Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Aging Neurosci. , 05 February 2025

Sec. Neurocognitive Aging and Behavior

Volume 17 - 2025 | https://doi.org/10.3389/fnagi.2025.1505815

This article is part of the Research Topic Mental, Sensory, Physical and Life Style Parameters Related to Cognitive Decline in Aging View all 22 articles

Role of visual function and performance of activities of daily living in cognitive function in patients with mild cognitive impairment: a cross-sectional study

\r\nChunhui ZhouChunhui Zhou1Ganfeng YangGanfeng Yang2Marc TheeboomMarc Theeboom3Hua YangHua Yang2Ruiting ZhuRuiting Zhu1Zijian ZhouZijian Zhou1Dong Zhu
Dong Zhu1*
  • 1School of Wushu, Shanghai University of Sport, Shanghai, China
  • 2Physical Education Institute, Soochow University, Suzhou, Jiangsu, China
  • 3Department of Exercise and Sport Science, Free University of Brussels, Brussels, Belgium

Background: Mild cognitive impairment (MCI) is a precursor to dementia, with many patients showing early decline in activities of daily living (ADLs). However, the role of visual impairment in this process is underexplored despite evidence suggesting it may accelerate cognitive decline. Current research does not understand how visual dysfunction affects cognitive abilities and how ADLs might moderate this relationship. This gap is crucial because early interventions targeting visual impairments could potentially delay progression to dementia, offering new avenues for supporting MCI patients.

Objective: This study investigates the relationship between visual function and cognitive abilities in patients with MCI. Specifically, it seeks to determine how different aspects of visual function, such as visual field indices and visual acuity, correlate with various cognitive domains measured by standardized assessments. Additionally, the study aims to examine the role of ADLs as a potential moderating factor in this relationship. By analyzing how ADL performance influences the strength and direction of the association between visual impairments and cognitive function, this research intends to identify key areas where visual deficits may contribute to cognitive decline in MCI.

Methods: This is a cross-sectional study. Two hundred and seventy four elderly patients were diagnosed with MCI from various communities in Taicang City, Jiangsu Province, China. Most participants were women (68.6%), and the average age was 69 years. Notable comorbidities included hypertension (41.6%) and diabetes (33.2%), indicating a higher burden of health conditions than typical older adult populations. Visual function, Montreal Cognitive Assessment (MoCA), and the ADLs were measured. Pearson's correlation coefficients were used to examine the unadjusted associations between visual function measures and cognitive performance. Multivariable linear regression models were employed to further explore these relationships and to assess the moderating role of ADLs.

Results: Significant positive correlations were found between visual function and cognitive performance, particularly with Visual Function Index (VFI) showing a strong initial correlation with the total MoCA score (r = 0.61, p < 0.001), which was attenuated after adjusting for ADL (β = 0.06, p = 0.23). Maximum Visual Field (Max VF) was positively correlated with language skills (r = 0.13, p < 0.05). The negative correlation of Minimum Visual Field (Min VF) with cognitive performance became positive after ADL adjustment (β = 0.12, p = 0.04).

Conclusions: These findings suggest that visual function plays a crucial role in the cognitive and functional abilities of patients with MCI. Early interventions targeting visual impairments may help mitigate cognitive decline and improve the quality of life for these patients.

1 Background

MCI is a pivotal phase in the spectrum of cognitive aging and represents an intermediate stage between cognitive changes typical of normal aging and those indicative of dementia, notably Alzheimer's disease (AD), with approximately 50% of individuals progressing to AD within 4–5 years (Gauthier et al., 2006). Given this uncertain trajectory, early identification, understanding of risk factors, and intervention strategies are critical to potentially delay or prevent further cognitive decline and functional impairment.

ADLs encompass a range of tasks necessary for independent living, including basic activities, such as eating and dressing (BADLs), and complex instrumental activities such, as managing finances and transportation (IADLs) (Fuentes et al., 2020). Impairments in basic ADLs typically emerge in later stages of cognitive decline; as such, deficits in IADLs may present earlier and are closely linked to cognitive abilities, such as memory, attention, and executive function (Liu-Seifert et al., 2015). Understanding the relationship between cognitive function and ADL performance is essential for assessing functional independence and designing interventions to support individuals with cognitive impairment.

