the fear-induced analgesia caused by alarm substance (Maximino et al., 2014c), and low doses of this drug blocked the effect of alarm substance on behavior in the novel tank test (Nathan et al., 2015). Methysergide, a non-selective 5-HT receptor antagonist, also blocked the effects of alarm substance in the novel tank test (Nathan et al., 2015). A role for the 5-HT1A receptor was also observed in the light/dark test, where antagonists show an anxiolytic-like effect (Maximino et al., 2013c), and in the novel tank test, where antagonists can either increase (Nowicki et al., 2014) or decrease (Maximino et al., 2013c) anxiety-like behavior. Finally, antagonists at the 5-HT2 and 5-HT3 receptors increase anxiety-like behavior in the novel tank test (Nowicki et al., 2014), while antagonists at the 5-HT1B receptor decrease anxiety-like behavior in this test, but not in the light/dark test (Maximino et al., 2013c).

The cholinergic system is emerging as another important target for the pharmacological modulation of anxiety-like behavior in zebrafish. The acetylcholinesterase inhibitor physostigmine has been shown to decrease bottom-dwelling in the novel tank test, an effect consistent with reduced anxiety (Cho et al., 2012). Nicotine, an antagonist at nicotinic cholinergic receptors, produces ample and consistent anxiolytic-like responses in the novel tank test (Levin et al., 2007), while in the aquatic plus-maze no effect was observed (Sackerman et al., 2010). The effects of nicotine in the novel tank test are mediated by the α7 and α4β2 nicotinic receptors, as antagonists for these receptors block the anxiolytic-like effects of nicotine (Bencan and Levin, 2008).

Adenosine and its receptors were implicated in the pathogenesis of anxiety-like behavior (Ruby et al., 2011). Caffeine is a non-selective adenosine rececptor antagonist, and has consistently been shown to increase anxiety-like behavior in zebrafish (Egan et al., 2009; Wong et al., 2010; Maximino et al., 2011a,b). These effects of caffeine on the light/dark test are mimicked by drugs which block the adenosine A1 receptor, but not A2 receptors, suggesting a participation of the first, but not the latter, in zebrafish anxiety (Maximino et al., 2011a). Interestingly, the A1 receptor has also been shown to protect against the convulsive actions of pentylenotetrazole (Siebel et al., 2015), while both receptors have been implicated in the amnestic effects of scopolamine in the inhibitory avoidance test in zebrafish (Bortolotto et al., 2014). Treatment with IB-MECA, an agonist at A3 receptors (which so far have not been described in zebrafish) reduces dark preference in a nitric oxide- and serotonin-dependent manner, while the reduction of bottom-dwelling is dependent on nitric oxide but not serotonin (Maximino et al., 2014b).

These results suggest the potential of adult zebrafish for studying the mechanisms of anxiety-like behavior and discovering novel drug targets. In general, a good balance between demonstration of pharmacological isomorphism and seeking novel targets is seen in the zebrafish anxiety literature. Two other examples illustrate the potential of this species in describing the substrates of anxiety-like behavior.

In the first study, authors capitalized on the knowledge regarding the role of fibroblast growth factor (FGF) receptors on the development of the zebrafish brain (Thisse and Thisse, 2005). spiegeldanio, a mutant with reduced FGF1A receptor function, was shown to have increased aggressive and exploratory behavior and decreased neophobia and anxiety (Norton et al., 2011). These animals show reduced dual specificity phosphatase enzyme dusp6 and phosphorylated extracellular signal-regulated kinase in the inferior lobe of the hypothalamus (Norton et al., 2011). While a decreased expression of the isoform A of the serotonin transporter in the raphe is also observed, treatment with fluoxetine does not rescue the behavioral phenotype (Norton et al., 2011); instead, histamine N-methyltransferase is upregulated in the brains of spiegeldanio mutants, which show decreased histamine levels in the preoptic area and raphe nucleus (Norton et al., 2011). Indeed, treatment with the histamine N-methyltransferase inhibitor tacrine rescues not only the hypo-histaminergic profile but also the behavioral syndrome associated with reduced FGF1A receptor signaling (Norton et al., 2011).

In another study, knockdown of otpa, a gene which is duplicated in zebrafish in relation to vertebrates is used to circumvent the lethality of homozygous null mutations in mice (Amir-Zilberstein et al., 2012). OTP is a homeodomain protein that is highly expressed in the neuroendocrine hypothalamus (Blechman et al., 2007), suggesting a role in regulating stress responses. In zebrafish, two isoforms are present; null mutants for otpa show normal basal expression of CRF in the brain, but exposure to a stressor does not increase CRF expression in the mutants (Amir-Zilberstein et al., 2012). These animals also show less bottom-dwelling in the novel tank test (Amir-Zilberstein et al., 2012). A series of experiments demonstrated that Otp associates phosphorylated cAMP response element-binding protein (pCREB) to recruit the crf and a2bp1 promoters in response to stressors; the latter promoter leads to the expression of a short variant of the PAC1 receptor for the pituitary adenylate cyclase-activating peptide (PACAP) (Amir-Zilberstein et al., 2012). Gene knockdown of the short variant of PAC1 leads to increased behavioral and CRF responses to stressor, and overexpression of the short form in otpa-positive neurons in the hypothalamus increases basal and stimulated CRF expression (Amir-Zilberstein et al., 2012).

PACAP and the PAC1 receptor have been implicated in post-traumatic stress disorder (Ressler et al., 2011). An interesting correlation with those results is the observation of the behavioral and neurochemical effects of PACAP intracerebroventricular (i.c.v.) injections in goldfish, a closely-related cyprinid (Maruyama et al., 2006). This peptide suppresses food intake and induces a significant increase in the expression of crf mRNA; both effects are blocked by an CRF1 receptor antagonist (Matsuda et al., 2006a). Moreover, PACAP also decreases locomotion in goldfish, albeit at a concentration higher than that needed to produce an anorexigenic effect (Matsuda et al., 2006a). While this locomotor effect is not necessarily suggestive of stress, an important research program has emerged on the role of hypothalamic neuropeptides in stress and feeding responses in goldfish (Matsuda, 2009; Matsuda et al., 2011a).

Some hypothalamic regions responsible for stress responses are also involved in food intake, and orexigen and anorexigen peptides produced and secreted from those areas regulate feeding. Some regulatory peptides involved in the organization of energetic homeostasis, such as ghrelin, orexin, galanin, thyrotropin-releasing hormone, PACAP, vasoactive intestinal peptide (VIP), and CRF, modulate different types of behavior after central or peripheral administration in mammals and teleost fish (Matsuda, 2009; Matsuda et al., 2011a). Some of these peptides have been linked to psychiatric disorders in humans (Figure 2). The i.c.v. and intraperitoneal injections of PACAP and VIP inhibits feeding and locomotion in goldfish (Matsuda et al., 2006a); PACAP injections also increase the expression of CRF mRNA in the brain, an effect which is mimicked by excessive feeding (Maruyama et al., 2006). While CRF has been implicated in anxiety and stress (Takahashi, 2001), there is also some evidence for a role in appetite control. CRF injections in the brain, but not in the periphery, is also anorexinogenic in goldfish (De Pedro et al., 1993). However, CRF injections increase locomotion (Maruyama et al., 2006) and produce an anxiogenic-like effect in the novel tank test (Matsuda et al., 2013).

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Neuropeptides involved in anxiety disorder in clinical samples (Steckler, 2008). Peptides marked with asteriks (*) have been investigated in goldfish (Carassius auratus) psychomotor activity, anxiety, or feeding assays. AVP, vasopressin; ANP, atrial natriuretic peptide; CCK, cholecystokinin; CRF, corticotropin-releasing factor; DYN, dynorphin; END, β-endorphin; GAD, generalized anxiety disorder; NPY, neuropeptide Y; PD, panic disorder; PTSD, post-traumatic stress disorder; SST, somatostatin.

The central effects of CRF on feeding in goldfish are independent on cortisol, as injections of this hormone do not alter food intake (de Pedro et al., 1997). CRF receptors also mediate the anorexinogenic effects of α-melanocyte stimulating hormone (α-MSH), since CRF antagonists block the effects of i.c.v. α-MSH injections on feeding but α-MSH antagonists do not block the effects of i.c.v. CRF (Matsuda et al., 2008). Subsequent studies demonstrated that the anorexigenic actions of both CRF and α-MSH are blocked by a gonadotrophin-releasing hormone (GnRH) type I receptor antagonist, suggesting that these stress peptides induce the release of GnRH2 which ultimately mediates their effects on feeding (Kang et al., 2011a).

Other neuropeptides involved in feeding have been shown to modulate locomotor activity and/or anxiety in goldfish. I.c.v. injections of ghrelin increase locomotion, while intraperitoneal injections decrease it; the intraperitoneal injection is also unable to alter dark preference (Kang et al., 2011c). Further studies using an acylated peptide demonstrated that this isoform increases food intake and increases locomotor activity when injected i.c.v. or intraperitoneally, while des-acyl ghrelin has no effect on locomotion, suggesting that the acylation is necessary for the psychomotor effects of ghrelin (Matsuda et al., 2006b).

The hypocretin/orexin system has been implicated in zebrafish feeding and arousal (Volkoff and Peter, 2006; Chiu and Prober, 2013). Orexin expression is upregulated in the goldfish hypothalamus after food deprivation and downregulated by intraperitoneal glucose injections (Nakamachi et al., 2006). I.c.v. injection of orexin A, but not orexin B, increases food intake in goldfish, while an anti-orexin antibody decreases food intake (Nakamachi et al., 2006). Orexin A, but not orexin B or an anti-orexin antibody, increases locomotion (Nakamachi et al., 2006), dark preference, and bottom-dwelling (Nakamachi et al., 2014) when injected i.c.v.; these effect are blocked by pre-treatment with a OX1R antagonist (Nakamachi et al., 2014).

Cholecystokinin (CCK) is a gut polypeptide involved in stimulating the digestion of fat and protein. Post-translational modifications produce hormones with variable number of amino acids, including CCK4 (which acts primarily in the central nervous system, with little effect on the gastrointestinal tract) and CCK8 (which acts both centrally and peripherally) (Fink et al., 1998). In goldfish, i.c.v. CCK8 administration is anorexigenic and increases pomc mRNA levels, but not crf, in the brain (Kang et al., 2010). Pretreatment with CRF antagonists block this anorexigenic effect of CCK8 (Kang et al., 2010). Intraperitoneal CCK8 also produces an anorexigenic effect (Kang et al., 2010) that is blocked by pretreatment with the NMDA receptor antagonist MK-801 (Kang et al., 2011b).

Neuropeptide Y (NPY) has been implicated in feeding and, recently, has been identified as a potential target for the treatment of anxiety disorders (Garner et al., 2009). I.c.v. injection of NPY reduces locomotor activity and dark preference in goldfish (Matsuda et al., 2011a) and increase food intake (López-Patiño et al., 1999; Miura et al., 2006). The orexigenic effect of NPY is blocked by pretreatment with Y1 receptor antagonists (López-Patiño et al., 1999; Miura et al., 2006), while the anxiolytic-like effect is not (Matsuda et al., 2011a). However, the injection of an Y4 receptor antagonist mimicks the effects of NPY (Matsuda et al., 2011a), suggesting that feeding and anxiety are mediated by different mechanisms in the goldfish brain. Interestingly, chronic fluoxetine treatment increases npy expression in the zebrafish brain (Wong et al., 2013).

In addition to feeding-related peptides, stress-related peptides were also tested in goldfish for their effects on stress, locomotion, and feeding. Octadecaneuropeptide is an endozepine which acts as an agonist at the translocator protein 18 kDa (formerly known as peripheral benzodiazepine receptor) and at a metabotropic receptor and an inverse agonist at benzodiazepine binding sites at central GABAA receptors (Gamier et al., 1994). I.c.v. ODN administration increases locomotor activity and dark preference in goldfish, an effect which is blocked by pretreatment with benzodiazepine antagonists, but not by antagonists at the metabotropic receptor, suggesting that its anxiogenic-like and locomotor effects are mediated by the benzodiazepine binding site (Matsuda et al., 2011b). ODN and derivative peptides also inhibit food intake in this species, an effect which is blocked by metabotropic receptor antagonists but not flumazenil, suggesting mediation by the metabotropic receptor but not by the benzodiazepine binding site (Matsuda et al., 2007). Interestingly, i.c.v. ODN injection increases proopiomelanocortin (pomc) mRNA levels, but not crf, and its anorexigenic effects are blocked by pretreatment with either a α-MSH antagonist or a CRF antagonist (Matsuda et al., 2010).

Aversive Conditioning in Goldfish

In addition to these experiments analyzing the behavioral effects of neuropeptides in goldfish, other experiments also analyzed learning and memory functions of specific brain regions. These experiments capitalize on the increased brain size of goldfish in relation to zebrafish, making ablation experiments and electrode implantation much easier in the first than in the latter cyprinid (Table 1). While functional considerations are not normally used to establish homology claims, these experiments were crucial in advancing a second wave of discussion regarding the limbic telecephalon of teleost fish (Butler and Hodos, 2005).

The amygdaloid nuclei possess a variety of neurobiological roles, among which its participation in emotional behavior and learning are intensely studied (LeDoux, 2012). Some amygdaloid nuclei, including the basolateral portions (BLA), have been demonstrated to be central for avoidance learning in rodents (Choi et al., 2010; Lázaro-Muñoz et al., 2011). In this form of instrumental conditioning, the animal associates a response (usually shuttling in a box) with avoidance of an aversive consequence, such as an electric shock, that has been signaled by a sound or light stimulus. Two-factor theory proposes that, in avoidance conditioning, the subject first learns that the signaling stimulus predicts the aversive stimulus (Pavlovian conditioning) and then learns that a particular behavior (avoidance) causes termination of both the signal and the aversive stimulus (operant conditioning) (Maia, 2010). In this sense, the conditioned fear to the signal drives learning of the avoidance response.

