- 1School of Psychology and Cognitive Science, East China Normal University, Shanghai, SH, China
- 2Key Laboratory of Brain Functional Genomics, Ministry of Education, Shanghai Key Laboratory of Brain Functional Genomics, East China Normal University, Shanghai, SH, China
- 3Shanghai Key Laboratory of Magnetic Resonance, East China Normal University, Shanghai, SH, China
- 4Shanghai Key Laboratory of Magnetic Resonance and School of Psychology and Cognitive Science, East China Normal University, Shanghai, SH, China
People often take either the role of an actor or that of recipient in positive and negative interpersonal events when they interact with others. The present study investigated how the actor-recipient role affected the neural responses to self in emotional situations. Twenty-five participants were scanned while they were presented with positive and negative interpersonal events and were asked to rate the degree to which the actor/the recipient was that kind of person who caused the interpersonal event. Half of the trials were self-relevant events and the other half were other-relevant events. Results showed that people were more likely to isolate self from negative events when they played the role of actor relative to recipient. Pregenual anterior cingulate cortex (pgACC) and posterior dorsal anterior cingulate cortex (pdACC) were more active for self than other only in negative events. More importantly, also in negative interpersonal events, dorsal medial prefrontal cortex (dmPFC) showed greater self-related activations (self-other) when participants played the role of recipient relative to actor, while activities in orbitofrontal cortex (OFC) were greater for self than other only when the evaluation target played the role of recipient. These results showed that the actor-recipient role affected neural responses to self in emotional situations, especially when a recipient role was played in negative situations.
Introduction
“Self” is a unique mental construct and self-processing is functionally dissociable from other forms of processing within the human brain (Kelley et al., 2002). Neuroimaging studies have demonstrated the involvement of the cortical midline structures (CMS), including medial prefrontal cortex (mPFC), posterior cingulate cortex (PCC) and precuneus, in self-processing (Kelley et al., 2002; Beer and Hughes, 2010; Korn et al., 2012). In these studies, participants were asked to rate self-descriptiveness and other-person-descriptiveness of favorable and unfavorable trait words. Meta-analytic evidence has suggested a spatial gradient within mPFC, i.e., ventral mPFC (vmPFC) and dorsal mPFC (dmPFC), based on the distinct functions during self-processing (Denny et al., 2012). Specifically, vmPFC is engaged in encoding self-relevance of stimuli, whereas dmPFC is involved in self-evaluation and self-related reappraisal (Northoff and Bermpohl, 2004; Beer and Hughes, 2010; Han et al., 2010; Korn et al., 2012).
One typical character of self-processing is that it often interacts with emotional valence (Moran et al., 2006). When considering personal relevance of information, individuals are more likely to endorse positive information as self-descriptive and isolate negative information (Mezulis et al., 2004; Moran et al., 2006; Beer and Hughes, 2010; D’Argembeau et al., 2012). Neuroimaging studies have demonstrated that brain activities in anterior cingulate cortex (ACC) and orbitofrontal cortex (OFC) were associated with interactions between self-processing and emotional valence (Moran et al., 2006; Sharot et al., 2007; Beer and Hughes, 2010; Seidel et al., 2010; Korn et al., 2012). Specifically, ACC was associated with conflict monitoring (Carter et al., 1998; Bush et al., 2000; Northoff and Bermpohl, 2004; Etkin et al., 2011), while OFC was associated with accurate self-view (Beer et al., 2006, 2010; Beer, 2007; Beer and Hughes, 2010). In addition, previous studies had demonstrated that people were especially sensitive to self-related negative information relative to positive information. They would preferentially process negative information, indicating “negativity bias” (Carretié et al., 2001; Huang and Luo, 2006; Hilgard et al., 2014). Neuroimaging studies have proposed that ACC and mPFC played a critical role in negative emotional processing (Ochsner et al., 2004; Cheng et al., 2010; Etkin et al., 2011; Guo et al., 2012).
Self-processing is not only to judge self-descriptiveness of trait words, we also, at various moments in life, might have cause to evaluate ourselves in interpersonal situations. No self-other difference in interpersonal situations is better known than the actor–observer asymmetry in social and cognitive psychology (Jones and Nisbett, 1971; Malle, 2006). Remarkably, when self plays the role of an actor (actively performs the action), he/she tends to attribute negative events to situations and positive events to dispositions of themselves, while a reverse attribution tendency is found when she/he is just observing the other-relevant event as a passive observer (Malle, 2006). Since the actor is referred to self actively performing the action in the self-relevant event and the observer condition involved the self-irrelevant events, it has been widely acknowledged that the classic actor–observer asymmetry was due to the involvement of self-processing (Cunningham et al., 1979; Malle, 2006). However, in social interpersonal situations, self also often plays the role of a recipient who actively observes the action from the actor in self-relevant events.
