- 1Department of Psychiatry, McKnight Brain Institute, University of Florida College of Medicine, Gainesville, FL, United States
- 2Department of Chemistry, Hemvati Nandan Bahuguna Garhwal University (A Central University), Srinagar Garhwal, Uttarakhand, India
- 3Department of Biochemistry and Molecular Biology, University of Florida College of Medicine, Gainesville, FL, United States
Introduction: The differential expression of emotional reactivity from early to late adulthood may involve maturation of prefrontal cortical responses to negative valence stimuli. In mice, age-related changes in affective behaviors have been reported, but the functional neural circuitry warrants further investigation.
Methods: We assessed age variations in affective behaviors and functional connectivity in male and female C57BL6/J mice. Mice aged 10, 30 and 60 weeks (wo) were tested over 8 weeks for open field activity, sucrose preference, social interactions, fear conditioning, and functional neuroimaging. Prefrontal cortical and hippocampal tissues were excised for metabolomics.
Results: Our results indicate that young and old mice differ significantly in affective behavioral, functional connectome and prefrontal cortical-hippocampal metabolome. Young mice show a greater responsivity to novel environmental and social stimuli compared to older mice. Conversely, late middle-aged mice (60wo group) display variable patterns of fear conditioning and during re-testing in a modified context. Functional connectivity between a temporal cortical/auditory cortex network and subregions of the anterior cingulate cortex and ventral hippocampus, and a greater network modularity and assortative mixing of nodes was stronger in young versus older adult mice. Metabolome analyses identified differences in several essential amino acids between 10wo mice and the other age groups.
Discussion: The results support differential expression of ‘emotionality’ across distinct stages of the mouse lifespan involving greater prefrontal-hippocampal connectivity and neurochemistry.
Introduction
The United States is seeing a rise in affective conditions among young adults (Kessler et al., 2012; Terlizzi and Villarroel, 2020). The most recent national mental health statistics show that young adults (ages 18–25 years) have a greater prevalence of anxiety and mood conditions but seek less treatment than older and middle-aged adults (ages 50 and older; Samhsa, 2022). Prior studies have revealed age-related changes in ‘emotionality’ and emotion processing that may contribute to a differential vulnerability of young individuals to experience an affective condition. Compared to older individuals, young adults cite greater negative emotional experiences, show less emotional control (Gross et al., 1997) and stability (Carstensen et al., 2011), have a greater tendency to report negative mood states (Machado et al., 2019), more worries (Brenes, 2006), and a greater recall of negative memories (Charles et al., 2003). Neuroimaging studies have determined the major components of neural circuitry involved in the differential processing of negative valence stimuli between young and older adults. The dorsal and rostral subregions of anterior cingulate cortex (ACg) show weaker responses to negative stimuli in old vs. young adults (Chaudhary et al., 2023). Functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) studies employing face perception tasks indicate that the amygdala has greater blood oxygenation level dependent (BOLD) response to negative emotion stimuli in young compared older adults (Iidaka et al., 2002; Fischer et al., 2005; Tessitore et al., 2005). Other studies, however, observed either no age associated differences in the amygdala response to negative stimuli (Wright et al., 2006) or report an increased amygdala functional connectivity from adolescence to adulthood (Ravindranath et al., 2020). Recent evidence suggests that the neurobiological adaptations associated with adulthood variations in affective behaviors likely involves a larger number of brain areas outside the amygdala and changes in neural interactions at a network level (Malezieux et al., 2023; Wang et al., 2023).
Age-related changes in affective behaviors in early vs. late adulthood have been reported in mice (Flurkey et al., 2007;Shoji and Miyakawa, 2019; Baumgartner et al., 2023). Young (2–3 month old; mo) C57BL/6 J male mice display greater anxiety-like behaviors compared to older adult, late middle age and aged mice (Ammassari-Teule et al., 1994; Shoji and Miyakawa, 2019). Although this remains unclear, since other studies provide evidence of greater anxiety-like behaviors in aged compared to young adult mice (Shoji and Miyakawa, 2019; Baumgartner et al., 2023). Nevertheless, the expression of fear-related associative memory (von Bohlen und Halbach et al., 2006; Evans et al., 2021; Ferreira et al., 2023), reward sensitivity (Ammassari-Teule et al., 1994; Malatynska et al., 2012), sociability and social recognition (Shoji and Miyakawa, 2019), and learned helplessness on a Porsolt test (Ferreira et al., 2023) are all observed to vary in young vs. older adulthood mice. In parallel with these affective behavior findings, there is recent evidence of brain structural differences between early and late adulthood (Clifford et al., 2023). In an analysis of structural covariance network data, it was shown that young mice have a higher modularity with greater node degree in anterior cingulate cortex and lower in hippocampal formation than older adult mice (Clifford et al., 2023). This age-related difference in structural network modularity was linked to cognitive differences (Clifford et al., 2023). It remains unclear whether functional network topology differs in a similar manner to the reported structural changes in young vs. older adult mice. The relationship between functional network activity, as measured by functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI), and affective behaviors in early and late adulthood warrants investigation. In the present study, we measured affective behaviors involving open field activity, reward sensitivity, sociability, and fear memory in young adult, older adult, and late middle aged C57BL/6 J male and female mice. We also evaluated the relationship between behavioral measures and fMRI markers of network connectivity. Finally, we conducted hippocampal and prefrontal cortical metabolomic analysis and observed several novel protein-related substrates differing between young, older adult and late middle-aged mice.
Materials and methods
Subjects
Male and female C57BL/6 J (B6) mice aged 10, 30, or 60 weeks old (wo) at the time of arrival were allowed a week of acclimation to the vivarium prior to the start of experiments (N = 30, total; n = 10 per age group, 50% females; The Jackson Laboratory, Bar Harbor, ME). Female mice initially weighed (in grams; mean ± standard deviation) 19 ± 0.93, 32 ± 4.6, 29 ± 2.8 and males 26.7 ± 2, 30 ± 2.5, 46.8 ± 5.6 (for 10, 30, 60wo, respectively). The selected age groups represent 3 stages of adulthood. B6 mice at 3–6MO (12–24wo) are considered mature adults and at 10–14MO (40–56wo) are considered middle age (Flurkey et al., 2007). The youngest 10wo group (2.5MO) was tested just before mature adulthood and the 60wo group was just at the end of middle age (15MO). At 30wo (7.5MO), mice start to show early signs of senescence (such as retirement from breeding at 8MO) and were included here as reference to compare early adulthood and late middle age changes (Flurkey et al., 2007). Mice were housed in age- and sex-matched groups of 3–4 in a temperature- and humidity-controlled room, inside conventional air filtered cages (dimensions: 29 × 18 × 13 cm) with food and water available ad libitum (vivarium lights on from 07:00 to 19:00 h). All procedures received prior approval from the Institutional Animal Care and Use Committee of the University of Florida and follow all applicable NIH guidelines.
