
94% of researchers rate our articles as excellent or good
Learn more about the work of our research integrity team to safeguard the quality of each article we publish.
Find out more
ORIGINAL RESEARCH article
Front. Aging Neurosci. , 05 March 2025
Sec. Parkinson’s Disease and Aging-related Movement Disorders
Volume 17 - 2025 | https://doi.org/10.3389/fnagi.2025.1524755
This article is part of the Research Topic Transcranial Magnetic Stimulation (TMS) in Motor Control and Motor Rehabilitation: Current Trends and Future Directions View all 8 articles
Background: Impaired motor inhibition in Parkinson’s disease (PD) is associated with functional alterations in the frontal-basal ganglia (BG) neural circuits. The right dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (DLPFC), pre-supplementary motor area (pre-SMA), and primary motor cortex (M1) play key roles in regulating this inhibition. However, the changes in interhemispheric interactions during motor inhibition in PD have not been clearly defined.
Methods: We used dual-site paired-pulse transcranial magnetic stimulation (ppTMS) to examine the interactions between the right DLPFC and pre-SMA and the left M1 in 30 patients with early-stage PD and 30 age-matched healthy controls (HC) during both resting and active conditions, specifically while performing a stop-signal task (SST).
Results: Stop-signal reaction times (SSRT) were significantly longer in PD patients compared to HC. The right DLPFC–left M1 interaction, at both short- and long-latency intervals, showed enhanced inhibition in PD following the stop-signal. In PD patients, SSRT was correlated with the inhibition of the right DLPFC–left M1 interaction, with stronger inhibition associated with shorter SSRT.
Conclusion: The deficit in reactive inhibition observed in PD is linked to an abnormal modulation of the right DLPFC–left M1 interaction during the stopping process.
Motor inhibition involves the cancellation of dominant or inappropriate responses and requires coordination within the sensorimotor system. It can be categorized into reactive inhibition and proactive inhibition. Reactive inhibition refers to the ability to immediately halt an ongoing action upon receiving a stop signal, whereas proactive inhibition involves adjusting motor strategies in anticipation of future actions based on the situation (Aron, 2011). In Parkinson’s disease (PD), inhibition deficits become more pronounced as the disease progresses (Gauggel et al., 2004; Obeso et al., 2011; Mirabella et al., 2012), affecting an individual’s ability to pursue future-oriented goals (Di Caprio et al., 2020). However, the underlying neural mechanisms contributing to motor inhibition deficits in PD are not yet fully understood.
Changes in the frontal-basal ganglia network was associated with changes in motor inhibition (Coxon et al., 2012). The primary motor cortex (M1), as the main output structure of this circuit, is crucial in “braking” movement output during motor inhibition (Berardelli et al., 2008; Wu et al., 2011a). The frontal cortex, particularly the pre-supplementary motor area (preSMA) and the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (DLPFC), play an important role in motor inhibition (Trujillo et al., 2019). When halting an already - initiated action, the pre-SMA and right inferior frontal gyrus send a stop signal through the BG network to intercept prepotent response, inhibiting BG output and resulting a global inhibitory effect on M1 (Aron et al., 2007; Badry et al., 2009). However, patients with PD commonly exhibit dysfunction and reduced activity in the supplementary motor complex (SMC) (Rahimpour et al., 2022), especially in pre-SMA (Herz et al., 2014). As a functional region essential for inhibiting competitive motor processes, damage to the pre-SMA can lead to impaired motor inhibition when there is competition between movements (Nachev et al., 2007). Effective connectivity analysis indicates that prefrontal regions, including the DLPFC, modulate task-specific activity by targeting the motor cortex, ultimately leading to motor cessation (Apšvalka et al., 2022). The right DLPFC is significantly related to inhibition control, and its activity changes can be used as an indicator of an individual’s motor inhibition ability (Hung et al., 2018). In the ON-condition, a higher regional blood flow in the right DLPFC, pre-SMA and M1 is predictive of better inhibitory control performance, especially in improving the reaction time of PD patients (Obeso et al., 2013; Criaud et al., 2016; Trujillo et al., 2019). However, whether behavioral performance during PD motor inhibition is regulated by DLPFC and pre-SMA interhemispheric interactions with M1 remains unclear.
Dual-site paired-pulse transcranial magnetic stimulation (ppTMS) is a technique that allows for the measurement of causal functional interactions between brain regions at a millisecond scale, under different movement states (Mars et al., 2009; Kroeger et al., 2010). Previous research has demonstrated that the interaction between motor-related cortical areas and contralateral M1 varies at different interstimulus intervals (ISI), which can be categorized as short-latency (6–10 ms) or long-latency (40–50 ms). This variation in cortical interaction reflects distinct cortico-cortical physiological pathways that regulate interhemispheric activity (Ni et al., 2009). In our study, we applied this method to examine cortico-cortical interactions from the right DLPFC and pre-SMA to the left M1 during motor inhibition, aiming to identify dynamic time windows that could indicate motor inhibition defects in PD.
Motor inhibition is commonly assessed using the stop-signal task (SST) (Logan et al., 1984). Building on our previous research, we selected the maybe stop task (MST) and never stop task (NST) to evaluate reactive and proactive inhibition performance (Wang et al., 2022b). In the NST, a go signal is never followed by a stop signal. In the MST, a stop signal may follow a go signal to cancel the individual’s response. The stop-signal reaction time (SSRT) refers to the response time to the stop signal in the MST, while the response delay effect (RDE) represents the difference in response times to the go signal between the NST and MST tasks. The SSRT is used to measure reactive inhibition, and the RDE is used to measure proactive inhibition (Pan et al., 2018).
In this study, we combined ppTMS with SST to observe changes in interhemispheric interactions at various time points following stimulus onset in patients with PD and healthy controls (HC). We hypothesized that interhemispheric interactions would be reduced, contributing to motor inhibition deficits in patients with PD.
We studied 30 patients with idiopathic PD and 30 HC, matched for age, sex, and educational level. All participants were right-handed, as confirmed using the Edinburgh Handedness Inventory (Oldfield, 1971), and had normal cognition (Montreal Cognitive Assessment, MoCA scores ≥26) (Nasreddine et al., 2005). PD diagnosis was based on the Movement Disorder Society (MDS) clinical diagnostic criteria (2015), and patients were recruited from the neurology clinic of Punan Hospital, Pudong New Area, Shanghai, China. Patients underwent the MDS-Unified Parkinson’s Disease Rating Scale, Part III (MDS–UPDRS–III) assessment at the start of the study. All participants were physically independent (Hoehn & Yahr stages I–II) and had no other neurological disorders or impulse control disorders. Patients maintained their regular medication regimens throughout the study, and all tests were conducted during the medication-ON period. Written informed consent was obtained from all participants after the study details were explained. The study protocol was approved by the Shanghai University of Sport Ethics Committee (102772020RT107) and registered with the Chinese Clinical Trial Registry (ChiCTR2000038517).