Among the factors influencing ADL performance, sensory impairments—such as vision and hearing loss—are prevalent in aging populations and have been shown to accelerate cognitive decline (Heyl and Wahl, 2012). The common-cause hypothesis posits that shared underlying factors influence sensory and cognitive functions, leading to strong covariability between the two domains (Lindenberger and Baltes, 1994). The information–degradation hypothesis suggests that sensory degradation may tax cognitive resources, thereby affecting cognitive processing abilities (Schneider and Pichora-Fuller, 2000). Research highlights the complex interplay among sensory function, cognitive function, and daily function in older adults. Therefore, understanding the effect of sensory impairments on cognitive function and everyday functioning is crucial to develop targeted interventions to mitigate cognitive decline in older adults.

Among sensory impairments, visual impairment holds particular significance because of its pervasive effect on cognitive function and daily functioning in older adults. Older adults with visual impairment are more likely to report subjective cognitive complaints and experience limitations in ADLs compared with those without visual impairment (Brown et al., 2014; Lee et al., 2019). Moreover, visual disturbances, such as impaired visual acuity, contrast sensitivity, and spatial perception, are common in individuals with AD and may manifest as early symptoms of the disease (Cormack et al., 2000; Armstrong, 1996). However, despite the growing recognition of the effect of visual impairment on cognitive function, a notable gap exists in understanding such relationship in individuals with MCI. Addressing this gap is critical for several reasons. First, elucidating the interplay among visual function, cognitive function, and ADL performance in individuals with MCI may provide valuable insights into the mechanisms underlying cognitive decline in this population. Second, understanding how visual impairment influences cognitive function and everyday functioning in MCI may inform the development of tailored interventions and support strategies to address the unique needs of this population. Finally, by identifying early markers of cognitive decline in MCI, clinicians and researchers may be equipped to intervene and potentially delay the progression to AD.

Recent advancements in the understanding of cognitive aging and sensory impairments have underscored the critical role of early intervention in mitigating cognitive decline (Bateman et al., 2012). Studies have increasingly highlighted the interconnectedness between sensory deficits, particularly visual impairment, and cognitive functions such as memory and executive function (Yoshii et al., 2019). State-of-the-art neuroimaging techniques have allowed researchers to explore the neural substrates of these interactions, revealing that sensory and cognitive processing often share common neural pathways. These findings suggest that sensory decline, especially visual impairment, may exacerbate cognitive decline by overloading cognitive resources and altering brain function (Zhou, 2020). In the context of MCI, the role of visual function in both cognitive and daily functioning remains underexplored.

In this comprehensive cross-sectional study, we aim to investigate the role of visual function and ADL performance in cognitive function among individuals with MCI. We hypothesize that visual impairment will be associated with poor cognitive function and decreased performance on ADLs in individuals with MCI. Results will reveal the underlying mechanisms of cognitive decline in MCI and identify targets for intervention and support strategies.

2 Methods

2.1 Study design

We employed a multistage, stratified, cluster-sampling procedure to ensure representative sampling of the elderly population in Taicang City, Jiangsu Province. Taicang City has a total elderly population of 163,100 individuals aged over 60 years, constituting 30.6% of the city's population. In the initial stage of sampling, we randomly selected four districts from Taicang City, with two districts classified as economically developed (above the city's median GDP), and the two remaining districts classified as economically underdeveloped (below the city's median GDP).

In the second stage of sampling, three communities were randomly chosen from each selected district. Each community typically comprises 500–700 households. Following community selection, we screened eligible elderly individuals based on predetermined recruitment criteria (Figure 1). The participants were required to have resided in Taicang for at least 1 year, while institutionalized individuals, such as those residing in nursing homes, were excluded from the study.

Figure 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Flow chart of the study.

Twelve research sites participated in this study, and each contributed to comprehensive data collection efforts. This study was approved by the Scientific Research Ethics Committee of the Shanghai University of Sport (102772023RT200), and written informed consent was obtained from all participants prior to their inclusion in the study.

2.2 Participants

Over 700 elderly subjects aged 60 years or above were recruited from various communities in Taicang City. Recruitment efforts targeted community-dwelling older adults through local senior centers, community health clinics, and advertisements in community newsletters. The inclusion criteria were as follows: age of 65 years and above and diagnosis of MCI according to standardized criteria (Petersen, 2004). Additionally, participants were required to exhibit no significant visual or hearing impairment unrelated to aging, such as cataracts or macular degeneration, and to be capable of providing informed consent. self-reported cognitive decline was corroborated by healthcare providers when available. This approach aligns with the recommendations in some diagnostic guidelines for MCI when longitudinal data is unavailable.

