Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Behav. Neurosci., 23 July 2014
Sec. Learning and Memory

Hygienic grooming is induced by contact chemicals in Drosophila melanogaster

  • 1Division of Creative Research and Development of Humanosphere, Research Institute for Sustainable Humanosphere, Kyoto University, Uji, Japan
  • 2Institut National de la Recherche Agronomique, UMR iEES-Paris, Versailles, France
  • 3Laboratoire Evolution, Génomes, Spéciation, Centre National de la Recherche Scientifique, UPR 9034, Gif-sur-Yvette, France
  • 4Département Sciences de la Vie et Santé, AgroParisTech, Paris, France

In social insects, grooming is considered as a behavioral defense against pathogen and parasite infections since it contributes to remove microbes from their cuticle. However, stimuli which trigger this behavior are not well characterized yet. We examined if activating contact chemoreceptive sensilla could trigger grooming activities in Drosophila melanogaster. We monitored the grooming responses of decapitated flies to compounds known to activate the immune system, e.g., dead Escherichia coli (Ec) and lipopolysaccharides (LPS), and to tastants such as quinine, sucrose, and salt. LPS, quinine, and Ec were quite effective in triggering grooming movements when touching the distal border of the wings and the legs, while sucrose had no effect. Contact chemoreceptors are necessary and sufficient to elicit such responses, as grooming could not be elicited by LPS in poxn mutants deprived of external taste sensilla, and as grooming was elicited by light when a channel rhodopsin receptor was expressed in bitter-sensitive cells expressing Gr33a. Contact chemoreceptors distributed along the distal border of the wings respond to these tastants by an increased spiking activity, in response to quinine, Ec, LPS, sucrose, and KCl. These results demonstrate for the first time that bacterial compounds trigger grooming activities in D. melanogaster, and indicate that contact chemoreceptors located on the wings participate in the detection of such chemicals.

Introduction

Insects, especially Diptera, devote a considerable amount of time to self-grooming. Grooming involves brushing the body and the wings with the legs, and cleaning the legs and the antenna with the mouthparts. In cockroaches, grooming helps cleaning external chemosensory receptors (Böröczky et al., 2013). Stereotyped grooming occur in response to mechanical stimulation of dorsal bristles in flies (Burg et al., 1993), of eye bristles in cricket (Hensler, 1986), or of leg and wing hairs in locusts (Burrows and Newland, 1994; Newland and Burrows, 1994, 1997; Page and Matheson, 2004). Grooming or scratching is also observed in response to noxious molecules in flies (Dethier, 1972) and in locusts (Newland, 1998; Page and Matheson, 2004). Grooming thus serves a number of purposes, related to maintaining the integrity of the body and avoiding noxious stimuli.

In analogy with the documented roles of grooming in other terrestrial animals, grooming may help insects to reduce the impact of ectoparasites (Mooring et al., 2004). In social insects like ants and termites, self- and allo-grooming activities contribute to reduce the pathogenicity of bacterial diseases and entomopathogenic fungi (Boucias et al., 1996; Shimizu and Yamaji, 2002; Traniello et al., 2002; Yanagawa and Shimizu, 2007; Aubert and Richard, 2008; Yanagawa et al., 2008). Self-grooming activities may also be important for solitary insects such as flies, which live in an environment littered with microorganisms (Wölfle et al., 2009; Stensmyr et al., 2012). In line with this hypothesis, flies groom themselves to clean a dust applied to them (Phillis et al., 1993). Social situations increase the rate of grooming, especially in females (Connolly, 1968) and grooming is always performed after oviposition (Yang et al., 2008), i.e., situations with a higher risk of contamination from microbes.

If one purpose of grooming is directly linked with the need of cleaning the body from potential ectoparasites (Zhukovskaya et al., 2013), then it would make sense that grooming were triggered by signals emanating from the microorganisms. One of these stimuli is probably noxiousness (Elwood, 2011), as spores sticking to the cuticula need first to pierce it to invade the body. Volatile stimuli could also be involved. In flies, specific olfactory receptors are devoted to the detection of harmful microbes (Stensmyr et al., 2012). In termites, we have shown that odors from pathogenic spores are detected by the olfactory system (Yanagawa et al., 2009) and that such odors trigger allo-grooming (Yanagawa et al., 2010, 2011, 2012). This opens the possibility that contact chemicals related to microbes could also trigger grooming in insects.

In this work, we examined if activating contact chemoreceptors triggers grooming activities in adult flies Drosophila melanogaster. In order to dispose of a simple behavioral test, we used decapitated flies. The main experimental advantage of this approach, is that decapitated flies do not fly and that they stand on their legs and remain responsive for up to 20 h (Vandervorst and Ghysen, 1980). In crickets, complete grooming sequences can be executed after transection of the connectives anterior to the mesothoracic ganglion (Berkowitz and Laurent, 1996a,b). In flies, localized grooming responses to mechanical stimulation of sensilla located on different segments of the body can be observed after decapitation (Vandervorst and Ghysen, 1980). This response is considered as a simple reflex that involves a reduced circuitry, which has been studied extensively (Corfas and Dudai, 1989; Burg et al., 1993; Phillis et al., 1993; Yellman et al., 1997; Ashton et al., 2001; Kays et al., 2014).

As contact chemoreceptors are present mostly on the legs and on the wings in decapitated flies (Stocker, 1994), we stimulated these appendages with bacterial compounds, e.g., dead Escherichia coli (Ec) and lipopolysaccharides (LPS), and with non-volatile chemicals known to be either aversive (quinine), appetitive (sucrose) or both depending on the concentration (NaCl and KCl) (Meunier et al., 2003). We first determined which substances and doses were more effective in triggering grooming upon contact with the distal border of the wings and the legs. We then evaluated if mechanosensation alone was sufficient to elicit a response to LPS by using mutant flies deprived of external taste sensilla (Nottebohm et al., 1994). We further tested if chemosensation alone was sufficient to induce a grooming response by inducing optogenetic specific activation of taste neurons responding to bitter molecules (Nagel et al., 2003; Hornstein et al., 2009). Finally, by using electrophysiology, we tested if the contact chemoreceptors located on the margin of the distal part of the wing were sensitive to the tastants tested in this study. Our observations support the hypothesis that contact chemicals play a decisive role in triggering grooming activities in D. melanogaster.