Although interpersonal situations, in which self played either the actor or recipient role, seem to be self-related, previous studies have argued that there were decreased self-other attribution differences for the recipient relative to actor (Kasof and Lee, 1993; Malle, 2006). For example, Kasof and Lee (1993) showed that, compared to accurate attributions in other-relevant events, individuals tended to attribute more positive (relative to negative) events to self when they played the role of actor, while this self-serving evaluations decreased when they played the role of recipient. Previous research had claimed that self-serving evaluations were heuristic judgments (Dunning et al., 1989; Chambers and Windschitl, 2004; Beer and Hughes, 2010) which were made more quickly and required fewer cognitive resources (Beer and Hughes, 2010). Weaker self-serving evaluations had been argued to be related to the involvement of more cognitive resources in judgments (Beer and Hughes, 2010). Reduced self-serving evaluations in recipient role in Kasof and Lee (1993) suggested that more complicated self-evaluation processes associated with more cognitive resources relative to heuristic judgments were engaged in the judgments.
The present study aimed to explore the neural mechanism underlying how the actor-recipient role would affect self-processing in emotional situations using functional MRI. Participants were scanned while they were presented with positive and negative interpersonal events and were asked to rate “how likely it is that the actor/the recipient is that kind of person who caused the interpersonal event”. In half of the trials, “self” was randomly assigned to an actor or recipient (e.g., “I hit Mary” or “Lisa hits me”) and “self” was the evaluation target. In the other half of the trials, other-relevant events were presented (e.g., “Ted hits Paul”) and “other” was the evaluation target. We predicted greater self-serving evaluations for actor than for recipient and this effect was stronger for negative relative to positive information based on the abundant evidence for negativity bias (Carretié et al., 2001; Hilgard et al., 2014). At the neural level, we expected that activities in mPFC, ACC and OFC would be associated with the impact of the actor-recipient role on self-processing in emotional interpersonal events, especially in negative events. Previous studies have claimed that dmPFC was a region associated with self-evaluation processes (Northoff and Bermpohl, 2004; Korn et al., 2012) and it has also been argued that people may consume more cognitive resources to engage in self-evaluation when they played the role of recipient. Therefore, the self-related activations (self-other) in dmPFC would be greater when the role of recipient relative to actor was played in negative interpersonal events and greater self-related dmPFC activations would be associated with longer reaction time (RT) for self-related evaluation processes. In addition, we expected that activities in OFC would be greater when participants made less self-serving judgments in the recipient relative to actor role, based on previous findings that OFC was associated with accurate self-view (Beer et al., 2006; Beer, 2007; Beer and Hughes, 2010).
Material and Methods
Participants
Twenty-nine right-handed volunteers from the university community with normal or corrected-to-normal vision participated in this experiment. Four participants had to be excluded due to excessive head movements, leaving 25 subjects for analyses (13 females, aged from 20 to 30 years, M = 24.16, SD = 2.59). The twenty-five participants included in the data analysis consisted of 5 undergraduate students (all females) and 20 graduate students (12 males and 8 females). None of the participants reported a significant abnormal neurological history. All the participants gave informed consent before scanning and were paid RMB50 for their participation. This study was approved by the Ethical Committee of East China Normal University.
Materials
Verbs with mean valence ratings of larger than 6 (positively) and smaller than 3 (negatively) based on 9-point pleasant-unpleasant ratings were selected from Chinese Affective Words System (CAWS; Wang et al., 2008). Forty two-character Chinese verbs (20 positively valenced and 20 negatively valenced) were used in the present study. These two sets of verbs differed in valence (t = 75.22, p < 0.001) but equated for arousal, familiarity and frequency.
These verbs were used to construct one-sentence interpersonal events. Each sentence was comprised of one subject, one verb and one object, such as “Ted hits Paul”. Each verb was used four times to construct four different types of conditions, according to the crossing of all possible combinations of evaluation Target (Self vs. Other) and Role (Actor vs. Recipient). An example of a verb with positive valence (i.e., like) is given below with four respective sentences and their judgment questions.
A1. I like John. How likely is it that I am that kind of person?
A2. Tom likes me. How likely is it that I am that kind of person?
B1. Ted likes Paul. How likely is it that Ted is that kind of person?
B2. Ted likes Paul. How likely is it that Paul is that kind of person?
For self-relevant events, “self” was randomly assigned as the actor (e.g., A1) or recipient (e.g., A2) and self was the target of evaluation. For other-relevant events, both the actor (e.g., B1) and the recipient (e.g., B2) would become the targets of evaluation separately. Similarly, the negatively valenced verbs were also used to construct four different types of conditions.