Dual-site ppTMS pulse was used to investigate interactions between the right DLPFC and pre-SMA to the left M1 during movement processing. Two 50 mm figure-of-eight coils (Alpha Branding Iron, Magstim) were connected to two Magstim 200 stimulators (Whitland, Dyfed, UK). The left and right M1 were defined as the locations where TMS induced motor evoked potentials (MEPs) with the highest peak-to-peak amplitude in the contralateral first dorsal interosseous (FDI) muscle at a given suprathreshold stimulator intensity. Resting motor threshold (RMT) was defined as the lowest TMS intensity required to generate MEPs greater than 50 μV in at least 5 of 10 trials, with the target muscle completely relaxed. The test stimulus (TS) coil was applied over the left M1 with the handle pointing backward at a 30–45° from the mid-sagittal line to produce a posterior–anterior directed current. The conditioning stimulus (CS) coil was placed over the right DLPFC or pre-SMA.
Surface electromyography (EMG) of the right FDI muscle were recorded with 9–mm–diameter Ag-AgCl surface electrodes. The active and reference electrodes were placed over the FDI muscle belly and metacarpophalangeal joint of the index finger, respectively. Ground electrode was placed on the dorsum of the hand. The signal was amplified (1000×), bandpass filtered (20 Hz −2.5 kHz; Intronix Technologies Model 2024F), digitized at 5 kHz by an analogue-to-digital interface (Micro1401; Cambridge Electronics Design, Cambridge, UK), and stored in a computer for off-line analysis using Signal 6.0 software.
The TMS coils and stimulus configurations are shown in Figures 1A,B. Participants sat in a relaxed position, with their elbows, hips, and knees flexed at 90–100°, in front of a computer screen placed 75–85 cm away. TMS coils were applied to various locations over the left and right hemispheres while participants completed the SST, presented in random order through MATLAB software. A blank screen was presented for 3–4 s after each response and the inter-pulse intervals were > 5 s to avoid changes in motor excitability due to TMS. Although a variety of intensities have been used in previous studies of cortical interactions, the suprathreshold conditioning pulses can elicit functional interactions between the frontal cortex and M1 (Hasan et al., 2013). The interaction between the cortex is related to the ISI, and the different cortico-cortical interaction also shows different changes under the same ISI (Picazio et al., 2014). The more significant interactions at short- and long-latency between right motor-related regions and left M1 were observed at 10 ms and 50 ms (Ni et al., 2009; Cao et al., 2022). Therefore, we refer to our pervious study and set the CS intensity at 110% RMT, while ISI selected 10 ms and 50 ms to represent the short- and long-latency, respectively (Wei et al., 2024).
Figure 1. Experimental setup and tasks (A). TMS coil sites and experimental setup. Participants sat in front of a computer in a relaxed position while different movement inhibition tasks were displayed on the screen. The CS coil (a small 50 mm figure-of-8 coil) was placed over the right hemisphere to induce medially (M) directed current in the brain (red arrow). The regions stimulated by CS coils were ① DLPFC and ② pre-SMA. The TS coil (a small 50 mm figure-of-8 coil) was applied over the hand representation of the left hemisphere to induce current in PA (posterior–anterior) direction in the brain (blue arrow). (B) Stimulus configurations used in study. (C) Outline of maybe stop task. The red arrow and gray triangle served as the visual stop signal. (D) Outline of never stop task. CS, conditioning stimulus; ISI, interstimulus intervals; SOA, stimulus-onset asynchrony; TS, test stimulus.
All participants underwent high-resolution T1 structural magnetic resonance imaging (MRI) using a three-dimensional fast spoiled gradient echo sequence (slice thickness: 1 mm; repetition time: 3130 ms; echo time: 2.98 ms; matrix: 256 × 256; field of view: 256; 176 sagittal slices) to localize individual right DLPFC and pre-SMA targets using the Visor 2 TMS Neuro-navigation system (eemagine, Berlin, Germany). Based on previous studies (Aron et al., 2007; Cao et al., 2022), the right DLPFC target was in Brodmann area (BA) 46 (Talairach coordinates: x = 40, y = 28, z = 30), and the pre-SMA target was in BA6 (Talairach coordinates: x = 6, y = 20, z = 44). The right DLPFC and pre-SMA stimulus locations were determined for each participant with the guidance of a coil tracker, and the CS coil was adjusted for each participant to ensure precise placement on the target gyrus.
In the SST, each trial began with the display of a black fixation cross on a white background for 500 ms, followed by a leftward or rightward arrow for 1,000 ms. Participants were instructed to respond as quickly and accurately as possible by pressing the “4” (leftward arrow) or “6” (rightward arrow) button on the keyboard using their right index finger.
The MST consisted of 75% go trials and 25% stop trials, with a total of 10 blocks, 600 trials (450 go trials and 150 stop trials). In the stop trials, a stop signal was presented by changing the arrow from black to red and displaying a gray triangle, after a variable stop-signal delay (SSD). To maintain a 50% stop success rate, a stepwise algorithm adjusted the SSD: it increased by 50 ms following a successful stop response and decreased by 50 ms after a failed stop response. The initial SSD was set to 250 ms. The TMS stimulus was applied at various stimulus-onset asynchronies (SOA) after the stop signal appeared (50 ms, 100 ms, 150 ms, 200 ms, 250 ms). For each block, three types of TMS stimulation (TS alone, 10 ms, and 50 ms) under different SOA were tested, with one trial of each stimulation collected. In the go trials, to prevent participants from guessing the trial type based on TMS, TMS was applied to all go trials. The TMS stimulus was applied at 50 ms, 100 ms, 150 ms, 200 ms, and 250 ms after the appearance of the go trials. Each block included three types of TMS stimulation (TS alone, 10 ms, and 50 ms) under different SOA, with three trials of each stimulation collected (Figure 1C).
The NST consisted of 180 go trials across three blocks, using the same stimuli as the MST go trials (Figure 1D). To avoid practice effect and reduce learning effect, the MST and NST were counterbalanced across participants.
The participants’ years of education and MoCA scores were not normally distributed, so the Mann–Whitney U test was used for analysis. Age was compared using a t-test, and sex differences were examined using a Chi-square test. Reaction times (RT) from go trials were screened for outliers, defined as responses that were incorrect or deviated from the mean by shorter or longer than the mean ± 3 standard deviations (SD), and these trials were excluded from further analysis. Proactive inhibition was assessed by calculating the RDE, defined as the difference between the go RT in the MST and NST (go RT_MST – go RT_NST). Both the SSRT and RDE were analyzed using a two-way repeated-measures analysis of variance (RM ANOVA), with the group (PD, HC) as the between-subjects factor and the brain region (R DLPFC–L M1, pre-SMA–L M1) as the within-subject factor.
Peak-to-peak MEP amplitudes were extracted using a custom script in SIGNAL 6.0 (Cambridge Electronics Design, Cambridge, UK). Resting-state interhemispheric interactions were analyzed using a two-way RM ANOVA [group (PD, HC) × ISI (10 ms, 50 ms)] for the R DLPFC–L M1 and pre-SMA–L M1 MEP ratios. The effect of the R DLPFC–L M1 and pre-SMA–L M1 interaction for each participant was quantified as the ratio of the mean MEP amplitude in the paired-pulse conditions relative to that in the TS alone.