Experimental design
Figure 1 shows the experimental timeline. All behavioral tests and functional neuroimaging were conducted over an 8-week period. Behavioral tests included locomotor activity in a novel environment, sociability and social recognition tests, sucrose preference tests, and cued fear conditioning (Figure 1). Functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) was conducted a week after sucrose preference testing and 2 weeks before the final behavioral test (fear conditioning). At the end of the experiments, mice were deeply anesthetized with 5% isoflurane, and brain tissue harvested for the metabolomic assay of excised prefrontal cortex and hippocampus tissue. Imaging was thus carried out when mice were 14, 34 and 63wo and these were euthanized 8 weeks after arrival, at 18, 38 and 68wo. However, to simplify presentation of the results, we keep group names in figures as 10, 30 and 60wo.
Locomotor activity in a novel environment
An automated rodent activity monitoring cage system (Accuscan Instruments, Columbus, OH) was used to measure spontaneous locomotor activity in a novel environment (Bruijnzeel et al., 2016). The day before locomotor measurements, mice were acclimatized for 1 h to the testing room. The activity monitors are made of clear Plexiglas (40 cm3) with an outer steel frame containing infrared emitting and receiving panels. Each panel has 16 equally spaced (2.5 cm) infrared beams crossing the length and width of the cages at a height of 2 cm (horizontal beams). Several patterns of sequential and repetitive infrared beam interruptions were registered and sent to a programmable counter, which sorted beam counts and sent sorted data to a PC running Versamax™ software. The sorted activity counts were classified as sequential beam breaks (forward locomotion), repetitive beam breaks (putative repetitive behaviors), total, center and margin distances traveled (in cm). Activity was measured during a single 30-min test.
Sucrose preference test
The sucrose preference test was conducted as described previously, with modifications (Bruijnzeel et al., 2016; Chellian et al., 2020). A decrease in preference to consume a 2% sucrose solution over water suggests anhedonia (D’Aquila et al., 1997). ‘Leakproof’ bottles (100 mL, graduated by 1 mL) with stoppers and sipper tubes were purchased (Braintree Scientific Inc., Braintree, MA; model no. WTRBTL). Filtered home cage lids with two bottle openings (Allentown, Inc., Allentown, NJ), were used for two bottle fluid delivery in mouse home cages. Mice were temporarily single housed for testing and once testing concluded they were returned to their original cages. First, mice were acclimatized to bottles filled with 2% sucrose solution for 3 days (96 h). On each of the 3 days, bottles were weighed (in grams) to monitor fluid intake and replaced with a full bottle. The 96 h acclimation period was followed by a single 24 h two-bottle choice test in which one bottle was filled with 2% sucrose solution and the other with water. The side of the cage lid where the sucrose and water filled bottles were placed was counterbalanced across animals. Water and sucrose consumed was measured after a 24 h period (McCall et al., 2013). Bottles were weighted and values converted from grams to ml. An index (SI) of how much sucrose (s) consumed vs. water (w) was calculated as: .
Sociability and social recognition test
We used a modified version of a previously reported sociability and social recognition test protocol (Tan et al., 2019). Test arenas were made of clear Plexiglas, divided by walls into 3 equal sized quadrants (arena total length x width x height dimensions: 60 × 45 × 20 cm). The middle quadrant is the neutral zone, and the side quadrants (left and right) were used for the placement of ‘bait’ mice for sociability and recognition tests. Each side quadrant can be entered through a central door in the dividing walls. A pair of clear plastic ‘bait’ cups covered with equally sized and equidistant perforations for air flow were inverted over the ‘bait’ mouse for sociability testing. Test mice were acclimatized to the room and the neutral quadrant of the test arena on 3 days, 15 min a day. During sociability testing, a novel (unfamiliar) mouse (an adult mouse of the same strain and sex; age range 3–6mo) was placed under a cup and the entrance to the ‘bait’ side of the arena was opened for exploration by the test mouse while the other side is kept sealed. In preliminary tests, this encouraged mice to explore the unfamiliar ‘bait’ mouse more vigorously during a first encounter. Mice were tested for 10 min for time spent exploring the unfamiliar mouse. Upon completion of the first test, and after a 5-min break to clean the arena, mice were placed back into the arena in the presence of a novel mouse and the previously encountered (now familiar) mouse and re-tested for 10 more minutes with both sides opened for exploration. In the first test, the degree of exploration of an unfamiliar mouse (“novel 1”) is considered here as a measure of the degree of sociability. In the second test, less exploration of the familiar mouse (“familiar 1”) relative to the new unfamiliar mouse (“novel 2”) is a sign of social recognition (Crawley et al., 2007). Behaviors were recorded using a digital camera directly connected via FireWire to a Linux computer. We used the ‘dvgrab’ tool from the Unix command terminal to program video capture settings and store the recorded videos.
Behavioral analyses were carried out using DeepLabCut (DLC, version 2.2.1; Mathis et al., 2018; Nath et al., 2019). We labeled 120 frames of mouse trajectories within the test cages taken from 6 different videos and 95% of those labels were used to train a machine learning algorithm. A ResNet-50-based neural network with default parameters for 2 training iterations was used for training. Data (digital videos) were split into training and test datasets. The training dataset was used to train a DLC model. Conversely, the test dataset was used to evaluate the performance of the model. We validated the DLC model with 7 train-test-split shuffles. The model with the smallest mean average Euclidean error (MAE) was selected. Before model selection, we applied a p-value cutoff of 0.6 to exclude model predictions with a likelihood under 0.6. The final selected model had a MAE of 2.9 pixels. This neural network was used to analyze videos from similar experimental settings to generate trajectory data. Using the R packages ‘trajr’ (McLean and Skowron Volponi, 2018) and ‘DLCanalyzer’ (Sturman et al., 2020), spurious trajectories were removed before analyses. The videos were analyzed in two parts, the first sociability test (T1) and the second social recognition test (T2). The immediate area surrounding and including the mouse ‘bait’ cup on one side of the arena was designated ‘ROI A’ and the other ‘ROI B’ (region of interest A and B, respectively). A python script was compiled to generate the number of entrances, the velocity (in pixels per frame or ppf), and the duration of initial approach and time spent within the ROIs.