Procedure
Participants were asked to complete 160 trials in the scanner with 20 trials in each condition. In each trial, participants were presented with a one-sentence interpersonal event, a question and a 4-point scale (Figure 1). They were required to read the sentence and to answer the question within 5 s on the 4-point scale by pressing the corresponding button (1 = very unlikely, 2 = moderately unlikely, 3 = moderately likely, 4 = very likely). Once the participant responded, a red circle would appear around the selected number and lasted for 1.5 s. Each trial was jittered with inter-stimulus intervals from 2 s to 6 s, during which a black fixation cross was presented against a white background. All the trials were presented in a random order.
Figure 1. Examples of situation stimuli used in the experiment. Each stimulus included a one-sentence interpersonal event, a question and a 4-point scale. (A) Self-relevant negative interpersonal event and the evaluation target “self” played the role of actor. (B) Self-relevant negative event and the evaluation target “self” played the role of recipient.
fMRI Image Acquisition and Analysis
Scanning was carried out on a 3T Siemens scanner at the Functional MRI Lab (East China Normal University, Shanghai). For functional images, 35 slices were acquired using a gradient-echo echo-planar imaging (EPI) sequence (TR = 2200 ms, TE = 30 ms, FOV = 220 mm, matrix size = 64 × 64, slice thickness = 3 mm, gap = 0.3 mm). Before the functional run, a high-resolution structural image was acquired using a T1-weighted, multiplanar reconstruction (MPR) sequence (TR = 1900 ms, TE = 3.42 ms, 192 slices, slice thickness = 1 mm, FOV = 256 mm, matrix size = 256 × 256).
Data preprocessing and statistical analyses were performed with Statistical Parametric Mapping (SPM12, Wellcome Department of Cognitive Neurology, London). Preprocessing included discarding the first five functional images to allow for scanner equilibrium effects, rigid-body motion correction, spatial normalization into the MNI space (resampled at 2 × 2 × 2 mm3 voxels), and spatial smoothing (using an 8-mm full-width half maximum isotropic Gaussian kernel). A high-pass filter with a cutoff period of 128 s was applied.
At the first level, eight conditions were defined according to Target (Self vs. Other), Role (Actor vs. Recipient) and Valence (Positive vs. Negative). They were modeled using a canonical hemodynamic response function with a temporal derivative. We have chosen the onset of the stimulus as the onset time point and the RT from the stimulus onset to button press as the duration (epoch with variable time length). Six regressors modeling movement-related variance and one modeling the overall mean were also employed in the design matrix. A general linear model analysis created 8 contrast images for each participant summarizing differences of interest. The eight first-level contrast images from each participant were then analyzed at the second level employing a random-effects model (flexible factorial design in SPM12).
We used F-contrast to analyze the Target × Valence interaction effect across roles. Then, in order to examine the effect of the actor-recipient role on self-processing in emotional situations, F-contrast was further used to examine the Target × Valence × Role interaction. Areas of activation were identified as significant only if they passed the threshold of p < 0.05 family-wise error (FWE) corrected for multiple comparisons at the cluster level with an underlying voxel level of p < 0.0001 uncorrected, unless otherwise indicated. Marsbar toolbox (Brett et al., 2002) was used to extract beta-values from the activated brain regions.
Results
Task Performance
A 2(Target: Self vs. Other) × 2(Valence: Positive vs. Negative) × 2(Role: Actor vs. Recipient) repeated measures ANOVA revealed that participants’ attribution ratings were characterized by a significant Target × Valence × Role interaction (F(1,24) = 31.79, p < 0.001) (Table 1). When the evaluation target took the role of actor, repeated measures ANOVA revealed that the interaction between target and valence was significant (F(1,24) = 103.01, p < 0.001). Paired-sample t test revealed that participants’ attribution ratings of self were significantly higher than those of other in positive events (t(24) = 2.68, p = 0.01), and participants’ attribution ratings were significant lower to self than other in negative events (t(24) = 9.68, p < 0.001). When the evaluation target played the role of recipient, repeated measures ANOVA revealed that the interaction between target and valence was also significant (F(1,24) = 42.84, p < 0.001). Paired-sample t test revealed that participants’ attribution ratings were significantly lower to self than other in negative events (t(24) = 7.68, p < 0.001), whereas there was no significant difference between self and other in positive events (t(24) = 0.83, p = 0.42).
Table 1. Means and standard deviations of participants’ attribution ratings of self and other when the evaluation target played the role of actor and recipient in positive and negative interpersonal events.
Moreover, paired-sample t test revealed a greater attribution rating difference (self-other) when participants played the role of actor relative to recipient in positive (t(24) = 3.06, p = 0.005) and negative (t(24) = 4.85, p < 0.001) interpersonal events. These results indicated that participants were more likely to endorse positive events to self and isolate self from negative events, and this attribution bias was greater when participants played the role of actor relative to recipient.