Materials and Methods

Flies

D. melanogaster flies were maintained on a standard cornmeal agar food at 20°C and 80% humidity. Most experiments were done with Canton Special (CS) flies. Flies devoid of external taste sensilla were Poxn70/Cyo; MKRS, Sb/TM6B, Tb (Poxn70).

In order to generate flies in which optogenetic activation of taste neurons responding to bitter substances was possible, we crossed flies carrying a Gr33a-Gal4 construction (generously given by J. Carlson) with flies carrying UAS-channel rhodopsin (UAS-CHR2) (Bloomington Drosophila Stock Center, stock no. 28995). Since the balancer chromosome of this construction carried the defect curly (CyO), we could select in the progeny individuals which did not express the construction (called mutant) and others (siblings) which expressed the phenotype. For Gr33a, the genotype was Gr33a-Gal4[1] /CyO; Dr/TM3, Sb, Ser × UAS-H134R-CHR2, where the siblings expressed the curly wing phenotype and the mutants had normal wings. During development, the larvae were fed on normal medium added with 1 mM trans-retinal (Hornstein et al., 2009). Adults were stimulated with blue light during 3 min, using an LED Laser at 480 nm (COO-pE-100F-WH1-20, CoolLED, UK).

Bacteria

The TOP 10 strain of E. coli was incubated in liquid LB medium. E. coli was washed by distilled water and heated at 95°C for 5 min. From this medium, we collected a 1.4 × 109/ml bacterial suspension (as measured with an absorption spectrometer). This suspension was subsequently diluted 100, 102, 104, and 106 fold.

Chemicals

LPS (L2630, Lipopolysaccharides from Escherichia coli 0111:B4, Sigma), sucrose, quinine, NaCl, and KCl were provided by Sigma-Aldrich and dissolved in distilled water.

Grooming Test

All behavioral observations were made on decapitated flies: such flies were reported to remain standing and responsive to stimuli during up to 20 h (Vandervorst and Ghysen, 1980); in our experimental conditions, flies looked responsive and alive during at least 2–3 h. Flies were lightly anesthetized by placing them on ice for 3–5 min. They were then placed under a stereoscopic microscope, and 10 flies were beheaded by a single cut at the neck made with micro-scissors. Flies woke up within 2–3 min. They were placed into an upright position and allowed to recover during about 10 min. The bioassays were performed at room temperature by placing the flies on a filter paper. We used a sharpened toothpick previously soaked into the test solution, to gently touch one of their appendage (wing, foreleg, or hindleg). The subsequent grooming activities were monitored and scored up to 3 min after the stimulation. 4-day old CS flies were tested with E. coli in suspension into water, with LPS, sucrose, quinine, and NaCl diluted in water. Poxn70 mutants were tested with LPS. Controls were performed by stimulating flies with distilled water. Age dependent responses were examined with LPS on 1, 7, and 10-day old CS flies. Each chemical was tested on 20 female and 20 male flies.

For optogenetic experiments, flies were exposed to a continuous pulse of blue light for 3 min, over the whole body. Headless flies were placed on a filter paper as usual, but the experiment was conducted in a dark condition. The intensity of the grooming response was scaled from 0 to 5 (see table in Figure 3B). 4-day old flies were used for all tests.

Visit Rate Test

In order to further evaluate the impact of LPS on another type of behavior, we recorded the number of flies visiting agar with or without a chemical treatment. This test monitors if flies avoid walking on a substrate or on the contrary spend more time on it (Marella et al., 2006), by counting at regular intervals how many flies are present on each substrate. Four day old flies were starved for 22 h in the presence of a wet filter paper and then transferred to cylindrical bottles (7 cm height, 3 cm diameter). The bottom was separated into two parts by mean of a strip of aluminum foil and each part was filled with 1 ml of 1% agarose and 100 mM sucrose. One side was treated with 20 μl of 10 mg/ml LPS. As a control, we proposed flies a situation where they were given the choice between agar and agar to check if their distribution was symmetrical, and between agar and agar with 100 mM sucrose to check the discriminative power of this test. Approximately 40 flies were placed in a bottle and allowed to explore the agarose for 30 min. Digital pictures of each bottle were taken every 30 s and the number of flies standing on each substrate was manually counted. A taste preference index was calculated as PI = (number flies on the test side - number flies on the water side)/(total number of flies). Data shown were obtained from 10 replicates.

Electrophysiological Recordings

Flies (4-day old) were secured to a support with tape and electrically grounded via a silver electrode contacting a drop of electrocardiogram gel (Redux® Gel, Parker Laboratories Inc., USA) placed over the abdomen. Recordings were performed on the wing part near LV2 (Figure 4A, square a). Taste bristles of the wing margin (Figure 4, ▲ in B and arrows in C) were stimulated by covering their tip with a glass electrode containing an electrolyte (1 mM KCl) and the stimulus during 2 s. To avoid adaptation, consecutive stimulations were applied at least 2 min apart. The stimulus tested were E. coli, LPS, sucrose, quinine, and NaCl at the same concentrations as in the grooming tests (see above). As a control, we used increasing concentrations of KCl (1, 10, 100 mM and 1 M). Recordings were performed on 10 females and 10 males for each stimulus category.

The active electrode was connected to a preamplifier (TastePROBE DTP-02, Syntech) (Marion-Poll and van der Pers, 1996), and the electric signals were further amplified and filtered (CyberAmp 320, Axon Instrument, Inc., gain = 200, 8th-order Bessel pass-band filter = 1–2800 Hz). These signals were digitized (DT9803, Data Translation; sampling rate = 10 kHz, 16 bits), stored on computer, and analyzed using dbWave (Marion-Poll, 1996). Spikes were detected and analyzed using software interactive procedures of custom software dbWave. We evaluated the action-potential frequency by counting the total number of spikes during each recording.

Statistical Analysis

To examine the concentration dependent increase of the grooming behavior in headless flies with respect to sex, chemicals, and fly-strains, a multiple logistic regression (JMP 10.0 software, SAS) was applied. For the analysis of fly visits, in the 2-choices assay, a Dunnett test was applied. Optogenetic induction of grooming via blue light stimulation was analyzed using a Wilcoxon test. The number of spikes from electrophysiological recordings were analyzed by Wilcoxon test including sex as factor, and then the following factors: dorsal/ventral location (D/V: Table S3), sensillum number (1–5: Table S3), and sensillum types (V1, D1, V2, D2, V3, …, Table S3) for each sex.