The medial pallium (MP) of teleost fish has been proposed to be homologous to the mammalian BLA (Maximino et al., 2013b). Interestingly, lesions in this region, but not in the lateral pallium (LP) impair two-way avoidance learning in goldfish (Portavella et al., 2002, 2004a,b; Portavella and Vargas, 2005; Vargas et al., 2012). Interestingly, the same region shows increased cfos expression in zebrafish after exposure to the light/dark test (Lau et al., 2011; von Trotha et al., 2014). LP lesions, on the other hand, impair spatial learning, but not active avoidance conditioning, except when a time interval is added between the signal and the electric shock (“trace conditioning”) (Portavella et al., 2002; Portavella and Vargas, 2005).

As is the case with avoidance learning, the medial pallium is also involved in conditioned taste aversion (CTA), which involves the tendency to avoid the ingestion of substances that were previously associated with visceral discomfort. CTA is impaired by BLA lesions in rodents (Reilly and Bornovalova, 2005), and is characterized by long interstimulus intervals, high stimulus specificity, and fast acquisition—in fact, CTA can be established by a single trial (Garcia et al., 1955). The medial pallium, especially at precommissural levels, receives gustatory information from the diencephalic tertiary gustatory nucleus in the Rainbow trout Onchorynchus mykiss (Folgueira et al., 2003), althought there is no evidence that general visceral information reach this region (Yoshimoto and Yamamoto, 2010). Goldfish are able to successfully avoid a gustatory stimulus which was followed by lithium chloride; whole-telencephalon ablation and lesions in the medial pallium, but not in the lateral pallium or cerebellum, impair the acquisition of CTA in this species (Martín et al., 2011). Interestingly, when the gustatory stimulus was paired with an electrical shock, a conditioned bradycardia develops that is not impaired by telencephalic ablation (Martín et al., 2011).

The pharmacological or neurochemical bases of these effects are unknown. However, recent experiments using microinjection of drugs in the telencephalon suggest a role for the glutamate-nitric oxide pathway in that process (Xu et al., 2003, 2009). Goldfish were trained in an active avoidance paradigm similar to that proposed by Portavella and colleagues (Vargas et al., 2012). Escape responses were defined as shuttling made after the onset of both the light signal and the electric shock, while avoidance responses were made after onset of light signal but before the electric shock. The injection of D-AP5, an antagonist at glutamatergic N-methyl-D-aspartate (NMDA) receptors, before training did not alter the performance of escape responses, but significantly impaired the acquisition of avoidance responses (Xu et al., 2003). When this drug was injected after training, no amnesic effect was observed, suggesting a participation of this receptor in the acquisition phase, but not in the consolidation of the aversive memory (Xu et al., 2003). In a second set of experiments, Xu et al. (2009) injected nitric oxide synthase or guanylate cyclase inhibitors, impairing the production of nitric oxide or cyclic guanosine monophospate (cGMP), respectively. Injection of these drugs before training impaired the acquisition of avoidance responses, without effects on escape responses. Interestingly, microinjection of these drugs in the medial pallium imediately after training also impaired avoidance responses, suggesting a participation of the nitric oxide-cGMP system on the consolidation of the active avoidance memory (Xu et al., 2009).

Both microinjection and ablation experiments underline the main advantage of goldfish in relation to zebrafish (Table 1): while zebrafish certainly has important advantages from the point of view of genetics and molecular toolboxes, the size of goldfish allows more easily for “classical” neuroscience techniques (drug microinjection, structure lesion, in vivo electrophysiological recordings). Indeed, while microinjection techniques are being introduced in zebrafish (Kizil and Brand, 2011; Barbosa et al., 2012), they are much easier to make in an animal with a bigger brain and body. Thus, experiments with goldfish could complement results found in zebrafish, especially regarding localized interventions.

Aggression in the Siamese Fighting-fish

In aggression research, the Siamese fighting-fish Betta splendens was widely used during the 1970s and 1980s due to the well-described characteristics of the appetitive and consummatory aspects of its aggressive behavior (Simpson, 1968). Moreover, as is the case with other ornamental fish species, maintenance, and housing costs are low, and keeping fighting-fish in laboratories is relatively simple (Clotfelter et al., 2007; Kania et al., 2012). Moreover, a stereotaxic atlas of the fighting-fish telencephalon has been constructed, facilitating the description of neural systems underlying behavior (Marino-Neto and Sabbatini, 1988).

Betta splendens usually exhibit salient aggressive behavior toward conspecifics, including a characteristic appetitive element—the aggressive display –, which is characterized by the extension of operculae, extension of medial, and caudal fins, and an intensification of body color (Simpson, 1968). When the adversary counter-displays, an escalation ensues, resulting in attacks that include offensive biting (Bronstein, 1983, 1994). Betta males also present aggressive display when exposed to a mirror (Miley and Burack, 1977); mirror presentation can therefore be used in lieu of a conspecific since it can simulate and aggressive encounter without harming the animals, since no difference between the strength of aggressive display toward a mirror or a conspecific (Miley and Burack, 1977).

While studying the aggressive display is useful for analyzing aggression, other techniques can also be used to assess the level of aggressive motivation in fighting-fish. Tapping on the classical studies of rodent motivation using runways, some studies analyzed the aggressive readiness by conditioning fighting-fish to swim through an “aquatic runway” in order to gain access to a conspecific or a mirror stimulus. It has been demonstrated that the level of aggressive motivation (as assessed by the time taken to reach the target area) is associated with combat readiness (number of displays) (Hogan and Bols, 1980). “Social reinforcement”—that is, instrumental behavior controlled by the opportunity for aggressive displays—has been used for a wide variety of applications in fighting-fish; interestingly, Betta splendens display self-control for mirror access, choosing delayed access to a mirror stimulus with longer duration instead of immediate access to a short duration of mirror presentation (Collins, 2008). These observations could complement work on the zebrafish three-choice serial reaction time task (Parker et al., 2012, 2013a,b) to build a research program on impulsive control disorders.

There is some evidence that Betta splendens possess significant cerebral laterality—the division of cognitive functions between both brain sides—in relation to other anabantoid fish, given that they demonstrate an eye use preference in aggressive interactions (Clotfelter and Kuperberg, 2007). Lateralized individuals also present higher group cohesion and better performance in spatial tasks in relation to non-lateralized individuals (Clotfelter and Kuperberg, 2007).

Serotonin (5-HT) has been implicated in the modulation of aggressive behavior in different species (Takahashi et al., 2011), including fish (Herculano and Maximino, 2014). Acute treatment with low doses of fluoxetine decreases the duration of aggressive display (Lynn et al., 2007; Dzieweczynski and Hebert, 2012; Forsatkar et al., 2013), suggesting an inhibitory role for 5-HT in fighting-fish aggression. However, neither the 5-HT synthesis inhibitor para-chlorophenylalanine nor the 5-HT precursor L-tryptophan changed display behavior (Clotfelter et al., 2007), suggesting that phasic, but not tonic, 5-HT controls aggressive behavior. Consistently with that hypothesis, intramuscular acute injections of 5-HT and 8-OH-DPAT (an agonist at 5-HT1A and 5-HT7 receptors) decrease the duration and readiness of aggressive displays, while the 5-HT1A receptor antagonist did not produce an effect (Clotfelter et al., 2007). The effects of chronic treatment with fluoxetine are mixed, with some authors describing decreases in aggressive displays (Kania et al., 2012) while other authors described no effect (Clotfelter et al., 2007).

Honeybees: Aversive Control and Impulsivity

Honeybees comprise the genus Apis, which comprises seven species and 44 subspecies (Martín et al., 2011). While distributed in the whole world, honeybees appear to have originated in South and Southeast Asia and Africa (Engel, 1999). The Western honey bee (Apis mellifera) had its genome fully sequenced in 2006 (http://hymenopteragenome.org/beebase/), and at least since the description of the waggle dance by Karl von Frisch it has been proposed as a model organism for ethology and comparative cognition (Smith et al., 2000; Whitfield et al., 2006). Moreover, as eusocial species, honeybees are increasingly being used to understand how the social environment can shape behavior, including social learning, predator cues, and social decision making (Menzel, 1983). A comparative overview of the advantages and disadvantages of using honeybees in neuroscience can be found in Table 2.

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Advantages and disadvantages for Drosophila melanogaster and Apis mellifera as models in behavioral neuroscience.

Honeybees, especially Apis mellifera, are increasingly demonstrating their potential as models in behavioral studies, following the inclusion of invertebrates in neurobehavioral research (Leadbeater and Chittka, 2007). In particular, honeybees are capable of complex decision making, presenting cognitive biases (Wilson-Sanders, 2011) and self-control (Bateson et al., 2011).

Honeybees have long been shown to be sensitive to aversive control (Hunt, 2007; Curran and Chalasani, 2012). Abramson (1986) demonstrated that honeybees quickly acquire aversive control in punishment, escape, and avoidance contingencies when an aversive odor (formic acid) is used. In a semi-naturalistic setting, honeybees trained to discriminated between two differently colored targets quickly acquire avoidance responses when response to one of the targets is associated with an electric shock in the proboscis (Abramson, 1986). Honeybees also present a sting extension reflex, which is exhibited when the animal is subjected to noxious stimuli; this reflex can be conditioned so that bees learn to extend their sting in response to odorants previously paired with an electric shock (Vergoz et al., 2007; Tedjakumala and Giurfa, 2013). Interestingly, dopamine and 5-HT (but not octopamine or 20-hydroxyecdisone) have been shown to decrease the conditioned sting extenstion response, while 5-HT2 receptor antagonists increase responsiveness (Vergoz et al., 2007; Tedjakumala et al., 2014).

While these results suggest a conserved role for specific 5-HT receptors in simple aversive control, other, more complex phenotypes can also be observed. In an interesting set of experiments, Bateson et al. (2011) first trained honeybees to associate a two-component odor mixture with either a reward (sucrose solution), punishment (quinine solution) or a less valuable reward (diluted sucrose solution); after training, animals were presented with unreinforced (test) trials in which three different odors mixtures, with intermediate concentrations of the mixture of the original compounds, were presented in addition to the two original odors. Animals responded to the original odor associated with the reward by extending the proboscis, while the odor that was associated with punishment or a less rewarding consequence did not elicit proboscis extension. Intermediate concentrations of the two-odor mixtures produced a mixed response, with mixtures with a higher concentration of the punishment-associated odors eliciting less proboscis extension responses. Interestingly, when animals were vigorously shaken for 60 s before testing, responding toward these concentrations was further decreased, suggesting a “cognitive bias”—that is, agitated honeybees classified ambiguous stimuli as predicting punishment (Bateson et al., 2011). Moreover, shaken bees showed decreased levels of dopamine, octopamine and serotonin in the hemolymph (Bateson et al., 2011). The authors suggested that this demonstration of a state-dependent modulation of categorization in honeybees has more in common with vertebrate behavior than previously thought.

Given that negative affective states such as anxiety and depression are associated with increased punishment expectancy, greater attention to potential threats, and a tendency to interpret ambiguous stimuli as threats, this “cognitive bias” demonstrates that this behavior can be used to model some aspects of psychiatric disorders. The neural mechanisms underlying negative cognitive biases are so far unexplored, but the observation that both dopamine and serotonin are diminished suggests similarities with the mechanisms underlying aversive control. However, this study also demonstrated a decrease in circulating octopamine levels in agitated bees—an effect which is difficult to reconcile with findings in vertebrates, since octopamine is found only in invertebrates. Moreover, in vertebrates the role of 5-HT on aversive control is highly dependent on receptor subtype and site of action, with 5-HT2 receptors increasing aversive responsiveness in the amygdala and decreasing it in the periaqueductal gray (Maximino, 2012; Zangrossi and Graeff, 2014).

In addition to aversive control and cognitive bias, important experiments demonstrated that honeybees are able to self-regulate their behavioral choices and make an economic choice for a delayed and bigger reward in opposition to an immediate small reward (Cheng et al., 2002). Experiments showing the capacity for “self-control” are important to understand impulsive choice, sometimes indexed by an alteration in the optimal delay discounting behavior described above (Arce and Santisteban, 2006). Cheng et al. (2002) described an experiment in which honeybees were trained to choose between a delayed sweet reward and an immediate less sweet reward, choosing the first over the latter. Food deprivation increases impulsive choice and brain dopamine levels (Mayack and Naug, 2015). In addition, successive negative contrast has been demonstrated in bumble bees (Bombus impatiens)—that is, animals adjust their choice toward less effort when the reward value is downshifted (Waldron et al., 2005). Although pharmacological, genetic, and biochemical experiments are still much needed to elucidate the isomorphism of these responses to vertebrate systems, they point to an exciting possibility of using bees to study impulse control and its social modulation.

Fruit Fly Exploratory Behavior and Aggression

Drosophila melanogaster are widely used in different fields of the biomedical sciences, especially in genetics (Ankeny and Leonelli, 2011). Drosophila share the broad actions of essential neurochemical substrates (specific receptors, signaling enzymes and proteins, neurotransmitters systems) that are involved in emotional behavior (Schafer, 2002; Iliadi, 2009). Genetic techniques produced Drosophila mutants for genes associated with neurodegenerative disorders, making the species suitable for studying the pathological bases of these diseases (Muqit and Feany, 2002). These advances were made possible by the characteristics of fruit flies which turned them into a central model organisms in genetics (Table 2): low maintenance cost, short generation time (c. 2 weeks), high fertility, and, of course, the availability of a research community and an extensive toolbox to manipulate gene expression in this species (Muqit and Feany, 2002; Schafer, 2002; van Alphen and van Swinderen, 2013).

