Skip to main content

REVIEW article

Front. Aging Neurosci., 24 June 2024
Sec. Neurocognitive Aging and Behavior

Aberrant decision-making as a risk factor for falls in aging

  • 1Division of Biokinesiology and Physical Therapy, University of Southern California, Los Angeles, CA, United States
  • 2Department of Biomedical Engineering, University of Southern California, Los Angeles, CA, United States
  • 3Neuroscience Graduate Program, University of Southern California, Los Angeles, CA, United States

Neuromotor impairments resulting from natural aging and aging-related diseases are often accompanied by a heightened prevalence of falls and fall-related injuries. Conventionally, the study of factors contributing to falls focuses on intrinsic characteristics, such as sensorimotor processing delays and weakness, and extrinsic factors, such as environmental obstacles. However, the impact of these factors only becomes evident in response to people’s decisions about how and where they will move in their environment. This decision-making process can be considered a behavioral risk factor, and it influences the extent to which a person engages in activities that place them near the limits of their capacity. While there are readily available tools for assessing intrinsic and extrinsic fall risk, our understanding of how to assess behavioral risk is limited. Measuring behavioral risk requires a systematic assessment of how people make decisions when walking in complex environments and how these decisions relate to their functional capacity. We propose that experimental methods and computational models derived from behavioral economics can stimulate the development of such assessments. Behavioral economics relies on theoretical models and empirical studies to characterize the factors that influence how people make decisions under risky conditions where a given decision can have variable outcomes. Applying a behavioral economic approach to walking can provide insight into how internal assessment of one’s fall risk influences the tasks that one is willing to perform. Ultimately, these assessments will allow us to identify people who make choices that increase their likelihood of fall-related injuries.

Introduction

Control of bipedal gait is an inherently risky task. As we walk, our body’s center of mass routinely exits our base of support, defined by the contact area between our feet and the ground, and as a result, we must actively control where we place our feet to maintain balance and prevent falls. In the absence of sensorimotor impairments, we learn to maintain balance while walking in various conditions, from smooth, uncluttered environments to uneven, mountainous terrain. This ability to maintain balance while walking is even more impressive when we consider that the nervous system must account for sensorimotor transmission and processing delays (Milton, 2011), signal-dependent noise (Harris and Wolpert, 1998), slow conversion of neural impulses to muscle force (Sandow, 1952), and a high-dimensional action space (Bernshteĭn, 1967). Although the nervous system readily solves the balance control problem despite these challenges, age-related impairments and a variety of neurological and musculoskeletal injuries can dramatically degrade balance and increase fall risk. Several studies over many years have characterized how factors such as slow response times (Smeesters et al., 2001; Pijnappels et al., 2005; Okubo et al., 2017), weakness (Moreland et al., 2004; Ding and Yang, 2016; Lauretani et al., 2018), and impairments in coordination (James et al., 2017; Pozaic et al., 2019; Liu and Finley, 2020) can increase fall risk by limiting people’s ability to respond appropriately to balance perturbations in risky environments. However, much less effort has been devoted to understanding the role of decision-making when people are exposed to situations that may put them at risk of falling.

The purpose of this review is to highlight how the choices people make regarding their future actions influence the likelihood of falls and how age-related changes influence this class of behavioral risk factors. We first highlight differences in how the concept of risk is operationalized in movement science and decision-making, then discuss limitations in contemporary approaches to examine risk-taking behavior during walking. Next, we discuss how theories and methods from behavioral economics can be used to develop models that explain how people make decisions in the context of risk. Finally, we conclude with a perspective of how we can integrate methods from behavioral economics with experimental methods from movement science to understand how age-related changes in the decision-making process may contribute to a heightened risk of falls in older versus younger adults (for key points, see Box 1).

Managing risk during walking

Risk during walking has classically been conceptualized in two forms within the movement sciences. First, fall risk is commonly considered as the likelihood that a person will experience a fall over a fixed period in the future (Tiedemann et al., 2010). The primary limitation of this definition is that it is not directly observable as there is currently no assessment that precisely predicts how likely someone is to fall over short timescales. Second, risk is often conceptualized with reference to dynamic balance control by characterizing how well people recover from losses of balance to prevent falls during walking (McAndrew et al., 2011; Hak et al., 2012; McAndrew Young et al., 2012; Aprigliano et al., 2016; Liu et al., 2018; Liu and Finley, 2020). For example, it is common for researchers to use biomechanical measures such as dynamic margins of stability (McAndrew Young et al., 2012; Hak et al., 2013; Park and Finley, 2017; Havens et al., 2018; Buurke et al., 2020) and whole-body angular momentum (Herr and Popovic, 2008; Nott et al., 2014; Aprigliano et al., 2016; Liu et al., 2018; Liu and Finley, 2020) to characterize balance control in natural and perturbed gait. Applying perturbations during walking allows researchers to characterize the control strategies used to maintain balance and assess how these strategies differ in people with neuromotor impairments. Although these biomechanical measures are precise, they fail to capture the probabilistic characteristics of behavior associated with the concept of risk.

In contrast to how risk is conceptualized in movement science, risk is formally defined within behavioral economics as the variance of the possible outcomes in a given situation (Markowitz, 1952; Kahneman and Tversky, 1979; Tobler and Weber, 2014). In the context of walking, one can consider a single step to be a form of gamble. Using the definition of risk from behavioral economics, taking steps on a wide path that is free of obstacles would be considered low risk because the probability of losing one’s balance is near zero for people who lack balance impairments. In contrast, walking on an uneven, rocky trail has more variable possible outcomes such as losing balance or falling and hence, has higher risk. What remains to be understood is how people weigh risk when choosing between alternative routes through the environment or when deciding if they should perform a task that may increase their probability of losing balance or falling.

Behavioral risk

Several factors contribute to the likelihood of an individual falling and these are most commonly divided into three types—intrinsic, extrinsic, and behavioral (Perell et al., 2001; Feldman and Chaudhury, 2008; Figure 1). Intrinsic factors include physical and cognitive characteristics such as sensorimotor processing delays, weakness, gait and balance deficits, and cognitive impairments. Extrinsic factors relate to the presence of hazards in the environment, such as obstacles and slippery surfaces. While these two categories include factors that one can observe and measure, behavioral fall risk is a relatively ill-defined category. Broadly, it relates to people’s choices regarding their actions (WHO, 2007; Feldman and Chaudhury, 2008). These can be directly related to movement, such as hurrying or carrying multiple objects while walking, or they can be indirectly related to movement, such as excessive alcohol use. Another example of a choice that would impact behavioral risk is the decision one makes when faced with multiple candidate paths linking one location to another. For example, when hiking through a forest, one might encounter two potential routes leading to the same destination: a long but smooth path and a short but uneven path with loose gravel and obstacles (Figure 2). Deciding between these routes requires a person to estimate the riskiness of each option based on estimates of their capacity and properties of the environment, and manage a trade-off between this perceived risk, time, and effort. This decision-making process is particularly critical for elderly individuals who are at risk of catastrophic injury from falls. Aging is often accompanied by cognitive impairments in domains such as executive function, working memory, and fluid intelligence, which may also influence decision-making (Del Missier et al., 2012; Brand and Schiebener, 2013; Tymula et al., 2013; Trevisan et al., 2019; Waltrip et al., 2023).

Figure 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Examples of intrinsic (aging and weakness, dizziness), extrinsic (obstacles, vehicles on the road), and behavioral (carrying heavy objects, walking in heels under the influence of alcohol) factors that influence fall risk.

Figure 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Examples of risky decision-making during walking. Decision between two hiking paths to the same destination—a long path without obstacles (green dashed line) and a short path with several obstacles (red dashed line) such as rocks and fallen trees. (Left) From the perspective of the decision-maker. (Right) Top view of the two options. This scenario represents a decision situation with a trade-off between risk and time such that while the route on the left (or green dashed line) has low risk, it would require more time to reach the destination whereas the opposite is true for the second option. The route on the right (or red dashed line) presents a greater fall and injury risk.