Results

Grooming Responses to Chemical Stimulation

Decapitated flies are capable of self-grooming movements following a stimulation. In order to induce this behavior, we gently brushed their wing margin or one of their leg with the tip of a toothpick. Flies which never exhibited grooming in response to stimulation with a toothpick dipped into a chemical stimulus never showed grooming after stimulation with water alone. These movements mostly involved the meta-thoracic legs which were raised and moved independently in a succession of strokes, brushing the wings, the abdomen and the dorsum or which were extended under the abdomen and touched each other in a series of reciprocal sliding movements (Supplemental Movies 1, 2). We scored these movements according to their intensity using a scale of 0–3 according to the number of grooming sequences induced over a period of observation of 3 min (Figure 1A).

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Grooming responses. (A) The grooming responses of decapitated flies were scored according to their intensity and duration. A stimulus is performed by touching the wing or leg with the tip of a tooth pick (red arrow). The animal responds to a proper stimulus by moving its metathoracic legs, browsing the wings or touching each other (double side arrow head). (B) Grooming responses induced by E. coli and LPS applied to the legs and the wings of 4 d old flies. (C) Grooming responses induced by general chemicals like sucrose, quinine, and salt. (D) Grooming responses to LPS appear early on the wings but develop progressively on the legs. The lowest concentration without number “·” indicated the response to water, which was used as control solution.

Dead E. coli in suspension (Ec) and LPS induced grooming responses in 4-day old adults (Figure 1B). Responses to Ec were observed following contact to the wings (p < 0.001, multiple logistic regression), the front legs (p < 0.001), and the hind legs (p < 0.001). Responses to LPS were obtained from the wings (p < 0.001) but not from the legs (front legs p = 0.158; hind legs p = 0.029).

We then asked if grooming could be triggered by stimulating the wings and the front legs with three type of tastants known to modulate feeding activities in flies, a sweet (sucrose), a bitter (quinine), and a salty one (NaCl). As in the previous experiments, water did not induce grooming responses. Also, sucrose (p = 0.692) did not trigger any grooming response at any concentration tested (Figure 1C). NaCl and quinine triggered a dose-dependent response when applied to the wings (p < 0.001). Quinine elicited a more intense grooming response than NaCl, and also triggered a dose-dependent response of the frontlegs (p = 0.002) but not of the hindlegs (p = 0.696). NaCl was not effective as quinine, but it induced a dose-dependent response from the hindleg (p = 0.008). The other tastants did not trigger responses when brought in contact with the legs (p > 0.1). There were no obvious differences between genders (Table S1).

Since all observations were made on 4-day old flies, we asked if grooming responses to LPS could change with age. As shown by Figure 1D, grooming responses to LPS applied to the different appendages increased from day 1 to day 10, developing first upon stimulation of the wings (day 4) and then being fully expressed at day 10 upon stimulation of the legs.

Disabling Contact Chemoreceptors Abolishes Grooming Responses

The previous results raise the question of which sensory modality elicited the grooming responses in the previous experiments. As chemoreceptive sensilla usually contain not only chemoreceptive neurons but also a mechanoreceptive neuron, it may be argued that the mechanosensory contact of the toothpick was a fundamental factor underlying responses recorded. Note, however, that if this were the case, no dose-response effect should have been seen in the previous experiments (Figure 1). In order to determine if the mechanosensory stimulation provided by the toothpick contact was sufficient to trigger grooming responses, we used Poxn70 flies, which are deprived of external taste chemoreceptors and present only mechanoreceptive sensilla (Nottebohm et al., 1994). We stimulated these flies with LPS which was the stimulus most effective for triggering grooming in the previous experiments (see Figure 1).

These flies did not respond to LPS following stimulation of the wings (P = 0.235) or of the legs (forelegs: p = 0.371 and hindlegs: p = 0.401, n = 40, Figure 2A). These results thus show that mechanosensory input alone cannot induce grooming so that responses to Ec, LPS, and tastants such as quinine or NaCl had a chemoreceptive basis mediated by external contact chemoreceptive sensilla.

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Grooming responses to LPS in mutant flies. (A) Grooming responses of mutant flies deprived of their taste receptors, Poxn70. (B) Grooming responses in bitter taste-evoked flies, Gr33a-Gal4 x UAS-ChR2. Cartoon depicting a beheaded fly illuminated with blue light. When stimulated by blue light, the fly will move its metathoracic legs as if it was a chemical stimulation (double side arrow head).

Stimulating Contact Chemoreceptors is Sufficient to Induce Grooming

In order to bypass stimulation of the mechanoreceptors, we expressed a channel rhodopsin receptor into taste neurons in order to activate them by light only. To this end, we used the ubiquitous UAS-Gal4 system (Brand and Perrimon, 1993) to express channelrhodopsin2 (ChR2) (Nagel et al., 2003; Hornstein et al., 2009) into cells expressing Gr33a, which encodes for a receptor essential for aversive taste in Drosophila (Moon et al., 2009). As stated in the Materials and Methods section, flies carrying the construction had straight wings (mutant flies). Those which carried half of the construction exhibited curled wing tips (sibling flies) and were used as control flies. Headless flies were stimulated with a pulse of 480 nm light during 3 min. Blue light did not affect control flies (Figure 2B). In Gr33a-CHR2 flies, photoactivation of Gr33a neurons induced grooming both in males (p < 0.001, Wilcoxon test) and females (p = 0.044) in the absence of any chemosensory stimulus. We found a sexual dimorphism in the flies carrying the construction as photoactivation induced more grooming in males than in females (p = 0.004 in Gr33a-Gal4 × UAS-ChR2, Figure 2B, Table S2), but not in control flies where the response was absent (Figure 2B).

LPS is Deterrent to Free-Moving Flies

In order to evaluate if LPS triggers only grooming or if it has other influence on the walking behavior of flies, we monitored the number of flies walking over 2 agar substrates, one of which was treated (Figure 3A). With pure agar, flies visited equally the 2 sides (Figure 3B). With sucrose, flies strongly preferred sucrose over agar (PI: females = 0.83 ± 0.06: p < 0.001, males = 0.56 ± 0.08: p < 0.001, Dunnett test). With LPS, flies avoided walking on side treated with LPS (females = −0.29 ± 0.08, p = 0.006; males = −0.36 ± 0.08, p = 0.049) (Figure 3). This suggests that LPS is detected by taste neurons located on the legs (and possibly on the proboscis), which mediate coordinated responses leading to the avoidance of areas where LPS is found.