To measure R DLPFC–L M1 and pre-SMA–L M1 interactions during stopping, two separate three-way RM ANOVA were conducted, with group (PD, HC) as the between-subjects factor, and ISI (10 ms, 50 ms) and SOA (50, 100, 150, 200, 250 ms) as within-subject factors. TMS trials with background root mean square EMG within 2SD of the mean for 100 ms before the TMS pulse were included in the analysis. A similar three-way RM ANOVA (group: PD, HC; ISI: 10 ms, 50 ms; SOA: 50, 100, 150, 200, 250 ms) was applied to analyze the R DLPFC–L M1 and pre-SMA–L M1 interactions during go trials in the MST and NST conditions separately. Normality of the data distribution was assessed and confirmed using the Shapiro–Wilk test.
Planned polynomial contrasts were performed to identify the trend models that best explained the reactive inhibition performance of the two groups at different SOA. The linear, quadratic, and cubic models were tested for significance. The Greenhouse–Geisser method was used to correct for violations of sphericity. Post-hoc comparisons were conducted using paired t-tests. Statistical significance was set at p < 0.05, with Bonferroni correction for multiple comparisons. Additionally, Pearson correlation analysis was used to examine the relationship between the slope of change in MEP ratio and the SSRT and UPDRS–III scores during 150-200 ms after the appearance of the stop signal at 10 ms.
The demographic and clinical data of the patients are summarized in Supplementary Table 1. As expected, no significant differences were found between patients with PD and HC in terms of age, sex, MoCA scores and educational level (Table 1).
Two-way RM ANOVA revealed a significant difference in SSRT between HC and patients with PD [F (1, 58) = 14.8, p < 0.001, η2p = 0.204]. Although no main effect of region was observed, the interaction between region and group was significant [F (1, 58) = 4.9, p = 0.031, η2p = 0.077]. Post-hoc paired t-tests showed that PD patients had significantly longer SSRT than HC for both R DLPFC–L M1 (p = 0.011) and pre-SMA–L M1 (p < 0.001) interactions (Figure 2A). Meanwhile, a two-way RM ANOVA on RDE revealed no significant effects for group, region, or group × region interactions (Figure 2B).
Figure 2. Behavioral and resting-state interhemispheric interaction results. (A) Results for reactive inhibition. SSRT in PD patients was significantly longer than that of HC, in both R DLPFC–L M1 and pre-SMA–L M1 conditions. (B) Results for proactive inhibition. There was no significant difference in RDE between PD patients and HC in the R DLPFC–L M1 and pre-SMA–L M1 conditions. (C) Interhemispheric interaction of R DLPFC–L M1 at ISI of 10 and 50 ms. (D) Interhemispheric interaction of pre-SMA–L M1 at ISI of 10 and 50 ms. Top and bottom lines of the box plot represent 1.5 times the interquartile range (IQR); the black horizontal line in the box plot indicates the median for each group; the black curve on the right shows the data distribution for each group. Each dot represents one subject. HC, healthy controls; ISI, interstimulus intervals; L M1, left primary motor cortex; PD, Parkinson’s disease; RDE, response delay effect; R DLPFC, right dorsal lateral prefrontal cortex; SSRT, stop-signal reaction time. ***p < 0.001, overall comparison of SSRT of PD and HC in R DLPFC–L M1 and pre-SMA–L M1; ※p < 0.05, R DLPFC–L M1 SSRT for PD vs. HC; ###p < 0.001, pre-SMA–L M1 SSRT for PD vs. HC.
Paired-samples t-tests revealed no significant difference between PD patients and HC in R DLPFC–M1 or pre-SMA–L M1 interactions for MEP amplitudes from TS alone (Table 2). Two-way RM ANOVA showed that the main effect of ISI was significant for both R DLPFC–L M1 [F (1, 58) = 23.1, p < 0.001, η2p = 0.284] and pre-SMA–L M1 interactions [F (1, 58) = 5.9, p = 0.018, η2p = 0.093]. However, there was no significant effect of group and no significant group × ISI interaction (Figures 2C,D).
In the go/stop trials of both MST and NST, MEP amplitudes for TS alone at each SOA did not differ between groups (Supplementary Table 2). For the R DLPFC–L M1 interaction, three-way RM ANOVA revealed significant main effects of ISI [F (1, 58) = 688.2, p < 0.001, η2p = 0.922], SOA [F (4, 58) = 20.6, p < 0.001, η2p = 0.262], and group [F (1, 58) = 7.8, p = 0.007, η2p = 0.119] during stopping. A significant group × ISI × SOA interaction was detected [F (4, 58) = 2.7, p = 0.033, η2p = 0.044].
At an ISI of 10 ms, the R DLPFC–L M1 interaction showed more inhibition in PD patients at SOA of 200 ms and 250 ms relative to 50 ms, 100 ms, and 150 ms (p < 0.001). HC also showed greater inhibition at 200 ms and 250 ms compared to 50 ms, 100 ms, and 150 ms (p < 0.001). Post-hoc paired t-tests revealed greater inhibition in PD patients compared to HC at SOA of 200 ms (p = 0.001) and 250 ms (p = 0.002) (Figure 3A).
Figure 3. Changes in R DLPFC–L M1 interactions with time after stop and go signals under different tasks conditions in PD and HC. (A) At ISI of 10 ms, changes in R DLPFC–LM1 interaction at different times after the appearance of stop signal. (B) At ISI of 50 ms, changes in R DLPFC–L M1 interaction at different times after the appearance of stop signal. (C) R DLPFC - L M1 interaction at ISI of 10 ms for go trials at different times after the go signal in the maybe stop condition. (D) R DLPFC–L M1 interaction at ISI of 50 ms for go trials at different times after the go signal in the maybe stop condition. (E) R DLPFC–L M1 interaction at ISI of 10 ms for go trials at different times after the go signal in the never stop condition. (F) R DLPFC–L M1 interaction at ISI of 50 ms for go trials at different times after the go signal in the never stop condition. The gray dashed lines indicate the MEP amplitude generated by TS alone (100%). Values below 100% represent inhibition and values above 100% represent facilitation. Values are shown as mean with standard deviation. HC, healthy controls; ISI, interstimulus intervals; MST, maybe stop task; NST, never stop task; PD, Parkinson’s disease; *p < 0.05, **p < 0.01, patients with PD vs. HC.
In contrast, at ISI of 50 ms, after Bonferroni correction for multiple SOA, the MEP ratios from stop trials in HC were significantly higher at SOA of 200 ms and 250 ms compared to 50 ms and 100 ms (p < 0.001). In PD patients, the MEP ratios during stopping were also higher at 200 ms and 250 ms compared to 50 ms and 100 ms (p < 0.05). Post-hoc analysis using paired t-test showed that interhemispheric disinhibition was more pronounced in HC than in PD patients at SOA of 200 ms (p = 0.038) and 250 ms (p = 0.045) (Figure 3B).