As is the case with most model organisms, the bottleneck for its introduction in the behavioral neurosciences was the availability of neurobehavioral assays (Iliadi, 2009; van Alphen and van Swinderen, 2013). Insects such as Drosophila and honeybees exhibit some defensive behaviors which can be interpreted as anxiety-like and/or fear-like. In Drosophila, centrophobism/thigmotaxis has been proposed to represent anxiety- or fear-like behavior (Besson and Martin, 2004; Iliadi, 2009).

Thigmotaxis was first observed in Drosophila as an after-effect of diethylether anesthesia, although it is present at basal levels in non-treated flies (Götz and Biesinger, 1985). It was observed that, when exploring a novel circular arena, flies avoid the arena center, an effect which is exacerbated after anesthesia (Götz and Biesinger, 1985). It was later observed that the preference for the arena boundaries is not controlled only by center avoidance (centrophobism) or wall preference due to tactile stimulation (thigmotaxis), and that flies prefer sheter-like environments (alcoves or dark corners), but only after the initial boundary exploration waned (Soibam et al., 2012). In both cases, an explicit “wall-following” is controlled by a complex combination of variables, including exploratory behavior (“curiosity”-driven) and avoidance due to the novelty of the environment. This is consistent with Montgomery's (1954) proposal that both approach and avoidance control exploratory behavior, the basis for the use of exploratory behavior in rodent models of anxiety.

This “wall-following” behavior is controlled by the mushroom bodies (MBs), one of the most well-studied central brain-like structures in the Drosophila brain. Hydroxyurea ablation of the MBs diminishes wall-following behavior, an effect that is replicated by genetic disruption of the synaptic transmission in γ lobes, but not in α/β lobes (Besson and Martin, 2004). Mutations that affect the cyclic adenosine monophosphaste (cAMP) pathway also decrease centrophobism, suggesting a participation of the cAMP-PKA pathway in that behavior (Lebreton and Martin, 2009). Finally, it has been shown that neurons expressing a substance P-like peptide in the fan-shaped body of the central complex are involved in wall-following, as genetic ablation of these cells increases this behavior (Kahsai et al., 2010).

These results highlight an important feature of Drosophila research: due to the genetic tractability of the model, manipulations which the use of Gal4/UAS lines to drive gene expression in a cell-specific manner (Aso et al., 2009). These powerful techniques, which are increasingly being used in zebrafish (Scott et al., 2007), allow for the expression of photosensitive proteins such as channelrhodopsin and halorhodopsin, as well as proteins such as tetanus toxin light chain (TeTxLC) or KillerRed in zebrafish or the temperature-sensitive protein Shibire in Drosophila, to inactivate a specific circuit or cell type expressed in a specific region. These powerful tools are increasingly being used in circuit neuroscience in genetically tractable organisms. On the other hand, the “allure” of high-technology research produces a tendency, in such organisms, for fundamental “low-tech” research to be ignored. In the case of Drosophila wall-following behavior, basic pharmacological research using clinically effective drugs has not yet been made, and therefore wall-following/thigmotaxis/centrophobism lacks pharmacological isomorphism.

Another important set of behaviors which have been studied in Drosophila and have consequences for biological psychiatry is aggression (Chen et al., 2002; Iliadi, 2009; van Alphen and van Swinderen, 2013). As is the case with most vertebrate species, fly aggressive behavior follows a pattern which includes behaviors without physical contact with the opponent, such as wing threat displays, and actions with direct physical contact, including fencing, holding, boxing, and tussling (Chen et al., 2002; Zwarts et al., 2012). Wing threat displays are threatening postures directed toward other males before fat charges. It is not known whether these display postures, as appetitive elements, represent an emotion such as anger (Iliadi, 2009).

Serotonin has been implicated in the control of aggressive behavior in vertebrates (Miczek et al., 2007; Carrillo et al., 2009). In Drosophila, the role of 5-HT in aggression is unclear, as 5-HT treatment does not alter aggression (Baier et al., 2002), and artificial selection for discordant levels of aggressive behavior does not alter the expression of genes involved in serotonergic signaling (Dierick and Greenspan, 2006). Nonetheless, pharmacological or genetic elevation of the serotonergic tonus increases aggression, and genetic silencing of 5-HTergic circuits blocks the effects of pharmacological induction while sparing aggression (Dierick and Greenspan, 2007). Expression of temperature-sensitive dTrpA1 channels in 5-HTergic neurons allows for the acute activation of these cells, accelerating the escalation of fights (Alekseyenko et al., 2010). In general, then, acute increases in 5-HT levels increase aggression, but it is not known whether this mechanism is present physiologically.

Reptile Neuroethology

While zebrafish and Drosophila are important reference species mainly due to their usefulness as model organisms, reptiles are important due to their position in the vertebrate phylogeny; reptile species are positioned at the anamniote-amniote transition (Figure 1), representing an important evolutionary junction that is mostly underrepresented in behavioral neuroscience.

Among reptile species, anole lizards are among the most widely studied in evolutionary ecology and ethology (Greenberg, 2002; Lovern et al., 2004). The genus Anolis is a diverse and widespread New World taxon that includes the green anole Anolis carolinensis, a small, diurnal, insectivorous lizard from the United States southeast that is convenient to observe and easy to maintain in the laboratory (Greenberg, 2002). Behavioral inventories (ethograms) that emphasize social dynamics have been produced for anole lizards (Greenberg and Noble, 1974; Greenberg, 1977) and other reptiles. Antipredator behavior has also been described in different lizard species, and include tonic immobility (Edson and Gallup, 1972; Hennig, 1977, 1979; Santos et al., 2010; Maximino et al., 2014a), flight (Hennig, 1979; Maximino et al., 2014a), modifications of exploratory behavior (Greenberg, 2002) and of refuge use (López et al., 2005). The advantages of using small reptiles as laboratory models (Table 3) include its high availability in the wild (allowing for the establishment of laboratory colonies which are continuously replenished with wild stock to obtain field-relevant laboratory studies), the variety of life history traits between species, their close phylogenetic relationship with birds (forming the most basal extant amniotes), and ease of maintenance and cost-effectiveness without sacrificing ecological relevance (Lovern et al., 2004). Moreover, brain atlases are also present for A. carolinensis (Greenberg, 1982), Gallotia galloti (Del Corral et al., 1990) and Gekko gekko (Wang et al., 2008).

TABLE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. Advantages and disadvantages for Anolis carolinensis and Gallus gallus as models in behavioral neuroscience.

Socially submissive green anoles show conspicuous physiological and behavioral alterations at both the immediate (catecholamine surges, increased plasmatic levels of corticosterone and α-melanocyte-stimulating hormone (MSH), increased serotonergic activity in the midbrain, hindbrain, hippocampus and nucleus accumbens, body color alterations) and the long term (reduced androgen tone, elevated corticosterone and α-MSH levels, decreased dopaminergic activity in the hindbrain and midbrain, decreased courtship and perch selection behavior). While the endocrinology of social stress responses is well-established in Anolis and other reptile species, a more careful observation of behavioral alterations is still lacking (Greenberg, 2002; Summers et al., 2003; Øverli et al., 2007).

Lizards are prey to many predators and therefore are subject to intense predation pressure, which is largely responsible for the development of multiple defensive strategies (Greenberg, 2002; López et al., 2005; Thaker et al., 2009). Leal and Rodríguez-Robles (1995, 1997) analyzed antipredatory responses in two different anole lizard species (Anolis cristatellus and Anolis cuvieri) during encounters with its natural predator, the snake Alsophis portoricensis, and observed 13 different behavioral responses, of which approximately half are also involved in social interactions in these species. A stereotypical sequence of behavioral acts was observed, depending on the phases of predator-prey interaction. Understanding the underlying mechanisms which organize these behavioral displays is essential to systematization and characterization of defensive responses these animals, as well to facilitate comparative studies relating to the establishment of animal models.

As an example, Machado et al. (2007) analyzed flight responses of Tropidurus montanus, another squamate lizard, to feigned attacks. The experimenters observed that maximum flight distance did not differ between the sexes; the presence of a neighbor did not affect flight for males; and that males with neighbors maximized the time of flight when compared to males without a neighbor. Moreover, body length was not predictive of flight behavior. During capture, T. montanus relied on attempted escapes, cloacal discharges, threat displays, tail breakage and tonic immobility (TI), in no particular order. TI duration is also increased in A. carolinensis by the presence of a simulated predator (Hennig, 1977) and is higher at shorter distances from the simulated predator (Hennig et al., 1976).

Hennig (1979) studied the effects of the physical environment, time in captivity, and distance between potential predator and prey on defensive behaviors Anolis carolinensis. In addition to TI duration, flight latency was also recorded in order to determine if environmental factors affect these defensive behaviors similarly. The results revealed that the immediate testing environment is more important when a potential predator was nearby, and housing is more important at greater distances and after the first few days of adaptation to the new conditions. TI seems to be responsive to both changes in testing environment and time in captivity, while flight latency was only sensitive to the type of housing environment during captivity.

Many variables such as those mentioned above can potentially change defense responses, but it remains unclear how individual animals from the same population, sex, age, reproductive status, and other similar conditions of risk and costs, differ in their antipredator behavior. López et al. (2005) suggested that the propensity to take risks in a homogeneous group of adult male Iberian rock lizards (Lacerta monticola) varies in a “shy–bold” continuum and observed than the possible source of variation in antipredator behavior might also be related to small subtle differences in morphology, body condition, and health.

Tonic immobility has been equated with thanatosis, a secondary defense mechanism in which the animal displays “death-feigning” in response to external stimuli. This peculiar defensive behavior is assumed by many different lizard species (Edson and Gallup, 1972; Leal and Rodríguez-Robles, 1995, 1997; Machado et al., 2007; Santos et al., 2010). Immobility responses to acute and intense threat have been proposed to be a part of acute stress responses (Bracha, 2004; Moskowitz, 2004) and peritraumatic tonic immobility is predictive of development of post-traumatic stress disorder (Maia et al., 2011; Pires and da Costa Maia, 2013). Thus, tonic immobility has a potential translational value for biological psychiatry. In that sense, Maximino et al. (2014a) demonstrated that the Brazilian wall lizard Tropidurus oreadicus has a well-organized pattern of defensive behaviors that emerge after tonic immobility: immediately following the cessation of TI lizards initiate either freezing or (more frequently) a pattern of flight behavior that, in a circular arena, is characterized as “circling”; after that, animals return to careful exploration of the environment, employing thigmotaxis/centrophobic behavior and tongue-flicking (Maximino et al., 2014a). Importantly, these authors observed that this pattern is amenable to pharmacological dissociation, with panicolytic drugs (alprazolam, imipramine) decreasing TI duration and post-TI freezing or circling, but not exploratory (risk assessment-like) behavior, while anxiolytic drugs (diazepam) increased tongue-flicking and decreased thigmotaxis (Maximino et al., 2014a).

A careful consideration of the different antipredator behavioral strategies employed by different lizard species in naturalistic, semi-naturalistic, or experimental settings suggest that, as is the case with mammals, reptiles adjust their behavior in relation to “predatory imminence continua” (Fanselow and Lester, 1988). In safe environments such as a nest or a burrow (low predatory imminence), animals do not exhibit defensive behaviors, resuming their normal activity. In a novel environment, predatory imminence increases because the probability of encountering a predator increases, and the animal engages pre-encounter defensive behaviors—including thigmotaxis, tongue-flicking, air-licking, and posture changes—and body color changes associated with crypsis. When a threat is present, predator imminence increase further and the animal engages in post-encounter defensive behaviors (freezing). If post-encounter defensive strategies have failed (i.e., during contact with the predator), the animal switches to circa-strike defensive behaviors (flight/threat, tonic immobility, body thrashing, tongue-bunch, tail autotomy). These sequences have been observed in lizards both in the wild (Machado et al., 2007) and in laboratory environments (Hennig et al., 1976; Leal and Rodríguez-Robles, 1995, 1997; Maximino et al., 2014a), and have consequences for the development of novel models of anxiety disorders.

The Chick Separation Stress Model

As is the case with other model organisms in developmental biology, laboratory chicks are used for a variety of reasons: availability and low price of fertilized eggs all the year round; fast development, with a duration similar to that of the mouse (21 days); absence of placenta and therefore of maternal effects after egg laying; and well-described developmental genetics (Table 3).

The chick separation stress paradigm exploits the strong attachment response of neonate fowl (Gallus gallus) and its typical distress vocalizations (DVoc) when socially isolated to model separation anxiety (Panksepp, 2011). Social separation initially produces an anxiety-like phase with high rates of DVoc, peaking in the first 3 min, followed by a decrease to about 50% of the initial rate within 10–25 min (Feltenstein et al., 2004; Sufka et al., 2006). While isolation for 3 min also produces stress-induced analgesia, hypothermia and ventral recumbency posturing (Sufka and Weed, 1994), the first effect is primarily mediated by novelty and not social isolation (Feltenstein et al., 2002). These phases can be pharmacologically dissociated, with anti-panic compounds (benzodiazepines, imipramine, clonidine, meprobamate, pentobarbital) attenuating DVocs in the first phase and antidepressant compounds (imipramine, maprotiline, fluoxetine) attenuating the second phase (Feltenstein et al., 2004; Sufka et al., 2006; Warnick et al., 2009). Moreover, social separation also increases plasmatic corticosterone levels (Feltenstein et al., 2003; Sufka et al., 2006). Interestingly, plasma corticosterone levels are higher when animals are isolated for 5–15 min, the period in which the first phase peaks, and this response is attenuated by isolation periods longer than 20 min (Sufka et al., 2006). Dopamine metabolism is increased in the telencephalon and diencephalon after 30 min (Hamasu et al., 2012). Interleukin-6, on the other hand, is elevated only by isolation for 120 min (Warnick et al., 2009). Chicks socially isolated for 5 min also show a “cognitive bias” in which they show higher responsiveness to aversive ambiguous cues, while animals isolated for 60 min are more responsive to both aversive and appetitive ambiguous cues (Salmeto et al., 2011); both forms of cognitive bias are attenuated by imipramine treatment, while clonidine did not alter responsiveness to any cue (Hymel and Sufka, 2012).