Classical fear conditioning
Fear conditioning was conducted 2 weeks following the fMRI studies. The fear conditioning cages were housed in sound attenuation chambers. The chambers had internal computer-controlled lighting, white noise generator, a fan, and control units for distinct aspects of the fear conditioning assay. These included audio speaker controls for tone generation, shock controllers for the internal cage floors, and infrared and white lighting controllers. The internal operant cage was made of translucent Plexiglas with a steel frame and steel shock grid floor connected to a computer controlled current stimulator (Med-Associates, Inc. St. Albans, VT). Activity within the fear conditioning cages was captured by a monochrome video camera and video recordings were optimized via externally controlled near infrared (NIR), white light and sound controllers. All cage accessories were controlled by PC running Video Freeze Software™. Camera lens brightness, gain, and shutter settings were verified and adjusted before collecting data and were kept constant in all mice. For each session, the detected motion-sensitive signals were calibrated with the mouse inside the cage and NIR lights on. The estimated motion index was set to the same threshold level (10 a.u.) in all mice prior to data exporting or analyses.
Videos were acquired at 30 frames per second (fps) and the minimum freeze duration was 30 frames. For training on the first day, a 180 s baseline was followed by four presentations of a 20 s tone (90 dB, 5 kHz, 50 ms risetime) with the presentation of moderate level current to the entire grid floor for 1 s (0.9 mA; inescapable shock) on the final second before the tone ended. This 90 dB tone is above the auditory brainstem response measured in C57BL/6 J mice, which declines with age and reaches 60–80 dB at 5–8 kHz (Henry, 2004; Yu et al., 2011; Youn et al., 2020). Each of the four tone-shock presentations were separated by 190 s and the entire session lasted 17 min. On days 2 and 3 (recall tests at 24 and 48 h of training day 1, respectively), the same protocol was carried out but in the absence of a shock. On day 3 (modified context test), the cage environment was modified by placing a plastic smooth white floor and diagonal walls, which covered the grid floor, light, and speaker.
For each of the time epochs corresponding to the 4 tone-shock pairings, a 20 s pre-shock tone interval and a 40 s post-shock interval were used to quantify number of freezing bouts (freezing counts) in a total 60 s epoch (per day there were four 60 s epochs summed to provide total freezing counts per day). Motion-index was also used as a secondary measure of general locomotor activity inside the cage during the same 60 s epochs/day.
Magnetic resonance imaging instrumentation
Images were collected on a magnetic resonance spectrometer tuned to 200.6 MHz proton resonant frequency (4.7 Tesla/33 cm actively shielded magnet). The MR system was equipped with Resonance Research Inc. spatial encoding gradients (RRI BFG-200/115-S14, maximum gradient strength of 670 mT/m at 300 Amps and a 120 μs risetime; RRI, Billerica, MA) and controlled by a VnmrJ 3.1 console (Agilent, Palo Alto, CA). A custom-built 8.8 cm x 11.6 cm radiofrequency (RF) transmit coil used in combination with a 3 cm x 3.5 cm actively decoupled/tunable RF receive quadrature surface coil (RF engineering lab, Advanced Magnetic Resonance Imaging and Spectroscopy Facility, Gainesville, FL).
Mouse imaging setup
Mice were scanned sedated under a continuous paranasal flow of 0.25% isoflurane gas (delivered at 0.04 L/min mixed with medical grade air containing 70% nitrogen and 30% oxygen, Airgas, Inc.) and a continuous subcutaneous infusion of dexmedetomidine. Prior to setup, mice were anesthetized under 2% isoflurane and administered an intraperitoneal (i.p.) injection of 0.1 mg/kg dexmedetomidine (at a 1 mL/kg volume). Isoflurane was reduced to 0.25% throughout the remaining imaging session. An infusion line was setup for subcutaneous delivery of dexmedetomidine over the course of scanning (0.1 mg/kg/ml at an infusion rate of 25 μL/h using a PHD-Ultra microinfusion pump, Harvard Apparatus, Holliston, MA). Functional MRI scans were collected at least 50 min after the i.p. injection. Respiratory rates were monitored continuously, and body temperature was maintained at 36–37°C using a warm water recirculation system (SA Instruments, Inc., New York).
Functional MRI acquisition protocol
We acquired one T2 weighted anatomical and an fMRI scan per mouse. The T2-weighted fast spin echo (FSE) sequence was acquired with the following parameters: effective echo time = 48 ms, repetition time (TR) = 2 s, echo train length = 8, number of averages = 12, field of view (FOV) of 19.2 mm x 19.2 mm and 0.8 mm thick slice, and a data matrix of 192 × 192 (0.1 μm2 in plane) and 12 interleaved ascending coronal (axial) slices covering the entire brain from the rostral-most extent of the anterior prefrontal cortical surface, caudally toward the upper brainstem and cerebellum (acquisition time 9 min 36 s). Functional images were collected using a single-shot gradient echo planar imaging sequence with the following parameters: TE = 22 ms, flip angle = 90°, TR = 1.2 s, 600 repetitions, FOV = 19.2 × 19.2 mm and 0.8 mm thick slice, and a data matrix of 48 × 48 (0.4 μm2 in plane) with 12 interleaved ascending coronal slices in the same position as the anatomical scan. Ten dummy EPI scans were run prior to acquiring data under steady state conditions. Respiratory rates, isoflurane and dexmedetomidine delivery, body temperature, lighting, and room conditions were kept constant across subjects.
Image pre-processing
The image processing workflow is illustrated in Figure 2 (Colon-Perez et al., 2019; Kotlarz et al., 2022; Sadaka et al., 2023; Sakthivel et al., 2023). Resting state processing was carried out using Analysis of Functional NeuroImages (AFNI; Cox, 1996), FMRIB Software Library (FSL; Jenkinson et al., 2002), and Advanced Normalization Tools (ANTs; Klein et al., 2009). Binary masks for anatomical and functional scans were created using ITKSNAP (Yushkevich et al., 2006). The brain binary masks were used for brain extraction prior to registration steps. Times series spikes were removed (3dDespike, AFNI), image repetition frames aligned to the first time series volume (3dvolreg, AFNI), and detrended (high pass filter <0.009 Hz using AFNI 3dTproject). Independent component analysis (ICA) using Multivariate Exploratory Optimized Decomposition into Independent Components (FSL MELODIC version 3.0) was used to assess structured ‘noise’ or artifact components in each subject scan, in their native space. Most, if not all ICA components in this first stage contained artifact signal voxels along brain edges, in ventricles, and large vessel regions. These components were suppressed using a soft (‘non-aggressive’) regression approach, as implemented in FMRIB Software Library (FSL 6.0.3) using fsl_regfilt (Jenkinson et al., 2002). A low-pass filter (>0.12 Hz) and spatial smoothing (0.4 mm FWHM) was next applied to the fMRI scans prior to registration steps. Post-regression ICA was carried out to verify removal of artifact components and preliminary assessment of putative ICA networks in individual scans.