RM ANOVA for MEP ratios in go trials of MST showed significant main effects of ISI [F (1, 58) = 37, p < 0.001, η2p = 0.389] and SOA [F (4, 58) = 85.3, p < 0.001, η2p = 0.595], as well as an ISI × SOA interaction [F (4, 58) = 7.0, p < 0.001, η2p = 0.108], but no significant effect of group or group × ISI × SOA interaction (Figures 3C,D). Similar results were observed for go trials in NST, with significant main effects of ISI [F (1, 58) = 49.1, p < 0.001, η2p = 0.459] and SOA [F (4, 58) = 40.7, p < 0.001, η2p = 0.412], as well as an ISI × SOA interaction [F (4, 58) = 9.9, p < 0.001, η2p = 0.146], but no group differences or group × ISI × SOA interaction (Figures 2E,F).
Trend model analysis showed that the relationship between interhemispheric interaction at 10 ms ISI and SOA was modeled by a linear trend during reactive inhibition in both PD patients [F (1, 29) = 260.7, p < 0.001] and HC [F (1, 29) = 30.8, p < 0.001]. The MEP ratio of R DLPFC–L M1 interaction declined with increasing SOA in the PD group at both quadratic [F (1, 29) = 26.7, p < 0.001] and tertiary [F (1, 29) = 28.3, p < 0.001] models. At ISI of 50 ms, the relationship between R DLPFC–L M1 interaction and SOA showed a significant linear trend for both HC [F (1, 29) = 36.5, p < 0.001] and PD [F (1, 29) = 24, p < 0.001], suggesting that the MEP ratio of R DLPFC–L M1 interaction increased linearly with longer SOA in both groups.
MEP amplitudes for TS alone of each SOA did not differ between groups for pre-SMA–L M1 interaction (Supplementary Table 2). Three-way RM ANOVA for pre-SMA–L M1 interaction showed a significant main effect for ISI [F (1, 58) = 729.7, p < 0.001, η2p = 0.926] and SOA [F (4, 58) = 41.2, p < 0.001, η2p = 0.415], as well as a significant ISI × SOA interaction [F (4, 58) = 148.9, p < 0.001, η2p = 0.720]. However, there was no significant effect of group and no significant group × ISI × SOA interaction (Figures 4A,B).
Figure 4. Changes in pre-SMA - L M1 interaction with time after stop and go signals under different tasks conditions in PD patients and HC. (A) Pre-SMA–L M1 interaction at ISI of 10 ms at different times after stop cue onset. (B) Pre-SMA–L M1 interaction at ISI of 50 ms at different times after stop cue onset. (C) pre-SMA–L M1 interaction at ISI of 10 ms for go trials in the maybe stop condition. (D) pre-SMA–L M1 interaction at ISI of 50 ms for go trials in the maybe stop condition. (E) pre-SMA–L M1 interaction at ISI of 10 ms for go trials in the never stop condition. (F) pre-SMA–L M1 interaction at ISI of 50 ms for go trials in the never stop condition. The gray dashed lines indicate the MEP amplitude generated by TS alone (100%). Values below 100% represent inhibition and values above 100% represent facilitation. Values are shown as mean with standard error. HC, healthy controls; ISI, interstimulus intervals; MST, maybe stop task; NST, never stop task; PD, Parkinson’s disease.
RM ANOVA for MEP ratios from MST go trials showed significant main effects of ISI [F (1, 58) = 61, p < 0.001, η2p = 0.513]and SOA [F (4, 58) = 93.1, p < 0.001, η2p = 0.616], and a significant ISI × SOA interaction [F (4, 58) = 9.9, p < 0.001, η2p = 0.146], with no significant effect of group and no significant group × ISI × SOA interaction (Figures 4C,D).
We also observed significant main effects of ISI [F (1, 58) = 130.2, p < 0.001, η2p = 0.692] and SOA [F (4, 58) = 57, p < 0.001, η2p = 0.496], and a significant ISI × SOA interaction [F (4, 58) = 11.4, p < 0.001, η2p = 0.164] for pre-SMA–L M1 interaction during the NST. However, there were no significant effects of group and no significant group × ISI × SOA interaction (Figures 4E,F).
Since the trend analysis indicated that the inhibitory interaction of R DLPFC–L M1 in PD patients at the ISI of 10 ms began to decline sharply at 150 ms, with significant differences from HC at 200 ms. To investigate whether this sharp decline is associated with behavioral performance, we first calculated the slope change in the difference from 150 ms to 200 ms relative to the 150 ms ratio. The slope was calculated using the formula: (MEP ratio150ms − MEP ratio200ms) / MEP ratio150ms, which was updated after outlined methods by Wei et al. (2024). We then analyzed the relationship between the slope changes and SSRT and UPDRS–III. Pearson correlation analysis revealed that SSRT was positively correlated with the slope of the MEP ratio for PD patients (r = 0.464, p = 0.010), but not for HC (Figure 5). Furthermore, there was no correlation between the slope changes in MEP ratio for R DLPFC–L M1 interaction and UPDRS–III at 150–200 ms.
Figure 5. The relationship between R DLPFC–L M1 interaction at interstimulus interval of 10 ms and stop-signal reaction time. (A) The relationship between the decline slope of MEP ratio R DLPFC - L M1 after stop signal at 150–200 ms and SSRT for ISI of 10 ms in PD patients. Pearson correlation analysis showed significant correlation between the slope of R DLPFC–L M1 interaction decline and SSRT (r = 0.464, p = 0.010). (B) The relationship between the decline slope of MEP ratio R DLPFC - L M1 at 150–200 ms after stop signal at ISI of 10 ms and SSRT in HC. Pearson correlation analysis showed no correlation between the slope of R DLPFC–L M1 interaction decline and SSRT. HC, healthy controls; ISI, interstimulus intervals; PD, Parkinson’s disease.
We investigated the cortical interactions between the right DLPFC and pre-SMA to left M1 in PD patients during stop task performance. The results showed that SSRT was significantly slower in PD patients compared to HC. Additionally, the inhibition interaction of R DLPFC–L M1 was greater at both 10 ms and 50 ms during the stop task. Specifically, when the ISI was 10 ms, the MEP ratio for the R DLPFC–L M1 interaction between 150–200 ms after the stop cue onset was positively correlated with SSRT in PD patients. No significant differences in pre-SMA–L M1 interaction were found between PD patients and HC.
The significantly prolonged SSRT observed in PD patients in this study reflects impaired reactive inhibition, consistent with our previous findings (Wang et al., 2022a). This deficit may be attributed to the degeneration of dopaminergic neurons, which leads to alterations in the frontal–basal ganglia circuit (MacDonald et al., 2000; Miller and Cohen, 2001). In PD patients, motor inhibition signals from the BG are diminished, impairing the ability to reduce the competition between movement and desired inhibition (Mink, 1996). However, no significant differences were observed between the groups in RDE, which may be related to the stage of the disease. A study showed that individuals with early PD have difficulty completely stopping an action compared to controls, but they can still adjust their behavioral strategies based on contextual cues. Additionally, the findings may be influenced by the cognitive demands of the task. Unlike NST, the MST requires participants to monitor and respond to both go and stop signals, increasing cognitive load. Proactive inhibition is influenced by a combination of cognitive processing and motor ability (Wang et al., 2022b). As the patients in our study had normal cognitive function, this may explain the normal levels of proactive inhibition observed in our PD participants.