The amount of effort directed to the pharmacological and construct validation of the chick separation stress model is not a coincidence, as this test has mainly been used as a biobehavioral assay (Willner, 1991) to identify pharmacological compounds. In this sense, at least two target systems have been evaluated successfully. A participation of the opioidergic system was suggested by rodent social behavior assays (Burgdorf et al., 2011); in chicks, a participation of μ-opioid receptors is suggested by the observation that an agonist, DAMGO, attenuates DVocs in a 3 min isolation, while drugs acting at δ, κ or orphanin receptors are not effective (Warnick et al., 2005); the opposite effect is not observed by treatment with the non-selective opioid antagonist naloxone or the μ-opioid receptor antagonist CTOP, suggesting a phasic rather than tonic modulation. A role for NMDA receptors is suggested by a series of experiments in which intracerebroventricular injection of L-cysteine (Yamane et al., 2009a), glutathione (Yamane et al., 2007) and NMDA (Yamane et al., 2009b) blocks DVocs during a 10 min isolation test; in addition to this effect, these drugs also induce hypnotic effects. These results are surprising, since activation of the NMDA receptor induces anxiety- and panic-like behavior in rodents (Bergink et al., 2004) and fish (Herculano et al., 2015), and has been suggested to be mediated by NMDA-evoked GABA release.

In general, the chick separation stress test has proved to be an interesting behavioral assay to screen for potential anti-panic and antidepressive drugs, being able to detect hits and dismiss drugs which produce false positives in other tests (Sufka et al., 2006; Warnick et al., 2009). Moreover, the neuroendocrine markers associated with stress follow the timecourse expected for acute stress, although their projection to anxiety and mood disorders is still blurred. While very little is known about the brain structures which mediate behavior in this model, the avian brain is well-described (The Avian Brain Nomenclature Consortium, 2005), and Gallus is a traditional model organism in developmental biology (Fields and Johnston, 2005). Thus, as is the case with zebrafish (Norton, 2013; Stewart et al., 2014b), Gallus gallus is well-positioned to produce models in developmental psychopathology. The case of the chick separation stress test is particularly interesting from this point of view, since about one third of adult patients with separation anxiety disorder developed it during their childhood (Black and Grant, 2014) and infant behavioral inhibition, a temperamental trait that is characterized by high reactivity to environmental and social novelty, is predictive of adult anxiety disorders (Fox et al., 2005).

Conclusions

The exclusive reliance of behavioral neurosciences on a small number of species is counterproductive; as a result, different research groups are starting to focus their efforts on a comparative perspective, using non-mammalian organisms in research. As can be inferred from this Review, the most readily transferable models using non-mammalian species are in the domains of anxiety, impulse control and aggression. While our knowledge of the neurobehavioral systems involved in disorders in these domains is far from complete, mammalian data suggests that a number of neurotransmitter and neuromodulator systems are dysfunctional in these pathologies (Figure 3). In Tables 13 and throughout this Review, the degree of conservation in these systems has been discussed. In some cases, a seemingly paradoxical situation is seen, in which the neurotransmitter system is not fully conserved from the molecular point of view, but from a functional perspective the degree of conservation is higher (Figure 3). While this state of affairs can simply represent the current limitations in the tools used to address these questions, it is possible that they represent discontinuities in the evolutionary histories of these traits. Future research—especially of the comparative kind—will answer these open questions.

FIGURE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 3. Apparent paradox in the divergence and conservation of monaminergic systems in mammals, fish, and insects. Receptor and enzyme sequences are not conserved (including gene duplication in the case of teleost fish), and the brain nuclei containing monoaminergic neurons are differently distributed throughout the brain in mammals, fish, and insects, but functions appear to be relatively well-conserved.

Each organism brings advantages and disadvantages, some of which we have discussed in the present Review. As is the case with rodents, no single species can be used to answer all research questions, and there is no such thing as a “perfect laboratory organism.” Ideally, all behavioral research using these species should be inserted into a comparative framework, assessing the same variables on different species and/or trying to extrapolate findings in different species; nonetheless, while at the present moment most research using non-mammalian organisms relies on data produced in rodents, the inverse is not necessarily true. The widespread adoption of other species is advantageous from a comparative and epistemic point of view, but still needs to go a long way to impact the field.

Conflict of Interest Statement

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Acknowledgments

AH is the recipient of a CNPq productivity grant (304130/2011-7). Universal-CNPq (grant number:479089/2013-2) and CAPES Pro-Amazonia (grant number:3288/2013).

References

Abramson, C. I. (1986). Aversive conditioning in honeybees (Apis mellifera). J. Comp. Psychol. 100, 108–116. doi: 10.1037/0735-7036.100.2.108

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ahmad, F., Noldus, L. P. J. J., Tegelenbosch, R. A. J., and Richardson, M. K. (2012). Zebrafish embryos and larvae in behavioural assays. Behaviour 149, 1241–1281. doi: 10.1163/1568539X-00003020

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Alekseyenko, O. V, Lee, C., and Kravitz, E. A. (2010). Targeted manipulation of serotonergic neurotransmission affects the escalation of aggression in adult male Drosophila melanogaster. PLoS ONE 5:e10806. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0010806

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Amir-Zilberstein, L., Blechman, J., Sztainberg, Y., Norton, W. H. J., Reuveny, A., Borodovsky, N., et al. (2012). Homeodomain protein Otp and activity-dependent splicing modulate neuronal adaptation to stress. Neuron 73, 279–291. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2011.11.019

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ankeny, R. A., and Leonelli, S. (2011). What's so special about model organisms? Stud. Hist. Philos. Sci. Part A 42, 313–323. doi: 10.1016/j.shpsa.2010.11.039

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Arce, E., and Santisteban, C. (2006). Impulsivity: a review. Psicothema 18, 213–220.

PubMed Abstract

Aso, Y., Grübel, K., Busch, S., Friedrich, A. B., Siwanowicz, I., and Tanimoto, H. (2009). The mushroom body of adult Drosophila characterized by GAL4 drivers. J. Neurogenet. 23, 156–172. doi: 10.1080/01677060802471718

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Baier, A., Wittek, B., and Brembs, B. (2002). Drosophila as a new model organism for the neurobiology of aggression? J. Exp. Biol. 205, 1233–1240.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Barbosa, A. Jr., Maximino, C., de Souza Fim Pereira, A., Wolkers, C. C. B., Alves, F. L., Ide, L. M., et al. (2012). “Rapid method for acute intracerebroventricular injection in adult zebrafish,” in Zebrafish Protocols for Neurobehavioral Research, eds A. V. Kalueff and A. M. Stewart (New Jersey, NJ: Humana Press), 323–330.

Bateson, M., Desire, S., Gartside, S. E., and Wright, G. A. (2011). Agitated honeybees exhibit pessimistic cognitive biases. Curr. Biol. 21, 1070–1073. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2011.05.017

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Belzung, C., and Lemoine, M. (2011). Criteria of validity for animal models of psychiatric disorders: focus on anxiety disorders and depression. Biol. Mood Anxiety Disord. 1:9. doi: 10.1186/2045-5380-1-9

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bencan, Z., and Levin, E. D. (2008). The role of α7 and α4β2 nicotinic receptors in the nicotine-induced anxiolytic effect in zebrafish. Physiol. Behav. 95, 408–412. doi: 10.1016/j.physbeh.2008.07.009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bencan, Z., Sledge, D., and Levin, E. D. (2009). Buspirone, chlordiazepoxide and diazepam effects in a zebrafish model of anxiety. Pharmacol. Biochem. Behav. 94, 75–80. doi: 10.1016/j.pbb.2009.07.009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bergink, V., van Megen, H. J. G. M., and Westenberg, H. G. M. (2004). Glutamate and anxiety. Eur. Neuropsychopharmacol. 14, 175–183. doi: 10.1016/S0924-977X(03)00100-7

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Besson, M., and Martin, J.-R. (2004). Centrophobism/thigmotaxis, a new role for the mushroom bodies in Drosophila. Int. J. Neurobiol. 62, 386–396. doi: 10.1002/neu.20111

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Black, D. W., and Grant, J. E. (2014). DSM-5 Guidebook. The essential companion to the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, 5th Edn. Arlington, VA: American Psychiatric Publishing.

Google Scholar

Blanchard, D. C., and Blanchard, R. J. (1988). Ethoexperimental approaches to the biology of emotion. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 39, 43–68. doi: 10.1146/annurev.ps.39.020188.000355

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Blechman, J., Borodovsky, N., Eisenberg, M., Nabel-Rosen, H., Grimm, J., and Levkowitz, G. (2007). Specification of hypothalamic neurons by dual regulation of the homeodomain protein Orthopedia. Development 134, 4417–4426. doi: 10.1242/dev.011262

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bortolotto, J. W., de Melo, G. M., Cognato, G., de, P., Vianna, M. R. M., and Bonan, C. D. (2014). Modulation of adenosine signaling prevents scopolamine-induced cognitive impairment in zebrafish. Neurobiol. Learn. Mem. 118, 113–119. doi: 10.1016/j.nlm.2014.11.016

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bracha, H. S. (2004). Freeze, flight, fight, fright, faint: adaptationist perspectives on the acute stress response spectrum. CNS Spectr. 9, 679–685.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Bronstein, P. (1994). On the predictability, sensitization and habituation of aggression in male bettas (Betta splendens). J. Comp. Psychol. 108, 45–57. doi: 10.1037/0735-7036.108.1.45

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bronstein, P. M. (1983). Onset of combat in male Betta splendens. J. Comp. Psychol. 97, 135–139. doi: 10.1037/0735-7036.97.2.135

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Burgdorf, J., Panksepp, J., and Moskal, J. R. (2011). Frequency-modulated 50 kHz ultrasonic vocalizations: a tool for uncovering the molecular substrates of positive affect. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 35, 1831–1836. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2010.11.011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Butler, A. B., and Hodos, W. (2005). Comparative Vertebrate Neuroanatomy. Evolution and Adaptation, 2nd Edn. Hoboken, NJ: John Wiley & Sons, Inc.

Google Scholar

Cachat, J., Stewart, A., Grossman, L., Gaikwad, S., Kadri, F., Chung, K. M., et al. (2010). Measuring behavioral and endocrine responses to novelty stress in adult zebrafish. Nat. Protoc. 5, 1786–1799. doi: 10.1038/nprot.2010.140

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cachat, J., Stewart, A., Utterback, E., Hart, P., Gaikwad, S., Wong, K., et al. (2011). Three-dimensional neurophenotyping of adult zebrafish behavior. PLoS ONE 6:e17597. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0017597

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Carrillo, M., Ricci, L. A., Coppersmith, G. A., and Melloni, R. H. Jr. (2009). The effect of increased serotonergic neurotransmission on aggression: a critical meta-analytical review of preclinical studies. Psychopharmacology (Berl). 205, 349–368. doi: 10.1007/s00213-009-1543-2

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chen, S., Lee, A. Y., Bowens, N. M., Huber, R., and Kravitz, E. A. (2002). Fighting fruit flies: a model system for the study of aggression. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 99, 5664–5668. doi: 10.1073/pnas.082102599

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cheng, K., Peña, J., Porter, M. A., and Irwin, J. D. (2002). Self-control in honeybees. Psychon. Bull. Rev. 9, 259–263.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Chiu, C. N., and Prober, D. A. (2013). Regulation of zebrafish sleep and arousal states: current and prospective approaches. Front. Neural Circuits 7:58. doi: 10.3389/fncir.2013.00058

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cho, H., Lee, C.-J., Choi, J., Hwang, J., and Lee, Y. (2012). Anxiolytic effects of an acetylcholinesterase inhibitor, physostigmine, in the adult zebrafish. Animal Cells Syst. (Seoul). 16, 198–206. doi: 10.1080/19768354.2011.642084

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Choi, J.-S., Cain, C. K., and LeDoux, J. E. (2010). The role of amygdala nuclei in the expression of auditory signaled two-way active avoidance in rats. Learn. Mem. 17, 139–147. doi: 10.1101/lm.1676610

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Clotfelter, E. D., and Kuperberg, E. S. (2007). Cerebral lateralization and its relationship to phylogeny and aggression in anabantoid fishes. Brain. Behav. Evol. 69, 169–175. doi: 10.1159/25200196985

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Clotfelter, E. D., O'Hare, E. P., McNitt, M. M., Carpenter, R. E., and Summers, C. H. (2007). Serotonin decreases aggression via 5-HT1A receptors in the fighting fish Betta splendens. Pharmacol. Biochem. Behav. 87, 222–231. doi: 10.1016/j.pbb.2007.04.018

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Collins, C. M. (2008). Siamese Fighting Fish (Betta splendens) Show Self-control for Access to a Mirror. Ph.D. thesis, University of Montana, Missoula.