Exclusion criteria were carefully delineated to ensure the integrity of the study sample. Individuals unable to cooperate with sensory and cognitive examinations due to conditions, such as deafness, blindness, or other sensory organ diseases, were excluded. Furthermore, participants suffering from major psychiatric disorders (e.g., depression, schizophrenia) or conditions that could confound cognitive function assessment (e.g., confusion, delirium) were excluded. Individuals experiencing cognitive impairment attributed to neuropsychiatric drug use were not eligible for participation. Finally, people who could not complete the cognitive test due to factors such as old age and low education (< 3 years) level were excluded. All participants were diagnosed by a physician who was a specialist in either geriatric medicine, psychiatry, or neurology. As a part of the diagnostic process, all subjects underwent ADL assessment, cognitive testing, and visual function examination. a trained Human Movement Sciences (HMS) research assistant tested the patients for the following additional inclusion criteria: Mini-Mental State Exam (MMSE) score of at least 9 to no more than 23 and the ability to perform the timed up-and-go test (Podsiadlo and Richardson, 2015), ADL score of < 26, indicating a reduced ability to perform daily tasks independently (Fuentes et al., 2020). In this study, 274 of the 288 participants (95.1%) had sufficient baseline data for variables of interest and were included.

2.3 Cognitive assessment

Cognitive assessments were performed in a quiet environment with no distractions. MoCA was used to assess global cognitive function (Yoshii et al., 2019). It includes tasks that assess various cognitive domains: visuospatial and executive functioning through a trail-making test (TMT), copying a cube (CC), and a clock-drawing test (CDT) by identifying three animals; memory with immediate and delayed recall of five words (M); attention via forward and backward digit span (FD and BD), a tapping test (TT), and serial 7s subtraction (S7S); language through sentence repetition (SR) and verbal fluency tasks (VFT); abstraction by explaining similarities between items; and orientation by providing the current date, month, year, day, place, and city. The total possible score on the MoCA is 30 points, with a score of 26 or above generally considered normal, while scores below 26 may indicate cognitive impairment, with lower scores suggesting significant impairment.

2.4 Assessment of ADL

ADL was assessed by the Alzheimer's Disease Co-operative Study-ADL (ADCS-ADL) scale (Galasko et al., 1997). The ADCS-ADL scale assesses 23 ADL items and is administered to the caregiver through an interview. The scale comprises two components, namely, basic ADL, which consists of six items, such as self-feeding, personal hygiene, and dressing; and instrumental ADL, which consists of 17 items, such as using telephone, reading books or magazines, managing finances, leisure activities, and household chores. The BADL score ranges from 0 to 22, and the IADL ranges from 0 to 56. BADL and IADL comprise the total ADL, with the range 0–78. High scores indicate less impairment.

2.5 Visual function assessment

Visual function was assessed utilizing an automated visual field test employing the Zeiss Visual Field Instrument HFA3 Model 860 to comprehensively evaluate the patient's visual field. The procedure entailed systematic analysis of the patient's visual field, wherein they fixated on a central point while targets were presented across various locations within their visual field. Responses were elicited through button presses or other means upon perception of the presented lights. Alongside determining the maximum visual field (Max VF) and minimum visual field (Min VF), the test outcomes were quantified through several key metrics. The VFI, which was expressed as a percentage, served as an indicator of overall visual field health, with 100% denoting normalcy and 0% indicating total blindness (Massof and Rubin, 2001). Mean Deviation (MD) represented the average deviation of all visual field points from the norm and reflected the extent of visual field loss; negative values signified defects, with larger negative values indicating greater severity (Rowe and Sarkies, 1998). Pattern Standard Deviation (PSD) gauged localized visual field damage by assessing inter-point variances, with elevated PSD values suggestive of irregularities, such as scotomasm, and typically correlating with localized visual field alterations. The examination included evaluation of visual acuity (VA), contributing to a comprehensive assessment of the patient's visual function (Warrian et al., 2010).

2.6 Statistical analysis

The unadjusted associations between cognitive function, as measured by the Montreal Cognitive Assessment (MoCA), and various measures of visual function (including Max VF, Min VF, VFI, MD, PSD, and VA, as well as total ADL, BADL, and IADL, were assessed using Pearson's correlation coefficient. In order to gain further insight into these relationships, multivariable linear regression models were constructed, taking into account age, sex, and educational levels. In these models, each measure of visual function and cognitive function was analyzed individually to determine the corresponding β-coefficients. A small number of patients had missing data, which were addressed through listwise deletion. All statistical analyses were conducted using SAS version 9.4 and R version 3.5.1. Statistical significance was set at p < 0.05.