Age-dependent changes in neurobiological processes can increase falls through their effects on decision-making. Decision-making is largely attributed to frontal areas of the brain such as the posterior parietal cortex and lateral prefrontal cortex, which show reductions in gray matter volume with aging (Resnick et al., 2003; Driscoll et al., 2009; Kennedy et al., 2009). In addition, dopamine plays a key role in assigning subjective values to different choice options in risky decision-making (Levy et al., 2010), and dopamine receptor availability in the prefrontal cortex has been shown to decline with aging (Karrer et al., 2017). It has been proposed that these declines lead to decisions made more from emotional or affect-based processes than from analytical processes (Lighthall, 2020). Additionally, the cholinergic system has been implicated in falls through its role in attentional focus such that older adults who fall have lower cholinergic activity than those who do not and among the fallers, the cholinergic activity is associated with gait speed (Pelosin et al., 2016). This is particularly interesting because of the role that inattention plays in promoting impulsivity in older adults during gait, thereby increasing fall risk (Harrison et al., 2010; Ferrari et al., 2012). Impulsivity refers to making decisions without considering the outcomes (Harrison et al., 2010) or failing to consider the immediate environment and safety concerns when moving (Ferrari et al., 2012) and therefore, has a direct impact on behavioral fall risk. Overall, these results provide a neurophysiological basis for a complex interplay between motor impairments, cognitive decline, and decision-making with aging, which may result in elevated fall risk in older adults.

While some studies and reviews allude to the presence of behavioral risk factors, these factors have historically been understudied. A multifactorial framework for fall risk has been proposed based on a review of 25 studies investigating relationships between features of the physical environment and falls in older adults (Feldman and Chaudhury, 2008). Three main fall risk factors were identified, the interactions between which determine the risk of falls: mobility, features of the physical environment, and risk-taking behavior. Mobility was defined as the individual’s ability to perform movements, which can be affected by aging, weakness, and disorders such as Parkinson’s disease and stroke. Features of the physical environment included the presence of hazards or safety features in people’s homes and daily use environments, for example, grab bars in the bathroom or railings on staircases. Risk-taking behaviors were defined as those that increase the likelihood of falling or those that challenge an individual’s dynamic balance. Thus, risk-taking behaviors are specific to each individual, such that a behavior that may be risk-taking for one individual might not be for another. Inappropriate risk-taking behaviors are reported as being a major cause of falls among older adults (Connell and Wolf, 1997; Feldman and Chaudhury, 2008). This fall risk framework, however, does not propose a method to objectively assess risk-taking.

Self-reported assessments of behavioral risk

Because an individual’s risk-taking behaviors are not easily observed, clinical assessments instead capture people’s perceptions of their ability to maintain balance in risky environments using scales such as the Falls Efficacy Scale, Activities-Specific Balance Confidence Scale, and the Modified Gait Efficacy Scale (Delbaere et al., 2010; Hadjistavropoulos et al., 2011; Moreira et al., 2017; Kluft et al., 2020). The Falls Efficacy Scale assesses the fear of falling by asking respondents to rate their level of concern about falling while performing different activities. The Activities-Specific Balance Confidence Scale and the Modified Gait Efficacy Scale measure balance confidence by having respondents rate their level of confidence in their ability to perform different activities without losing their balance. Though neither falls efficacy nor balance confidence assess decision-making, they can be used to gain insight into risk-seeking tendencies such that individuals with high falls efficacy and high balance confidence may be more willing to take risk, regardless of their actual physical ability. The Falls Behavioral Scale for Older People assesses people’s perceptions of their behaviors when faced with risky situations (Clemson et al., 2003). Respondents can rate the frequency with which they engage in protective behaviors, such as holding on to a handrail when using stairs and using a walking aid when needed, and risky behaviors, such as hurrying when doing things. Each of these questionnaires relies on self-reports and, hence, necessarily assesses people’s perceptions of their behaviors and not their actual behaviors. These types of questionnaires are also subject to self-reporting biases (Raphael, 1987; Grimm, 2010; Althubaiti, 2016), including recall and social desirability such that people may tend toward reporting their behaviors as being more cautious than they actually are.

Experimental methods of assessing behavioral risk while walking

Plank-crossing is a walking task that has been used to assess behavioral risk in older adults, such that when participants are free to choose from planks of different widths and heights to walk across, their choice can indicate their level of behavioral risk (Butler et al., 2015). Older adults who took higher behavioral risk in this task self-reported as being cautious in their everyday lives (Butler et al., 2015). This mismatch could potentially be due to the previously discussed self-reporting biases, incorrect estimates of gait ability during plank-crossing, impaired sensorimotor integration or cognitive deficits (Li and Lindenberger, 2002). These same older adults also experienced more falls in a 12-month follow-up period. Additionally, there was no association between precision walking ability and behavioral risk as those with better ability chose lower-risk planks and vice versa. One limitation of this study, however, is that plank crossing speed was not controlled or accounted for and can itself be a source of risk due to its influence on foot placement accuracy (Bradshaw and Sparrow, 2000; Roerdink et al., 2021). A second limitation is that participants did not cross the chosen planks, and thus the chosen speed of plank-crossing and the likelihood of making a misstep were unknown.

A mismatch has also been found between reported risk-taking during road-crossing and actual observed road-crossing decisions in older adults (Butler et al., 2016). Road-crossing is a common risky activity that requires a good estimate of one’s ability and accurate perceptions of the speed of moving vehicles. In a study of road-crossing decisions, a simulated pedestrian crossing was created and participants were instructed to cross in front of a styrofoam car, at the shortest possible distance from it (Butler et al., 2016). One group crossed within a small distance from the car, suggesting that they accurately judged their ability and their environment. This group also performed best on a battery of physical and cognitive tests. However, they reported engaging in risky behavior and being less cautious in everyday life. Conversely, the groups that were either “hit” by the car or had to retreat to avoid being “hit” reported being less risky and more cautious on the everyday risk-taking scale.

A major limitation of the studies described above was that only a single decision was made in each condition. In everyday life, we tend to face similar decision-making situations multiple times and our decisions generally change over time as we learn from experience. It is important to understand how experience influences decision-making and whether experience-dependent changes are appropriate, given the individual’s motor ability. These studies were also limited in the range of risky conditions that could safely be used. One potential way to address this limitation is to use virtual reality (VR) in combination with physical perturbations to create complex walking scenarios that mimic the real world (Cano Porras et al., 2018, 2019; Raffegeau et al., 2023). An advantage of this method is that it would allow for the evaluation of decision-making in real-world scenarios within a controlled and safe environment. Using physical perturbations delivered via motion platforms or specialized treadmills can further improve ecological validity by introducing balance-disturbing consequences to decisions that may better reflect what people experience in the real world (Park and Finley, 2017; Lee et al., 2019; Buurke et al., 2020; Debelle et al., 2020). This would allow researchers to systematically capture the effects of experience, the visual representation of risk, and the physical consequences of errors on decision-making while walking.

The role of misjudgment in risky decision-making during walking

The results above suggest a mismatch between people’s perception and memory of how they manage risk while walking and their behaviors. Similarly, there is a mismatch between perceived and actual motor ability. In older adults, it is possible that a decline in physical function with aging may not always be accompanied by a perception of this decline (Butler et al., 2016). In risky situations, this mismatch between perceived and actual ability has the potential to increase the probability of a fall. For example, if an individual who overestimates their stepping ability faces an obstacle on a hiking trail, they may incorrectly choose to step over it instead of walking around it, thereby putting themselves at an increased risk of tripping or falling and injuring themselves. Therefore, there is a need to investigate this construct of “misjudgment” between perceived and actual ability and its contribution to fall risk.

In general, misjudgment is quantified by measuring perceived ability through an oral response and actual ability through the performance of the task. Studies of misjudgment in motor tasks are summarized in Table 1. One example of such a task is precision walking wherein perceived ability was measured by asking participants to estimate the narrowest path within which they could walk without stepping outside its boundaries (Kluft et al., 2017). Actual ability was measured by determining the actual narrowest path within which they could accurately walk. The degree of misjudgment in this paradigm was found to be highly variable among participants, with many older adults overestimating their walking ability (Lighthall, 2020). Additionally, older adults with a better precision walking ability were not better judges of their accuracy than those with a lower ability. Therefore, judgment of ability appears to be a separate skill in older adults, independent of one’s actual ability.

Table 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Summary of studies of misjudgement of ability in different gait-related motor tasks.