FIGURE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 3. Two-choice test. (A) Schematics of the behavioral test. (B) The preferences for either side were examined by computing a preference index (P.I.) (**p < 0.01, ***p < 0.001, ANOVA). Black bars: males; white bars: females (mean ± s.e.m., n = 10).

Electrophysiological Recordings

We investigated sensilla distributed on the anterior wing margin, located over the marginal, and sub-marginal cells across LV2 (Figure 4A). In this area, contact chemoreceptive sensilla are interspersed between stout bristles and are organized along two rows (Figure 4B), pointing upward (dorsal sensilla) or downward (ventral) (Figure 4C). Contact chemoreceptors are easily spotted under the microscope because of their curved and slender appearance, and also because their tip stands out of other bristles (see Palka et al., 1979; Isono and Morita, 2010, Figure 5). This area was selected based on pilot tests which showed that flies were more responsive when they were contacted there as compared to other regions of the wing.

FIGURE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 4. Contact chemoreceptors on the wing. (A) Drosophila wing. ACV, anterior cross vein; PCV, posterior cross vein. The section from which recordings were performed is outlined by a rectangle (a), which displayed at a higher magnification in Figure 2B. (B) Anterior wing margin, with 3 rows of bristles. Symbols on the picture show: ◯, singly innervated stout bristle; X, singly innervated slender bristle; ▲, multiple innervated curved bristles (from which recordings were obtained). (C) Sensilla on the vein area between LV2 and LV3 in rectangle a. Arrows indicate sensilla recorded in this study (▲ in B).

FIGURE 5
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 5. Electrophysiological recordings from contact chemoreceptors on the wings. Recording samples obtained from wing taste sensilla to 1 mM KCl (used as an electrolyte in all recordings), 1 M NaCl, 10 mM Quinine, 0.1 M Sucrose, 1 mg/ml LPS, and 107/ml of E. coli. Bars on the bottom of the graph represent a scale (vertical: 5 mV, horizontal: 50 ms). Recordings were all taken from female flies, except the response to KCl.

We sampled sensilla labeled D1–D5 and V1–V5 (Figure 4). Between 30 and 50% of the sensilla were not responsive or no electrical contact could be established when contacting their tip. These sensilla are relatively slender and tend to vibrate upon contact with the stimulus electrode, much more than taste sensilla on the proboscis or on the legs. Upon contact, these sensilla occasionally responded with a phasic burst of spikes (Figure 5), possibly fired from several nerve cells.

We thus counted the total number of spikes occurring within the first 2 s of the stimulation (Figure 6). When analyzing these responses over the whole set of data, we found that the number of spikes changed according to the chemical stimulus (p < 0.001, Wilcoxon test), to the concentration (p < 0.001), to sex (p < 0.001), and according to the sensillum position in the row (p < 0.001, V1–V5 and D1–D5). However, we did not find any difference between dorsal and ventral sensilla (p = 0.329, D/V). These observations confirm that wing contact chemoreceptive sensilla are functional, that they respond to classical taste stimuli. These sensilla show dose-dependent responses to LPS and E. coli suspensions. They also respond in a dose-dependent way to NaCl, to KCl, to quinine, but apparently not to sugar.

FIGURE 6
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 6. Responses of wing taste sensilla to different chemicals. Spiking activity recorded from wing taste sensilla in males and females in response to KCl, NaCl, quinine, sucrose, E. coli, and LPS. (A) Responses of female flies. (B) Responses of male flies. Each point represents the average ± s.e.m. from n = 51 to 89 recordings obtained from a total of 10 insects. The concentration of each stimulus is reported on the abscissa as the log10 of the molar concentration for KCl, NaCl, quinine, and sucrose, as log10 of the density per ml for dead E. coli and as mg/ml for LPS.

We further analyzed the temporal course of the responses by computing the number of spikes occurring during consecutive 100 ms bins (Figures S1, S2). At the lowest concentration tested, we observed a burst of spikes starting from about 20 spikes/s and decaying rapidly within 1 s. This initial response was probably elicited by the mechanoreceptor associated with these taste sensilla (in males and females, bin: p < 0.001 for all chemicals except KCl, p = 0.002 in males and p < 0.001 in females and NaCl, p = 0.254 in males and p = 0.420 in females, analysis of covariance). However, with sucrose, the initial burst peaked at 30–35 spikes/s which may indicate that 1 mM sucrose already elicits a response. As the concentration of each stimulus increased, the firing rate globally increased. For salts, it was mostly the tonic part of the response which was affected, while with quinine, E. coli and LPS both the phasic and tonic parts of the responses increased with the dose tested. On the contrary, the spiking activities were markedly depressed in both sexes when the concentration of sucrose was raised from 1 mM to 1 M.

Discussion

We demonstrate for the first time that grooming activities in D. melanogaster adults are triggered by bacterial suspensions of E. coli, its surface compound, LPS, and by aversive chemicals like quinine and NaCl at a high concentration. Mechanosensation is not required to elicit this response as sucrose and water did not induce grooming, and Poxn70 mutants deprived of their external taste receptors were insensitive to LPS. Furthermore, grooming activities could be elicited without physical contact with a substrate by optogenetic activation of cells expressing the bitter receptor Gr33a. Since our behavioral observations indicated that grooming reactions were elicited by stimulating the costal outer vein of the wings, we examined the contact chemoreceptors located there using tip-recording. These sensilla were found to house contact chemoreceptive neurons increasing their firing activity in response bacterial extracts, LPS, quinine, and to high salt concentrations thus supporting the hypothesis that these wing contact chemoreceptors contribute to triggering grooming responses in decapitated flies.

In this work, we recorded grooming by scoring the intensity of the behavior in relation to the number of strokes made by the legs and by the duration of this reaction. A more detailed analysis of this behavior was not relevant here as we focused on finding chemical stimuli which could trigger this behavior. In order to simplify the experimental procedures, we also observed the reactions of decapitated insects. In such insects, the responses induced bypass the downstream control normally exerted by higher order nervous centers in intact animals. Our observations are thus by no mean representative of the normal grooming behavior but they give us a good indication of the strength of the sensory stimuli sent to the local ganglia.

While self-grooming has been described to occur in response to touch (Page and Matheson, 2004) and by noxious chemicals (Newland, 1998; Elwood, 2011), or spontaneously (Yellman et al., 1997), we demonstrate for the first time that grooming is elicited by bacterial chemicals (LPS, Ec). Since it is often said that insects use behavioral resistance to complement their immune reactions (Hughes et al., 2002), detecting LPS and bacterial-related compounds may contribute to help insects avoiding pathogens, by triggering hygienic behaviors like grooming.