Curran, K. P., and Chalasani, S. H. (2012). Serotonin circuits and anxiety: what can invertebrates teach us? Invertebr. Neurosci. 12, 81–92. doi: 10.1007/s10158-012-0140-y

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Del Corral, J. M., Miralles, A., Nicolau, M. C., Planas, B., and Rial, R. V. (1990). Stereotaxic atlas for the lizard Gallotia galloti. Prog. Neurobiol. 34, 185–196. doi: 10.1016/0301-0082(90)90011-5

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

de Mooij-van Malsen, A. J. G., Vinkers, C. H., Peterse, D. P., Olivier, B., and Kas, M. J. H. (2011). Cross-species behavioural genetics: a starting point for unravelling the neurobiology of human psychiatric disorders. Prog. Neuropsychopharmacol. Biol. Psychiatry 35, 1383–1390. doi: 10.1016/j.pnpbp.2010.10.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

De Pedro, N., Alonso-Gómez, A. L., Gancedo, B., Delgado, M. J., and Alonso-Bedate, M. (1993). Role of corticotropin-releasing factor (CRF) as a food intake regulator in goldfish. Physiol. Behav. 53, 517–520. doi: 10.1016/0031-9384(93)90146-7

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

de Pedro, N., Alonso-Gómez, A. L., Gancedo, B., Valenciano, A. I., Delgado, M. J., and Alonso-Bedate, M. (1997). Effect of alpha-helical-CRF[9-41] on feeding in goldfish: involvement of cortisol and catecholamines. Behav. Neurosci. 111, 398–403. doi: 10.1037/0735-7044.111.2.398

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dierick, H. A., and Greenspan, R. J. (2006). Molecular analysis of flies selected for aggressive behavior. Nat. Genet. 38, 1023–1031. doi: 10.1038/ng1864

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dierick, H. A., and Greenspan, R. J. (2007). Serotonin and neuropeptide F have opposite modulatory effects on fly aggression. Nat. Genet. 39, 678–682. doi: 10.1038/ng2029

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dzieweczynski, T. L., and Hebert, O. L. (2012). Fluoxetine alters behavioral consistency of aggression and courtship in male Siamese fighting fish, Betta splendens. Physiol. Behav. 107, 92–97. doi: 10.1016/j.physbeh.2012.06.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Edson, P. H., and Gallup, G. G. (1972). Tonic immobility as a fear response in lizards (Anolis carolinensis). Psychon. Sci. 26, 27–28. doi: 10.3758/BF03337874

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Egan, R. J., Bergner, C. L., Hart, P. C., Cachat, J. M., Canavello, P. R., Elegante, M. F., et al. (2009). Understanding behavioral and physiological phenotypes of stress and anxiety in zebrafish. Behav. Brain Res. 205, 38–44. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2009.06.022

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ellis, L. D., Seibert, J., and Soanes, K. H. (2012). Distinct models of induced hyperactivity in zebrafish larvae. Brain Res. 1449, 46–59. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2012.02.022

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Engel, M. S. (1999). The taxonomy of recent and fossil honey bees (Hymenoptera: Apidae; Apis). J. Hymenopt. Res. 8, 165–196.

Google Scholar

Engert, F. (2014). The big data problem: turning maps into knowledge. Neuron 83, 1246–1248. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2014.09.008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fanselow, M. S., and Lester, L. (1988). “A functional behavioristic approach to aversively motivated behavior: predatory imminence as a determinant of the topography of defensive behavior,” in Evolution and Learning, eds R. C. Bolles and M. D. Beecher (Hillsdale, MI: Erlbaum), 185–211.

Feltenstein, M. W., Ford, N. G., Freeman, K. B., and Sufka, K. J. (2002). Dissociation of stress behaviors in the chick social-separation-stress procedure. Physiol. Behav. 75, 675–679. doi: 10.1016/S0031-9384(02)00660-1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Feltenstein, M. W., Lambdin, L. C., Webb, H. E., Warnick, J. E., Khan, S. I., Khan, I. A., et al. (2003). Corticosterone response in the chick separation–stress paradigm. Physiol. Behav. 78, 489–493. doi: 10.1016/S0031-9384(03)00030-1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Feltenstein, M. W., Warnick, J. E., Guth, A. N., and Sufka, K. J. (2004). The chick separation stress paradigm: a validation study. Pharmacol. Biochem. Behav. 77, 221–226. doi: 10.1016/j.pbb.2003.10.019

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fields, S., and Johnston, M. (2005). Cell biology. Whither model organism research? Science 307, 1885–1886.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Fink, H., Rex, A., Voits, M., and Voigt, J.-P. (1998). Major biological actions of CCK—a critical evaluation of research findings. Exp. Brain Res. 123, 77–83. doi: 10.1007/s002210050546

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Folgueira, M., Anadón, R., and Yáñez, J. (2003). Experimental study of the connections of the gustatory system in the rainbow trout, Oncorhynchus mykiss. J. Comp. Neurol. 465, 604–619. doi: 10.1002/cne.10879

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Forsatkar, M. N., Abedi, M., Nematollahi, M. A., and Rahbari, E. (2013). Effect of testosterone and fluoxetine on aggressive behaviors of fighting fish, Betta splendens. Int. J. Aquat. Biol. 1, 289–293.

Google Scholar

Fox, N. A., Henderson, H. A., Marshall, P. J., Nichols, K. E., and Ghera, M. M. (2005). Behavioral inhibition: linking biology and behavior within a developmental framework. Ann. Rev. Psychol. 56, 235–262. doi: 10.1146/annurev.psych.55.090902.141532

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gamier, M., Boujrad, N., Ogwuegbu, S. O., Hudson, J. R. Jr., and Papadopoulos, V. (1994). The polypeptide diazepam-binding inhibitor and a higher affinity mitochondrial peripheral-type benzodiazepine receptor sustain constitutive steroidogenesis in the R2C Leydig tumor cell line. J. Biol. Chem. 269, 22105–22112.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Garcia, J., Kimeldorf, D. J., and Koelling, R. A. (1955). Conditioned aversion to saccharin resulting from exposure to gamma radiation. Science 122, 157–158.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Garland, T. Jr. (2001). “Phylogenetic comparison and artificial selection. Two approaches in evolutionary physiology,” in Hypoxia: From Genes to the Bedside, eds R. C. Roach, P. D. Wanger, and P. H. Hackett (New York, NY: Kluwer Academic/Plenum Publishers), 107–132.

Garner, M., Möhler, H., Stein, D. J., Mueggler, T., and Baldwin, D. S. (2009). Research in anxiety disorders: from the bench to the bedside. Eur. Neuropsychopharmacol. 19, 381–390. doi: 10.1016/j.euroneuro.2009.01.011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gerlai, R. (2003). Zebra fish: an uncharted behavior genetic model. Behav. Genet. 33, 461–468. doi: 10.1023/A:1025762314250

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gerlai, R. (2010). High-throughput behavioral screens: the first step towards finding genes involved in vertebrate brain function using zebrafish. Molecules 15, 2609–2622. doi: 10.3390/molecules15042609

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gerlai, R. (2014). Fish in behavior research: unique tools with a great promise! J. Neurosci. Methods. doi: 10.1016/j.jneumeth.2014.04.015

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gerlai, R. (2015). Zebrafish phenomics: behavioral screens and phenotyping of mutagenized fish. Curr. Opin. Behav. Sci. 2, 21–27. doi: 10.1016/j.cobeha.2014.07.007

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Götz, K. G., and Biesinger, R. (1985). Centrophobism in Drosophila melanogaster. J. Comp. Physiol. A 156, 319–327. doi: 10.1007/BF00610725

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Greenberg, N. (1977). A neuroethological investigation of display behavior in the lizard, carolinensis (Lacertilia, Iguanidae). Am. Zool. 17, 191–201.

Greenberg, N. (1982). A forebrain atlas and stereotaxic technique for the lizard, Anolis carolinensis. J. Morphol. 174, 217–236. doi: 10.1002/jmor.1051740210

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Greenberg, N. (2002). Ethological aspects of stress in a model lizard, Anolis carolinensis. Integr. Comp. Biol. 42, 526–540. doi: 10.1093/icb/42.3.526

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Greenberg, N., and Noble, G. K. (1974). Social behavior of the American chameleon (Anolis carolinensis Voight). Physiol. Zool. 17, 392–439.

Griebel, G., and Holmes, A. (2013). 50 years of hurdles and hope in anxiolytic drug discovery. Nat. Rev. Drug Discov. 12, 667–687. doi: 10.1038/nrd4075

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gupta, P., Khobragade, S. B., Shingatgeri, V. M., and Rajaram, S. M. (2014). Assessment of locomotion behavior in adult Zebrafish after acute exposure to different pharmacological reference compounds. Drug Dev. Ther. 5, 127–133. doi: 10.4103/2394-2002.139626

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hale, M. E. (2014). Mapping circuits beyond the models: integrating connectomics and comparative neuroscience. Neuron 83, 1256–1258. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2014.08.032

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hall, Z. J., De Serrano, A. R., Rodd, F. H., and Tropepe, V. (2014). Casting a wider fish net on animal models in neuropsychiatric research. Prog. Neuropsychopharmacol. Biol. Psychiatry 55, 7–15. doi: 10.1016/j.pnpbp.2014.04.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hamasu, K., Kabuki, Y., Tomonaga, S., Denbow, D. M., and Furuse, M. (2012). Changes in brain monoamine metabolism of neonatal chicks under two different acute stress conditions. Br. Poult. Sci. 53, 145–149. doi: 10.1080/00071668.2012.662271

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hennig, C. W. (1977). Effects of simulated predation on tonic immobility in Anolis carolinensis: the role of eye contact. Bull. Psychon. Soc. 9, 239–242. doi: 10.3758/BF03336987

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hennig, C. W. (1979). The effects of physical environment, time in captivity, and defensive distance on tonic immobility, freezing, and flight behaviors in Anolis carolinensis. Anim. Learn. Behav. 7, 106–110. doi: 10.3758/BF03209666

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hennig, C. W., Dunlap, W. P., and Gallup, G. G. Jr. (1976). The effect of distance between predator and prey and the opportunity to escape on tonic immobility in Anolis carolinensis. Psychol. Rec. 26, 313–320.

Google Scholar

Herculano, A. M., and Maximino, C. (2014). Serotonergic modulation of zebrafish behavior: towards a paradox. Prog. Neuropsychopharmacol. Biol. Psychiatry 55, 50–66. doi: 10.1016/j.pnpbp.2014.03.008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Herculano, A. M., Puty, B., Miranda, V., Lima, M. G., and Maximino, C. (2015). Interactions between serotonin and glutamate-nitric oxide pathways in zebrafish scototaxis. Pharmacol. Biochem. Behav. doi: 10.1016/j.pbb.2014.12.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hesse, M. (1963). Models and Analogies in Science. London: Sheed and Ward.

Google Scholar

Hogan, J. A., and Bols, R. J. (1980). Priming of aggressive motivation in Betta splendens. Anim. Behav. 28, 135–142. doi: 10.1016/S0003-3472(80)80018-2

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hunt, G. J. (2007). Flight and fight: a comparative view of the neurophysiology and genetics of honey bee defensive behavior. J. Insect Physiol. 53, 399–410. doi: 10.1016/j.jinsphys.2007.01.010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hymel, K. A., and Sufka, K. J. (2012). Pharmacological reversal of cognitive bias in the chick anxiety-depression model. Neuropharmacology 62, 161–166. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropharm.2011.06.009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Iliadi, K. G. (2009). The genetic basis of emotional behavior: has the time come for a Drosophila model? J. Neurogenet. 23, 136–146. doi: 10.1080/01677060802471650

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kahsai, L., Martin, J.-R., and Winther, A. M. E. (2010). Neuropeptides in the Drosophila central complex in modulation of locomotor behavior. J. Exp. Biol. 213, 2256–2265. doi: 10.1242/jeb.043190

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kalueff, A. V, Echevarria, D. J., and Stewart, A. M. (2014a). Gaining translational momentum: more zebrafish models for neuroscience research. Prog. Neuropsychopharmacol. Biol. Psychiatry 55, 1–6. doi: 10.1016/j.pnpbp.2014.01.022

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kalueff, A. V, Ren-Patterson, R. F., LaPorte, J. L., and Murphy, D. L. (2008). Domain interplay concept in animal models of neuropsychiatric disorders: a new strategy for high-throughput neurophenotyping research. Behav. Brain Res. 188, 243–249. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2007.11.011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kalueff, A. V., and Stewart, A. M. (2014). Modeling neuropsychiatric spectra to empower translational biological psychiatry. Behav. Brain Res. 276, 1–7. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2014.01.038

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kalueff, A. V, Stewart, A. M., and Gerlai, R. (2014b). Zebrafish as an emerging model for studying complex brain disorders. Trends Pharmacol. Sci. 35, 63–75. doi: 10.1016/j.tips.2013.12.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kalueff, A. V, Wheaton, M., and Murphy, D. L. (2007). What's wrong with my mouse model? Advances and strategies in animal modeling of anxiety and depression. Behav. Brain Res. 179, 1–18. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2007.01.023

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kang, K. S., Shimizu, K., Azuma, M., Ui, Y., Nakamura, K., Uchiyama, M., et al. (2011a). Gonadotropin-releasing hormone II (GnRH II) mediates the anorexigenic actions of α-melanocyte-stimulating hormone (α-MSH) and corticotropin-releasing hormone (CRH) in goldfish. Peptides 32, 31–35. doi: 10.1016/j.peptides.2010.10.013

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kang, K. S., Yahashi, S., Azuma, M., and Matsuda, K. (2010). The anorexigenic effect of cholecystokinin octapeptide in a goldfish model is mediated by the vagal afferent and subsequently through the melanocortin- and corticotropin-releasing hormone-signaling pathways. Peptides 31, 2130–2134. doi: 10.1016/j.peptides.2010.07.019