Preliminary evidence suggests that misjudgment of ability is task-specific and not an inherent trait. Misjudgment was quantified across four different stepping tasks that involved stepping over a height, stepping across a certain distance, or taking a step to recover from a forward lean (Kluft et al., 2017). There was no association between the degrees of misjudgment across these tasks, suggesting that this may not be a trait inherent to an individual but rather a task-specific skill. While further work has been done to develop more tasks to better assess misjudgment, a good method for quantification of misjudgment has not yet been identified (Kluft et al., 2018).

Decision-making under risk

While judgment of ability is one factor in walking-related behavioral decisions, these are complex decisions that require an evaluation of the walking environment and the possible outcomes. As a result, there is a need to develop a quantitative framework to better explain how people integrate information about themselves and the environment to inform their decisions. Behavioral economics is a field of study that investigates the psychological and cognitive processes underlying decision-making (Glimcher and Fehr, 2014). Theories in this field provide a framework to study people’s choices in different contexts and the underlying processes that lead to these choices. In the context of behavioral economics, a risky prospect is defined as a situation in which the possible outcome of choosing a specific option is uncertain, and risk is defined as the variance in the distribution of possible outcomes (Braun et al., 2011; Nagengast et al., 2011).

Models of risky decision-making typically maximize a utility function. For example, the mean–variance model expresses the utility of an option, U(x), as a function of the expected value of the possible outcomes, E(x), and their variance (Equation 1) (Markowitz, 1952; Nagengast et al., 2011).

U x = E x θVar x     (1)

Here, θ is the risk-sensitivity parameter where a value of zero indicates risk neutrality, a positive value indicates risk aversion, and a negative value indicates a risk-seeking tendency. This model has previously been applied in effort-based decision-making, in which the “sure bet” required the exertion of a known fixed force by the hand, whereas the risky option could require the individual to exert a lower or higher force than the fixed option, thereby having a larger variance (Nagengast et al., 2011). This and other studies that have applied behavioral economic models of decision-making to motor control are summarized in Table 2. Fitting this model to the participants’ behavior demonstrated that they were sensitive to the level of risk and had tendencies to choose riskier options.

Table 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Summary of studies examining the influence of risk on decision-making in motor tasks.

A model of motor decision-making has been proposed that accounts for the inherent uncertainty in movement planning and execution in addition to biomechanical costs such as effort (Trommershäuser et al., 2003a). To test this model, partially overlapping target and penalty areas were presented on a screen. A reaching movement that ended in the target circle yielded reward points, whereas the penalty circle led to a loss. A prediction was made for each individual’s optimal movement endpoint that would maximize reward while accounting for their natural movement variability. This prediction was compared to their actual chosen endpoints. Actual decisions closely matched those of an “optimal performer” suggesting that people accurately take both their own movement variability or uncertainty as well as explicit costs into account when making decisions for such pointing tasks (Trommershäuser et al., 2003a).

Prospect Theory is a model of risky decision-making that was developed to explain commonly observed “irrationalities” in people’s behaviors (Kahneman and Tversky, 1979; Tversky and Kahneman, 1992). These include risk-seeking tendencies in the presence of small probability gains (e.g., gambling), and risk-aversive tendencies in the presence of small probability losses (e.g., purchasing insurance). According to this model, people’s choices can be explained by a process in which they transform and represent the probability and values of options available to them and use these representations to make a decision (Kahneman and Tversky, 1979; Tversky and Kahneman, 1992). Prospect Theory considers three key elements of decision-making: diminishing sensitivity, loss aversion, and probability distortion. For an option with a possible outcome x that has a probability p, Prospect Theory defines its subjective value v(x) and decision weight w(p) as in the following equations.

v x = { x α f o r x 0 λ x β f o r x < 0     (2)
w p = exp ln p γ     (3)

Consider a lottery that offers $50 with probability p and $0 with probability 1-p. First, Prospect Theory suggests that the subjective value of a gain of $50 is higher for someone who only has $100 than for someone with $10,000. This feature reflects a value function that has a diminishing sensitivity to change (Equation 2, when ɑ < 1 or 𝛽 < 1). Second, the theory captures loss aversion for which the value function is steeper for losses than for gains. This leads to losses having a higher impact on value than a gain of equivalent magnitude (𝜆 in Equation 2). Third, the theory characterizes probability distortion, according to which people do not objectively represent the probabilities of possible outcomes in a lottery. Instead, they transform the probabilities to decision weights, w(p), that determine the impact of each outcome on the person’s decision (Equation 3). This distortion function is generally represented by a sigmoidal function, which is steepest near probabilities of 0 and 1 (Tversky and Kahneman, 1992). A more recent version of this theory, called Cumulative Prospect Theory, applies the probability weighting function to cumulative probabilities of outcomes and is generalizable to decisions that include more than two possible outcomes (Tversky and Kahneman, 1992).

Several studies have used the Prospect Theory framework to investigate motor decision-making, subjective valuation, and probability distortion during motor tasks (Wu et al., 2009; O’Brien and Ahmed, 2013; O’Brien and Ahmed, 2014; O’Brien and Ahmed, 2015). A comparison between equivalent economic and motor decision-making tasks observed typical overweighting of small probabilities and underweighting of moderate to large probabilities in the economic domain but the opposite tendency in the motor domain (Wu et al., 2009). “Motor lotteries” were presented in the form of targets of varying widths, which, combined with each individual’s motor variability, determined the probability of success of a reaching movement. Each target was associated with some reward equivalent to those in the economic task. Therefore, with all else being equal, the only difference between the two tasks was the manner in which probability information was provided—explicitly in the economic task and implicitly based on the target width in the motor task. These results suggest that the probability weighting function depends on the task, the nature in which probability information is provided, or both (Box 2).

Within the motor domain, risk-seeking has been observed in both upper-body reaching and whole-body leaning type of movements (O’Brien and Ahmed, 2013). Similar to previously reported results, underweighting of small and overweighting of moderate to large probabilities was observed in both these movements, along with the overvaluation of rewards and undervaluation of penalties (O’Brien and Ahmed, 2013). In both tasks, swift out-and-back movements were performed to control a cursor on a screen using either a manipulandum for the reaching task or by leaning forward on a force plate for the whole-body task. The cursor was taken from its home position to as close as possible to the edge of a virtual “cliff.” Risk was manipulated by either varying the penalty associated with the cliff region or by adding noise to the cursor position. Surprisingly, risk-seeking was greater in the whole-body task than in the arm-reaching task. This result is counterintuitive because one would expect that a whole-body movement, which has a more unstable posture than a seated reaching movement, might lead to risk-aversive tendencies. However, it is important to note that the cliff and the consequences of risk-taking in this task are artificial, and therefore, these results may not generalize to the presence of real, physical consequences.

When the same two tasks described above were performed on a platform at a height of 0.8 m above the ground, the difference in the extent of risk-seeking between the two was eliminated (O’Brien and Ahmed, 2015). Decision analysis using CPT revealed a significant difference in probability weighting in the whole-body movement between the two elevations, such that small probability losses were overestimated to a greater extent at high elevations, leading to more risk-averse behavior. Therefore, it seems that implicit postural threat increases risk sensitivity in the context of movement-related decisions. While these two studies were the first to assess risk-sensitive decision-making in goal-directed whole-body movements using a behavioral economic approach, more work remains to be done using tasks with actual physical consequences to understand how the perception of risk and its effects on decision-making is affected by movement type, context, and experience.

While these studies have considerably advanced the understanding of motor decision-making in young adults, the effects of aging in this decision domain are not well studied. There is some evidence of decreased risk-taking in older adults compared to young adults in a reach-to-target task where risk is manipulated using penalty points (Valsecchi et al., 2018). In a plank-crossing task described in a previous section, mixed results were reported among older adults with some choosing to cross risky planks and some choosing safer options (Butler et al., 2015). However, this study did not compare young and older adults, making it difficult to ascertain whether risk-taking in this task is age-dependent. Studies comparing decision-making across multiple domains suggest an interaction between domain and age such that risk-seeking generally decreases with age in the financial, health, ethical, and recreational domains but might increase with age in the social domain (Weber et al., 2002; Blais and Weber, 2006; Rolison et al., 2014; Waltrip et al., 2023). More work is needed, specifically in the context of gait-related decision-making, to understand how aging influences behavioral fall risk.