3 Results

The baseline characteristics for the 274 participants are presented in Table 1. The average age was 69 years, with a mean MoCA score of 18.5, indicating MCI. A majority of participants were women (68.6%). The mean total ADL was 20.6, suggesting moderate impairment in daily living activities. Notable comorbidities included hypertension (41.6%) and diabetes (33.2%), indicating a higher burden of health conditions than typical older adult populations.

Table 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Demographics.

Table 2 shows the β-coefficients from multivariable linear regression models adjusted for age and sex. Max VF was significantly positively correlated with the total MoCA score (r = 0.17, p < 0.05), but this correlation was not significant after adjusting for ADL (β = 0.12, p = 0.39). VFI showed a strong positive correlation with the total MoCA score (r = 0.61, p < 0.001), which weakened significantly after adjusting for ADL (β = 0.06, p = 0.23). Min VF had a negative correlation with the total MoCA score, which became positive after adjusting for ADL (β = 0.12, p = 0.04).

Table 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Correlation and regression coefficients.

VFI was significantly positively correlated with memory (r = 0.33, p < 0.001) and attention (r = 0.40, p < 0.001), but these correlations were not significant after adjusting for ADL (memory: β = 0.02, p = 0.72; attention: β = −0.01, p = 0.89). Max VF was significantly positively correlated with language (r = 0.13, p < 0.05), and this correlation remained significant after adjusting for ADL (β = 0.11, p = 0.11).

Regression analysis indicated that ADL moderated the relationship between visual function and cognitive performance. VFI's strong positive correlation with the total MoCA score before adjustment (β = 0.58, p < 0.001) weakened after adjustment (β = 0.06, p = 0.23). Min VF's negative correlation with memory became positive after ADL adjustment (β = 0.10, p = 0.09). Overall ADL was negatively correlated with memory (r = −0.14, p < 0.05), indicating that lower ADL capabilities are associated with poorer memory performance.

4 Discussion

This study underscores the intricate relationship between visual function and cognitive performance in older adults, highlighting the critical moderating role of ADL. The observed correlations between visual function indices (Max VF, VFI, Min VF) and cognitive performance (MoCA score) align with previous research showing that visual impairments can detrimentally affect cognitive abilities (Zanto et al., 2011; Kalkstein et al., 2011). The positive correlation between Max VF and cognitive performance, especially in language abilities, suggests broader visual fields contribute to improved cognition (Clemmensen et al., 2020; Lindenberger et al., 2008). However, the attenuation of these correlations after adjusting for ADL emphasizes that daily functional activities significantly mediate the effect of visual function on cognitive performance (Raimo et al., 2024; Clemmensen et al., 2020; Tulliani et al., 2022). This finding suggests that interventions should not only focus on visual rehabilitation but also on enhancing ADL capabilities.

The initialnegative correlation between Min VF and cognitive performance, which became positive after adjusting for ADL, indicates a nuanced interaction (Wolf et al., 2021; Eckstein et al., 2017). While a reduced visual field might initially hinder cognitive abilities, the context of daily activities can shift this effect. This may be due to compensatory mechanisms where individuals develop alternative strategies to maintain cognitive performance despite visual impairments (Buele et al., 2023; Jia et al., 2019). Additionally, the reversal of the Min VF and cognitive performance relationship post-ADL adjustment is a novel finding that suggests a complex interaction requiring further investigation (Guo et al., 2021). Enhancing ADL capabilities could buffer the effects of visual impairments on cognitive health, offering a more detailed understanding of how visual function impacts different cognitive processes.

Healthcare professionals should adopt a holistic approach that integrates assessments of visual function, cognitive performance, and ADL to develop effective intervention strategies. Rehabilitation programs should enhance visual fields and ADL skills, recognizing their synergistic potential in improving cognitive outcomes (Wolf et al., 2021; Eckstein et al., 2017). This strategy is particularly relevant for memory and language domains, where the interplay between visual function and ADL is most pronounced (Jia et al., 2019). Future research should use longitudinal designs to track changes over time and better understand how visual impairments and ADL interact to influence cognitive health.