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kang, K. S., Yahashi, S., and Matsuda, K. (2011b). Effect of the N-methyl-d-aspartate receptor antagonist on locomotor activity and cholecystokinin-induced anorexigenic action in a goldfish model. Neurosci. Lett. 488, 238–241. doi: 10.1016/j.neulet.2010.11.036

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kang, K. S., Yahashi, S., and Matsuda, K. (2011c). The effects of ghrelin on energy balance and psychomotor activity in a goldfish model: an overview. Int. J. Pept. 2011:171034. doi: 10.1155/2011/171034

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kania, B. F., Gralak, M. A., and Wielgosz, M. (2012). Four-week fluoxetine (SSRI) exposure diminishes aggressive behaviour of male Siamese fighting fish (Betta splendens). J. Behav. Brain Sci. 2, 185–190. doi: 10.4236/jbbs.2012.22022

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kas, M. J. H., Krishnan, V., Gould, T. D., Collier, D. A., Olivier, B., Lesch, K.-P., et al. (2011). Advances in multidisciplinary and cross-species approaches to examine the neurobiology of psychiatric disorders. Eur. Neuropsychopharmacol. 21, 532–544. doi: 10.1016/j.euroneuro.2010.12.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kimmel, C. B. (1989). Genetics and early development of zebrafish. Trends Genet. 5, 283–288. doi: 10.1016/0168-9525(89)90103-0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kizil, C., and Brand, M. (2011). Cerebroventricular microinjection (CVMI) into adult zebrafish brain is an efficient misexpression method for forebrain ventricular cells. PLoS ONE 6:e27395. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0027395

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kokel, D., Bryan, J., Laggner, C., White, R., Cheung, C. Y. J., Mateus, R., et al. (2010). Rapid behavior-based identification of neuroactive small molecules in the zebrafish. Nat. Chem. Biol. 6, 231–237. doi: 10.1038/nchembio.307

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kokel, D., and Peterson, R. T. (2008). Chemobehavioural phenomics and behaviour-based psychiatric drug discovery in the zebrafish. Briefings Funct. Genomics Proteomics 7, 483–490. doi: 10.1093/bfgp/eln040

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

LaFollette, H., and Shanks, N. (1994). Animal experimentation: the legacy of Claude Bernard. Int. Stud. Philos. Sci. 8, 195–210. doi: 10.1080/02698599408573495

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

LaFollette, H., and Shanks, N. (1995). Two models of models in biomedical research. Philos. Q. 45, 141–160. doi: 10.2307/2220412

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

LaPorte, J. L., Ren-Patterson, R. F., Murphy, D. L., and Kalueff, A. V (2008). Refining psychiatric genetics: from “mouse psychiatry” to understanding complex human disorders. Behav. Pharmacol. 19, 377–384. doi: 10.1097/FBP.0b013e32830dc09b

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lau, B. Y. B., Mathur, P., Gould, G. G., and Guo, S. (2011). Identification of a brain center whose activity discriminates a choice behavior in zebrafish. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 108, 2581–2586. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1018275108

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lázaro-Muñoz, G., LeDoux, J. E., and Cain, C. K. (2011). Sidman instrumental avoidance initially depends on lateral and basal amygdala and is constrained by central amygdala-mediated Pavlovian processes. Biol. Psychiatry 67, 1120–1127. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2009.12.002

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Leadbeater, E., and Chittka, L. (2007). Social learning in insects—From miniature brains to consensus building. Curr. Biol. 17, R703–R713. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2007.06.012

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Leal, M., and Rodríguez-Robles, J. A. (1995). Antipredator responses of Anolis cristatellus (Sauria: Polychrotidae). Copeia 1, 155–161. doi: 10.2307/1446810

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Leal, M., and Rodríguez-Robles, J. A. (1997). Antipredator responses of the Puerto Rican giant anole, Anolis cuvieri (Squamata: Polychrotidae). Biotropica 29, 372–375. doi: 10.1111/j.1744-7429.1997.tb00440.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lebreton, S., and Martin, J.-R. (2009). Mutations affecting the cAMP transduction pathway disrupt the centrophobism behavior. J. Neurogenet. 23, 225–234. doi: 10.1080/01677060802509160

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

LeDoux, J. E. (2012). Evolution of human emotion: a view through fear. Prog. Brain Res. 195, 431–442. doi: 10.1016/B978-0-444-53860-4.00021-0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Leonelli, S., and Ankeny, R. A. (2012). Re-thinking organisms: the impact of databases on model organism biology. Stud. Hist. Philos. Sci. Part A 43, 29–36. doi: 10.1016/j.shpsc.2011.10.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Leonelli, S., and Ankeny, R. A. (2013). What makes a model organism? Endeavour 37, 209–212. doi: 10.1016/j.endeavour.2013.06.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Levin, E. D., Bencan, Z., and Cerutti, D. T. (2007). Anxiolytic effects of nicotine in zebrafish. Physiol. Behav. 90, 54–58. doi: 10.1016/j.physbeh.2006.08.026

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lillesaar, C. (2011). The serotonergic system in fish. J. Chem. Neuroanat. 41, 294–308. doi: 10.1016/j.jchemneu.2011.05.009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

López, P., Hawlena, D., Polo, V., Amo, L., and Martín, J. (2005). Sources of individual shy–bold variations in antipredator behaviour of male Iberian rock lizards. Anim. Behav. 69, 1–9. doi: 10.1016/j.anbehav.2004.05.010

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

López-Patiño, M. A., Guijarro, A. I., Isorna, E., Delgado, M. J., Alonso-Bedate, M., and De Pedro, N. (1999). Neuropeptide Y has a stimulatory action on feeding behavior in goldfish (Carassius auratus). Eur. J. Pharmacol. 377, 147–153.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

López-Patiño, M. A., Yu, L., Cabral, H., and Zhdanova, I. V (2008). Anxiogenic effects of cocaine withdrawal in zebrafish. Physiol. Behav. 93, 160–171. doi: 10.1016/j.physbeh.2007.08.013

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lovern, M. B., Holmes, M. M., and Wade, J. (2004). The green anole (Anolis carolinensis): a reptilian model for laboratory studies of reproductive morphology and behavior. ILAR J. 45, 54–64. doi: 10.1093/ilar.45.1.54

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lynn, S. E., Egar, J. M., Walker, B. G., Sperry, T. S., and Ramenofsky, M. (2007). Fish on Prozac: a simple, noninvasive physiology laboratory investigating the mechanisms of aggressive behavior in Betta splendens. Adv. Physiol. Edu. 31, 358–363. doi: 10.1152/advan.00024.2007

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Machado, L. L., Galdino, C. A. B., and Sousa, B. M. (2007). Defensive behavior of the lizard Tropidurus montanus (Tropiduridae): effects of sex, body size and social context. South Am. J. Herpetol. 2, 136–140. doi: 10.2994/1808-9798(2007)2[136:DBOTLT]2.0.CO;2

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Maia, D. B., Marmar, C. R., Henn-Haase, C., Nóbrega, A., Fiszman, A., Marques-Portella, C., et al. (2011). Predictors of PTSD symptoms in Brazilian police officers: the synergy of negative affect and peritraumatic dissociation. Rev. Bras. Psiquiatr. 33, 362–366. doi: 10.1590/S1516-4446201100042520019

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Maia, T. V. (2010). Two-factor theory, the actor-critic model, and conditioned avoidance. Learn. Behav. 38, 50–67. doi: 10.3758/LB.38.1.50

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Marino-Neto, J., and Sabbatini, R. M. (1988). A stereotaxic atlas for the telencephalon of the Siamese fighting-fish (Betta splendens). Braz. J. Med. Biol. Res. 21, 971–986.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Martín, I., Gómez, A., Salas, C., Puerto, A., and Rodríguez, F. (2011). Dorsomedial pallium lesions impair taste aversion learning in goldfish. Neurobiol. Learn. Mem. 96, 297–305. doi: 10.1016/j.nlm.2011.06.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Maruyama, K., Miura, T., Uchiyama, M., Shioda, S., and Matsuda, K. (2006). Relationship between anorexigenic action of pituitary adenylate cyclase-activating polypeptide (PACAP) and that of corticotropin-releasing hormone (CRH) in the goldfish, Carassius auratus. Peptides 27, 1820–1826. doi: 10.1016/j.peptides.2006.01.013

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Matsuda, K. (2009). Recent advances in the regulation of feeding behavior by neuropeptides in fish. Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci. 1163, 241–250. doi: 10.1111/j.1749-6632.2008.03619.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Matsuda, K., Hagiwara, Y., Shibata, H., Sakashita, A., and Wada, K. (2013). Ovine corticotropin-releasing hormone (oCRH) exerts an anxiogenic-like action in the goldfish, Carassius auratus. Gen. Comp. Endocrinol. 188, 118–122. doi: 10.1016/j.ygcen.2013.01.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Matsuda, K., Kang, K. S., Sakashita, A., Yahashi, S., and Vaudry, H. (2011a). Behavioral effect of neuropeptides related to feeding regulation in fish. Ann. N.Y. Acad. Sci. 1220, 117–126. doi: 10.1111/j.1749-6632.2010.05884.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Matsuda, K., Kojima, K., Shimakura, S.-I., Wada, K., Maruyama, K., Uchiyama, M., et al. (2008). Corticotropin-releasing hormone mediates α-melanocyte-stimulating hormone-induced anorexigenic action in goldfish. Peptides 29, 1930–1936. doi: 10.1016/j.peptides.2008.06.028

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Matsuda, K., Kojima, K., Wada, K., Maruyama, K., Leprince, J., Tonon, M. C., et al. (2010). The anorexigenic action of the octadecaneuropeptide (ODN) in goldfish is mediated through the MC4R- and subsequently the CRH receptor-signaling pathways. J. Mol. Neurosci. 42, 74–79. doi: 10.1007/s12031-010-9346-9

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Matsuda, K., Maruyama, K., Nakamachi, T., Miura, T., and Shioda, S. (2006a). Effects of pituitary adenylate cyclase-activating polypeptide and vasoactive intestinal polypeptide on food intake and locomotor activity in the goldfish, Carassius auratus. Ann. N.Y. Acad. Sci. 1070, 417–421. doi: 10.1196/annals.1317.054

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Matsuda, K., Miura, T., Kaiya, H., and Maruyama, K. (2006b). Stimulatory effect of n-octanoylated ghrelin on locomotor activity in the goldfish, Carassius auratus. Peptides 27, 1335–1340. doi: 10.1016/j.peptides.2005.10.011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Matsuda, K., Wada, K., Azuma, M., Leprince, J., Tonon, M. C., Sakashita, A., et al. (2011b). The octadecaneuropeptide exerts an anxiogenic-like action in goldfish. Neuroscience 181, 100–108. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2011.02.058

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Matsuda, K., Wada, K., Miura, T., Maruyama, K., Shimakura, S. I., Uchiyama, M., et al. (2007). Effect of the diazepam-binding inhibitor-derived peptide, octadecaneuropeptide, on food intake in goldfish. Neuroscience 150, 425–432. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2007.09.012

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Maximino, C. (2012). Serotonin and Anxiety: Neuroanatomical, Pharmacological, and Functional Aspects. New York, NY: Springer Science+Business Media, LLC. doi: 10.1007/978-1-4614-4048-2

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Maximino, C., de Brito, T. M., Colmanetti, R., Pontes, A. A. A., de Castro, H. M., de Lacerda, R. I. T., et al. (2010a). Parametric analyses of anxiety in zebrafish scototaxis. Behav. Brain Res. 210, 1–7. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2010.01.031

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Maximino, C., Brito, T. M., de Moraes, F. D., de Oliveira, F. V. C., Taccolini, I. B., Pereira, P. M., et al. (2007). A comparative analysis of the preference for dark environments in five teleosts. Int. J. Comp. Psychol. 20, 351–367.

Google Scholar

Maximino, C., Brito, T. M., and De Gouveia, A. Jr. (2010c). Construct validity of behavioral models of anxiety: where experimental psychopathology meets ecology and evolution. Psychol. Neurosci. 3, 117–123. doi: 10.3922/j.psns.2010.1.015

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Maximino, C., de Brito, T. M., de Silva, A. W. B., da Herculano, A. M., Morato, S., Gouveia, A. Jr., et al. (2010d). Measuring anxiety in zebrafish: a critical review. Behav. Brain Res. 214, 157–171. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2010.05.031

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Maximino, C., Carvalho, C. M., and Morato, S. (2014a). Discrimination of anxiety- versus panic-like behavior in the wall lizard Tropidurus oreadicus. Psychol. Neurosci. 7, 227–231. doi: 10.3922/j.psns.2014.020

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Maximino, C., da Silva, A. W. B., Araújo, J., Lima, M. G., Miranda, V., Puty, B., et al. (2014d). Fingerprinting of psychoactive drugs in zebrafish anxiety-like behaviors. PLoS ONE 9:e103943. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0103943

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Maximino, C., de Brito, T. M., Dias, C. A. M, Gouveia, A. Jr., and Morato, S. (2010b). Scototaxis as anxiety-like behavior in fish. Nat. Protoc. 5, 209–216. doi: 10.1038/nprot.2009.225

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Maximino, C., Gemaque, J., Benzecry, R, Lima, M. G., Batista Ede, J, Picanço-Diniz, D. L. W., et al. (2014b). Role of nitric oxide in the behavioral and neurochemical effects of IB-MECA in zebrafish. Psychopharmacology (Berl). 232, 1671–1680. doi: 10.1007/s00213-014-3799-4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Maximino, C., Lima, M. G., Araujo, J., Oliveira, K. R. M., Herculano, A. M., Stewart, A. M., et al. (2013a). “The serotonergic system of zebrafish: genomics, neuroanatomy and neuropharmacology,” in Serotonin: Biosynthesis, Regulation and Health Implications, ed F. S. Hall (New York, NY: Nova Science), 53–67.