LPS is not only triggering grooming responses, but it is also a deterrent as flies avoided to walk on a substrate treated with LPS (Figure 3). LPS could represent a specific avoidance signals as these molecules are present in the cell wall of all Gram-negative bacteria. However, LPS are complex molecules consisting of a lipid anchor, a polysaccharide core, and chains of carbohydrates (Salton and Kim, 1996). It remains to be seen if flies are capable of detecting such large and complex molecules either directly or following a degradation inside of the sensillum lymph. LPS may also act indirectly on the physiology of the taste neurons by virtue of its structure which resembles a surfactant. Alternately, as commercial sources of LPS are extracted from bacteria, flies could be sensitive to contaminants of LPS, such as endotoxins proteins (Hirschfeld et al., 2000) or peptidoglycans (MacKenzie et al., 2010), which trigger immune reactions in vertebrates more efficiently than LPS.

The chemicals contained in dead E. coli bacteria suspension are a mixture of proteins, nucleic acids and to a lesser extent lipids and polysaccharides, and the recognition and behavioral induction cascade can be much more complex. Like LPS, this mixture is a powerful stimulant for the immune system of flies (Lemaitre and Hoffmann, 2007). However, it can also be a signal for food source or for oviposition since E. coli easily stays on meats or ripen fruits (Van Elsas et al., 2011; Nguyen-The, 2012). Thus chemical signals from E. coli and LPS could work differently in hygiene behavior by the interaction with other behaviors like feeding.

Our work stresses the need to further examine the role wing contact chemoreceptors. According to Stocker (1994), each wing of D. melanogaster is equipped with about 40 taste sensilla, each of which hosts 4 chemosensitive neurons and 1 mechanoreceptor. Unexpectedly, we found that contact chemicals delivered specifically to the wings were quite effective in triggering grooming, and that the response to LPS was established earlier on the wings than on the legs. This role of wing contact chemoreceptors in Drosophila contrasts the lesser role they play in locusts, where it was found that stimulating leg contact chemoreceptors was much more effective in triggering a cleaning reaction than by stimulating their wing contact chemoreceptors (Page and Matheson, 2004).

To our knowledge, electrophysiological responses from wing contact chemoreceptors have been recorded in very few insects and the role of these chemosensitive sensilla is not clear. Wolbarsht and Dethier (1958), stressed that in the blowfly, Phormia regina, these sensilla respond to salts and sugar, and are quite sensitive to mechanical vibrations, suggesting that one of their function is to provide proprioceptive information during flight. However, they also reported that the axons projecting to the central nervous system are very thin which suggests that their conduction velocity is slow, making their role in controlling flight unlikely. Angioy et al. (1981a,b) suggested that wing contact chemoreceptors in P. regina are involved in feeding. They observed that proboscis extension and feeding attempts could be triggered by touching single wing hairs with sugar and that salt inhibits proboscis extension. In tse-tse flies, Deportes et al. (1994) examined wing contact chemoreceptors on the costal vein and recorded electrophysiological responses to mechanical stimulation and to salt, sugar, and extracts from conspecifics. They suggested that wing contact chemoreceptors play a role in social or sexual interactions. In the locust, Page and Matheson (2004) recorded responses of basiconica sensilla to noxious stimuli, which were thus considered more as playing a role in proprioception. Our observations suggest that contact chemoreceptors of the wings might play a decisive role in signaling the presence of potentially harmful microbes. Recently, Raad (2013) remarked that contact chemoreceptive sensilla are located mainly on the costal vein of the wings, precisely where a vortex occurs during wings flapping. This lead him to propose that flying insects could use their wing taste sensilla to monitor molecules trapped within this vortex, and thus detect chemicals from a flower for example, without actually touching them.

Except in the locust (Page and Matheson, 2004), all attempts to record electrophysiological responses from wing contact chemoreceptors reported a high number of unresponsive sensilla (Dethier, 1972; Angioy et al., 1979, 1981a,b; Deportes et al., 1994). Our study makes no exception. We were unable to record responses from other parts than the outer margin of the wing, although nothing distinguishes these hairs from the others from a morphological point of view. In our hands, only about 40% of the hairs of the outer margin were responsive which is much less than on the proboscis (Hiroi et al., 2002) or on the legs (Meunier et al., 2003), where the rate of success was closer to 80–90%. Deportes et al. (1994) reported similar figures with regions of the wing where no successful recordings could be obtained and with proportions of unresponsive hairs depending on the stimulus used. Angioy et al. (1981a,b) also reported unusual proportions of unresponsive hairs in Phormia. They suggested that unresponsiveness could be related to satiety as these contact chemoreceptors increased their resistance markedly after forced-feeding (Angioy et al., 1979). At this stage, we have no explanation for this and further experiments are warranted to determine if contact chemoreceptors of the wings are less functional than taste sensilla located on other appendages.

Raad (2013) introduced a functional technique to evaluate the sensitivity of wing chemosensitive sensilla which may alleviate the problems faced by extracellular electrophysiology. In Drosophila, using a Gal4 driver strongly expressed in neurons (tubulin) to drive the expression of a calcium-sensitive probe, G-cAMP (Nakai et al., 2001), he showed well-defined temporal changes of calcium concentrations following stimulation with sugars and bitter compounds in contact chemoreceptive sensilla located in the proximal part of the costal vein of the wing. He further reported that contact chemoreceptive sensilla of this area are expressing a range of sugar- and bitter-sensitive gustatory receptors of the Gr family, thus confirming that contact chemoreceptive sensilla of the wings of Drosophila are fully functional and respond at least to some sapid molecules detected by contact chemoreceptors located on the legs and on the proboscis.

In our tests, sucrose is the only stimulus which did not elicit grooming in our panel of stimuli. The electrophysiological responses obtained from wing taste sensilla did not show any increase of the firing activity with the dose, but on the contrary revealed an inhibition of the basal firing activity. This observation is consistent with recent results obtained by Raad (2013) on fly wing contact chemoreceptors using calcium imaging which failed to reveal any response to sucrose, while strong activities were found in response to glucose, fructose, and quinine. Therefore, the most likely explanation is that wing taste chemoreceptors are not sensitive to sucrose. That sucrose would inhibit the firing activity of taste neurons in a dose-dependent way is unprecedented to our knowledge, but it could be a new case of inhibitory interactions between tastants (Meunier et al., 2003; Cocco and Glendinning, 2012; Charlu et al., 2013; Jeong et al., 2013). Further observations are warranted to confirm this observation and to clarify which mechanism is at work.