We found no differences in interhemispheric interaction at rest between PD patients and HC at both short and long ISI (10 ms and 50 ms). These circuits may not be active during the resting state, and interhemispheric interactions could be driven by dynamic movements, as neural representations are influenced by a combination of external stimuli and internal biases (Nachev et al., 2007). In a study of subcortical stroke, connectivity between motor areas at rest was found to be normal, as assessed by functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) (Grefkes et al., 2008). However, during voluntary movements with the paretic limbs, significant differences in interhemispheric connectivity were observed compared to HC (Rehme et al., 2011; Volz et al., 2015).
At 10 ms ISI, MEP ratios for the R DLPFC–L M1 interaction were lower at 200 and 250 ms after the stop signal in both PD and HC compared to earlier time points, with greater inhibition observed in PD patients. Motor inhibition is associated with increased cortical inhibition (Julie Duque et al., 2017). Approximately 200 ms is needed from the onset of the stop signal to movement cancellation (Derosiere and Duque, 2020). The DLPFC is linked to the successful inhibition of incompatible actions, resolving competition and establishing the mappings necessary to perform the task, converting conceptual information into action (Miller and Cohen, 2001; Duque et al., 2012; Lucci et al., 2014). Specifically, the right DLPFC plays a crucial role in the process of reactive inhibition (Chen et al., 2021). The BG-mediated inhibition network integrates multiple sources of information from frontal regions, including the DLPFC, and passes this integrated information to M1 to determine the most appropriate decision and execute it (Miyachi et al., 2005; Trujillo et al., 2019). However, functional impairment in the BG leads to a loss of motor cortex selectivity, affecting the activity of the cortical inhibitory system (Bares et al., 2003). Additionally, PD patients exhibit decreased regional cerebral blood flow in the right DLPFC due to pathological factors (Kikuchi et al., 2001). Dopamine deficits reduce cortical activity (Fecteau et al., 2007; Kehagia et al., 2012), which disrupts the activation of the right DLPFC during task performance in PD patients (Disbrow et al., 2013; Loayza et al., 2022). Impaired neurotransmitter system activation or disrupted connectivity between regions can disrupt the balance between excitatory and inhibitory processes in the brain, further affecting the integration of sensory information necessary for inhibitory actions (Langan et al., 2010; Passamonti et al., 2012; Kapogiannis et al., 2013). Consequently, the regulation of the right DLPFC over M1 during automatic movement is compromised in PD, potentially leading to deficits in reactive inhibition in PD patients. Suppression of activities in the R DLPFC using cathodal transcranial direct current stimulation (tDCS) improved response inhibition capacity (Weidacker et al., 2016).
We observed distinct changes in interhemispheric interaction at 50 ms ISI. The MEP ratio in HC showed an increasing trend of disinhibition during stopping, whereas a weaker trend was observed in PD patients, particularly at 200 and 250 ms after the stop cue onset. This finding may be attributed to abnormal mediating activity in interhemispheric transmission in PD patients. Although the right DLPFC and left M1 lack a direct anatomical connection due to the lack of white matter fiber connections (Guye et al., 2003), evidence suggests conduction via relays in homologous M1 at long latencies (Ni et al., 2009). Abnormal interhemispheric M1–M1 interactions in PD may impede the right DLPFC-left M1 interhemispheric nerve conduction, affecting the regulation of the DLPFC on the motor system. This is reflected as sustained inhibition of cortico-cortical interaction. Furthermore, interhemispheric interactions transmitted through the corpus callosum (CC) are glutamate-dependent and typically excitatory (disinhibitory) (van der Knaap and van der Ham, 2011). Previous studies have shown that interhemispheric disinhibition through the DLPFC projection via the CC to the contralateral M1 enhances bimanual performance in the elderly (Fujiyama et al., 2016). However, in PD patients, pathological damage to CC fibers occurs in the early stages of the disease (Wittstock, 2009), potentially leading to insufficient disinhibition and poor motor performance during the task.
Although the MEP ratios for pre-SMA–L M1 interaction showed inhibition during stopping, we found no significant group differences. The pre-SMA interacts with M1 to regulate planned motor adaptation in response to environmental stimuli (Nachev et al., 2008; Rossi et al., 2009; Neubert et al., 2010). This behavioral modulation, occurring when the participant anticipates a stop signal, is a manifestation of proactive inhibition (Obeso et al., 2013). The SST findings suggest that pre-SMA activity reflects the motivation to regulate actions (Scangos and Stuphorn, 2010) and is associated with preparation-related activity (Chikazoe et al., 2009; Jahfari et al., 2010).
Our results show that a greater decline in the slope of the MEP ratio for R DLPFC–L M1 correlated with slower SSRT at 150–200 ms after the stop signal onset at an ISI of 10 ms. Interhemispheric interaction is associated with behavioral performance (van Ruitenbeek et al., 2017). Decreased DLPFC–M1 interaction not only affects the execution of self-initiated movements (Wu et al., 2011b), but is also associated with decreased bimanual coordination in older adults (Fujiyama et al., 2016). The DLPFC is anatomically connected to the pre-SMA, subthalamic nucleus, and other regions, with these polysynaptic connections providing multiple pathways for the DLPFC to modulate M1 excitability (Brown et al., 2019). However, the regulatory effect of the right DLPFC on the M1 during automatic movement is affected in PD (François-Brosseau et al., 2009). Therefore, we suggest that excessive inhibition at 150–200 ms after stop-signal onset may be a physiological manifestation of reduced movement inhibition deficits in PD patients, and changes in brain interaction regulation during the task can be regarded as a signal of behavioral performance (Hinder et al., 2012).
Our study has limitations. Although we used coordinates to identify the target stimulating brain region for each participant, individual brain differences may introduce some biases. In future studies, we will use effective field modeling to improve accuracy. Additionally, due to the limited number of participants in the early stage of recruitment, this study has not thoroughly analyzed the impact of clinical peculiarities on electrophysiology and behavior. Future studies will explore clinical symptoms such as disease duration, gender, age, cognitive level, and other factors, recruit more PD patients at different stages, and further explore the relationship between clinical variability and neurophysiology and its impact on behavioral performance. Moreover, combined with fMRI and TMS-EEG, we will explore the impact of interactions between SMC and other regions on motor preparation, initiation and execution in PD patients.
To sum up, this study used dual-site ppTMS for the first time to examine PD patients during SST performance. Impaired reactive inhibition in PD patients may be associated with abnormal regulation of R DLPFC–L M1 inhibition, which exhibited excessive inhibition at both short (10 ms) and long (50 ms) ISIs during stopping. The degree of inhibition interaction at short ISI was found to correlate with stopping efficiency, as measured by SSRT. This abnormal interhemispheric interaction may provide a physiological correlation for some of the behavioral deficits observed in PD patients.
The raw data supporting the conclusions of this article will be made available by the authors, without undue reservation.
The studies involving humans were approved by Shanghai University of Sport Ethics Committee (102772020RT107). The studies were conducted in accordance with the local legislation and institutional requirements. The participants provided their written informed consent to participate in this study.