This study provides valuable insights into the relationship between sensory impairments, cognitive function, and daily living activities in older adults; however, several limitations should be considered. The sample was relatively homogenous, limiting the generalizability of the findings, and the cross-sectional design prevents establishing causal relationships between sensory deficits and cognitive decline. Future research should incorporate longitudinal designs to explore causality and include more diverse populations to improve generalizability. Additionally, the reliance on self-reported IADL performance may introduce bias, and future studies should utilize objective, performance-based assessments. While this study focused on cognitive and sensory impairments, other factors such as mental health and social engagement should also be considered in future research. Exploring interventions that target both sensory and cognitive domains, particularly those integrating technologies like wearable devices or cognitive training apps, could offer promising avenues for intervention. Finally, investigating the neurobiological mechanisms linking sensory impairments and cognitive decline, including biomarkers and neuroimaging, could further enhance our understanding and inform preventive strategies. Addressing these limitations and exploring these future directions will be crucial for advancing the field and developing effective interventions for older adults at risk of cognitive decline.

5 Conclusion

This study elucidates that visual function significantly influences cognitive performance, with ADL as a crucial moderating factor. Enhanced visual function is correlated with superior cognitive performance, and the moderating effect of ADL suggests that improvements in ADL capabilities could positively affect cognitive health. These findings underscore the importance of comprehensive assessments and interventions to enhance visual function and ADL capacities and bolster cognitive health in the elderly population. Future research should focus on developing and evaluating interventions designed to simultaneously improve ADL and visual function to optimize cognitive outcomes in older adults.

Data availability statement

The original contributions presented in the study are included in the article/supplementary material, further inquiries can be directed to the corresponding author.

Ethics statement

This study was approved by the Scientific Research Ethics Committee of Shanghai University of Sport on March 15, 2024 (102772023RT200) and approved by the Chinese Clinical Trial Registry on April 24, 2024 (ChiCTR2400083424). All volunteers or their legal representatives provided written informed consent.

Author contributions

CZ: Investigation, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing. GY: Data curation, Methodology, Writing – review & editing. MT: Data curation, Formal analysis, Funding acquisition, Writing – review & editing. HY: Data curation, Investigation, Project administration, Writing – review & editing. RZ: Conceptualization, Supervision, Writing – review & editing. ZZ: Data curation, Supervision, Writing – review & editing. DZ: Data curation, Formal analysis, Methodology, Writing – review & editing.

Funding

The author(s) declare financial support was received for the research, authorship, and/or publication of this article. This work was funded by the National Planning Office of Philosophy and Social Science (National Social Science Foundation of China) [grant no. 23ATY010] and the Shanghai Municipal Science and Technology Commission (grant no. 19080503200).

Acknowledgments

We sincerely thank our colleagues for their valuable assistance and the funding agency for financial support. Thanks to the National Planning Office of Philosophy and Social Science for our support.

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Generative AI statement

The author(s) declare that no Gen AI was used in the creation of this manuscript.

Publisher's note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

Abbreviations

MoCA, Montreal Cognitive Assessment; Max VF, Maximum Visual Field; Min VF, Minimum Visual Field; VFI, Visual Field Index; MD, Mean Deviation; PSD, Pattern Standard Deviation; VA, Visual Acuity; ADL, Activities of Daily Living; BADL, Basic Activities of Daily Living; IADL, Instrumental Activities of Daily Living; SD, Standard Deviation; TMT, trail-making test; CC copying a cube; CDT, a clock-drawing test; FD and BD, forward and backward digit span; TT, a tapping test; S7S, serial 7s subtraction; SR, sentence repetition; VFT, verbal fluency tasks; ADCS-ADL, Alzheimer's Disease Co-operative Study-ADL; MCI, Mild cognitive impairment; AD, Alzheimer's disease.