Opportunities for advancing theoretical understanding and assessment of behavioral risk during walking

As reviewed above, fall risk assessments in the clinic largely focus on identifying intrinsic and extrinsic fall risk factors, often rely on self-reports, and do not have a high predictive value for falls (Lee et al., 2013). One potential reason for the limited predictive ability of current assessments is that they neglect an assessment of actual decisions in the context of risk. The emergence of intrinsic risk factors with age or disease is likely accompanied by changes in decisions that people make. For example, a decline in visual acuity may lead to an individual consciously adopting more cautious behaviors such as walking slower. Conversely, the lack of intrinsic or extrinsic risk factors is not necessarily indicative of low fall risk as the individual’s behavioral decisions could be putting them in risky situations. Therefore, this interplay between intrinsic and extrinsic risk factors and behavioral decisions needs to be investigated, and this requires the means to systematically assess people’s motor decision-making in risky environments.

The need to assess behavioral risk has been addressed in part by developing self-report questionnaires that ask respondents to rate their level of engagement in risky behaviors in everyday life (Clemson et al., 2003; Kwan et al., 2013; Butler et al., 2015). However, such questionnaires rely on people’s perceptions and memories of their actions and can, therefore be subject to common biases such as recall bias and social desirability bias (Althubaiti, 2016). In fact, as reviewed above, people’s self-reports of risky behaviors in everyday life do not always match the nature of their actual decisions in behavioral tasks (Butler et al., 2015, 2016). This points toward the need to assess people’s actual decision-making during walking, the factors that influence this decision-making process, and the relationship between walking decisions and fall risk.

We propose that experimental methods and computational models derived from behavioral economics can overcome the limitations associated with assessments of behavioral risk that are based primarily on self-reporting. A behavioral economic approach can be used to identify the cognitive processes that underlie decision-making in complex environments and identify individuals who may be at a higher risk of falls because they engage in risk-seeking behaviors. The Mean–Variance model and Prospect Theory are examples of integrative or algebraic models of decision-making, which assume that people assess all the information about all available options before making a decision. While these models have been fairly successful in explaining people’s economic decisions, they are not considered to be plausible representations of the underlying cognitive computations that mediate decision-making (Payne et al., 1978; Pachur et al., 2017). For example, when we decide between alternate paths along a rocky trail, we do not have explicit information about the probabilities of different outcomes (e.g., losses of balance) associated with each path. Even in situations where all this information is explicitly available, the ability to perform a Prospect Theory-like algebraic analysis may not be psychologically plausible (Payne et al., 1978; Lopes, 1995; Pachur et al., 2017).

To this end, heuristic models of decision-making have been proposed as more plausible strategies by which people make decisions. Individuals with high levels of statistical numeracy have been shown to use heuristic rather than algebraic models of decision-making, which leads to more consistent choices between risky economic gambles (Ashby, 2017). These models assume that people have a limited capacity for memory and information processing and as a result, filter the available information to simplify the decision-making process (Slovic et al., 2004; Gonzalez and Mehlhorn, 2016; Pachur et al., 2017). An example of this filtering process is the recency bias, according to which people more heavily weigh events that were experienced closer in time to the decision (Hertwig and Erev, 2009). In the context of gait, if a decision about a walking path must be made quickly, this heuristic may be utilized and allow for more recent experiences of losses of balance to influence the decision. Another heuristic, which is particularly relevant in the context of walking and falls, is the affect heuristic (Slovic et al., 2004). Affect is the feeling of positivity or negativity that we associate with any event or object. By associating each of our experiences with a general “feeling” or “affect,” we can perform a quick analysis of possible outcomes of a decision in the future by identifying the affect associated with each. This becomes particularly relevant when studying motor decisions that involve whole-body movements because of the affective response that near-falls or falls elicit. The use of heuristic processes in simple choices has also been shown to lead to more consistent decisions than in complex choices with higher cognitive demands (Bessette et al., 2021). However, the extent to which the use of more cognitively demanding deliberative and simpler heuristic decision-making processes explains individual differences in behavioral risk during walking remains to be seen.

The development of a comprehensive framework of decision-making during walking requires an understanding of how different factors, such as risk or uncertainty, time, movement goals, movement context, and personality traits, influence decisions. Future studies should aim to study these factors in isolation and in combination to best approximate people’s decision-making behaviors in more complex, real-world situations. The influence of risk or uncertainty can be determined by having people choose between walking options with different levels of variability in the possible outcomes of each. These outcomes may be presented in the form of trips or slips, which are often the cause of falls in both young and older adults and are, therefore, ecologically valid decision consequences. Technologies such as augmented and virtual reality (VR) can be particularly useful in these contexts as they allow for the simulation of real-world environments within which one or more factors can be systematically manipulated (Cano Porras et al., 2018, 2019; Raffegeau et al., 2023). For example, the effects of movement context may be studied by using multiple virtual environments such as road-crossing in traffic, hiking on a trail, or navigating in a crowded mall. In combination with motion platforms or specialized treadmills that can be used to deliver the physical consequences of decisions, these virtual environments can be made to match real-world environments more closely.

While technologies such as VR and specialized treadmills can help advance the research endeavor to understand behavioral fall risk, they may not always be practical options for use in clinical practice due to financial and physical space constraints. More practical solutions for evaluating decision-making in the clinic include those similar to the plank-crossing task described previously where planks of differing levels of difficulty can be presented to walk across (Butler et al., 2015). While participants in this study did not actually cross their plank of choice due to safety concerns, future studies could implement safety measures to enable clinicians to assess their patients’ risk-taking tendencies based on which planks they choose to cross or avoid. Further work is required to develop low-cost and efficient methods to assess gait-related decision-making in the clinic.

Assessments of behavioral fall risk can also be improved by modifying assessments of fall history to better capture the circumstances surrounding a fall. These circumstances could include the individual’s goals and motivation at the time of the fall, and the exact movement or behavior that led to the fall. Although this information is prone to each of the biases associated with self-reports (Raphael, 1987), it may be used to track the conditions surrounding falls more comprehensively in both research and the clinic. Additionally, it is just as important to consider the circumstances leading to near-falls or balance disturbances, as they can provide insight into daily behaviors that put the individual in risky situations but may otherwise be overlooked.

A final challenge when translating the risky decision-making framework to locomotor control and fall risk is that of identifying ideal decisions and decision-making processes. When walking, decisions are made within the context of the individual’s intrinsic characteristics and features of their environment (Feldman and Chaudhury, 2008). These decisions then directly inform gait behavior and fall risk. Because of the many sources of sensory information available, identifying the key factors that decisions are based on and those that they should be based on is important, particularly in terms of translating this work to the clinic. Ultimately, the goal is for physical therapists to be able to recognize key features of a patient’s decision-making process and prescribe specific and actionable ways to alter it in a way that reduces fall risk. Fall risk itself is inherently multidimensional and its reliable estimation continues to be an elusive goal despite a wealth of research (Perell et al., 2001; Gates et al., 2008; Ejupi et al., 2014; Kluft et al., 2019). This is primarily because assessments of gait and balance ability at a single time point in a clinic or research setting do not entirely reflect daily behaviors in the outside world. As the use of wearable technology (Howcroft et al., 2013, 2017; Bet et al., 2019; Nouredanesh et al., 2021) for health and behavioral monitoring improves, clinicians may be able to combine data from daily gait behavior and assessments of risky decision-making to address excessive risk-seeking or risk-aversive tendencies in their patients.

Conclusion

While it is well-established that aging and the presence of neurological impairments increase the risk of falls, much less is known about how these factors influence behavioral risk. As reviewed here, behavioral risk is influenced by an individual’s ability to assess their own capacity, the requirements of the task, the features of the environment, and the ability to integrate this information to make appropriate decisions. Impairments in any of these processes can cause impaired decision-making and when this occurs in the context of gait and balance-related movements, it can lead to potentially catastrophic outcomes. Age-related declines in physical ability are not always accompanied by a simultaneous adjustment of perceived ability, leading to higher engagement in risky behaviors (Sakurai et al., 2013; Butler et al., 2015; Liphart et al., 2016). This misjudgment of ability can stem from factors including, but not limited to, sensory impairments such as information transmission delays, increased noise, and reductions in sensitivity that affect multiple sensory modalities. The development of a rigorous computational framework to explain people’s decisions during walking may help identify the type of information that an individual uses when performing a risk assessment for walking, how the perception of this information may be distorted, and how this distortion influences the mobility-related decisions that people make in everyday life. By using VR in combination with perturbations capable of triggering losses of balance (Lee et al., 2019; Liss et al., 2022), one can create simulations of complex real-world environments to expand the current understanding of how decision-making influences fall risk in older adults and people with mobility impairments.