ZW (1st author): Conceptualization, Data curation, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing. JW: Data curation, Writing – original draft. YS: Data curation, Software, Writing – original draft. YL: Data curation, Writing – review & editing. YW: Data curation, Formal analysis, Writing – review & editing. RC: Data curation, Writing – review & editing. ZW (7th author): Resources, Writing – review & editing. JZ: Conceptualization, Funding acquisition, Writing – review & editing. XT: Funding acquisition, Writing – review & editing. KL: Data curation, Funding acquisition, Resources, Writing – original draft.
The author(s) declare that financial support was received for the research, authorship, and/or publication of this article. The financial support from the National Natural Science Foundation of China (31971024) and Macao Polytechnic University (RP/FCSD-04/2022).
The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.
The authors declare that no Gen AI was used in the creation of this manuscript.
All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.
The Supplementary material for this article can be found online at: https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fnagi.2025.1524755/full#supplementary-material
Apšvalka, D., Ferreira, C. S., Schmitz, T. W., Rowe, J. B., and Anderson, M. C. (2022). Dynamic targeting enables domain-general inhibitory control over action and thought by the prefrontal cortex. Nat. Commun. 13, 274–221. doi: 10.1038/s41467-021-27926-w
Aron, A. R. (2011). From reactive to proactive and selective control: developing a richer model for stopping inappropriate responses. Biol. Psychiatry 69, e55–e68. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2010.07.024
Aron, A. R., Behrens, T. E., Smith, S., Frank, M. J., and Poldrack, R. A. (2007). Triangulating a cognitive control network using diffusion-weighted magnetic resonance imaging (MRI) and functional MRI. J. Neurosci. 27, 3743–3752. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0519-07.2007
Badry, R., Mima, T., Aso, T., Nakatsuka, M., Abe, M., Fathi, D., et al. (2009). Suppression of human cortico-motoneuronal excitability during the stop-signal task. Clin. Neurophysiol. 120, 1717–1723. doi: 10.1016/j.clinph.2009.06.027
Bares, M., Kanovsky, P., Klajblova, H., and Rektor, I. (2003). Intracortical inhibition and facilitation are impaired in patients with early Parkinson’s disease: a paired TMS study. Eur. J. Neurol. 10, 385–389. doi: 10.1046/j.1468-1331.2003.00610.x
Berardelli, A., Abbruzzese, G., Chen, R., Orth, M., Ridding, M. C., Stinear, C., et al. (2008). Consensus paper on short-interval intracortical inhibition and other transcranial magnetic stimulation intracortical paradigms in movement disorders. Brain Stimul. 1, 183–191. doi: 10.1016/j.brs.2008.06.005
Brown, M. J. N., Goldenkoff, E. R., Chen, R., Gunraj, C., and Vesia, M. (2019). Using dual-site transcranial magnetic stimulation to probe connectivity between the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex and ipsilateral primary motor cortex in humans. Brain Sci. 9:177. doi: 10.3390/brainsci9080177
Cao, N., Pi, Y., Qiu, F., Wang, Y., Xia, X., Liu, Y., et al. (2022). Plasticity changes in dorsolateral prefrontal cortex associated with procedural sequence learning are hemisphere-specific. NeuroImage 259:119406. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2022.119406
Chen, T., Wang, H., Wang, X., Zhu, C., Zhang, L., Wang, K., et al. (2021). Transcranial direct current stimulation of the right dorsolateral prefrontal cortex improves response inhibition. Int. J. Psychophysiol. 162, 34–39. doi: 10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2021.01.014
Chikazoe, J., Jimura, K., Hirose, S., Yamashita, K. I., Miyashita, Y., and Konishi, S. (2009). Preparation to inhibit a response complements response inhibition during performance of a stop-signal task. J. Neurosci. 29, 15870–15877. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3645-09.2009
Coxon, J. P., van Impe, A., Wenderoth, N., and Swinnen, S. P. (2012). Aging and inhibitory control of action: Cortico-subthalamic connection strength predicts stopping performance. J. Neurosci. 32, 8401–8412. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.6360-11.2012
Criaud, M., Poisson, A., Thobois, S., Metereau, E., Redouté, J., Ibarrola, D., et al. (2016). Slowness in movement initiation is associated with proactive inhibitory network dysfunction in Parkinson’s disease. J. Parkinsons Dis. 6, 433–440. doi: 10.3233/JPD-150750
Derosiere, G., and Duque, J. (2020). Tuning the corticospinal system: how distributed brain circuits shape human actions. Neuroscientist 26, 359–379. doi: 10.1177/1073858419896751
Di Caprio, V., Modugno, N., Mancini, C., Olivola, E., and Mirabella, G. (2020). Early-stage Parkinson’s patients show selective impairment in reactive but not proactive inhibition. Mov. Disord. 35, 409–418. doi: 10.1002/mds.27920
Disbrow, E. A., Sigvardt, K. A., Franz, E. A., Turner, R. S., Russo, K. A., Hinkley, L. B., et al. (2013). Movement activation and inhibition in Parkinson’s disease: a functional imaging study. J. Parkinsons Dis. 3, 181–192. doi: 10.3233/JPD-130181
Duque, J., Greenhouse, I., Labruna, L., and Ivry, R. B. (2017). Physiological markers of motor inhibition during human behavior. Trends Neurosci. 40, 219–236. doi: 10.1016/j.tins.2017.02.006
Duque, J., Labruna, L., Verset, S., Olivier, E., and Ivry, R. B. (2012). Dissociating the role of prefrontal and premotor cortices in controlling inhibitory mechanisms during motor preparation. J. Neurosci. 32, 806–816. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4299-12.2012
Fecteau, S., Knoch, D., Fregni, F., Sultani, N., Boggio, P., and Pascual-Leone, A. (2007). Diminishing risk-taking behavior by modulating activity in the prefrontal cortex: a direct current stimulation study. J. Neurosci. 27, 12500–12505. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3283-07.2007
François-Brosseau, F. E., Martinu, K., Strafella, A. P., Petrides, M., Simard, F., and Monchi, O. (2009). Basal ganglia and frontal involvement in self-generated and externally-triggered finger movements in the dominant and non-dominant hand. Eur. J. Neurosci. 29, 1277–1286. doi: 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2009.06671.x
Fujiyama, H., Van Soom, J., Rens, G., Gooijers, J., Leunissen, I., Levin, O., et al. (2016). Age-related changes in frontal network structural and functional connectivity in relation to bimanual movement control. J. Neurosci. 36, 1808–1822. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3355-15.2016
Gauggel, S., Rieger, M., and Feghoff, T.-A. (2004). Inhibition of ongoing responses in patients with Parkinson’s disease. J. Neurol. Neurosurg. Psychiatry 75, 539–544. doi: 10.1136/jnnp.2003.016469