Maximino, C., Lima, M. G., Costa, C. C., Guedes, I. M. L., and Herculano, A. M. (2014c). Fluoxetine and WAY 100,635 dissociate increases in scototaxis and analgesia induced by conspecific alarm substance in zebrafish (Danio rerio Hamilton 1822). Pharmacol. Biochem. Behav. 124C, 425–433. doi: 10.1016/j.pbb.2014.07.003

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Maximino, C., Lima, M. G., Oliveira, K. R. M., Batista, E., de, J. O., and Herculano, A. M. (2013b). “Limbic associative” and “autonomic” amygdala in teleosts: a review of the evidence. J. Chem. Neuroanat. 48-49, 1–13. doi: 10.1016/j.jchemneu.2012.10.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Maximino, C., Lima, M. G., Oliveira, K. R. M., Picanço-diniz, D. L. W., and Herculano, A. M. (2011a). Adenosine A1, but not A2, receptor blockade increases anxiety and arousal in zebrafish. Basic Clin. Pharmacol. Toxicol. 109, 203–207. doi: 10.1111/j.1742-7843.2011.00710.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Maximino, C., Puty, B., Benzecry, R., Araújo, J., Lima, M. G., Batista, E., et al. (2013c). Role of serotonin in zebrafish (Danio rerio) anxiety: relationship with serotonin levels and effect of buspirone, WAY 100635, SB 224289, fluoxetine and para-chlorophenylalanine in two behavioral models. Neuropharmacology 71, 83–97. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropharm.2013.03.006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Maximino, C., Silva, A. W. B., da Gouveia, A. Jr., and Herculano, A. M. (2011b). Pharmacological analysis of zebrafish (Danio rerio) scototaxis. Prog. Neuropsychopharmacol. Biol. Psychiatry 35, 624–631. doi: 10.1016/j.pnpbp.2011.01.006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mayack, C., and Naug, D. (2015). Starving honeybees lose self-control. Biol. Lett. 11:20140820. doi: 10.1098/rsbl.2014.0820

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

McKinney, W. T., and Bunney, W. E. (1969). Animal model of depression: I. Review of evidence: implications for research. Arch. Gen. Psychiatry 21, 240–248. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.1969.01740200112015

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

McNaughton, N., and Zangrossi, H. Jr. (2008). “Theoretical approaches to the modeling of anxiety in animals,” in Handbook of Anxiety and Fear, eds R. J. Blanchard, D. C. Blanchard, G. Griebel, and D. J. Nutt (Amsterdam: Elsevier B. V.), 11–27.

Menzel, R. (1983). Neurobiology of learning and memory: the honeybee as a model system. Naturwissenschaften 70, 504–511. doi: 10.1007/BF00394056

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Miczek, K. A., de Almeida, R. M. M., Kravitz, E. A., Rissman, E. F., de Boer, S. F., and Raine, A. (2007). Neurobiology of escalated aggression and violence. J. Neurosci. 27, 11803–11806. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3500-07.2007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Miley, W. M., and Burack, G. (1977). Strength of aggressive display in Siamese fighting fish (Betta splendens) toward a conspecific, an alien species (Macropodus opercularis), and a mirror image as affected by prior conspecific visual experience. Behav. Biol. 21, 267–272. doi: 10.1016/S0091-6773(77)90352-2

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mitra, P. P. (2014). The circuit architecture of whole brains at the mesoscopic scale. Neuron 83, 1273–1283. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2014.08.055

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Miura, T., Maruyama, K., Shimakura, S. I., Kaiya, H., Uchiyama, M., Kangawa, K., et al. (2006). Neuropeptide Y mediates ghrelin-induced feeding in the goldfish, Carassius auratus. Neurosci. Lett. 407, 279–283. doi: 10.1016/j.neulet.2006.08.071

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Montgomery, K. C. (1954). The relation between fear induced by novel stimulation and exploratory behavior. J. Comp. Physiol. Psychol. 47, 254–260.

PubMed Abstract

Moskowitz, A. K. (2004). “Scared stiff”: catatonia as an evolutionary-based fear response. Psychol. Rev. 111, 984–1002. doi: 10.1037/0033-295X.111.4.984

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Muqit, M. M. K., and Feany, M. B. (2002). Modelling neurodegenerative diseases in Drosophila: a fruitful approach? Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 3, 237–243. doi: 10.1038/nrn751

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nakamachi, T., Matsuda, K., Maruyama, K., Miura, T., Uchiyama, M., Funahashi, H., et al. (2006). Regulation by orexin of feeding behaviour and locomotor activity in the goldfish. J. Neuro 18, 290–297. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2826.2006.01415.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nakamachi, T., Shibata, H., Sakashita, A., Iinuma, N., Wada, K., Konno, N., et al. (2014). Orexin A enhances locomotor activity and induces anxiogenic-like action in the goldfish, Carassius auratus. Horm. Behav. 66, 317–323. doi: 10.1016/j.yhbeh.2014.06.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nathan, F. M., Ogawa, S., and Parhar, I. S. (2015). Kisspeptin1 modulates odorant-evoked fear response via two serotonin receptor subtypes (5-HT1A and 5-HT2) in zebrafish. J. Neurochem. 133, 870–878. doi: 10.1111/jnc.13105

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nelson, N. C. (2012). Modeling mouse, human, and discipline: epistemic scaffolds in animal behavior genetics. Soc. Stud. Sci. 43, 3–29. doi: 10.1177/0306312712463815

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

The Avian Brain Nomenclature Consortium. (2005). Avian brains and a new understanding of vertebrate brain evolution. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 6, 151–159. doi: 10.1038/nrn1606

CrossRef Full Text

Norton, W., and Bally-Cuif, L. (2010). Adult zebrafish as a model organism for behavioural genetics. BMC Neurosci. 11, 90. doi: 10.1186/1471-2202-11-90

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Norton, W. H. J. (2013). Toward developmental models of psychiatric disorders in zebrafish. Front. Neural Circuits 7:79. doi: 10.3389/fncir.2013.00079

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Norton, W. H. J., Stumpenhorst, K., Faus-Kessler, T., Folchert, A., Rohner, N., Harris, M. P., et al. (2011). Modulation of fgfr1a signaling in zebrafish reveals a genetic basis for the aggression–boldness syndrome. J. Neurosci. 31, 13796–13807. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2892-11.2011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nowicki, M., Tran, S., Muraleetharan, A., Markovic, S., and Gerlai, R. (2014). Serotonin antagonists induce anxiolytic and anxiogenic-like behavior in zebrafish in a receptor-subtype dependent manner. Pharmacol. Biochem. Behav. 126, 170–180. doi: 10.1016/j.pbb.2014.09.022

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Øverli, Ø., Sørensen, C., Pulman, K. G. T., Pottinger, T. G., Korzan, W., Summers, C. H., and Nilsson, G. E. (2007). Evolutionary background for stress-coping styles: relationships between physiological, behavioral, and cognitive traits in non-mammalian vertebrates. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 31, 396–412. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2006.10.006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Panksepp, J. (2004). “Emerging neuroscience of fear and anxiety: therapeutic practice and clinical implications,” in Textbook of Biological Psychiatry (Nova Iorque: Wiley), 489–519.

Google Scholar

Panksepp, J. (2006). Emotional endophenotypes in evolutionary psychiatry. Prog. Neuropsychopharmacol. Biol. Psychiatry 30, 774–784. doi: 10.1016/j.pnpbp.2006.01.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Panksepp, J. (2011). Cross-species affective neuroscience decoding of the primal affective experiences of humans and related animals. PLoS ONE 6:e21236. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0021236

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Panksepp, J., Moskal, J. R., Panksepp, J. B., and Kroes, R. A. (2002). Comparative approaches in evolutionary psychology: molecular neuroscience meets the mind. Neuroendocrinol. Lett. 23, 105–115.

Google Scholar

Parker, M. O., Brock, A. J., Walton, R. T., and Brennan, C. H. (2013a). The role of zebrafish (Danio rerio) in dissecting the genetics and neural circuits of executive function. Front. Neural Circuits 7:63. doi: 10.3389/fncir.2013.00063

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Parker, M. O., Ife, D., Ma, J., Pancholi, M., Smeraldi, F., Straw, C., et al. (2013b). Development and automation of a test of impulse control in zebrafish. Front. Syst. Neurosci. 7:65. doi: 10.3389/fnsys.2013.00065

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Parker, M. O., Millington, M. E., Combe, F. J., and Brennan, C. H. (2012). Development and implementation of a three-choice serial reaction time task for zebrafish (Danio rerio). Behav. Brain Res. 227, 73–80. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2011.10.037

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pavlidis, M., Theodoridi, A., and Tsalafouta, A. (2015). Neuroendocrine regulation of the stress response in adult zebrafish, Danio rerio. Prog. Neuropsychopharmacol. Biol. Psychiatry 60, 121–131. doi: 10.1016/j.pnpbp.2015.02.014

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pinna, G. (2010). In a mouse model relevant for PTSD, selective brain steroidogenicstimulants (SBSSs) improve behavioral deficits by normalizing allopregnanolone biosynthesis. Behav. Pharmacol. 21, 438–450. doi: 10.1097/FBP.0b013e32833d8ba0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pires, T. S. F., and da Costa Maia, Â. (2013). Posttraumatic stress disorder among victims of serious motor vehicle accidents: an analysis of predictors. Rev. Bras. Psiquiatr. Clínica 40, 211–214. doi: 10.1590/S0101-6083201300062520011

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pollen, A. A., and Hofmann, H. A. (2008). Beyond neuroanatomy: novel approaches to studying brain evolution. Brain. Behav. Evol. 72, 145–158. doi: 10.1159/000151474

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Portavella, M., Torres, B., and Salas, C. (2004a). Avoidance response in goldfish: emotional and temporal involvement of medial and lateral telencephalic pallium. J. Neurosci. 24, 2335–2342. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4930-03.2004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Portavella, M., Torres, B., Salas, C., and Papini, M. R. (2004b). Lesions of the medial pallium, but not of the lateral pallium, disrupt spaced-trial avoidance learning in goldfish (Carassius auratus). Neurosci. Lett. 362, 75–78. doi: 10.1016/j.neulet.2004.01.083

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Portavella, M., and Vargas, J. P. (2005). Emotional and spatial learning in goldfish is dependent on different telencephalic pallial systems. Eur. J. Neurosci. 21, 2800–2806. doi: 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2005.04114.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Portavella, M., Vargas, J. P., Torres, B., and Salas, C. (2002). The effects of telencephalic pallial lesions on spatial, temporal, and emotional learning in goldfish. Brain Res. Bull. 57, 397–399. doi: 10.1016/S0361-9230(01)00699-2

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Reilly, S., and Bornovalova, M. A. (2005). Conditioned taste aversion and amygdala lesions in the rat: a critical review. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 29, 1067–1088. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2005.03.025

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ressler, K. J., Mercer, K. B., Bradley, B., Jovanovic, T., Mahan, A., and Kerley, K. (2011). Post-traumatic stress disorder is associated with PACAP and PAC1 receptor. Nature 470, 492–497. doi: 10.1038/nature09856

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Richendrfer, H., Pelkowski, S. D., Colwill, R. M., and Creton, R. (2012). On the edge: pharmacological evidence for anxiety-related behavior in zebrafish larvae. Behav. Brain Res. 228, 99–106. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2011.11.041

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rinkwitz, S., Mourrain, P., and Becker, T. S. (2011). Zebrafish: an integrative system for neurogenomics and neurosciences. Prog. Neurobiol. 93, 231–243. doi: 10.1016/j.pneurobio.2010.11.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ruby, C. L., Adams, C. A., Mrazek, D. A., and Choi, D.-S. (2011). “Adenosine signaling in anxiety,” in Anxiety Disorders, ed V. Kalinin (InTech). doi: 10.5772/19423

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sackerman, J., Donegan, J. J., Cunningham, C. S., Nguyen, N. N., Lawless, K., Long, A., et al. (2010). Zebrafish behavior in novel environments: effects of acute exposure to anxiolytic compounds and choice of Danio rerio line. Int. J. Comp. Psychol. 23, 43–61.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Salmeto, A. L., Hymel, K. A., Carpenter, E. C., Brilot, B. O., Bateson, M., and Sufka, K. J. (2011). Cognitive bias in the chick anxiety-depression model. Brain Res. 1373, 124–130. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2010.12.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Santos, M. B., dos, Oliveira, M. C. L. M., de, Verrastro, L., and Tozetti, A. M. (2010). Playing dead to stay alive: death-feigning in Liolaemus occipitalis (Squamata: Liolaemidae). Biota Neotrop. 10, 361–364. doi: 10.1590/S1676-0603201252001400043

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schafer, W. R. (2002). “Neuropsychopharmacology of worms and flies,” in Neuropsychopharmacology. The Fifth Generation of Progress, eds K. L. Davis, D. Charney, J. T. Coyle, and C. Nemeroff (New York, NY: Raven Press/American College of Neuropsychopharmacology), 263–274.