Repeated measures ANOVA revealed that participants’ RT was characterized by main effects of Target (F(1,24) = 26.93, p < 0.001), Valence (F(1,24) = 10.05, p = 0.004) and Role (F(1,24) = 36.11, p < 0.001) (Table 2). Judgments in the self condition (M = 2319 ms, SD = 93) were made significantly faster than those in the other condition (M = 2716 ms, SD = 122). Judgments in the positive condition (M = 2475 ms, SD = 104) were made significantly faster than those in the negative condition (M = 2560 ms, SD = 100). Judgments in the actor condition (M = 2439 ms, SD = 98) were also made significantly faster than those in the recipient condition (M = 2596 ms, SD = 106).
Table 2. Means and standard deviations of participants’ RT (ms) of judgment for self and other when the evaluation target played the role of actor and recipient in positive and negative interpersonalevents.
fMRI Results
Target × Valence Interaction
The F-contrast of Target (Self vs. Other) × Valence (Positive vs. Negative) interaction across roles revealed significant activations in pregenual ACC (pgACC) (MNI 16, 48, 8) and posterior dACC (pdACC) (MNI 6, −18, 42) (Figure 2; Table 3). Additionally, this contrast revealed significant activations in postcentral gyrus (MNI 44, −24, 54), lingual gyrus (MNI −12, −70, 4), thalamus (MNI 12, −18, 16) and middle temporal gyrus (mTG) (MNI 50, −32, −2).
Figure 2. Brain activities in Target × Valence interaction effect across roles. Activities in pgACC (MNI 16, 48, 8) and pdACC (MNI 6, −18, 42) were greater for self than other only when interpersonal events were negative. Error bars indicated standard errors of mean beta-values. Asterisks indicated that beta-values were greater for self than other(*p < 0.05, **p < 0.001).
Table 3. Identification of BOLD signal changes association with the Target × Valence interaction and Target × Valence × Role interaction.
Parameter estimates across pgACC and pdACC were extracted. Paired-sample t test revealed similar pattern of activations in pgACC and pdACC, that is, in both the actor and recipient conditions, the activities in these two regions were greater for self than other only in negative events (actor: t(24) = 3.41, p = 0.002, and recipient: t(24) = 4.95, p < 0.001 for pgACC; actor: t(24) = 2.47, p = 0.02, and recipient: t(24) = 5.16, p < 0.001 for pdACC) but not in positive events (actor: t(24) = 1.04, p = 0.31, and recipient: t(24) = 1.76, p = 0.10 for pgACC; actor: t(24) = 0.36, p = 0.72, and recipient: t(24) = 1.23, p = 0.23 for pdACC).
Target × Valence × Role Interaction
The F-contrast of Target × Valence × Role interaction revealed significant activations in dmPFC (MNI −18, 38, 50) and OFC (MNI −32, 60, 0) (Figure 3; Table 3). Additionally, this contrast revealed activations in lingual gyrus (MNI 16, −58, −2) and calcarine gyrus (MNI 16, −68, 8). We extracted the parameter estimates across dmPFC and OFC. Paired-sample t test revealed that, in both positive and negative events, the brain activity in dmPFC was greater for self than other in both the actor (positive: t(24) = 3.74, p = 0.001; negative: t(24) = 2.08, p = 0.05) and recipient conditions (positive: t(24) = 3.57, p = 0.002; negative: t(24) = 6.93, p < 0.001). Interestingly, paired-sample t-test revealed greater self-related dmPFC activations (self-other) for the recipient relative to actor in negative interpersonal events (t(24) = 4.61, p < 0.001) but not in positive events (t(24) = 1.77, p = 0.09). The dmPFC has been argued to be associated with self-evaluation processes (Northoff and Bermpohl, 2004; Beer and Hughes, 2010; Han et al., 2010). It could be assumed that people consumed more cognitive resources to engage in self-evaluation processes when they played the role of recipient in negative events. Correlation analyses were performed to determine brain regions whose BOLD signal change detected from the self vs. other contrast when the recipient role was played in negative events varied with the corresponding RT (after log-transformation). A significant cluster of activation was detected in dmPFC (MNI −12, 44, 48, 22 voxels, survived a voxel-level intensity threshold of p < 0.001, uncorrected), indicating greater self-related dmPFC activations were associated with longer RT for self relative to other for the recipient in negative events. We then overlapped activations from correlation analyses with activations from the F-contrast of Target × Valence × Role interaction. Clusters in dmPFC overlapped (Figure 4).
Figure 3. Brain activities in Target × Valence × Role interaction. Activities in dmPFC (MNI −18, 38, 50) were greater for self than other when the actor or recipient role was played in both positive and negative events. Activities in orbitofrontal cortex (OFC) (MNI −32, 60, 0) were greater for self than other only when the evaluation target played the role of recipient in negative interpersonal events. Error bars indicated standard errors of mean beta-values. Asterisks indicated that beta-values were greater for self than other (*p < 0.05, **p < 0.001).