Figure 2. Image processing workflow. Anatomical scans from all subjects were used to generate a group-wise template for anatomical and functional image registrations and individual and group ICA-based signal processing steps. The decomposition of whole brain signals into 60 components was used to generate region of interest masks for signal extraction and pairwise correlations. The study specific node-set were normalized to the common coordinate framework version 3 mouse atlas for 3D visualizations of nodes and edges. See text for details.
Image template construction, subject normalization, and resting state signal extraction
Anatomical scans were cropped, and bias field corrected (N4BiasFieldCorrection, ANTs). Functional scans were cropped, and a temporal mean image was registered to the anatomical. The functional-to-anatomical registration matrix was applied to all fMRI volumes. A multi-T2 anatomical template was generated using ANTs ‘antsMultivariateTemplateConstruction2.sh’ script (cross correlation used as a similarity metric and a greedy SyN as transformation model). Individual subject-to-template transformation matrices were applied to fMRI datasets. A group ICA was applied to template-registered concatenated fMRI scans to generate a set of 60 nodes for ROI-based intrinsic fMRI signal extraction, pairwise cross-correlation statistics, and adjacency matrix construction. The ICA was set to an upper limit 60 component signal decomposition and a mixture model threshold of 0.7. The resulting 60 ICA components included peak signal (peak T statistic) nodes in midline prefrontal cortex (anterior cingulate), somatosensory regions, temporal, visual, auditory cortices, entorhinal cortex, several hippocampal nodes, subiculum, amygdala, thalamic nuclei, and striatum. Each of the 60 nodes were binarized and assessed to ensure non-overlapping coverage of brain areas on the template and registered scans (Figure 2). Both the template and ICA nodes were normalized to the Allen Brain Institutes common coordinate framework version 3 (CCFv3) atlas of the B6 mouse brain for final 3D visualizations (Lein et al., 2007). Signal timeseries were extracted from preprocessed fMRI scans with the assistance of 60 ROI mask overlays. This generated 60 individual ROI text files per subject that contained L2-normalized resting state signals as a vector of 600 data points.
The timeseries files were used in cross-correlations and in calculations of Pearson r coefficients for every pairwise combinations of ROIs (using corrcoef function in MATLAB). The resulting number of pairwise correlations was 1,710 per subject (after removing 60 self-correlations). Correlation coefficients were Fisher’s transform to ensure normality prior to statistical analyses. The center voxel coordinates for the ICA-based nodes normalized to the CCFv3 were used in 3D network visualizations using BrainNet viewer (Xia et al., 2013).
Network analysis
Weighted undirected matrices were analyzed using Brain Connectivity Toolbox (Rubinov and Sporns, 2010), igraph (version 1.5.1.9005; Csardi and Nepusz, 2006) in R, and tools in MATLAB (Mathworks, Natick, MA). Global graph metrics were calculated for edge density thresholds ranging from 6 to 32%. Global network measures for this density range were converted to area under the curve (AUC) values prior to multivariate statistical assessments. Node-specific (ROI) network measures were calculated at a 16% threshold, which in the present dataset corresponds to a Pearson r of approximately 0.2 or more, an average degree of 9.1, and network strength of 2.5–5.0 (Supplementary Figure 1). As a comparison, graph density based on bootstrapped calculations of confidence intervals for significant Pearson correlations corresponds to a significant r of ≥0.06, 40% density (δ ≥ 0.4), degree(k) ≥ 20, and strength ≥4 (Supplementary Figure 1).
A major objective of this study was to assess maturational and aging changes in functional network topology and determine links between network measures and spontaneous ‘affective’ behaviors. To this end, we assessed several graph measures of network integration and communication efficiency. These included node clustering coefficient (CC; number of interconnected node pairs connected to a node; Onnela et al., 2005), node strength (sum of edge weights/node) and degree (sum of edges/node) and global measures, such as transitivity (related to CC; number of triad groups normalized by all possible triad nodes in a network), mean distance or characteristic path length (CPL; the average edges or edge weights between node pairs), the small world coefficient (SWC; Humphries et al., 2006; Humphries and Gurney, 2008) and small world measure (SWM; Telesford et al., 2011). Global efficiency is the inverse of CPL. For local node efficiency, a length matrix was used to calculate inverse values in vicinity of a node, with added weights used to emphasize the highest efficiency node paths (Rubinov and Sporns, 2010; Wang et al., 2017). To corroborate results relative to random networks, all network measures were calculated on original and randomized versions of the functional connectivity matrices. Positive and negative edges were randomized by ~5 binary swaps with edge weights re-sorted at each step. The approach preserves weights, degree, and strength distributions (Rubinov and Sporns, 2010). We confirmed that the randomization approach preserved weights per nodes, node degree and strength distributions (Supplementary Figure 2). Node strength and degree distributions were similar between random and original networks, with distributions having long tail at low density threshold and near Gaussian at high density (Supplementary Figures 2A–F). Degree and strength were approximately preserved across nodes, although the specific organization of edge weights differed when viewed on 3D brains (Supplementary Figures 2G–K). The positive and negative matrix strength sequence accuracies for input and output matrices were r = 0.99 and r = 0.98. SWC was calculated as the ratio of gamma to lambda coefficients, where gamma is the ratio of real to random network CC, and lambda the ratio of real to random network CPL (Humphries and Gurney, 2008). SWM was calculated with an artificial non-ring lattice network as reference, as previously reported (Telesford et al., 2011).
A probabilistic approach for community detection was used to calculate a modularity statistic (Q), which indexes the rate of intra-group connections vs. connections due to chance (Blondel et al., 2008). The procedure starts with a random grouping of nodes and iteratively moving nodes into groups which maximize the value of Q. The final number of modules and node assignments to each group (e.g., community affiliation assignments) was taken as the median of 100 iterations of the modularity maximization procedure (Pompilus et al., 2020). The number of communities per mouse brain functional network and population size of each community were also assessed. We evaluated other community detection routines in igraph as a comparison to results obtained with the implementation of Louvain’s algorithm in Brain Connectivity Toolbox (results in Table 1). The role each node plays within their assigned module can vary according to their participation coefficient. The participation coefficient is calculated as: , where dim is the within-module node degree and di is the overall network degree for a node.