So far, our findings support that contact chemicals play a decisive role in triggering hygiene grooming in D. melanogaster. Our study shows that LPS induces vigorous grooming activities from flies and also elicits consistent spiking activities in contact chemoreceptors of the wing. LPS has been used repeatedly in multiple organisms as an inducer of the immune system. LPS and bacterial peptidoglycans are strong inducers of the immune system in Drosophila (Charroux et al., 2009). Bacterial peptidoglycans are recognized by peptidoglycan recognition proteins, which can be soluble or membrane-bound, and activate both Toll and Imd pathways (Gottar et al., 2006; Gendrin et al., 2009).

Conflict of Interest Statement

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Acknowledgments

We thank Prof. K. Scott, Prof. J. Carlson, Dr. L. Soustelle and Dr. Y. Grau for the fly strains and Dr. E. Jacquin-Joly for the E. coli strain used in this study. We also thank A. French for her discussions and support. The authors thank Dr. T. Yanagawa (Biostatistics Center, Kurume University, Japan) for guidance and comments about statistical analysis. Frédéric Marion-Poll and Alexandra M. A. Guigue were supported in part by an ANR grant ADAPTANTHROP and DESIRABLE, Aya Yanagawa was supported by the John Mung program in Kyoto University and a grant for basic science research projects supported by Sumitomo foundation.

Supplementary Material

The Supplementary Material for this article can be found online at: http://www.frontiersin.org/journal/10.3389/fnbeh.2014.00254/abstract

References

Angioy, A. M., Liscia, A., and Pietra, P. (1979). Influence of feeding conditions on wing, labellar and tarsal hair resistance in Phormia regina (Meig.). Experientia 35, 60–62. doi: 10.1007/BF01917877

CrossRef Full Text

Angioy, A. M., Liscia, A., and Pietra, P. (1981a). Electrophysiological responses of wing chemosensilla in Phormia regina (Meig.) to NaCl and sucrose. Boll. Soc. Ital. Biol. Sper. 57, 588–594.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Angioy, A. M., Liscia, A., and Pietra, P. (1981b). Some functional aspects of the wing chemosensilla in Phormia regina (Meig.) (Diptera Calliphoridae). Monit. Zool. Ital. 15, 221–228.

Ashton, K., Wagoner, A. P., Carrillo, R., and Gibson, G. (2001). Quantitative trait loci for the monoamine-related traits heart rate and headless behavior in Drosophila melanogaster. Genetics 157, 283–294.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Aubert, A., and Richard, F. J. (2008). Social management of LPS-induced inflammation in Formica polyctena ants. Brain Behav. Immun. 22, 833–837. doi: 10.1016/j.bbi.2008.01.010

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Berkowitz, A., and Laurent, G. (1996a). Central generation of grooming motor patterns and interlimb coordination in locusts. J. Neurosci. 16, 8079–8091.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Berkowitz, A., and Laurent, G. (1996b). Local control of leg movements and motor patterns during grooming in locusts. J. Neurosci. 16, 8067–8078.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Böröczky, K., Wada-Katsumata, A., Batchelor, D., Zhukovskaya, M., and Schal, C. (2013). Insects groom their antennae to enhance olfactory acuity. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 110, 3615–3620. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1212466110

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Boucias, D. G., Stokes, C., Storey, G., and Pendland, J. C. (1996). The effects of imidacloprid on the termites Reticulitermes flavipes and its interaction with the mycopathogen Beauveria bassiana. Pflanzenschutz-Nachr. Bayer 49, 103–144.

Brand, A. H., and Perrimon, N. (1993). Targeted gene expression as a means of altering cell fates and generating dominant phenotypes. Development 118, 401–415.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Burg, M. G., Hanna, L., Kim, Y. T., and Wu, C. F. (1993). Development and maintenance of a simple reflex circuit in small-patch mosaics of Drosophila: effects of altered neuronal function and developmental arrest. J. Neurobiol. 24, 803–823. doi: 10.1002/neu.480240608

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Burrows, M., and Newland, P. L. (1994). Convergence of mechanosensory afferents from different classes of exteroceptors onto spiking local interneurons in the locust. J. Neurosci. 14, 3341–3350.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Charlu, S., Wisotsky, Z., Medina, A., and Dahanukar, A. (2013). Acid sensing by sweet and bitter taste neurons in Drosophila melanogaster. Nat. Commun. 4, 2042. doi: 10.1038/ncomms3042

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Charroux, B., Rival, T., Narbonne-Reveau, K., and Royet, J. (2009). Bacterial detection by Drosophila peptidoglycan recognition proteins. Microbes Infect. 11, 631–636. doi: 10.1016/j.micinf.2009.03.004

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Cocco, N., and Glendinning, J. I. (2012). Not all sugars are created equal: some mask aversive tastes better than others in an herbivorous insect. J. Exp. Biol. 215, 1412–1421. doi: 10.1242/jeb.059832

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Connolly, K. (1968). The social facilitation of preening behaviour in Drosophila melanogaster. Anim. Behav. 16, 385–391. doi: 10.1016/0003-3472(68)90023-7

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Corfas, G., and Dudai, Y. (1989). Habituation and dishabituation of a cleaning reflex in normal and mutant Drosophila. J. Neurosci. 9, 56–62.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Deportes, I., Geoffroy, B., Cuisance, D., Otter, C. J. D., Carlson, D. A., and Ravallec, M. (1994). Chemoreceptors in the wings of tsetse fly (Diptera: Glossinidae). A structural and electrophysiological study in Glossina fuscipes fuscipes. Rev. Elev. Med. Vet. Pays Trop. 47, 81–88.