Figure 4. Overlap. Clusters in dmPFC overlapped between correlation analyses and the Target × Valence × Role interaction (p < 0.001, uncorrected).
Paired-sample t test revealed that, in positive events, the brain activity in OFC showed no difference for self than other in both the actor (t(24) = 1.89, p = 0.07) and recipient (t(24) = 1.38, p = 0.18) conditions. In negative events, the brain activity in OFC was greater for self than other only in the recipient condition (t(24) = 5.98, p < 0.001) but not in the actor condition (t(24) = 0.65, p = 0.52). Moreover, paired-sample t test revealed that OFC showed greater self-related activations when participants played the role of recipient relative to actor in negative interpersonal events (t(24) = 3.69, p = 0.001) but not in positive events (t(24) = 0.84, p = 0.41).
Discussion
The present study explored the impact of the actor-recipient role on neural responses to self in emotional situations. Results of attribution ratings revealed that people tended to endorse positive events as self-relevant only when they performed the role of actor, whereas they were more likely to isolate self from negative events when they played the role of actor relative to recipient, indicating greater self-serving evaluations when actor role was played in negative situations. Activities in pgACC and pdACC were greater for self than other only when the interpersonal events were negative. Moreover, activities in dmPFC were greater for self than other when the actor or recipient role was played in both positive and negative events, whereas activities in OFC were greater while responding to self than to other only when the evaluation target played the role of recipient in negative interpersonal events. Importantly, OFC and dmPFC showed greater self-related activations when participants played the role of recipient relative to actor in negative interpersonal events. These results showed that the actor-recipient role affected neural responses to self in emotional situations, especially when a recipient role was played in negative situations.
Previous research had claimed that people had the tendency to make claims that cast the self in a favorable light (Sedikides et al., 1998; Heine et al., 1999; Duval and Silvia, 2002; Mezulis et al., 2004). People have a need to view themselves positively (Heine et al., 1999; Mezulis et al., 2004). However, participants’ positive self-image would be threatened when they are involved in negative interpersonal events. Thus, encountering negative events would conflict with the need to maintain individuals’ positive outlook. In order to diminish the conflict, people tend to isolate themselves from negative events (Miller and Ross, 1975; Cunningham et al., 1979; Duval and Silvia, 2002; Mezulis et al., 2004). This tendency was confirmed by attribution ratings data. That is, participants were more likely to endorse negative events as self-irrelevant. At the neural level, greater activations of pgACC and pdACC were found during response to self than to other only when the interpersonal events were negative. ACC has long been conceived in neuroimaging studies to have a regulatory role with respect to limbic regions. It is involved in generating negative emotional responses and is important for emotional conflict monitoring and regulation (Carter et al., 1998; Bush et al., 2000; Northoff and Bermpohl, 2004; Etkin et al., 2011). The greater self-related activations in pgACC and pdACC in negative events further confirmed the role of ACC in monitoring and regulating the conflict between negative information and the maintenance of a positive outlook.
It has been suggested that the dmPFC is engaged in self-evaluation and self-related reappraisal (Northoff and Bermpohl, 2004; Northoff et al., 2006; Han et al., 2010; Korn et al., 2012). The more efforts are involved in the cognitive processing in self-evaluation, the greater dmPFC is activated (Beer and Hughes, 2010). In the current study, dmPFC exhibited greater self-related activations in the recipient relative to actor condition only in negative interpersonal events. Correlation analysis further revealed increased self-related activations in this dmPFC for people with longer RT for self relative to other for the recipient in negative events (observed at a looser threshold), indicating greater cognitive self-evaluation processing in negative events when the recipient role was played. Moreover, there were reduced self-serving evaluations for the recipient relative to actor in negative events found in our study. Previous studies had demonstrated that self-serving evaluations were heuristic judgments and self-serving judgments required fewer cognitive resources than the other judgments (Dunning et al., 1989; Chambers and Windschitl, 2004; Beer and Hughes, 2010). Reduced self-serving evaluations for the recipient relative to actor in negative events in our study indicated more cognitive processing when self received negative events, providing further support for the role of dmPFC in self-evaluation.
The current study also showed that activities in OFC were greater while responding to self than to other only when the evaluation target played the role of recipient in negative interpersonal events. That is, OFC was modulated by the actor-recipient role in negative interpersonal events. Previous studies had shown that OFC was associated with accurate self-view (Beer et al., 2006; Beer, 2007; Beer and Hughes, 2010). More heuristic self-serving evaluations were associated with less activities in OFC (De Martino et al., 2006; Beer and Hughes, 2010). In the present research, participants made more heuristic self-serving judgments when they played the role of actor than when they played the role of recipient in negative interpersonal events. This results were consistent with the neural activations in OFC, that is, the self-related activations were greater when the role of recipient was played than that of actor was played in negative interpersonal events.