References

Armstrong, R. A. (1996). Visual field defects in Alzheimer's disease patients may reflect differential pathology in the primary visual cortex. Optom. Vision Sci. 73, 677–682. doi: 10.1097/00006324-199611000-00001

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Bateman, R. J., Xiong, C., Benzinger, T. L., Fagan, A. M., Goate, A., Fox, N. C., et al. (2012). Clinical and biomarker changes in dominantly inherited Alzheimer's disease. N. Engl. J. Med. 367, 795–804. doi: 10.1056/NEJMoa1202753

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Brown, J. C., Goldstein, J. E., Chan, T. L., Massof, R., Ramulu, P., Group, L. V. R. N. S., et al. (2014). Characterizing functional complaints in patients seeking outpatient low-vision services in the United States. Ophthalmology 121, 1655–62.e1. doi: 10.1016/j.ophtha.2014.02.030

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Buele, J., Varela-Aldás, J. L., and Palacios-Navarro, G. (2023). Virtual reality applications based on instrumental activities of daily living (iADLs) for cognitive intervention in older adults: a systematic review. J. Neuroeng. Rehabil. 20:168. doi: 10.1186/s12984-023-01292-8

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Clemmensen, F. K., Hoffmann, K., Siersma, V., Sobol, N., Beyer, N., Andersen, B. B., et al. (2020). The role of physical and cognitive function in performance of activities of daily living in patients with mild-to-moderate Alzheimer's disease - a cross-sectional study. BMC Geriatr. 20:513. doi: 10.1186/s12877-020-01926-9

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Cormack, F. K., Tovee, M., and Ballard, C. (2000). Contrast sensitivity and visual acuity in patients with Alzheimer's disease. Int. J. Geriatr. Psychiatry 15, 614–620. doi: 10.1002/1099-1166(200007)15:7<614::AID-GPS153>3.0.CO;2-0

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Eckstein, M. K., Guerra-Carrillo, B., Singley, A. T. M., and Bunge, S. A. (2017). Beyond eye gaze: what else can eyetracking reveal about cognition and cognitive development? Dev. Cogn. Neurosci. 25, 69–91. doi: 10.1016/j.dcn.2016.11.001

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Fuentes, M., Klostermann, A., Kleineidam, L., Bauer, C., Schuchhardt, J., Maier, W., et al. (2020). Identification of a cascade of changes in activities of daily living preceding short-term clinical deterioration in mild Alzheimer's disease dementia via lead-lag analysis. J. Alzheimers. Dis. 76, 1005–1015. doi: 10.3233/JAD-200230

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Galasko, D., Bennett, D., Sano, M., Ernesto, C., Thomas, R., Grundman, M., et al. (1997). An inventory to assess activities of daily living for clinical trials in Alzheimer's disease. Alzheimer Dis. Assoc. Disor. 11, S33–S39. doi: 10.1097/00002093-199700112-00005

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Gauthier, S., Reisberg, B., Zaudig, M., Petersen, R. C., Ritchie, K., Broich, K., et al. (2006). Mild cognitive impairment. Lancet 367, 1262–1270. doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736(06)68542-5

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Guo, X., Arsiwala, L. T., Dong, Y., Mihailovic, A., Ramulu, P. Y., Sharrett, A. R., et al. (2021). Visual function, physical function, and activities of daily living in two aging communities. Transl. Vis. Sci. Technol. 10:15. doi: 10.1167/tvst.10.14.15

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Heyl, V., and Wahl, H. W. (2012). Managing daily life with age-related sensory loss: cognitive resources gain in importance. Psychol. Aging. 27, 510–521. doi: 10.1037/a0025471

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Jia, R. X., Liang, J. H., Xu, Y., and Wang, Y. Q. (2019). Effects of physical activity and exercise on the cognitive function of patients with Alzheimer disease: a meta-analysis. BMC Geriatr. 19:181. doi: 10.1186/s12877-019-1175-2

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Kalkstein, J., Checksfield, K., Bollinger, J., and Gazzaley, A. (2011). Diminished top-down control underlies a visual imagery deficit in normal aging. J. Neurosci. 31, 15768–15774. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3209-11.2011

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Lee, M. J., Varadaraj, V., Ramulu, P. Y., Whitson, H. E., Deal, J. A., Swenor, B. K., et al. (2019). Memory and confusion complaints in visually impaired older adults: an understudied aspect of well-being. Gerontol. Geriatr. Med. 5:2333721418818944. doi: 10.1177/2333721418818944

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Lindenberger, U., and Baltes, P. B. (1994). Sensory functioning and intelligence in old age: a strong connection. Psychol. Aging. 9, 339–355. doi: 10.1037/0882-7974.9.3.339

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Lindenberger, U., Nagel, I. E., Chicherio, C., Li, S. C., Heekeren, H. R., Bäckman, L., et al. (2008). Age-related decline in brain resources modulates genetic effects on cognitive functioning. Front. Neurosci. 2, 234–244. doi: 10.3389/neuro.01.039.2008