Grefkes, C., Nowak, D. A., Eickhoff, S. B., Dafotakis, M., Küst, J., Karbe, H., et al. (2008). Cortical connectivity after subcortical stroke assessed with functional magnetic resonance imaging. Ann. Neurol. 63, 236–246. doi: 10.1002/ana.21228
Guye, M., Parker, G. J. M., Symms, M., Boulby, P., Wheeler-Kingshott, C. A. M., Salek-Haddadi, A., et al. (2003). Combined functional MRI and tractography to demonstrate the connectivity of the human primary motor cortex in vivo. NeuroImage 19, 1349–1360. doi: 10.1016/S1053-8119(03)00165-4
Hasan, A., Galea, J. M., Casula, E. P., Falkai, P., Bestmann, S., and Rothwell, J. C. (2013). Muscle and timing-specific functional connectivity between the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex and the primary motor cortex. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 25, 558–570. doi: 10.1162/jocn_a_00338
Herz, D. M., Eickhoff, S. B., Løkkegaard, A., and Siebner, H. R. (2014). Functional neuroimaging of motor control in parkinson’s disease: a meta-analysis. Hum. Brain Mapp. 35, 3227–3237. doi: 10.1002/hbm.22397
Hinder, M. R., Fujiyama, H., and Summers, J. J. (2012). Premotor-motor interhemispheric inhibition is released during movement initiation in older but not young adults. PLoS One 7:e52573. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0052573
Hung, Y., Gaillard, S. L., Yarmak, P., and Arsalidou, M. (2018). Dissociations of cognitive inhibition, response inhibition, and emotional interference: Voxelwise ALE meta-analyses of fMRI studies. Hum. Brain Mapp. 39, 4065–4082. doi: 10.1002/hbm.24232
Jahfari, S., Stinear, C. M., Claffey, M., Verbruggen, F., and Aron, A. R. (2010). Responding with restraint: what are the neurocognitive mechanisms? J. Cogn. Neurosci. 22, 1479–1492. doi: 10.1162/jocn.2009.21307
Kapogiannis, D., Reiter, D. A., Willette, A. A., and Mattson, M. P. (2013). Posteromedial cortex glutamate and GABA predict intrinsic functional connectivity of the default mode network. NeuroImage 64, 112–119. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2012.09.029
Kehagia, A. A., Barker, R. A., and Robbins, T. W. (2012). Cognitive impairment in Parkinson’s disease: the dual syndrome hypothesis. Neurodegener Dis 11, 79–92. doi: 10.1159/000341998
Kikuchi, A., Takeda, A., Kimpara, T., Nakagawa, M., Kawashima, R., Sugiura, M., et al. (2001). Hypoperfusion in the supplementary motor area, dorsolateral prefrontal cortex and insular cortex in Parkinson’s disease. J. Neurol. Sci. 193, 29–36. doi: 10.1016/S0022-510X(01)00641-4
Kroeger, J., Bäumer, T., Jonas, M., Rothwell, J. C., Siebner, H. R., and Münchau, A. (2010). Charting the excitability of premotor to motor connections while withholding or initiating a selected movement. Eur. J. Neurosci. 32, 1771–1779. doi: 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2010.07442.x
Langan, J., Peltier, S. J., Bo, J., Fling, B. W., Welsh, R. C., and Seidler, R. D. (2010). Functional implications of age differences in motor system connectivity. Front. Syst. Neurosci. 4, 1–11. doi: 10.3389/fnsys.2010.00017
Loayza, F. R., Obeso, I., Redondo, R. G., Villagra, F., Luis, E., Obeso, J. A., et al. (2022). The motor inhibitory network in patients with asymmetrical Parkinson’s disease: an fMRI study. Brain Imaging Behav. 16, 1349–1361. doi: 10.1007/s11682-021-00587-5
Logan, G. D., Cowan, W. B., and Davis, K. A. (1984). On the ability to inhibit simple and choice reaction time responses: a model and a method. J. Exp. Psychol. Hum. Percept. Perform. 10, 276–291. doi: 10.1037/0096-1523.10.2.276
Lucci, G., Berchicci, M., Spinelli, D., and Di Russo, F. (2014). The motor preparation of directionally incompatible movements. NeuroImage 91, 33–42. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2014.01.013
MacDonald, A. W., Cohen, J. D., Andrew Stenger, V., and Carter, C. S. (2000). Dissociating the role of the dorsolateral prefrontal and anterior cingulate cortex in cognitive control. Science 288, 1835–1838. doi: 10.1126/science.288.5472.1835
Mars, R. B., Klein, M. C., Neubert, F. X., Olivier, E., Buch, E. R., Boorman, E. D., et al. (2009). Short-latency influence of medial frontal cortex on primary motor cortex during action selection under conflict. J. Neurosci. 29, 6926–6931. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1396-09.2009
Miller, E. K., and Cohen, J. D. (2001). An integrate theory of PFC function. Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 24, 167–202. doi: 10.1146/annurev.neuro.24.1.167
Mirabella, G., Iaconelli, S., Romanelli, P., Modugno, N., Lena, F., Manfredi, M., et al. (2012). Deep brain stimulation of subthalamic nuclei affects arm response inhibition in Parkinson’s patients. Cereb. Cortex 22, 1124–1132. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhr187
Miyachi, S., Lu, X., Inoue, S., Iwasaki, T., Koike, S., Nambu, A., et al. (2005). Organization of multisynaptic inputs from prefrontal cortex to primary motor cortex as revealed by retrograde transneuronal transport of rabies virus. J. Neurosci. 25, 2547–2556. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4186-04.2005
Nachev, P., Kennard, C., and Husain, M. (2008). Functional role of the supplementary and pre-supplementary motor areas. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 9, 856–869. doi: 10.1038/nrn2478
Nachev, P., Wydell, H., O’Neill, K., Husain, M., and Kennard, C. (2007). The role of the pre-supplementary motor area in the control of action. NeuroImage 36, T155–T163. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2007.03.034
Nasreddine, Z. S., Phillips, N. A., Bédirian, V., Charbonneau, S., Whitehead, V., Collin, I., et al. (2005). The Montreal cognitive assessment, MoCA: a brief screening tool for mild cognitive impairment. Available online at: www.mocatest
Neubert, F.-X., Mars, R. B., Buch, E. R., Olivier, E., and Rushworth, M. F. S. (2010). Cortical and subcortical interactions during action reprogramming and their related white matter pathways. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. 107, 13240–13245. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1000674107
Ni, Z., Gunraj, C., Nelson, A. J., Yeh, I. J., Castillo, G., Hoque, T., et al. (2009). Two phases of interhemispheric inhibition between motor related cortical areas and the primary motor cortex in human. Cereb. Cortex 19, 1654–1665. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhn201
Obeso, I., Cho, S. S., Antonelli, F., Houle, S., Jahanshahi, M., Ko, J. H., et al. (2013). Stimulation of the pre-SMA influences cerebral blood flow in frontal areas involved with inhibitory control of action. Brain Stimul. 6, 769–776. doi: 10.1016/j.brs.2013.02.002
Obeso, I., Wilkinson, L., Casabona, E., Bringas, M. L., Álvarez, M., Álvarez, L., et al. (2011). Deficits in inhibitory control and conflict resolution on cognitive and motor tasks in Parkinson’s disease. Exp. Brain Res. 212, 371–384. doi: 10.1007/s00221-011-2736-6
Oldfield, R. C. (1971). The assessment and analysis of HANDEDNESS: The Edinburgh INVENTORY, vol. 9. Oxford: Pergamon Press, 97–113.