We analyzed the tendency of assortative vs. dissortative mixing of nodes (Newman, 2002). The assortativity index is a correlation coefficient comparing node strength values between pairs of edge-connected nodes. Positive r values indicate connectivity between pairs of nodes with similar strengths (e.g., high strength nodes pairs with high and low with low), while negative r values indicate cross-pairings between low and high strength nodes. We generated and analyzed weighted rich club coefficient curves (Φ) at an edge density of 16% (Colizza et al., 2006). Nodes belonging to a rich club subnetwork have an above-chance tendency to tightly connect with each other and share the bulk of connections within the network (Colizza et al., 2006; van den Heuvel and Sporns, 2011). The approach creates subgraphs containing nodes with strength values at a predetermined degree value, k. For each k-level subgraph, Φw is calculated as the ratio of the sum of edge weights that connect nodes of degree k or higher (W>k) to the total possible number of summed edge-weights (van den Heuvel and Sporns, 2011). Rich club coefficient values were calculated for original and randomized networks and a normalized rich club index analyzed (i.e., ).
Functional connectivity networks were visualized in BrainNet viewer (Xia et al., 2013). Center coordinates for each node were based on CCFv3 parcellations, as indicated above. Several 3D mouse brain connectome maps were generated in which size of nodes (spheres) was scaled according to AUC values for several node-centric measures at various thresholds. Additional 3D maps were generated with node colors representing the module assignment (e.g., community affiliation vector) and node size weighted by modularity index.
Sample preparation for Gas Chromatography–Mass Spectrometry (GC–MS)
Fresh brain tissue was extracted, and two isolated regions were weighed, and flash frozen for metabolomics (Supplementary Figure 3). A 1 mm thick slice of the prefrontal cortex was manually dissected from approximately Bregma anterior–posterior −2.96 mm to −1.94 mm (Paxinos and Franklin, 2001), and a 2 mm wide midline cut including from the median −/+ 1 mm medial lateral structures −3 to −3.5 mm deep. The dissected regions thus included areas of the infralimbic, prelimbic, medial orbital, anterior cingulate and midline primary motor cortices. The dorsal hippocampus was dissected at −1.46 mm rostrally to about −2.46 mm caudally (Bregma coordinates) and stripped of its overlying cortex and white matter. These regions have been implicated previously in age related differences in structural connectivity (Clifford et al., 2023).
Mice brain tissue (20–25 mg) was extracted with 1 mL of Acetonitrile:Isopropanol:Water (3:3:2, v:v:v), centrifuged for 25 min at 10,000× g. The supernatant was transferred into a new tube and dried in a speedvac. The dried extract was resuspended in 0.5 mL of Acetonitrile:Water (1:1, v:v) and centrifuged for 15 min and the supernatant was transferred into reaction vial and dried down under nitrogen. Fifty microliters of 2% MOX reagent were added to the vial and incubated at 30°C for 90 min followed by addition of 50 μL of MTBSTFA +TBDMS and incubation at 60°C for 1 h. Finally, reaction solution was transferred into GC vial for GC–MS analysis (Mahar et al., 2021).
GC–MS analysis
GC–MS data was recorded with Single Quadrupole Mass Spectrometer and GC (Trace 1,310; Thermo Fisher Scientific, United States). The column was 30 m long dimethyl (95%)/diphenyl polysiloxane (5%) RTX-5MS with 0.25 mm ID, and guard column (10 m; Restek). The initial temperature of GC oven was 60°C for 60 s and the temperature increased to 325°C (@10°C/min). Metabolites were identified using NIST library with Xcalibur software.
Statistical analysis
Statistical analyses used MATLAB, GraphPad Prism (version 10.2) or the R statistics package. Unless otherwise noted, all analyses involved either a two-way full factorial analysis of variance (ANOVA) without or with repeated measures, depending on the specific test (critical α < 0.05). Post-hoc tests used Tukey’s honest significant difference (HSD) procedure. Where appropriate, false discovery rate (FDR, q ≤ 0.05) correction was used. The resultant statistical test parameters are indicated in figure legends or in the results section. Unless otherwise specified, male and female data were pooled to increase statistical power. The dependent variables in locomotor tests were sequential and repetitive beam breaks, total, margin, and center distances. Dependent variables for sucrose preference tests are fluid intake consumed in 24 h (ml) and sucrose preference index. For social interaction, the dependent variables are number of ROI entrances, average speed in ROI (in pixels per frame, or ppf), time spent in ROI, and time of initial approach toward ROI. For cued fear conditioning, the dependent variable is freezing counts summed over four 60 s trial bins per day.
Global network measures (i.e., SW, modularity, CPL, efficiency, transitivity, assortativity) were analyzed first as AUC values. Measures with significant differences were again analyzed across density thresholds to determine the threshold level where significant differences are observed. Upon determining the significance of modularity and assortativity, we analyzed the number of modules per group, number of nodes per module, and the normalized rich club index. All single threshold nodal analyses were carried out for a network density threshold of 16%. These included node degree, node strength, participation coefficient, clustering coefficient, and local efficiency. These were corrected for multiple comparisons across 60 nodes using the Benjamini-Hochberg FDR procedure (q ≤ 0.05).
Additional exploratory analyses were carried out on several variables to assess novel links between network measures and affective behavioral measures. For this, we used 9 variables, including AUC values for SW, modularity, CPL, transitivity, assortativity, freezing counts on days 1–3 and margin distance. This data set provided 9 variables with 30 observations each. Factor analysis was first used to explore associations between variables of most importance in the data set (i.e., evaluate variable loadings for the first two factors contributing most to the variance in the data). Projections of variables and data points onto the first 2 dimensions was carried out with principal components analysis (PCA) and cluster analysis using FactoMineR (Lê et al., 2008). A multivariate ANOVA (MANOVA) was used to examine overall differences and report specific univariate associations of potential future interest (α < 0.05).
Further analysis of fMRI scans was carried out using probabilistic ICA (Beckmann and Smith, 2004) as implemented in MELODIC Version 3.15, part of FSL. The following data pre-processing was applied to the input data: masking of non-brain voxels; voxel-wise de-meaning of the data; normalization of the voxel-wise variance. Pre-processed data were whitened and projected into a 20-dimensional subspace using PCA. The whitened observations were decomposed into sets of vectors which describe signal variation across the temporal domain (time-courses), the session/subject domain and across the spatial domain (maps) by optimizing for non-Gaussian spatial source distributions using a fixed-point iteration technique (Hyvarinen, 1999). Estimated component maps were divided by the standard deviation of the residual noise and thresholded (0.6) by fitting a mixture model to the histogram of intensity values (Beckmann and Smith, 2004). The resulting components were overlaid on the multi-subject template and classified according to peak z score anatomical location. A series of two-stage multiple linear regressions were used to back-project the group ICA components to subject-specific time courses (using spatial regressions) and spatial components (using temporal regressions). The subject-specific spatial component maps were then used in statistical comparisons for each component in the FSL randomize tool. The statistical design matrix was generated using the FSL Glm tool. We used a one-way ANOVA general linear model design. Randomization tests were carried out with 500 permutations (corrected p-level for significance is 0.05 and statistical thresholding of maps by threshold free cluster enhancement).