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Liu-Seifert, H., Siemers, E., Price, K., Han, B., Selzler, K. J., Henley, D., et al. (2015). Cognitive impairment precedes and predicts functional impairment in mild Alzheimer's disease. J. Alzheimers. Dis. 47, 205–214. doi: 10.3233/JAD-142508

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Massof, R. W., and Rubin, G. S. (2001). Visual function assessment questionnaires. Surv. Ophthalmol. 45, 531–548. doi: 10.1016/S0039-6257(01)00194-1

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Petersen, R. C. (2004). Mild cognitive impairment as a diagnostic entity. J. Intern. Med. 256, 183–194. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2796.2004.01388.x

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Podsiadlo, D., and Richardson, S. (2015). The timed “up and go”: a test of basic functional mobility for frail elderly persons. J. Am. Geriatr. Soc. 39, 142–148. doi: 10.1111/j.1532-5415.1991.tb01616.x

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Raimo, S., Maggi, G., Ilardi, C. R., Cavallo, N. D., Torchia, V., Pilgrom, M. A., et al. (2024). The relation between cognitive functioning and activities of daily living in normal aging, mild cognitive impairment, and dementia: a meta-analysis. Neurol. Sci. 45, 2427–2443. doi: 10.1007/s10072-024-07366-2

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Rowe, F. J., and Sarkies, N. J. (1998). Assessment of visual function in idiopathic intracranial hypertension: a prospective study. Eye 12, 111–118. doi: 10.1038/eye.1998.18

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Schneider, B. A., and Pichora-Fuller, M. K. (2000). “Implications of perceptual deterioration for cognitive aging research,” in The handbook of aging and cognition, 2nd ed. (Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates Publishers), 155–219.

Google Scholar

Tulliani, N., Bissett, M., Fahey, P., Bye, R., and Liu, K. P. Y. (2022). Efficacy of cognitive remediation on activities of daily living in individuals with mild cognitive impairment or early-stage dementia: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Syst. Rev. 11:156. doi: 10.1186/s13643-022-02032-0

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Warrian, K. J., Altangerel, U., and Spaeth, G. L. (2010). Performance-based measures of visual function. Surv. Ophthalmol. 55, 146–161. doi: 10.1016/j.survophthal.2009.06.006

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Wolf, A., Ueda, K., and Hirano, Y. (2021). Recent updates of eye movement abnormalities in patients with schizophrenia: a scoping review. Psychiatry Clin. Neurosci. 75, 82–100. doi: 10.1111/pcn.13188

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Yoshii, F., Onaka, H., Kohara, S., Ryo, M., and Takahashi, W. (2019). Association of smell identification deficit with Alzheimer's disease assessment scale-cognitive subscale, japanese version scores and brain atrophy in patients with dementia. Eur. Neurol. 81, 145–151. doi: 10.1159/000501311

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Zanto, T. P., Rubens, M. T., Thangavel, A., and Gazzaley, A. (2011). Causal role of the prefrontal cortex in top-down modulation of visual processing and working memory. Nat. Neurosci. 14, 656–661. doi: 10.1038/nn.2773

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Zhou, Y. (2020). Imaging and Multiomic Biomarker Applications. Hauppauge: Nova Science Publishers.

Google Scholar

Keywords: Alzheimer's disease, mild cognitive impairment, cognitive function, visual function, activities of daily living

Citation: Zhou C, Yang G, Theeboom M, Yang H, Zhu R, Zhou Z and Zhu D (2025) Role of visual function and performance of activities of daily living in cognitive function in patients with mild cognitive impairment: a cross-sectional study. Front. Aging Neurosci. 17:1505815. doi: 10.3389/fnagi.2025.1505815

Received: 03 October 2024; Accepted: 15 January 2025;
Published: 05 February 2025.

Edited by:

Chinedu Udeh-Momoh, Wake Forest University, United States

Reviewed by:

Fatma Sena Dost, Dokuz Eylül University, Türkiye
Jorge Buele, Universidad Indoamérica, Ecuador

Copyright © 2025 Zhou, Yang, Theeboom, Yang, Zhu, Zhou and Zhu. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Dong Zhu, emh1ZG9uZ0BzdXMuZWR1LmNu

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

Research integrity at Frontiers

Man ultramarathon runner in the mountains he trains at sunset

94% of researchers rate our articles as excellent or good

Learn more about the work of our research integrity team to safeguard the quality of each article we publish.


Find out more