Previous studies had revealed the engagement of mTG in attribution decisions (Blackwood et al., 2003; Harris et al., 2005; Seidel et al., 2010; Cabanis et al., 2013) and activities in thalamus in self-related and/or emotion-related processing (Seidel et al., 2010; Somerville et al., 2010; Turk et al., 2011; Korn et al., 2012). These brain regions were also activated in the interaction between target and valence in our data, providing further support for the role of mTG and thalamus in self-related emotional attribution. In addition, this study has a limitation which should be noted. Arousal-matched positive and negative materials were used in the present research. However, valence and arousal as two dimensions of emotion have been argued to work in an interactive way (Robinson et al., 2004; Citron et al., 2014). Future studies should further examine the impact of the actor-recipient role on self-related processing in different valence and arousal situations.
In conclusion, the current study further illustrated neural models of social cognition by examining the impact of the actor-recipient role on self-processing in emotional situations. The pgACC, pdACC, OFC, and dmPFC were involved in emotional self-processing. Self-related pgACC and pdACC activations in negative events indicated its association with monitoring conflict in negative self-related processing. More importantly, dmPFC and OFC were modulated differently by the actor-recipient role in emotional self-processing, with dmPFC associated with self-evaluation processes and OFC engaged in accurate self-view. Taken together, these results demonstrated that the actor-recipient role affected neural responses to self in emotional situations.
Author Contributions
All authors were involved in the design and implementation of the study and the writing of the manuscript. Authors Xiaoyan Wang, Li Zheng, Xuemei Cheng, Lin Li and Xiuyan Guo devised the concept and supervised the study. Authors Xuemei Cheng and Qianfeng Wang collected the imaging data. Authors Xiaoyan Wang, Li Zheng carried out the analysis. Authors Li Zheng, Xuemei Cheng, Lin Li and Xiuyan Guo joined in the interpretation of data. Authors Lining Sun joined in the writing of the manuscript.
Conflict of Interest Statement
The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.
Acknowledgments
This research was supported by National Natural Science Foundation of China (31271090), Innovation Program of Shanghai Municipal Education Commission (12ZS046), Overseas Publication Project of Art and Social Science of East China Normal University in 2014 (2014HW046), and National Social Science Foundation of China (14AZD106).
References
Beer, J. S. (2007). The default self: feeling good or being right? Trends Cogn. Sci. 11, 187–189. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2007.02.004
PubMed Abstract | Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Beer, J. S., and Hughes, B. L. (2010). Neural systems of social comparison and the “above-average” effect. Neuroimage 49, 2671–2679. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.10.075
PubMed Abstract | Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Beer, J. S., John, O. P., Scabini, D., and Knight, R. T. (2006). Orbitofrontal cortex and social behavior: integrating self-monitoring and emotion-cognition interactions. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 18, 871–879. doi: 10.1162/jocn.2006.18.6.871
PubMed Abstract | Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Beer, J. S., Lombardo, M. V., and Bhanji, J. P. (2010). Roles of medial prefrontal cortex and orbitofrontal cortex in self-evaluation. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 22, 2108–2119. doi: 10.1162/jocn.2009.21359
PubMed Abstract | Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Blackwood, N., Bentall, R., ffytche, D. H., Simmons, A., Murray, R., and Howard, R. (2003). Self-responsibility and the self-serving bias: an fMRI investigation of causal attributions. Neuroimage 20, 1076–1085. doi: 10.1016/s1053-8119(03)00331-8
PubMed Abstract | Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Brett, M., Anton, J.-L., Valabregue, R., and Poline, J.-B. (2002). “Region of interest analysis using an SPM toolbox,” in the 8th International Conference on Functional Mapping of the Human Brain (Sendai, Japan).