BOX 1. Key points.

• Fall risk is influenced by three types of factors—intrinsic (e.g., weakness), extrinsic (e.g., obstacles), and behavioral (e.g., risk-taking behaviors) (Connell and Wolf, 1997; Perell et al., 2001; Feldman and Chaudhury, 2008).

• We lack a systematic method to assess behavioral fall risk that does not rely on self-reports, which often do not match actual real-world behaviors.

• Aging can lead to a mismatch between actual and perceived motor ability, potentially increasing fall risk by encouraging risky motor decisions that do not align with actual ability (Butler et al., 2015, 2016; Kluft et al., 2017, 2018, 2019).

• Decision-making under risk is extensively studied in the field of behavioral economics, where risk is a function of the variability in outcomes when the same decision is repeated (Tobler and Weber, 2014).

• Computational models of decision-making used in behavioral economics have successfully explained people’s movement choices during upper extremity tasks (Tversky and Kahneman, 1992; Trommershäuser et al., 2003b; Wu et al., 2009; Delbaere et al., 2010; Grimm, 2010; Braun et al., 2011; Nagengast et al., 2011; Hak et al., 2013; O’Brien and Ahmed, 2013, 2016; Buurke et al., 2020).

• Given the success of these behavioral economic approaches in explaining upper extremity movement choices and their underlying processes, we propose that this framework can be extended to gait decisions to quantify behavioral fall risk.

• While prior studies use monetary rewards or artificial points as decision outcomes, the investigation of gait-related decisions should focus on incorporating outcomes that relate to fall risk.

• By studying how people walk in realistic virtual environments while experiencing balance disturbances such as slips and trips, we can better understand how individual differences in mobility-related decision-making influence fall risk (Lee et al., 2019; Liss et al., 2022).

BOX 2. The description-experience gap.

The most common form of decision-making tasks in behavioral economics involve a clear, numerical description of the possible outcomes of a decision and their associated probabilities. While such studies have provided tremendous insight into human decision-making under conditions of risk and uncertainty, such decisions are not commonplace in everyday life. These decisions which are made based on explicit descriptions are examples of “decisions from description.” More commonly, decisions in real life are made from experience in the same or similar situations. The study of such “decisions from experience” is relatively recent and has led to the discovery of differences in decisions based on these two modalities, commonly termed the “description-experience gap” (Hertwig et al., 2004; Hertwig and Erev, 2009; Wu et al., 2009; Ludvig and Spetch, 2011; Camilleri and Newell, 2013).

Early in the study of this phenomenon, it was most widely studied and reported in situations with rare events (Hertwig et al., 2004; Hertwig and Erev, 2009). The common finding is that rare events are overweighted in decisions from description but underweighted in decisions from experience. Because rare events by nature occur with low probabilities, two explanations for this finding are limited sampling because rare events are not experienced often in a decision from experience paradigm, and a recency heuristic because even if they are experienced sufficiently, more likely events may be experienced more recently and hence, receive more weight (Hertwig et al., 2004). In walking in everyday life, falls can be considered rare events, underweighting of which can lead to repeated engagement in activities that are likely to cause falls.

More recent work has found that this description-experience gap exists not only for low probability, rare outcomes but even for those with higher probabilities (Ludvig and Spetch, 2011). Specifically in the context of Prospect Theory, the common finding is that of overweighting of low probabilities and underweighting of moderate-large probabilities in decisions from description, but the opposite in decisions from experience (Wu et al., 2009; Ludvig and Spetch, 2011). In addition to the limited sampling and recency effects, a potential reason for this observed difference is that explicit probabilities are known in description-based decisions but must be inferred or learned in experience-based decisions (Hertwig and Erev, 2009; Wu et al., 2009). However, it is unclear how this difference in information format might influence probability weighting. Finally, another proposed cause of this gap is contingent sampling where in experience-based decisions, people may rely more heavily on information gained in situations similar to the current one, thereby disregarding any other important information which would otherwise be readily available in a description paradigm (Hertwig and Erev, 2009).

Most movement and gait-related decisions in everyday life are based on previous experiences. These decisions are different from lottery-based decisions from experience because of the added aspects of voluntary motor control and motor learning. It has previously been suggested that the differences seen in motor and economic decision-making, in addition to stemming from this description-experience gap, may also be due to people’s perceived ability to control the outcome of a decision when the outcome is based on their motor skill (Wu et al., 2009; O’Brien and Ahmed, 2013, 2014, 2015). Due to these differences, it is important to further investigate the description-experience gap specifically in the context of movement-related decisions.

Author contributions

SJ: Conceptualization, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing, Data curation, Visualization. NS: Funding acquisition, Writing – review & editing. JF: Conceptualization, Funding acquisition, Supervision, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing.

Funding

The author(s) declare that financial support was received for the research, authorship, and/or publication of this article. This work was supported by NSF Award 2043637 from the Division of Behavioral and Cognitive Sciences.

Acknowledgments

The authors would like to thank Amanda Frataccia for illustrating Figures 1 and 2.

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

The author(s) declared that they were an editorial board member of Frontiers, at the time of submission. This had no impact on the peer review process and the final decision.

Publisher’s note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

References

Althubaiti, A. (2016). Information bias in health research: definition, pitfalls, and adjustment methods. J. Multidiscip. Healthc. 9, 211–217. doi: 10.2147/JMDH.S104807

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Aprigliano, F., Martelli, D., Micera, S., and Monaco, V. (2016). Intersegmental coordination elicited by unexpected multidirectional slipping-like perturbations resembles that adopted during steady locomotion. J. Neurophysiol. 115, 728–740. doi: 10.1152/jn.00327.2015

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Ashby, N. J. S. (2017). Numeracy predicts preference consistency: deliberative search heuristics increase choice consistency for choices from description and experience. Judgm. Decis. Mak. 12, 128–139. doi: 10.1017/S1930297500005672

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Bernshteĭn, N. A. (1967). The co-ordination and regulation of movements. Oxford, London: Pergamon Press.

Google Scholar

Bessette, D. L., Wilson, R. S., and Arvai, J. L. (2021). Do people disagree with themselves? Exploring the internal consistency of complex, unfamiliar, and risky decisions. J. Risk Res. 24, 593–605. doi: 10.1080/13669877.2019.1569107

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Bet, P., Castro, P. C., and Ponti, M. A. (2019). Fall detection and fall risk assessment in older person using wearable sensors: a systematic review. Int. J. Med. Inform. 130:103946. doi: 10.1016/j.ijmedinf.2019.08.006

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Blais, A. R., and Weber, E. U. (2006). A domain-specific risk-taking (DOSPERT) scale for adult populations. Judgm. Decis. Mak. 1, 33–47. doi: 10.1017/S1930297500000334

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Bradshaw, E., and Sparrow, W. A. (2000). The speed-accuracy trade-off in human gait control when running towards targets. J. Appl. Biomech. 16, 331–341. doi: 10.1123/jab.16.4.331

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Brand, M., and Schiebener, J. (2013). Interactions of age and cognitive functions in predicting decision making under risky conditions over the life span. J. Clin. Exp. Neuropsychol. 35, 9–23. doi: 10.1080/13803395.2012.740000

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Braun, D. A., Nagengast, A. J., and Wolpert, D. M. (2011). Risk-sensitivity in sensorimotor control. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 5:1. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2011.00001

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Butler, A. A., Lord, S. R., and Fitzpatrick, R. C. (2011). Reach distance but not judgment error is associated with falls in older people. J. Gerontol. A Biol. Sci. Med. Sci. 66, 896–903. doi: 10.1093/gerona/glr071

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Butler, A. A., Lord, S. R., and Fitzpatrick, R. C. (2016). Perceptions of speed and risk: experimental studies of road crossing by older people. PLoS One 11:e0152617. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0152617

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Butler, A. A., Lord, S. R., Taylor, J. L., and Fitzpatrick, R. C. (2015). Ability versus hazard: risk-taking and falls in older people. J. Gerontol. A Biol. Sci. Med. Sci. 70, 628–634. doi: 10.1093/gerona/glu201

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Buurke, T. J. W., Liu, C., Park, S., den Otter, R., and Finley, J. M. (2020). Maintaining sagittal plane balance compromises frontal plane balance during reactive stepping in people post-stroke. Clin. Biomech. 80:105135. doi: 10.1016/j.clinbiomech.2020.105135

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Camilleri, A. R., and Newell, B. R. (2013). “Chapter 4 - mind the gap? Description, experience, and the continuum of uncertainty in risky choice” in Progress in brain research: decision making. eds. P. VSC and N. Srinivasan, vol. 202 (Amsterdam, The Netherlands: Elsevier), 55–71.