For metabolomics data, the intensity of metabolites was imported to the web-based MetaboAnalyst 5.0 platform1 and normalized by the sum of intensities followed by generalized log10 transformation and pareto scaling before multivariate analysis. Further, principal component analysis (PCA), partial least square discriminant analysis (PLS-DA), and Hierarchical Clustering analysis were employed on the dataset (Pang et al., 2021). The Q2 and R2 values were calculated to assess supervised PLS-DA model. The Variable Importance in Projection (VIP) derived from PLS-DA was used to extract the importance of metabolites to PLS-DA model. Metabolites with a VIP score > 1 is weighed to be influential in PLS-DA.
Results
10wo mice display higher locomotor activity than 30 and 60wo mice
Mice were tested in a novel open field environment 1 week after arrival. The ages at the time of testing were 11, 31, and 61wo. We used a one-way ANOVA with Tukey’s multiple comparison post-hoc test to analyze the data. The 10wo group showed a significantly greater total distance (F2,27 = 4.5, p = 0.02; Tukey’s post hoc p = 0.03 and 0.04 comparing 10wo vs. 30–60wo, respectively) and margin distance than 30 and 60wo mice (F2,27 = 6.3, p = 0.006; Tukey’s post hoc p = 0.01 comparing 10wo vs. both 30–60wo mice; Figure 3). The greater margin distance activity in 10wo mice was driven by female mice (sex main effect F1,24 = 5.9, p = 0.02, with margin distance higher in 10wo female mice compared to 30–60wo females and 10wo males; Tukey’s post hoc p < 0.05; Supplementary Figure 4). No significant age or sex differences were observed in other locomotor activity measures, such as sequential and repetitive beam breaks, total and center distance.
Figure 3. Young 10wo mice showed greater activity in a novel test environment compared to 30 and 60wo mice. (A,B) No differences were observed for sequential and repetitive beam breaks activities between the age groups. (C) Total distance (in cm) differed between the age groups, with 10wo mice showing a non-significant trend compared to 60wo mice (F2,27 = 4.5, p = 0.02; *Tukey’s post hoc p = 0.03 and 0.04 comparing 10wo vs 30–60wo, respectively). (D) Movement along the margins of the novel test cages different between the age groups, with 10wo mice showing greater movement relative to both 30 and 60wo mice (F2,27 = 6.3, p = 0.006; *Tukey’s post hoc p = 0.01 comparing 10wo vs both 30–60wo mice). The greater margin activity in 10wo mice was largely driven by female mice (sex main effect F1,24 = 5.9, p = 0.02, with margin activity levels higher in female vs male 10wo and 30wo mice, p = 0.004 for female vs male 10 and 30wo mice; n = 10/pooled groups). (E) No differences were observed for center distance. Data in are box-whisker with interquartile range and minimum-maximum values around the median (dots are data points).
10wo mice display brief and faster patterns of social exploratory activity compared to 30–60wo mice
Sociability tests were carried out when mice were 12, 32, and 62wo. We used a two-way ANOVA (age x test session) with repeated measures. No effect of sex was observed. Thus, data were pooled in our analyses. No age differences in the number of ROI entrances was observed but a significant difference across test sessions was observed (main effect of session F2,46 = 49.4, p < 0.00001; Figure 4A). Thus, all age groups entered the ROI where the novel test mouse was located a similar number of times and displayed reductions in the number of entrances during a second encounter with the same test mouse by the same degree. 10wo mice showed a greater mean speed within the ROI than 60wo mice, during the re-encounter with the familiar mouse and exploration of the 2nd novel mouse (F2,23 = 5.9, p = 0.008; Tukey’s post hoc p = 0.02 and 0.03, respectively; Figure 4B). We also observed significant effects of test session and age, but not age x session interactions, on time spent in the test mouse ROI (session F2,46 = 5.5, p = 0.007; age F2,23 = 4.5, p = 0.02; Figure 4C). 30wo mice spent more time in the ROI during the first novel mouse encounter than 10wo mice (Tukey’s post hoc p = 0.007). Only 30wo mice showed reductions in their time spent in the ROI on the second encounter with the familiar mouse and with the new novel mouse (Tukey’s post hoc p = 0.02 and 0.005, respectively). No effects of age or session were observed on time in initial approach (Figure 4C).
Figure 4. Age differences in sociability toward novel mouse. (A) All mice showed similar number of entrances in the novel mouse ROI in the first test (T1), reduced entrances during second exposure to the familiar mouse in a second test (T2) and showed high levels of entrances in a second novel mouse ROI during the second test (T2; main effect of session F2,46 = 49.4, p < 0.00001). Guided asterisks indicate significant differences at *p < 0.05, ***p < 0.001, and ****p < 0.0001 over T1-T2 sessions. (B) 10wo mice showed a greater mean speed within the ROI than 60wo mice, during the re-encounter with the familiar mouse and exploration of the 2nd novel mouse (F2,23 = 5.9, p = 0.008; Tukey’s post hoc p = 0.02 and 0.03, respectively). (C) The 10wo mice spent less time in ROI where ‘bait’ mouse was located compared to 30wo mice (age F2,23 = 4.5, p = 0.02; Tukey’s post hoc *p < 0.05, ***p < 0.001). (D) No significant differences in time to initial approach. Group sizes are: 10wo n = 8, 30wo n = 9, 60wo n = 9. (E,F) Representative video frames capturing exploratory activity by the experimental mouse and novel and familiar mice under inverted clear plastic ‘bait’ cups.
No age differences in sucrose consumption
Sucrose preference testing was conducted a week after sociability tests when mice were 13, 33, and 63wo. No effect of sex was observed. Thus, data were pooled in our analyses. All mice consumed more sucrose solution than water. No age-related differences in the amount of sucrose and water consumed nor differences in the sucrose preference index were found (Supplementary Figure 5). We also analyzed sucrose consumption during the 3 days of acclimation and observed no differences between the groups (Supplementary Figure 5).