Dethier, V. G. (1972). Sensitivity of the contact chemoreceptors of the blowfly to vapors. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 69, 2189–2192. doi: 10.1073/pnas.69.8.2189

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Elwood, R. W. (2011). Pain and suffering in invertebrates? Ilar J. 52, 175–184. doi: 10.1093/ilar.52.2.175

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Gendrin, M., Welchman, D. P., Poidevin, M., Herve, M., and Lemaitre, B. (2009). Long-range activation of systemic immunity through peptidoglycan diffusion in Drosophila. PLoS Pathog. 5:e1000694. doi: 10.1371/journal.ppat.1000694

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Gottar, M., Gobert, V., Matskevich, A. A., Reichhart, J. M., Wang, C., Butt, T. M., et al. (2006). Dual detection of fungal infections in Drosophila via recognition of glucans and sensing of virulence factors. Cell 127, 1425–1437. doi: 10.1016/j.cell.2006.10.046

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Hensler, K. (1986). Intracellular recordings of neck muscle motoneurones during eye cleaning behaviour of the cricket. J. Exp. Biol. 120, 153–172.

Hiroi, M., Marion-Poll, F., and Tanimura, T. (2002). Differentiated response to sugars among labellar chemosensilla in Drosophila. Zoolog. Sci. 19, 1009–1018. doi: 10.2108/zsj.19.1009

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Hirschfeld, M., Ma, Y., Weis, J. H., Vogel, S. N., and Weis, J. J. (2000). Cutting edge: repurification of lipopolysaccharide eliminates signaling through both human and murine toll-like receptor 2. J. Immunol. 165, 618–622. doi: 10.4049/jimmunol.165.2.618

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Hornstein, N. J., Pulver, S. R., and Griffith, L. C. (2009). Channelrhodopsin2 mediated stimulation of synaptic potentials at Drosophila neuromuscular junctions. J. Vis. Exp. 25:e1133. doi: 10.3791/1133

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Hughes, W. O., Eilenberg, J., and Boomsma, J. J. (2002). Trade-offs in group living: transmission and disease resistance in leaf-cutting ants. Proc. Biol. Sci. R. Soc. 269, 1811–1819. doi: 10.1098/rspb.2002.2113

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Isono, K., and Morita, H. (2010). Molecular and cellular designs of insect taste receptor system. Front. Cell. Neurosci. 4:20. doi: 10.3389/fncel.2010.00020

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Jeong, Y. T., Shim, J., Oh, S. R., Yoon, H. I., Kim, C. H., Moon, S. J., et al. (2013). An odorant-binding protein required for suppression of sweet taste by bitter chemicals. Neuron 79, 725–737. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2013.06.025

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Kays, I., Cvetkovska, V., and Chen, B. E. (2014). Structural and functional analysis of single neurons to correlate synaptic connectivity with grooming behavior. Nat. Protoc. 9, 1–10. doi: 10.1038/nprot.2013.157

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Lemaitre, B., and Hoffmann, J. (2007). The host defense of Drosophila melanogaster. Annu. Rev. Immunol. 25, 697–743. doi: 10.1146/annurev.immunol.25.022106.141615

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

MacKenzie, S. A., Roher, N., Boltana, S., and Goetz, F. W. (2010). Peptidoglycan, not endotoxin, is the key mediator of cytokine gene expression induced in rainbow trout macrophages by crude LPS. Mol. Immunol. 47, 1450–1457. doi: 10.1016/j.molimm.2010.02.009

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Marella, S., Fischler, W., Kong, P., Asgarian, S., Rueckert, E., and Scott, K. (2006). Imaging taste responses in the fly brain reveals a functional map of taste category and behavior. Neuron 49, 285–295. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2005.11.037

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Marion-Poll, F. (1996). Display and analysis of electrophysiological data under MS-Windows. Entomol. Exp. Appl. 80, 116–119. doi: 10.1111/j.1570-7458.1996.tb00900.x

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Marion-Poll, F., and van der Pers, J. (1996). Un-filtered recordings from insect taste sensilla. Entomol. Exp. Appl. 80, 113–115. doi: 10.1111/j.1570-7458.1996.tb00899.x

CrossRef Full Text

Meunier, N., Marion-Poll, F., Rospars, J. P., and Tanimura, T. (2003). Peripheral coding of bitter taste in Drosophila. J. Neurobiol. 56, 139–152. doi: 10.1002/neu.10235

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Moon, S. J., Lee, Y., Jiao, Y., and Montell, C. (2009). A Drosophila gustatory receptor essential for aversive taste and inhibiting male-to-male courtship. Curr. Biol. 19, 1623–1627. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2009.07.061

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Mooring, M. S., Blumstein, D. T., and Stoner, C. J. (2004). The evolution of parasite-defence grooming in ungulates. Biol. J. Linn. Soc. 81, 17–37. doi: 10.1111/j.1095-8312.2004.00273.x

CrossRef Full Text

Nagel, G., Szellas, T., Huhn, W., Kateriya, S., Adeishvili, N., Berthold, P., et al. (2003). Channelrhodopsin-2, a directly light-gated cation-selective membrane channel. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 100, 13940–13945. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1936192100

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Nakai, J., Ohkura, M., and Imoto, K. (2001). A high signal-to-noise Ca2+ probe composed of a single green fluorescent protein. Nat. Biotechnol. 19, 137–141. doi: 10.1038/84397

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Newland, P. L. (1998). Avoidance reflexes mediated by contact chemoreceptors on the legs of locusts. J. Comp. Physiol. A 183, 313–324. doi: 10.1007/s003590050258

CrossRef Full Text

Newland, P. L., and Burrows, M. (1994). Processing of mechanosensory information from gustatory receptors on a hind leg of the locust. J. Comp. Physiol. A 174, 399–410. doi: 10.1007/BF00191706

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Newland, P. L., and Burrows, M. (1997). Processing of tactile information in neuronal networks controlling leg movements of the locust. J. Insect Physiol. 43, 107–123. doi: 10.1016/S0022-1910(96)00081-9

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Nguyen-The, C. (2012). Biological hazards in processed fruits and vegetables - Risk factors and impact of processing techniques. LWT-Food Sci. Technol. 49, 172–177. doi: 10.1016/j.lwt.2012.07.002

CrossRef Full Text

Nottebohm, E., Usui, A., Therianos, S., Kimura, K., Dambly-Chaudière, C., and Ghysen, A. (1994). The gene poxn controls different steps of the formation of chemosensory organs in Drosophila. Neuron 12, 25–34. doi: 10.1016/0896-6273(94)90149-X

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Page, K. L., and Matheson, T. (2004). Wing hair sensilla underlying aimed hindleg scratching of the locust. J. Exp. Biol. 207, 2691–2703. doi: 10.1242/jeb.01096

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Palka, J., Lawrence, P. A., and Hart, H. S. (1979). Neural projection patterns from homeotic tissue of drosophila studied in bithorax mutants and mosaics. Dev. Biol. 69, 549–575. doi: 10.1016/0012-1606(79)90311-7

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Phillis, R. W., Bramlage, A. T., Wotus, C., Whittaker, A., Gramates, L. S., Seppala, D., et al. (1993). Isolation of mutations affecting neural circuitry required for grooming behavior in Drosophila melanogaster. Genetics 133, 581–592.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Raad, H. (2013). Rôle et fonctionnalité des récepteurs gustatifs dans les ailes de drosophiles. Docteur en Sciences Thèse de 3ème cycle, Nice-Sophia Antipolis, Nice.