Google Scholar

Cano Porras, D., Sharon, H., Inzelberg, R., Ziv-Ner, Y., Zeilig, G., and Plotnik, M. (2019). Advanced virtual reality-based rehabilitation of balance and gait in clinical practice. Ther. Adv. Chronic. Dis. 10:2040622319868379. doi: 10.1177/2040622319868379

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Cano Porras, D., Siemonsma, P., Inzelberg, R., Zeilig, G., and Plotnik, M. (2018). Advantages of virtual reality in the rehabilitation of balance and gait. Neurology 90, 1017–1025. doi: 10.1212/WNL.0000000000005603

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Clemson, L., Cumming, R. G., and Heard, R. (2003). The development of an assessment to evaluate behavioral factors associated with falling. Am. J. Occup. Ther. 57, 380–388. doi: 10.5014/ajot.57.4.380

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Connell, B. R., and Wolf, S. L. (1997). Environmental and behavioral circumstances associated with falls at home among healthy elderly individuals. Arch. Phys. Med. Rehabil. 78, 179–186. doi: 10.1016/S0003-9993(97)90261-6

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Debelle, H., Harkness-Armstrong, C., Hadwin, K., Maganaris, C. N., and O’Brien, T. D. (2020). Recovery from a forward falling slip: measurement of dynamic stability and strength requirements using a Split-Belt instrumented treadmill. Front. Sports Act. Living. 2:2. doi: 10.3389/fspor.2020.00082

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Delbaere, K., Close, J. C. T., Brodaty, H., Sachdev, P., and Lord, S. R. (2010). Determinants of disparities between perceived and physiological risk of falling among elderly people: cohort study. BMJ 341:c4165. doi: 10.1136/bmj.c4165

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Del Missier, F., Mäntylä, T., and de Bruin, W. B. (2012). Decision-making competence, executive functioning, and general cognitive abilities. J. Behav. Decis. Mak. 25, 331–351. doi: 10.1002/bdm.731

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Ding, L., and Yang, F. (2016). Muscle weakness is related to slip-initiated falls among community-dwelling older adults. J. Biomech. 49, 238–243. doi: 10.1016/j.jbiomech.2015.12.009

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Driscoll, I., Davatzikos, C., An, Y., Wu, X., Shen, D., Kraut, M., et al. (2009). Longitudinal pattern of regional brain volume change differentiates normal aging from MCI. Neurology 72, 1906–1913. doi: 10.1212/WNL.0b013e3181a82634

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Ejupi, A., Lord, S. R., and Delbaere, K. (2014). New methods for fall risk prediction. Curr. Opin. Clin. Nutr. Metab. Care 17, 407–411. doi: 10.1097/MCO.0000000000000081

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Feldman, F., and Chaudhury, H. (2008). Falls and the physical environment: a review and a new multifactorial falls-risk conceptual framework. Can. J. Occup. Ther. 75, 82–95. doi: 10.1177/000841740807500204

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Ferrari, M., Harrison, B., and Lewis, D. (2012). The risk factors for impulsivity-related falls among hospitalized older adults. Rehabil. Nurs. J. 37, 145–150. doi: 10.1002/RNJ.00046

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Gates, S., Smith, L. A., Fisher, J. D., and Lamb, S. E. (2008). Systematic review of accuracy of screening instruments for predicting fall risk among independently living older adults : Centre for Reviews and Dissemination Accessed April 9, 2021. Available at: https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/books/NBK75880/.

Google Scholar

Glimcher, P. W., and Fehr, E. (eds.). (2014). “Introduction: a brief history of neuroeconomics” in Neuroeconomics (Cambridge, MA: Elsevier), xvii–xxviii.

Google Scholar

Gonzalez, C., and Mehlhorn, K. (2016). Framing from experience: cognitive processes and predictions of risky choice. Cogn. Sci. 40, 1163–1191. doi: 10.1111/cogs.12268

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Grau-Moya, J., Ortega, P. A., and Braun, D. A. (2012). Risk-sensitivity in Bayesian sensorimotor integration. PLoS Comput. Biol. 8:e1002698. doi: 10.1371/journal.pcbi.1002698

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Grimm, P. (2010). “Social desirability bias” in Wiley International encyclopedia of marketing. eds. J. N. Sheth and N. Malhotra (Hoboken, NJ: John Wiley & Sons, Ltd).

Google Scholar

Hadjistavropoulos, T., Delbaere, K., and Fitzgerald, T. D. (2011). Reconceptualizing the role of fear of falling and balance confidence in fall risk. J. Aging Health 23, 3–23. doi: 10.1177/0898264310378039

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Hak, L., Houdijk, H., Steenbrink, F., Mert, A., van der Wurff, P., Beek, P. J., et al. (2012). Speeding up or slowing down? Gait adaptations to preserve gait stability in response to balance perturbations. Gait Posture 36, 260–264. doi: 10.1016/j.gaitpost.2012.03.005

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Hak, L., Houdijk, H., van der Wurff, P., Prins, M. R., Mert, A., Beek, P. J., et al. (2013). Stepping strategies used by post-stroke individuals to maintain margins of stability during walking. Clin. Biomech. 28, 1041–1048. doi: 10.1016/j.clinbiomech.2013.10.010

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Harris, C. M., and Wolpert, D. M. (1998). Signal-dependent noise determines motor planning. Nature 394, 780–784. doi: 10.1038/29528

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Harrison, B. E., Ferrari, M., Campbell, C., Maddens, M., and Whall, A. L. (2010). Evaluating the relationship between inattention and impulsivity-related falls in hospitalized older adults. Geriatr. Nur. 31, 8–16. doi: 10.1016/j.gerinurse.2009.09.002

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Havens, K. L., Mukherjee, T., and Finley, J. M. (2018). Analysis of biases in dynamic margins of stability introduced by the use of simplified center of mass estimates during walking and turning. Gait Posture 59, 162–167. doi: 10.1016/j.gaitpost.2017.10.002

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Herr, H., and Popovic, M. (2008). Angular momentum in human walking. J. Exp. Biol. 211, 467–481. doi: 10.1242/jeb.008573

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Hertwig, R., Barron, G., Weber, E. U., and Erev, I. (2004). Decisions from experience and the effect of rare events in risky choice. Psychol. Sci. 15, 534–539. doi: 10.1111/j.0956-7976.2004.00715.x

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Hertwig, R., and Erev, I. (2009). The description–experience gap in risky choice. Trends Cogn. Sci. 13, 517–523. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2009.09.004

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Howcroft, J., Kofman, J., and Lemaire, E. D. (2013). Review of fall risk assessment in geriatric populations using inertial sensors. J. NeuroEngineering Rehabil. 10:91. doi: 10.1186/1743-0003-10-91

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Howcroft, J., Kofman, J., and Lemaire, E. D. (2017). Prospective fall-risk prediction models for older adults based on wearable sensors. IEEE Trans. Neural Syst. Rehabil. Eng. 25, 1812–1820. doi: 10.1109/TNSRE.2017.2687100

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

James, E. G., Leveille, S. G., Hausdorff, J. M., Barton, B., Cote, S., Karabulut, M., et al. (2017). Coordination impairments are associated with falling among older adults. Exp. Aging Res. 43, 430–439. doi: 10.1080/0361073X.2017.1369634

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Kahneman, D., and Tversky, A. (1979). Prospect theory: An analysis of decision under risk. Econometrica 47, 263–292. doi: 10.2307/1914185