60wo adult mice display high variability in their freezing responses during initial fear conditioning but express fear memory at 24 and 48 h
Fear conditioning was conducted a week after completing fMRI scans when mice were 16, 36, and 66wo. Percent time freezing was analyzed using a two-way repeated measures ANOVA. No effect of sex was observed. Thus, data were pooled in our analyses. On day 1 of fear conditioning, all age groups progressively developed unconditioned-conditioned stimulus (US+/CS+) associations (Figure 5). We observed a rise in percent time freezing over the course of each tone-shock exposure in all age groups (effect of trial F3,81 = 66, p < 0.0001 and age F2,27 = 7.5, p = 0.0025; Figure 5A). 60wo mice showed higher percent freezing across cue-shock trials than the other age groups (Tukey’s post-hoc test indicated p < 0.05 comparing 60wo to 10wo and 30wo mice on trials 1–3 and p = 0.02 compared to 10wo mice on trial 4), although the freezing responses were more variable in the 60wo mice than in the other two age groups. Data were next analyzed as average percent time freezing per day (Figure 5B). When compared across days, all age groups showed robust recall of fear conditioning, observed as high levels of freezing in response to the tone alone, at 24 and 48 h (main effect of age F2,27 = 4.6, p = 0.02; main effect test day F2,54 = 66.8, p < 0.0001). At 24 h, all mice maintained a high percent freezing relative to day 1 (Dunnett’s test p < 0.05 in all age groups comparing day 2 to 1). At 48 h, the cages were modified such that the tone was delivered in a context with different sensory features (walls and floors were smooth white plastic in a triangular configuration, as reported in Anagnostaras et al., 2010). Still, 10wo and 60wo mice maintained high levels of freezing in response to the conditioned cue, relative to day 1 (Dunnett’s test p < 0.05). The 30wo mice showed a similar but non-significant trend toward increased fear recall at 48 h (Figure 5B). The freezing responses by 60wo mice were higher than those of 10wo mice on the 3rd day of testing (Dunnett’s test p < 0.05).
Figure 5. 60wo mice demonstrate high freezing responses at 24 and 48 h post despite variable initial development of conditioned freezing responses. (A) Although all mice develop conditioned freezing by trials 3 and 4, the oldest age group showed high levels of freezing compared to 10–30wo mice (Tukey’s HSD p < 0.05 comparing 60wo to 10* and 30wo** mice on trials 1 and 2). (B) Average percent time spent freezing over 3 days of testing (conditioning, recall at 24 h, recall in modified context at 48 h). All mice show high levels of freezing at 24 and 48 h, although in comparison to 10wo mice, 60wo mice maintain high freezing responses in a modified context on day 3 (asterisks indicate significant differences at *p < 0.05, **p < 0.01, ***p < 0.001, and ****p < 0.0001, Tukey’s post-hoc test). (C) Motion index (arbitrary units, a.u.) over 3 days of testing. All mice show reductions in general locomotor activity inside the operant cages on days 2–3 compared to day 1 (asterisks indicate significant differences at **p < 0.01, and ****p < 0.0001, Tukey’s post-hoc test). Data in A are mean standard ± error and in B-C are box-whisker with interquartile range and minimum-maximum values around the median (dots are data points; n = 10/group).
We analyzed a motion index as a surrogate measure of general locomotor activity using an infrared motion sensitive camera inside the test cages. Consistent with results in Figure 3, 10wo mice showed higher motion index levels than 60wo mice in the novel test cage on day 1 (Figure 5C; day by age interaction F4,54 = 2.7, p = 0.04, Tukey’s HSD p = 0.008 day 1 comparing 10wo vs. the 60wo group; main effect test day F2,54 = 84.9, p < 0.0001). The motion index was significantly reduced in all groups by days 2 and 3 of testing (Tukey’s HSD p < 0.05 comparing days 1 vs. 2 and 3 for all age groups). A closer assessment of the motion index over the tone-shock presentation trials on day 1 and tone responses on day 2 provided further evidence of highly variable responses to conditioned and unconditioned stimuli in 60wo mice compared to the other two age groups (Supplementary Figures 6–8). The 60wo mice did have a reduced initial motion response to the tone, but this was only significant during the second tone-shock presentation (two-way ANOVA with repeated measures showed effect of time, F2.8,76.1 = 46, p < 0.0001, and age, F2,27 = 7.5, p = 0.0025, but no interactions; Tukey’s multiple comparison test indicated p < 0.05 comparing 60wo to 30wo, on tone-shock trial 2). However, all mice responded to tone, shock, and showed reduced motion during baseline over the course of all 4 trials on day 1 (Supplementary Figures 6D, 8). Further declines in motion were not observed on day 2 (Supplementary Figure 7D), suggesting acclimation of motion overlapping with conditioned fear learning.
Young adult mice show a higher network modularity compared to older adult mice
Functional MRI scanning was carried out over a two-week period following sucrose preference testing. Mice were 14–15, 34–35, and 64–65wo at the time of scanning. Data were analyzed using two-way ANOVA with repeated measures and Tukey’s multiple comparison test. No effect of sex was observed. Thus, data were pooled in our analyses. Analysis of network measures obtained from functional connectivity matrices indicated significant main effects of age, but not sex, for SWC (age by threshold interaction F26,312 = 2.4, p = 0.0002), modularity (age by threshold interaction F26,312 = 1.79, p = 0.01), and assortativity (age by threshold interaction F26,312 = 2.6, p = 0.00006; Figure 6). Tukey’s post-hoc comparisons further revealed that SWC was highest in 30wo mice relative to 10wo mice and both modularity and assortativity were highest in 10wo mice relative to 30 and 60wo mice (Figure 6). Significant pairwise differences were observed across several edge density thresholds (see Figures 6C–E; details in figure legend). We analyzed the number of modules (at 16% edge density) per age group and observed a significantly greater number of modules in 30 vs. 10wo mice (Figures 6A,F; Kruskall-Wallis test p = 0.02). Network randomization removed these differences (Figure 6B). The number of nodes per module did not differ between groups (Figure 6G). To confirm the significant age related changes in functional network modularity, we further investigated 5 additional community detection routines available in igraph, including greedy optimization (Clauset et al., 2004), edge betweenness (Newman and Girvan, 2004), random walk (Pons and Latapy, 2005), lead Eigenvector (Newman, 2006), and a different implementation of Louvain’s algorithm (Blondel et al., 2008; Table 1). These were all estimated at 16% edge density. Across most of these algorithms, the number of modules detected was lowest in 10wo mice compared to the other two age groups. In addition, the modularity index was highest in 10wo mice compared to the two older age groups (Table 1 provides statistical results).