Scott, E. K., Mason, L., Arrenberg, A. B., Ziv, L., Gosse, N. J., Xiao, T., et al. (2007). Targeting neural circuitry in zebrafish using GAL4 enhancer trapping. Nat. Methods 4, 323–326. doi: 10.1038/nmeth1033

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Siebel, A. M., Menezes, F. P., Capiotti, K. M., Kist, L. W., da Costa Schaefer, I., Frantz, J. Z., et al. (2015). Role of adenosine signaling on pentylenetetrazole-induced seizures in zebrafish. Zebrafish 12, 127–136. doi: 10.1089/zeb.2014.1004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Signore, I. A., Guerrero, N., Loosli, F., Colombo, A., Villalón, A., Wittbrodt, J., et al. (2009). Zebrafish and medaka: model organisms for a comparative developmental approach of brain asymmetry. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. Part B 364, 991–1003. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2008.0260

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Simpson, M. J. A. (1968). The display of the Siamese fighting fish, Betta splendens. Anim. Behav. Monogr. 1, 1–73. doi: 10.1016/S0066-1856(68)80001-9

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Smith, D. R., Villafuerte, L., Otis, G., and Palmer, M. R. (2000). Biogeography of Apis cerana F. and A. nigrocincta Smith: insights from mtDNA studies. Apidologie 31, 265–279. doi: 10.1051/apido:2000121

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Soibam, B., Mann, M., Liu, L., Tran, J., Lobaina, M., Kang, Y. Y., et al. (2012). Open-field arena boundary is a primary object of exploration for Drosophila. Brain Behav. 2, 97–108. doi: 10.1002/brb3.36

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Steckler, T. (2008). “Peptide receptor ligands to treat anxiety disorders,” in Handbook of Anxiety and Fear, R. J. Blanchard, D. C. Blanchard, G. Griebel, and D. Nutt (Amsterdam: Elsevier B. V.), 157–221.

Steenbergen, P. J., Richardson, M. K., and Champagne, D. L. (2011). The use of the zebrafish model in stress research. Prog. Neuropsychopharmacol. Biol. Psychiatry 35, 1432–1451. doi: 10.1016/j.pnpbp.2010.10.010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Stewart, A., Gaikwad, S., Kyzar, E., Green, J., Roth, A., and Kalueff, A. V (2011a). Modeling anxiety using adult zebrafish: a conceptual review. Neuropharmacology 62, 135–143. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropharm.2011.07.037

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Stewart, A., Maximino, C., de Brito, T. M., Herculano, A. M., Gouveia, A. Jr., Morato, S., et al. (2011b). “Neurophenotyping of adult zebrafish using the light/dark box paradigm,” in Zebrafish Neurobehavioral Protocols, eds. A. V Kalueff and J. M. Cachat (New York, NY: Humana Press), 157–167.

Stewart, A. M., Braubach, O., Spitsbergen, J., Gerlai, R., and Kalueff, A. V. (2014a). Zebrafish models for translational neuroscience research: from tank to bedside. Trends Neurosci. 37, 264–278. doi: 10.1016/j.tins.2014.02.011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Stewart, A. M., Cachat, J., Gaikwad, S., Robinson, K. S., Gebhardt, M., and Kalueff, A. V (2013). Perspectives on experimental models of serotonin syndrome in zebrafish. Neurochem. Int. 62, 893–902. doi: 10.1016/j.neuint.2013.02.018

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Stewart, A. M., and Kalueff, A. V. (2014). Developing better and more valid animal models of brain disorders. Behav. Brain Res. 276, 28–31. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2013.12.024

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Stewart, A. M., Ullmann, J. F. P., Norton, W. H. J., Parker, M. O., Brennan, C. H., Gerlai, R., et al. (2014b). Molecular psychiatry of zebrafish. Mol. Psychiatry 20, 2–17. doi: 10.1038/mp.2014.128

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Stewart, A., Wu, N., Cachat, J., Hart, P., Gaikwad, S., Wong, K., et al. (2011c). Pharmacological modulation of anxiety-like phenotypes in adult zebrafish behavioral models. Prog. Neuropsychopharmacol. Biol. Psychiatry 35, 1421–1431. doi: 10.1016/j.pnpbp.2010.11.035

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Striedter, G. F., Belgard, T. G., Chen, C., Davis, F. P., Finlay, B. L., Güntürkün, O., et al. (2014). NSF Workshop Report: discovering general principles of nervous system organization by comparing brain maps across species. Brain. Behav. Evol. 83, 1–8. doi: 10.1159/000360152

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sufka, K. J., Feltenstein, M. W., Warnick, J. E., Acevedo, E. O., Webb, H. E., and Cartwright, C. M. (2006). Modeling the anxiety-depression continuum hypothesis in domestic fowl chicks. Behav. Pharmacol. 17, 681–689. doi: 10.1097/FBP.0b013e3280115fac

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sufka, K. J., and Weed, N. C. (1994). Construct validation of behavioral indices of isolation stress and inflammatory nociception in young domestic fowl. Physiol. Behav. 55, 741–746. doi: 10.1016/0031-9384(94)90054-X

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Summers, C. H., Summers, T. R., Moore, M. C., Korzan, W. J., Woodley, S. K., Ronan, P. J., et al. (2003). Temporal patterns of limbic monoamine and plasma corticosterone response during social stress. Neuroscience 116, 553–563. doi: 10.1016/S0306-4522(02)00708-X

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Takahashi, A., Quadros, I. M., de Almeida, R. M., and Miczek, K. A. (2011). Brain serotonin receptors and transporters: initiation vs. termination of escalated aggression. Psychopharmacology (Berl). 213, 183–212. doi: 10.1007/s00213-010-2000-y

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Takahashi, L. K. (2001). Role of CRF1 and CRF2 receptors in fear and anxiety. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 25, 627–636. doi: 10.1016/S0149-7634(01)00046-X

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tedjakumala, S. R., Aimable, M., and Giurfa, M. (2014). Pharmacological modulation of aversive responsiveness in honey bees. Front. Behav. Neurosci. 7:221. doi: 10.3389/fnbeh.2013.00221

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tedjakumala, S. R., and Giurfa, M. (2013). Rules and mechanisms of punishment learning in honey bees: the aversive conditioning of the sting extension response. J. Exp. Biol. 216, 2985–2997. doi: 10.1242/jeb.086629

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Thaker, M., Lima, S. L., and Hews, D. K. (2009). Alternative antipredator tactics in tree lizard morphs: hormonal and behavioural responses to a predator encounter. Anim. Behav. 77, 395–401. doi: 10.1016/j.anbehav.2008.10.014

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Thisse, B., and Thisse, C. (2005). Functions and regulations of fibroblast growth factor signaling during embryonic development. Dev. Biol. 287, 390–402. doi: 10.1016/j.ydbio.2005.09.011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vada, S., Goli, D., Bose, A., and Sharma, U. R. (2015). Effects of anti anxiety drug midazolam as pretreatment therapy for anxiety of zebra fishes induced by the exposure of unfamiliar environment of aquatic white/black maze. Asian J. Pharm. Clin. Res. 8, 3–6.

Google Scholar

van Alphen, B., and van Swinderen, B. (2013). Drosophila strategies to study psychiatric disorders. Brain Res. Bull. 92, 1–11. doi: 10.1016/j.brainresbull.2011.09.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

van der Staay, F. J. (2006). Animal models of behavioral dysfunctions: basic concepts and classifications, and an evaluation strategy. Brain Res. Rev. 52, 131–159. doi: 10.1016/j.brainresrev.2006.01.006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vargas, J. P., López, J. C., and Portavella, M. (2012). “Amygdala and emotional learning in vertebrates—a comparative perspective,” in The Amygdala—A Discrete Multitasking Manager (InTech). doi: 10.5772/51552

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vergoz, V., Roussel, E., Sandoz, J. C., and Giurfa, M. (2007). Aversive learning in honeybees revealed by the olfactory conditioning of the sting extension reflex. PLoS ONE 2:e288. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.252001288

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Volkoff, H., and Peter, R. E. (2006). Feeding behavior of fish and its control. Zebrafish 3, 131–140. doi: 10.1089/zeb.2006.3.131

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

von Trotha, J. W., Vernier, P., and Bally-Cuif, L. (2014). Emotions and motivated behavior converge on an amygdala-like structure in the zebrafish. Eur. J. Neurosci. doi: 10.1111/ejn.12692

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Waldron, F. A., Wiegmann, D. D., and Wiegmann, D. A. (2005). Negative incentive contrast induces economic choice behavior in bumble bees. Int. J. Comp. Psychol. 18, 358–371.

Google Scholar

Wang, W., Dai, Z., Tan, H., Guo, C., and Sun, J. (2008). A stereotaxic method and apparatus for the Gekko gecko. Chinese Sci. Bull. 53, 1107–1112. doi: 10.1007/s11434-008-0175-y

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Warnick, J. E., Huang, C. J., Acevedo, E. O., and Sufka, K. J. (2009). Modelling the anxiety-depression continuum in chicks. J. Psychopharmacol. 23, 143–156. doi: 10.1177/0269881108089805

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Warnick, J. E., McCurdy, C. R., and Sufka, K. J. (2005). Opioid receptor function in social attachment in young domestic fowl. Behav. Brain Res. 160, 277–285. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2004.12.009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Whitfield, C. W., Behura, S. K., Berlocher, S. H., Clark, A. G., Johnston, J. S., Sheppard, W. S., et al. (2006). Thrice out of Africa: ancient and recent expansions of the honey bee, Apis mellifera. Science 314, 642–645. doi: 10.1126/science.1132772

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Willner, P. (1991). “Methods for assessing the validity of animal models of human psychopathology,” in Animal Models in Psychiatry, eds. A. A. Boulton, G. B. Baker, and M. T. Martin-Iverson (Clifton, NJ: Humana Press), 1–23.

Wilson-Sanders, S. E. (2011). Invertebrate models for biomedical research, testing, and education. ILAR J. 52, 126–152. doi: 10.1093/ilar.52.2.126

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wong, K., Elegante, M., Bartels, B., Elkhayat, S., Tien, D., Roy, S., et al. (2010). Analyzing habituation responses to novelty in zebrafish (Danio rerio). Behav. Brain Res. 208, 450–457. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2009.12.023

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wong, R. Y., Oxendine, S. E., and Godwin, J. (2013). Behavioral and neurogenomic transcriptome changes in wild-derived zebrafish with fluoxetine treatment. BMC Genomics 14:348. doi: 10.1186/1471-2164-14-348

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wright, C. (2002). Animal Models of Depression in Neuropsychopharmacology Qua Feyerabendian Philosophy of Science [Preprint]. PhilSci-Archive. Available online at: http://philsci-archive.pitt.edu/813/

Xu, X., Bazner, J., Qi, M., Johnson, E., and Freidhoff, R. (2003). The role of telencephalic NMDA receptors in avoidance learning in goldfish (Carassius auratus). Behav. Neurosci. 117, 548–554. doi: 10.1037/0735-7044.117.3.548

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Xu, X., Bentley, J., Miller, T., Zmolek, K., Kovaleinen, T., Goodman, E., et al. (2009). The role of telencephalic NO and cGMP in avoidance conditioning in goldfish (Carassius auratus). Behav. Neurosci. 123, 614–623. doi: 10.1037/a0015243

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Yamane, H., Asechi, M., Tsuneyoshi, Y., Denbow, D. M., and Furuse, M. (2009a). Central L-cysteine induces sleep, and D-cysteine induces sleep and abnormal behavior during acute stress in neonatal chicks. Anim. Sci. J. 80, 428–432. doi: 10.1111/j.1740-0929.2009.00647.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Yamane, H., Tomonaga, S., Suenaga, R., Denbow, D. M., and Furuse, M. (2007). Intracerebroventricular injection of glutathione and its derivative induces sedative and hypnotic effects under an acute stress in neonatal chicks. Neurosci. Lett. 418, 87–91. doi: 10.1016/j.neulet.2007.03.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Yamane, H., Tsuneyoshi, Y., Denbow, D. M., and Furuse, M. (2009b). N-methyl-d-aspartate and alpha-amino-3-hydroxy-5-methyl-4-isoxazolepropionate receptors involved in the induction of sedative effects under an acute stress in neonatal chicks. Amino Acids 37, 733–739. doi: 10.1007/s00726-008-0203-x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Yoshimoto, M., and Yamamoto, N. (2010). Ascending general visceral sensory pathways from the brainstem to the forebrain in a cichlid fish, Oreochromis (Tilapia) niloticus. J. Comp. Neurol. 518, 3570–3603. doi: 10.1002/cne.22415

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zangrossi, H. Jr., and Graeff, F. G. (2014). Serotonin in anxiety and panic: contributions of the elevated T-maze. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2014.03.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zwarts, L., Versteven, M., and Callaerts, P. (2012). Genetics and neurobiology of aggression in Drosophila. Fly (Austin). 6, 35–48. doi: 10.4161/fly.19249

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: behavioral models, anamniotes, invertebrates, biological psychiatry, teleost fish, sauropsida

Citation: Maximino C, Silva RXC, da Silva SNS, Rodrigues LSS, Barbosa H, de Carvalho TS, Leão LKR, Lima MG, Oliveira KRM and Herculano AM (2015) Non-mammalian models in behavioral neuroscience: consequences for biological psychiatry. Front. Behav. Neurosci. 9:233. doi: 10.3389/fnbeh.2015.00233

Received: 07 May 2015; Accepted: 18 August 2015;
Published: 08 September 2015.

Edited by:

Allan V. Kalueff, ZENEREI Institute, USA and Guangdong Ocean University, China

Reviewed by:

Chen-Min Yeh, Salk Institute for Biological Studies, USA
Jason E. Warnick, Arkansas Tech University, USA

Copyright © 2015 Maximino, Silva, da Silva, Rodrigues, Barbosa, de Carvalho, Leão, Lima, Oliveira and Herculano. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) or licensor are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Anderson Manoel Herculano, Laboratório de Neurofarmacologia Experimental, Instituto de Ciências Biológicas, Universidade Federal do Pará, Av., Augusto Corrêa n. 1, 66075-110 Belém/PA, Brazil, aherculanos@gmail.com

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.