Bush, G., Luu, P., and Posner, M. I. (2000). Cognitive and emotional influences in anterior cingulate cortex. Trends Cogn. Sci. 4, 215–222. doi: 10.1016/s1364-6613(00)01483-2
PubMed Abstract | Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Cabanis, M., Pyka, M., Mehl, S., Müller, B., Loos-Jankowiak, S., Winterer, G., et al. (2013). The precuneus and the insula in self-attributional processes. Cogn. Affect. Behav. Neurosci. 13, 330–345. doi: 10.3758/s13415-012-0143-5
PubMed Abstract | Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Carretié, L., Mercado, F., Tapia, M., and Hinojosa, J. A. (2001). Emotion, attention and the ‘negativity bias’, studied through event-related potentials. Int. J. Psychophysiol. 41, 75–85. doi: 10.1016/s0167-8760(00)00195-1
PubMed Abstract | Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Carter, C. S., Braver, T. S., Barch, D. M., Botvinick, M. M., Noll, D., and Cohen, J. D. (1998). Anterior cingulate cortex, error detection and the online monitoring of performance. Science 280, 747–749. doi: 10.1126/science.280.5364.747
PubMed Abstract | Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Chambers, J. R., and Windschitl, P. D. (2004). Biases in social comparative judgments: the role of nonmotivated factors in above-average and comparative-optimism effects. Psychol. Bull. 130, 813–838. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.130.5.813
PubMed Abstract | Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Cheng, Y., Chen, C., Lin, C.-P., Chou, K.-H., and Decety, J. (2010). Love hurts: an fMRI study. Neuroimage 51, 923–929. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.02.047
PubMed Abstract | Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Citron, F. M. M., Gray, M. A., Critchley, H. D., Weekes, B. S., and Ferstl, E. C. (2014). Emotional valence and arousal affect reading in an interactive way: neuroimaging evidence for an approach-withdrawal framework. Neuropsychologia 56, 79–89. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2014.01.002
PubMed Abstract | Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Cunningham, J. D., Starr, P. A., and Kanouse, D. E. (1979). Self as actor, active observer and passive observer: implications for causal attributions. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 37, 1146–1152. doi: 10.1037//0022-3514.37.7.1146
D’Argembeau, A., Jedidi, H., Balteau, E., Bahri, M., Phillips, C., and Salmon, E. (2012). Valuing one’s self: medial prefrontal involvement in epistemic and emotive investments in self-views. Cereb. Cortex 22, 659–667. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhr144
PubMed Abstract | Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
De Martino, B., Kumaran, D., Seymour, B., and Dolan, R. J. (2006). Frames, biases and rational decision-making in the human brain. Science 313, 684–687. doi: 10.1126/science.1128356
PubMed Abstract | Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Denny, B. T., Kober, H., Wager, T. D., and Ochsner, K. N. (2012). A meta-analysis of functional neuroimaging studies of self- and other judgments reveals a spatial gradient for mentalizing in medial prefrontal cortex. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 24, 1742–1752. doi: 10.1162/jocn_a_00233
PubMed Abstract | Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Dunning, D., Meyerowitz, J. A., and Holzberg, A. D. (1989). Ambiguity and self-evaluation: the role of idiosyncratic trait definitions in self-serving assessments of ability. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 57, 1082–1090. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.57.6.1082
Duval, T. S., and Silvia, P. J. (2002). Self-awareness, probability of improvement and the self-serving bias. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 82, 49–61. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.82.1.49
PubMed Abstract | Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Etkin, A., Egner, T., and Kalisch, R. (2011). Emotional processing in anterior cingulate and medial prefrontal cortex. Trends Cogn. Sci. 15, 85–93. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2010.11.004
PubMed Abstract | Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Guo, X., Zheng, L., Zhang, W., Zhu, L., Li, J., Wang, Q., et al. (2012). Empathic neural responses to others’ pain depend on monetary reward. Soc. Cogn. Affect. Neurosci. 7, 535–541. doi: 10.1093/scan/nsr034
PubMed Abstract | Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Han, S., Gu, X., Mao, L., Ge, J., Wang, G., and Ma, Y. (2010). Neural substrates of self-referential processing in Chinese Buddhists. Soc. Cogn. Affect. Neurosci. 5, 332–339. doi: 10.1093/scan/nsp027
PubMed Abstract | Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Harris, L. T., Todorov, A., and Fiske, S. T. (2005). Attributions on the brain: neuro-imaging dispositional inferences, beyond theory of mind. Neuroimage 28, 763–769. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2005.05.021
PubMed Abstract | Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Heine, S. J., Lehman, D. R., Markus, H. R., and Kitayama, S. (1999). Is there a universal need for positive self-regard? Psychol. Rev. 106, 766–794. doi: 10.1037/0033-295x.106.4.766
PubMed Abstract | Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Hilgard, J., Weinberg, A., Hajcak Proudfit, G., and Bartholow, B. D. (2014). The negativity bias in affective picture processing depends on top-down and bottom-up motivational significance. Emotion 14, 940–949. doi: 10.1037/a0036791
PubMed Abstract | Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Huang, Y.-X., and Luo, Y.-J. (2006). Temporal course of emotional negativity bias: an ERP study. Neurosci. Lett. 398, 91–96. doi: 10.1016/j.neulet.2005.12.074
PubMed Abstract | Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Jones, E. E., and Nisbett, R. E. (1971). “The actor and the observer: divergent perceptions of the causes of behavior,” in Attribution: Perceiving the Causes of Behavior, eds E. E. Jones, D. E. Kanouse, R. E. Kelley, R. E. Nisbett, S. Valins and B. Weiner (Morristown: General Learning Press), 79–94.