Salton, M. R. J., and Kim, K. S. (1996). “Structure,” in Medical Microbiology, 4th Edn., Chapter 2, ed S. Baron (Galveston, TX: The University of Texas Medical Branch at Galveston), 1262. Available online at: http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/books/NBK7627/

Shimizu, S., and Yamaji, M. (2002). Pathogenicity of entomopathogenic fungi to the termite, Reticulitermes speratus. Jap. J. Appl. Entomol. Zool. 46, 89–91. doi: 10.1303/jjaez.2002.89

CrossRef Full Text

Stensmyr, M. C., Dweck, H. K. M., Farhan, A., Ibba, I., Strutz, A., Mukunda, L., et al. (2012). A conserved dedicated olfactory circuit for detecting harmful microbes in Drosophila. Cell 151, 1345–1357. doi: 10.1016/j.cell.2012.09.046

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Stocker, R. F. (1994). The organization of the chemosensory system in Drosophila melanogaster - a review. Cell Tissue Res. 275, 3–26. doi: 10.1007/BF00305372

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Traniello, J. F. A., Rosengaus, R. B., and Savoie, K. (2002). The development of immunity in a social insect: evidence for the group facilitation of disease resistance. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 99, 6838–6842. doi: 10.1073/pnas.102176599

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Vandervorst, P., and Ghysen, A. (1980). Genetic control of sensory connections in Drosophila. Nature 286, 65–67. doi: 10.1038/286065a0

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Van Elsas, J. D., Semenov, A. V., Costa, R., and Trevors, J. T. (2011). Survival of Escherichia coli in the environment: fundamental and public health aspects. ISME J. 5, 173–183. doi: 10.1038/ismej.2010.80

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Wolbarsht, M. L., and Dethier, V. G. (1958). Electrical activity in the chemoreceptors of the blowfly: I. Responses to chemical and mechanical stimulation. J. Gen. Physiol. 42, 393–412. doi: 10.1085/jgp.42.2.393

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Wölfle, S., Trienens, M., and Rohlfs, M. (2009). Experimental evolution of resistance against a competing fungus in Drosophila melanogaster. Oecologia 161, 781–790. doi: 10.1007/s00442-009-1414-x

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Yanagawa, A., Fujiwara-Tsujii, N., Akino, T., Yoshimura, T., Yanagawa, T., and Shimizu, S. (2011). Musty odor of entomopathogens enhances disease-prevention behaviors in the termite Coptotermes formosanus. J. Invertebr. Pathol. 108, 1–6. doi: 10.1016/j.jip.2011.06.001

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Yanagawa, A., Fujiwara-Tsujii, N., Akino, T., Yoshimura, T., Yanagawa, T., and Shimizu, S. (2012). Odor aversion and pathogen-removal efficiency in grooming behavior of the termite Coptotermes Formosanus. PLoS ONE 7:e47412. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0047412

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Yanagawa, A., and Shimizu, S. (2007). Resistance of the termite, Coptotermes formosanus Shiraki to Metarhizium anisopliae due to grooming. Biocontrol 52, 75–85. doi: 10.1007/s10526-006-9020-x

CrossRef Full Text

Yanagawa, A., Yokohari, F., and Shimizu, S. (2008). Defense mechanism of the termite, Coptotermes formosanus Shiraki, to entomopathogenic fungi. J. Invertebr. Pathol. 97, 165–170. doi: 10.1016/j.jip.2007.09.005

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Yanagawa, A., Yokohari, F., and Shimizu, S. (2009). The role of antennae in removing entomopathogenic fungi from cuticle of the termite, Coptotermes formosanus. J. Insect Sci. 9:6. doi: 10.1673/031.009.0601

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Yanagawa, A., Yokohari, F., and Shimizu, S. (2010). Influence of fungal odor on grooming behavior of the termite, Coptotermes formosanus. J. Insect Sci. 10:141. doi: 10.1673/031.010.14101

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Yang, C.-H., Belawat, P., Hafen, E., Jan, L. Y., and Jan, Y.-N. (2008). Drosophila egg-laying site selection as a system to study simple decision-making processes. Science 319, 1679–1683. doi: 10.1126/science.1151842

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Yellman, C., Tao, H., He, B., and Hirsh, J. (1997). Conserved and sexually dimorphic behavioral responses to biogenic amines in decapitated Drosophila. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 94, 4131–4136. doi: 10.1073/pnas.94.8.4131

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Zhukovskaya, M., Yanagawa, A., and Forschler, B. (2013). Grooming behavior as a mechanism of insect disease defense. Insects 4, 609–630. doi: 10.3390/insects4040609

CrossRef Full Text

Keywords: Escherichia coli, LPS, contact chemoreceptors, wing margin, grooming behavior

Citation: Yanagawa A, Guigue AMA and Marion-Poll F (2014) Hygienic grooming is induced by contact chemicals in Drosophila melanogaster. Front. Behav. Neurosci. 8:254. doi: 10.3389/fnbeh.2014.00254

Received: 09 December 2013; Accepted: 03 July 2014;
Published online: 23 July 2014.

Edited by:

Martin Giurfa, Centre National de la Recherche Scientifi que - Université Paul Sabatier-Toulouse III, France

Reviewed by:

Martin Giurfa, Centre National de la Recherche Scientifi que - Université Paul Sabatier-Toulouse III, France
Bertram Gerber, Leibniz Institut für Neurobiologie, Germany

Copyright © 2014 Yanagawa, Guigue and Marion-Poll. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) or licensor are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Frédéric Marion-Poll, Laboratoire Evolution, Génomes, Spéciation, Centre National de la Recherche Scientifique, UPR 9034, Bat. 13, 1 rue de la Terrasse, 91198 Gif-sur-Yvette, France e-mail: frederic.marion-poll@legs.cnrs-gif.fr

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.