Pan, Y., Wang, L., Zhang, Y., Zhang, C., Qiu, X., Tan, Y., et al. (2018). Deep brain stimulation of the internal globus pallidus improves response initiation and proactive inhibition in patients with Parkinson’s disease. Front. Psychol. 9, 1–10. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2018.00351
Passamonti, L., Crockett, M. J., Apergis-Schoute, A. M., Clark, L., Rowe, J. B., Calder, A. J., et al. (2012). Effects of acute tryptophan depletion on prefrontal-amygdala connectivity while viewing facial signals of aggression. Biol. Psychiatry 71, 36–43. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2011.07.033
Picazio, S., Veniero, D., Ponzo, V., Caltagirone, C., Gross, J., Thut, G., et al. (2014). Prefrontal control over motor cortex cycles at beta frequency during movement inhibition. Curr. Biol. 24, 2940–2945. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2014.10.043
Rahimpour, S., Rajkumar, S., and Hallett, M. (2022). The supplementary motor complex in Parkinson’s disease. J. Mov. Disord. doi: 10.14802/jmd.21075/J
Rehme, A. K., Eickhoff, S. B., Wang, L. E., Fink, G. R., and Grefkes, C. (2011). Dynamic causal modeling of cortical activity from the acute to the chronic stage after stroke. NeuroImage 55, 1147–1158. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.01.014
Rossi, S., Hallett, M., Rossini, P. M., Pascual-Leone, A., Avanzini, G., Bestmann, S., et al. (2009). Safety, ethical considerations, and application guidelines for the use of transcranial magnetic stimulation in clinical practice and research. Clin. Neurophysiol. 120, 2008–2039. doi: 10.1016/j.clinph.2009.08.016
Scangos, K. W., and Stuphorn, V. (2010). Medial frontal cortex motivates but does not control movement initiation in the countermanding task. J. Neurosci. 30, 1968–1982. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4509-09.2010
Trujillo, P., van Wouwe, N. C., Lin, Y. C., Stark, A. J., Petersen, K. J., Kang, H., et al. (2019). Dopamine effects on frontal cortical blood flow and motor inhibition in Parkinson’s disease. Cortex 115, 99–111. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2019.01.016
van der Knaap, L. J., and van der Ham, I. J. M. (2011). How does the corpus callosum mediate interhemispheric transfer? A review. Behav. Brain Res. 223, 211–221. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2011.04.018
van Ruitenbeek, P., Serbruyns, L., Solesio-Jofre, E., Meesen, R., Cuypers, K., and Swinnen, S. P. (2017). Cortical grey matter content is associated with both age and bimanual performance, but is not observed to mediate age-related behavioural decline. Brain Struct. Funct. 222, 437–448. doi: 10.1007/s00429-016-1226-9
Volz, L. J., Sarfeld, A. S., Diekhoff, S., Rehme, A. K., Pool, E. M., Eickhoff, S. B., et al. (2015). Motor cortex excitability and connectivity in chronic stroke: a multimodal model of functional reorganization. Brain Struct. Funct. 220, 1093–1107. doi: 10.1007/s00429-013-0702-8
Wang, Z., Pi, Y., Tan, X., Wang, Z., Chen, R., Liu, Y., et al. (2022b). Effects of Wu Qin xi exercise on reactive inhibition in Parkinson’s disease: a randomized controlled clinical trial. Front. Aging Neurosci. 14, 1–13. doi: 10.3389/fnagi.2022.961938
Wang, Z., Pi, Y. L., Wu, Y., Wei, J., Li, Y., Zhang, J., et al. (2022a). Selective effects of exercise on reactive and proactive inhibition in Parkinson’s disease. PeerJ 10, e13628–e13619. doi: 10.7717/peerj.13628
Wei, J. N., Zhang, M. K., Wang, Z., Liu, Y., and Zhang, J. (2024). Table tennis experience enhances motor control in older adults: insights into sensorimotor-related cortical connectivity. Int. J. Clin. Health Psychol. 24:100464. doi: 10.1016/j.ijchp.2024.100464
Weidacker, K., Weidemann, C. T., Boy, F., and Johnston, S. J. (2016). Cathodal tDCS improves task performance in participants high in Coldheartedness. Clin. Neurophysiol. 127, 3102–3109. doi: 10.1016/j.clinph.2016.05.274
Wittstock, M. (2009). Neurophysiological analysis of interhemispheric motor tracts in neurodegenerative diseases. Fortschr. Neurol. Psychiatr. 77, S42–S44. doi: 10.1055/s-0028-1109607
Wu, T., Long, X., Wang, L., Hallett, M., Zang, Y., Li, K., et al. (2011a). Functional connectivity of cortical motor areas in the resting state in Parkinson’s disease. Hum. Brain Mapp. 32, 1443–1457. doi: 10.1002/hbm.21118
Wu, T., Wang, L., Hallett, M., Chen, Y., Li, K., and Chan, P. (2011b). Effective connectivity of brain networks during self-initiated movement in Parkinson’s disease. NeuroImage 55, 204–215. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.11.074
CS – Conditioning stimulus
DLPFC – Dorsolateral prefrontal cortex
FDI – First dorsal interosseous
fMRI – Functional magnetic resonance imaging
HC – Healthy controls
ISI – Interstimulus intervals
L – Left
M1 – Primary motor cortex
MEPs – Motor evoked potentials
MST – Maybe stop task
MoCA – Montreal Cognitive Assessment
NST – Never stop task
PD – Parkinson’s disease
pre-SMA – Pre-supplementary motor area
ppTMS – Paired-pulse transcranial magnetic stimulation
RT – Reaction time
R – Right
RDE – Response delay effect
RMT – Resting motor threshold
SSRT – Stop-signal reaction time
SST – Stop-signal task
SOA – Stimulus–onset asynchrony
SSD – Stop signal delay
SMC – Supplementary motor complex
EMG – Surface electromyography
TMS – Transcranial magnetic stimulation
tDCS – Transcranial direct current stimulation
TS – Test stimulus
MDS–UPDRS–III – Movement Disorder Society-Unified Parkinson’s Disease Rating Scale-Part III
Keywords: Parkinson’s disease, motor inhibition, interhemispheric interaction, dorsolateral prefrontal cortex, pre-supplementary motor area
Citation: Wang Z, Wei J, Song Y, Li Y, Wu Y, Chen R, Wang Z, Zhang J, Tan X and Liu K (2025) Role of right dorsolateral prefrontal cortex–left primary motor cortex interaction in motor inhibition in Parkinson’s disease. Front. Aging Neurosci. 17:1524755. doi: 10.3389/fnagi.2025.1524755
Received: 19 November 2024; Accepted: 18 February 2025;
Published: 05 March 2025.
Edited by:
Elisa Kallioniemi, New Jersey Institute of Technology, United StatesReviewed by:
Anna-Lisa Schuler, Max Planck Institute for Human Cognitive and Brain Sciences, GermanyCopyright © 2025 Wang, Wei, Song, Li, Wu, Chen, Wang, Zhang, Tan and Liu. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.
*Correspondence: Ke Liu, Nzg3MjE3ODMwQHFxLmNvbQ==
Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.
Research integrity at Frontiers
Learn more about the work of our research integrity team to safeguard the quality of each article we publish.