Kasof, J., and Lee, J. Y. (1993). Implicit causality as implicit salience. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 65, 877–891. doi: 10.1037//0022-3514.65.5.877
Kelley, W. M., Macrae, C. N., Wyland, C. L., Caglar, S., Inati, S., and Heatherton, T. F. (2002). Finding the self? An event-related fMRI study. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 14, 785–794. doi: 10.1162/08989290260138672
PubMed Abstract | Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Korn, C. W., Prehn, K., Park, S. Q., Walter, H., and Heekeren, H. R. (2012). Positively biased processing of self-relevant social feedback. J. Neurosci. 32, 16832–16844. doi: 10.1523/jneurosci.3016-12.2012
PubMed Abstract | Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Malle, B. F. (2006). The actor-observer asymmetry in attribution: a (surprising) meta-analysis. Psychol. Bull. 132, 895–919. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.132.6.895
PubMed Abstract | Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Mezulis, A. H., Abramson, L. Y., Hyde, J. S., and Hankin, B. L. (2004). Is there a universal positivity bias in attributions? A meta-analytic review of individual, developmental and cultural differences in the self-serving attributional bias. Psychol. Bull. 130, 711–747. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.130.5.711
PubMed Abstract | Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Miller, D. T., and Ross, M. (1975). Self-serving biases in the attribution of causality: fact or fiction? Psychol. Bull. 82, 213–225. doi: 10.1037/h0076486
Moran, J., Macrae, C., Heatherton, T. F., Wyland, C., and Kelley, W. M. (2006). Neuroanatomical evidence for distinct cognitive and affective components of self. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 18, 1586–1594. doi: 10.1162/jocn.2006.18.9.1586
PubMed Abstract | Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Northoff, G., and Bermpohl, F. (2004). Cortical midline structures and the self. Trends Cogn. Sci. 8, 102–107. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2004.01.004
PubMed Abstract | Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Northoff, G., Heinzel, A., de Greck, M., Bermpohl, F., Dobrowolny, H., and Panksepp, J. (2006). Self-referential processing in our brain—A meta-analysis of imaging studies on the self. Neuroimage 31, 440–457. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2005.12.002
PubMed Abstract | Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Ochsner, K. N., Ray, R. D., Cooper, J. C., Robertson, E. R., Chopra, S., Gabrieli, J. D., et al. (2004). For better or for worse: neural systems supporting the cognitive down-and up-regulation of negative emotion. Neuroimage 23, 483–499. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2004.06.030
PubMed Abstract | Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Robinson, M. D., Storbeck, J., Meier, B. P., and Kirkeby, B. S. (2004). Watch out! that could be dangerous: valence-arousal interactions in evaluative processing. Pers. Soc. Psychol. Bull. 30, 1472–1484. doi: 10.1177/0146167204266647
PubMed Abstract | Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Sedikides, C., Campbell, W. K., Reeder, G. D., and Elliot, A. J. (1998). The self-serving bias in relational context. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 74, 378–386. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.74.2.378
Seidel, E.-M., Eickhoff, S. B., Kellermann, T., Schneider, F., Gur, R. C., Habel, U., et al. (2010). Who is to blame? Neural correlates of causal attribution in social situations. Soc. Neurosci. 5, 335–350. doi: 10.1080/17470911003615997
PubMed Abstract | Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Sharot, T., Riccardi, A. M., Raio, C. M., and Phelps, E. A. (2007). Neural mechanisms mediating optimism bias. Nature 450, 102–105. doi: 10.1038/nature06280
PubMed Abstract | Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Somerville, L. H., Kelley, W. M., and Heatherton, T. F. (2010). Self-esteem modulates medial prefrontal cortical responses to evaluative social feedback. Cereb. Cortex 20, 3005–3013. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhq049
PubMed Abstract | Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Turk, D. J., van Bussel, K., Waiter, G. D., and Macrae, C. N. (2011). Mine and me: exploring the neural basis of object ownership. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 23, 3657–3668. doi: 10.1162/jocn_a_00042
PubMed Abstract | Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Keywords: self-evaluation, actor-recipient role, ACC, dmPFC, OFC
Citation: Wang X, Zheng L, Cheng X, Li L, Sun L, Wang Q and Guo X (2015) Actor-recipient role affects neural responses to self in emotional situations. Front. Behav. Neurosci. 9:83. doi: 10.3389/fnbeh.2015.00083
Received: 02 December 2014; Accepted: 17 March 2015;
Published online: 15 April 2015.
Edited by:
Lars Schwabe, University of Hamburg, GermanyReviewed by:
Chen-Min Yeh, Salk Institute for Biological Studies, USAC. W. E. M Quaedflieg, Maastricht University, Netherlands
Copyright © 2015 Wang, Zheng, Cheng, Li, Sun, Wang and Guo. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution and reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) or licensor are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.
*Correspondence: Xiuyan Guo, Shanghai Key Laboratory of Magnetic Resonance and School of Psychology and Cognitive Science, East China Normal University, North Zhongshan Road 3663, Shanghai, SH, 200062, China xyguo@psy.ecnu.edu.cn