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Karrer, T. M., Josef, A. K., Mata, R., Morris, E. D., and Samanez-Larkin, G. R. (2017). Reduced dopamine receptors and transporters but not synthesis capacity in normal aging adults: a meta-analysis. Neurobiol. Aging 57, 36–46. doi: 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2017.05.006

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Kennedy, K. M., Erickson, K. I., Rodrigue, K. M., Voss, M. W., Colcombe, S. J., Kramer, A. F., et al. (2009). Age-related differences in regional brain volumes: a comparison of optimized voxel-based morphometry to manual volumetry. Neurobiol. Aging 30, 1657–1676. doi: 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2007.12.020

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Kluft, N., Bruijn, S. M., Luu, M. J., Dieën, J. H., Carpenter, M. G., and Pijnappels, M. (2020). The influence of postural threat on strategy selection in a stepping-down paradigm. Sci. Rep. 10:10815. doi: 10.1038/s41598-020-66352-8

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Kluft, N., Bruijn, S. M., van Dieën, J. H., and Pijnappels, M. (2018). Do older adults select appropriate motor strategies in a stepping-down paradigm? Front. Physiol. 9:9. doi: 10.3389/fphys.2018.01419

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Kluft, N., Bruijn, S. M., Weijer, R. H. A., van Dieën, J. H., and Pijnappels, M. (2017). On the validity and consistency of misjudgment of stepping ability in young and older adults. PLoS One 12:e0190088. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0190088

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Kluft, N., Bruijn, S. M., Weijer, R. H. A., van Dieën, J. H., and Pijnappels, M. (2019). Does misjudgement in a stepping down paradigm predict falls in an older population? R. Soc. Open Sci. 6:190786. doi: 10.1098/rsos.190786

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Kluft, N., van Dieën, J. H., and Pijnappels, M. (2017). The degree of misjudgment between perceived and actual gait ability in older adults. Gait Posture 51, 275–280. doi: 10.1016/j.gaitpost.2016.10.019

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Kwan, M. M. S., Tsang, W. W. N., Lin, S. I., Greenaway, M., Close, J. C. T., and Lord, S. R. (2013). Increased concern is protective for falls in Chinese older people: the Chopstix fall risk study. J. Gerontol. Ser. A. 68, 946–953. doi: 10.1093/gerona/gls338

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Lauretani, F., Maggio, M., Ticinesi, A., Tana, C., Prati, B., Gionti, L., et al. (2018). Muscle weakness, cognitive impairment and their interaction on altered balance in elderly outpatients: results from the TRIP observational study. Clin. Interv. Aging 13, 1437–1443. doi: 10.2147/CIA.S165085

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Lee, B. C., Kim, C. S., and Seo, K. H. (2019). The Body’s compensatory responses to unpredictable trip and slip perturbations induced by a programmable Split-Belt treadmill. IEEE Trans. Neural Syst. Rehabil. Eng. 27, 1389–1396. doi: 10.1109/TNSRE.2019.2921710

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Lee, J., Geller, A. I., and Strasser, D. C. (2013). Analytical review: focus on fall screening assessments. PM&R 5, 609–621. doi: 10.1016/j.pmrj.2013.04.001

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Levy, I., Snell, J., Nelson, A. J., Rustichini, A., and Glimcher, P. W. (2010). Neural representation of subjective value under risk and ambiguity. J. Neurophysiol. 103, 1036–1047. doi: 10.1152/jn.00853.2009

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Lighthall, N. R. (2020). Neural mechanisms of decision-making in aging. WIREs Cogn. Sci. 11:e1519. doi: 10.1002/wcs.1519

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Li, K. Z. H., and Lindenberger, U. (2002). Relations between aging sensory/sensorimotor and cognitive functions. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 26, 777–783. doi: 10.1016/S0149-7634(02)00073-8

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Liphart, J., Gallichio, J., Tilson, J. K., Pei, Q., Wu, S. S., and Duncan, P. W. (2016). Concordance and discordance between measured and perceived balance and the effect on gait speed and falls following stroke. Clin. Rehabil. 30, 294–302. doi: 10.1177/0269215515578294

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Liss, D. J., Carey, H. D., Yakovenko, S., and Allen, J. L. (2022). Young adults perceive small disturbances to their walking balance even when distracted. Gait Posture 91, 198–204. doi: 10.1016/j.gaitpost.2021.10.019

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Liu, C., and Finley, J. M. (2020). Asymmetric gait patterns alter the reactive control of intersegmental coordination patterns in the sagittal plane during walking. PLoS One 15:e0224187. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0224187

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Liu, C., Macedo, L. D., and Finley, J. M. (2018). Conservation of reactive stabilization strategies in the presence of step length asymmetries during walking. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 12:12. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2018.00251

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Lopes, L. L. (1995). “Algebra and process in the modeling of risky choice” in Psychology of learning and motivation. eds. J. Busemeyer, R. Hastie, and D. L. Medin (Cambridge, MA: Academic Press), 177–220.

Google Scholar

Ludvig, E. A., and Spetch, M. L. (2011). Of black swans and tossed coins: is the description-experience gap in risky choice limited to rare events? PLoS One 6:e20262. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0020262

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Markowitz, H. (1952). Portfolio selection. J. Finan. 7, 77–91. doi: 10.2307/2975974

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

McAndrew, P. M., Wilken, J. M., and Dingwell, J. B. (2011). Dynamic stability of human walking in visually and mechanically destabilizing environments. J. Biomech. 44, 644–649. doi: 10.1016/j.jbiomech.2010.11.007

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

McAndrew Young, P. M., Wilken, J. M., and Dingwell, J. B. (2012). Dynamic margins of stability during human walking in destabilizing environments. J. Biomech. 45, 1053–1059. doi: 10.1016/j.jbiomech.2011.12.027

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Milton, J. G. (2011). The delayed and noisy nervous system: implications for neural control. J. Neural Eng. 8:065005. doi: 10.1088/1741-2560/8/6/065005

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Moreira, M. N., Bilton, T. L., Dias, R. C., Ferriolli, E., and Perracini, M. R. (2017). What are the Main physical functioning factors associated with falls among older people with different perceived fall risk? Physiother. Res. Int. 22:e1664. doi: 10.1002/pri.1664

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Moreland, J. D., Richardson, J. A., Goldsmith, C. H., and Clase, C. M. (2004). Muscle weakness and falls in older adults: a systematic review and meta-analysis. J. Am. Geriatr. Soc. 52, 1121–1129. doi: 10.1111/j.1532-5415.2004.52310.x

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Nagengast, A. J., Braun, D. A., and Wolpert, D. M. (2010). Risk-sensitive optimal feedback control accounts for sensorimotor behavior under uncertainty. PLoS Comput. Biol. 6:e1000857. doi: 10.1371/journal.pcbi.1000857

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Nagengast, A. J., Braun, D. A., and Wolpert, D. M. (2011). Risk-sensitivity and the mean-variance trade-off: decision making in sensorimotor control. Proc. R. Soc. B Biol. Sci. 278, 2325–2332. doi: 10.1098/rspb.2010.2518

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Nott, C. R., Neptune, R. R., and Kautz, S. A. (2014). Relationships between frontal-plane angular momentum and clinical balance measures during post-stroke hemiparetic walking. Gait Posture 39, 129–134. doi: 10.1016/j.gaitpost.2013.06.008

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Nouredanesh, M., Godfrey, A., Howcroft, J., Lemaire, E. D., and Tung, J. (2021). Fall risk assessment in the wild: a critical examination of wearable sensor use in free-living conditions. Gait Posture 85, 178–190. doi: 10.1016/j.gaitpost.2020.04.010

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

O’Brien, M. K., and Ahmed, A. A. (2013). Does risk-sensitivity transfer across movements? J. Neurophysiol. 109, 1866–1875. doi: 10.1152/jn.00826.2012

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

O’Brien, M. K., and Ahmed, A. A. (2014). Take a stand on your decisions, or take a sit: posture does not affect risk preferences in an economic task. PeerJ 2:e475. doi: 10.7717/peerj.475

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

O’Brien, M. K., and Ahmed, A. A. (2015). Threat affects risk preferences in movement decision making. Front. Behav. Neurosci. 9:9. doi: 10.3389/fnbeh.2015.00150

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

O’Brien, M. K., and Ahmed, A. A. (2016). Rationality in human movement. Exerc. Sport Sci. Rev. 44, 20–28. doi: 10.1249/JES.0000000000000066

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Okubo, Y., Schoene, D., and Lord, S. R. (2017). Step training improves reaction time, gait and balance and reduces falls in older people: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Br. J. Sports Med. 51, 586–593. doi: 10.1136/bjsports-2015-095452

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Pachur, T., Suter, R. S., and Hertwig, R. (2017). How the twain can meet: Prospect theory and models of heuristics in risky choice. Cogn. Psychol. 93, 44–73. doi: 10.1016/j.cogpsych.2017.01.001

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Park, S., and Finley, J. M. (2017). “Characterizing dynamic balance during adaptive locomotor learning,” in 2017 39th Annual International Conference of the IEEE Engineering in Medicine and Biology Society (EMBC). Jeju, Korea (South): EEE, 50–53.

Google Scholar

Payne, J. W., Braunstein, M. L., and Carroll, J. S. (1978). Exploring predecisional behavior: An alternative approach to decision research. Organ. Behav. Hum. Perform. 22, 17–44. doi: 10.1016/0030-5073(78)90003-X

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Pelosin, E., Ogliastro, C., Lagravinese, G., Bonassi, G., Mirelman, A., Hausdorff, J. M., et al. (2016). Attentional control of gait and falls: is cholinergic dysfunction a common substrate in the elderly and Parkinson’s disease? Front. Aging Neurosci. 8:8. doi: 10.3389/fnagi.2016.00104

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Perell, K. L., Nelson, A., Goldman, R. L., Luther, S. L., Prieto-Lewis, N., and Rubenstein, L. Z. (2001). Fall risk assessment measures: an analytic review. J. Gerontol. Ser. A. 56, M761–M766. doi: 10.1093/gerona/56.12.M761

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Pijnappels, M., Bobbert, M. F., and van Dieën, J. H. (2005). How early reactions in the support limb contribute to balance recovery after tripping. J. Biomech. 38, 627–634. doi: 10.1016/j.jbiomech.2004.03.029

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Pozaic, T., Grebe, A. K., Grollmuss, M., Haeberlen, N., and Stork, W. (2019). “Inter-limb coordination assessment and fall risk in ADL” in International conference on biomedical and health informatics. eds. Y. T. Zhang, P. Carvalho, and R. Magjarevic (Singapore: Springer), 1–5.

Google Scholar

Raffegeau, T. E., Young, W. R., Fino, P. C., and Williams, A. M. (2023). A perspective on using virtual reality to incorporate the affective context of everyday falls into fall prevention. JMIR Aging. 6:e36325. doi: 10.2196/36325

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Raphael, K. (1987). Recall bias: a proposal for assessment and control. Int. J. Epidemiol. 16, 167–170. doi: 10.1093/ije/16.2.167

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Resnick, S. M., Pham, D. L., Kraut, M. A., Zonderman, A. B., and Davatzikos, C. (2003). Longitudinal magnetic resonance imaging studies of older adults: a shrinking brain. J. Neurosci. 23, 3295–3301. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.23-08-03295.2003

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Roerdink, M., Geerse, D. J., and Peper, C. (2021). ‘Haste makes waste’: the tradeoff between walking speed and target-stepping accuracy. Gait Posture 85, 110–116. doi: 10.1016/j.gaitpost.2021.01.004

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Rolison, J. J., Hanoch, Y., Wood, S., and Liu, P. J. (2014). Risk-taking differences across the adult life span: a question of age and domain. J. Gerontol. Ser. B. 69, 870–880. doi: 10.1093/geronb/gbt081

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Sakurai, R., Fujiwara, Y., Ishihara, M., Higuchi, T., Uchida, H., and Imanaka, K. (2013). Age-related self-overestimation of step-over ability in healthy older adults and its relationship to fall risk. BMC Geriatr. 13:44. doi: 10.1186/1471-2318-13-44

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Sandow, A. (1952). Excitation-contraction coupling in muscular response. Yale J. Biol. Med. 25, 176–201

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Slovic, P., Finucane, M. L., Peters, E., and MacGregor, D. G. (2004). Risk as analysis and risk as feelings: some thoughts about affect, reason, risk, and rationality. Risk Anal. 24, 311–322. doi: 10.1111/j.0272-4332.2004.00433.x

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Smeesters, C., Hayes, W. C., and McMahon, T. A. (2001). The threshold trip duration for which recovery is no longer possible is associated with strength and reaction time. J. Biomech. 34, 589–595. doi: 10.1016/S0021-9290(01)00005-7

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Tiedemann, A., Lord, S. R., and Sherrington, C. (2010). The development and validation of a brief performance-based fall risk assessment tool for use in primary care. J. Gerontol. Ser. A. 65A, 896–903. doi: 10.1093/gerona/glq067

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Tobler, P. N., and Weber, E. U. (2014). “Valuation for risky and uncertain choices” in Neuroeconomics. eds. P. W. Glimcher and E. Fehr (Amsterdam: Elsevier), 149–172.

Google Scholar

Trevisan, C., Zanforlini, B. M., Maggi, S., Noale, M., Limongi, F., de Rui, M., et al. (2019). Judgment capacity, fear of falling, and the risk of falls in community-dwelling older adults: the Progetto Veneto Anziani longitudinal study. Rejuvenation Res. 23, 237–244. doi: 10.1089/rej.2019.2197

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Trommershäuser, J., Maloney, L. T., and Landy, M. S. (2003a). Statistical decision theory and trade-offs in the control of motor response. Spat. Vis. 16, 255–275. doi: 10.1163/156856803322467527

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Trommershäuser, J., Maloney, L. T., and Landy, M. S. (2003b). Statistical decision theory and the selection of rapid, goal-directed movements. J. Opt. Soc. Am. A 20, 1419–1433. doi: 10.1364/JOSAA.20.001419

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Tversky, A., and Kahneman, D. (1992). Advances in prospect theory: cumulative representation of uncertainty. J. Risk Uncertain. 5, 297–323. doi: 10.1007/BF00122574

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Tymula, A., Belmaker, L. A. R., Ruderman, L., Glimcher, P. W., and Levy, I. (2013). Like cognitive function, decision making across the life span shows profound age-related changes. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. 110, 17143–17148. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1309909110

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Valsecchi, M., Billino, J., and Gegenfurtner, K. R. (2018). Healthy aging is associated with decreased risk-taking in motor decision-making. J. Exp. Psychol. Hum. Percept. Perform. 44, 154–167. doi: 10.1037/xhp0000436

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Waltrip, L. H., Joyce, J. L., Chapman, S., Cosentino, S., and Sunderaraman, P. (2023). Risk-taking behavior in older adults is related to cognitive outcomes in a domain-specific manner. Alzheimers Dement. 19:e078930. doi: 10.1002/alz.078930

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Weber, E. U., Blais, A. R., and Betz, N. E. (2002). A domain-specific risk-attitude scale: measuring risk perceptions and risk behaviors. J. Behav. Decis. Mak. 15, 263–290. doi: 10.1002/bdm.414

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

WHO (2007). WHO global report on falls prevention in older age: Prevention in older age : World Health Organization Accessed February 11, 2021. http://ebookcentral.proquest.com/lib/socal/detail.action?docID=349983.

Google Scholar

Wu, S. W., Delgado, M. R., and Maloney, L. T. (2009). Economic decision-making compared with an equivalent motor task. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 106, 6088–6093. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0900102106

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: decision-making, aging, gait, falls, locomotion, risk-taking

Citation: Jain S, Schweighofer N and Finley JM (2024) Aberrant decision-making as a risk factor for falls in aging. Front. Aging Neurosci. 16:1384242. doi: 10.3389/fnagi.2024.1384242

Received: 08 February 2024; Accepted: 20 May 2024;
Published: 24 June 2024.

Edited by:

Alessandro Martorana, University of Rome Tor Vergata, Italy

Reviewed by:

Shmuel Springer, Ariel University, Israel
Francesco Di Lorenzo, Santa Lucia Foundation (IRCCS), Italy

Copyright © 2024 Jain, Schweighofer and Finley. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: James M. Finley, jmfinley@usc.edu

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.