Figure 6. Young 10wo mice had greater modularity and assortativity of functional connectivity networks than 30 and 60wo mice. (A) 3D functional connectome maps illustrating mean modularity value (node size) and node community affiliation (node and edge color) for 10, 30, and 60wo mice (density threshold = 16% edges retained). The bar below maps indicates the number of identified modules. (B) Randomized 3D connectome maps for the same data in A. (C) Small world coefficient was lowest in 10wo mice (age by threshold interaction F26,312 = 2.4, p = 0.0002, Tukey’s HSD * and ***p < 0.05 comparing 10wo and 30wo to 60 weeks, respectively). (D) Modularity was highest in 10wo mice (age by threshold interaction F26,312 = 1.79, p = 0.01, Tukey’s HSD * and **p < 0.05 comparing 10wo to 30wo and 60 weeks, respectively). (E) Assortativity was highest in 10wo mice (age by threshold interaction F26,312 = 2.6, p = 0.00006, Tukey’s HSD * and **p < 0.05 comparing 10wo to 30wo and 60 weeks, respectively). (F) Number of modules identified at density-16% was higher in 30wo compared to 10wo mice (Kruskall-Wallis test p = 0.02). (G) No difference in number of nodes per module was observed. (H) No difference in the normalized rich club coefficient was observed. N = 10/group. Data in A–E and G,H are mean standard ± error and in F are box-whisker with interquartile range and minimum-maximum values around the median (dots are data points).
At 16% edge density, we also observed significant main effects of age on transitivity and assortativity (Table 1). Transitivity was lowest in 60wo vs. 30 and 10wo mice. Assortativity was significantly greater in 10wo vs. 30wo mice. This was observed using Brain Connectivity Toolbox’s routine for calculating network assortativity. Although a similar trend was observed using igraph, it did not reach statistical significance (Table 1).
Young adult mice have robust functional connectivity between affective brain regions in auditory/temporal cortical and striatal networks
Group ICA identified 9 of 20 functional network components similar to previously identified in mice (Jonckers et al., 2011; Sforazzini et al., 2014; Grandjean et al., 2014a; Bukhari et al., 2017). The ICA components included anterior and posterior cingulate, somatosensory, visual, auditory/temporal, piriform/amygdala, hypothalamic and dorsal striatal networks (Supplementary Figure 9). Figure 7 shows the results for statistical comparisons between age groups. The 10wo mice had greater functional connectivity in the auditory/temporal network than 60wo mice. The areas showing greater connectivity within the right hemisphere of this network in 10wo mice included anterior cingulate, auditory/temporal subregions, ventral hippocampus, dorsal striatum, midbrain reticular nucleus, bed nucleus of stria terminalis, posterior thalamic areas, and superior colliculus. The left hemisphere had less differences and included primary and secondary somatosensory areas, striatum, visual cortex, hippocampus, and thalamic subregions (Figure 7). The left hemisphere striatal network had greater functional connectivity in 10wo compared to 60wo mice. The greater functional connectivity in the left striatal network of 10wo mice included piriform, hippocampus and superior colliculus. We also observed greater functional connectivity in a left hemisphere striatal network in 30wo compared to 60wo mice and in a right hemisphere somatosensory network in 10wo compared to 30wo mice.
Figure 7. Age associated reductions in functional connectivity in primary sensory and association cortical areas and striatum. Lines indicate regions of the brain with statistically significant differences in connectivity with the network component (indicated above maps). Statistical analysis used a one-way ANOVA with mixed effects (p < 0.05, cluster corrected; higher level permutation tests with 5,000 permutations; n = 10/group). Maps thresholded at t > 2.3 (bar graph below maps) and overlaid onto multi-subject T2 anatomical template. Titles over the maps indicate the original network component identified by group pICA (see methods for details). Column maps on the left correspond to CCFv3 atlas of the mouse brain. Abbreviations: ACg, anterior cingulate; AMYG, amygdala; APN, anteroposterior nucleus of thalamus; AUD, auditory cortex; BNST, bed nucleus of stria terminalis; CA3, ‘Ammons Cornus’ 3 subfield of hippocampus; DStr, dorsal striatum; HPC, hippocampus; LDT, laterodorsal thalamus; MDT, mediodorsal thalamus; MRN, median Raphe nucleus; NAc, nucleus accumbens; PHA, posterior hypothalamic area; Pir, piriform cortex; PfT, parafascicular thalamus; S2, secondary somatosensory cortex; Sept, septum SCol, superior colliculus; SI, substantia innominate; SSC1, primary somatosensory cortex; V1, primary visual cortex; VALT, ventral anterolateral thalamus; vHPC, ventral hippocampus; VMT, ventromedial thalamus; VPMT, ventral posteromedial thalamus; vSub, ventral subiculum.
Statistical interactions between age, locomotor activity, fear conditioning and indices of functional network connectivity
MANOVA revealed a significant effect of age across several connectomic and behavior variables (F9,20 = 4.6, p = 0.002). Principal components analysis (PCA) revealed a relatively strong relationship between several of these variables. This is observed by contributions to the first 2 dimensions, which jointly explain 56.08% of variance in the data. To establish significance, PCA results for the first 2 dimensions (56.08%) were compared against a reference value (43.65%) representing 95% quartiles of distribution of percent explained variances (inertia) of simulated data tables of equivalent size and normal distribution (Figure 8). The positive direction along dimension 1 opposes 90% of mice in the youngest group (1–8 and 10 are in the youngest 10mo group) from many of the mice in the other two age groups (Figure 8A; Supplementary Figure 10A). This positive direction is characterized by high values freezing counts on days 2–3 of fear conditioning and high margin distance activity in a novel open field environment and high values for small world coefficient, transitivity, and modularity in 70% of the 10mo mice (Supplementary Figure 10B). Cluster dendrogram in Figure 8B classified 3 clusters. A majority of young 10mo mice were classified in cluster 3 (green). This group is again characterized by high values for modularity, transitivity, small world coefficient, margin distance activity and days 2–3 of fear conditioning.
Figure 8. Exploratory multivariate analyses reveal significant associations of interest between functional network measures and affective behavior variables. The first two dimensions of the PCA explain 56.08%, which is above 95%-quartiles of random distributions predicting 43.62% of variance explain with equal size data tables. (A) Individual factor map with labeled individuals contributing to a high degree to the first two dimensions. (B) Cluster tree partitioning data by ascending hierarchical classification of principal components. Cluster 3, made mostly of individuals in the youngest age group, is characterized by high values for modularity, transitivity, small worldness, marginal distance activity and fear recall (Supplementary Figure 10).
Metabolomic profiles in prefrontal cortex and hippocampus differ between 10, 30 and 60wo mice
Results for PCA and partial least squares discriminant analysis (PLS-DA) for GC–MS data obtained from the mouse hippocampus are shown in Supplementary Figure 11 and Figures 9A,B. Variable importance in projection (VIP) scores plot of the PLS-DA model for mouse hippocampus indicated differences in the top 25 metabolites between 10, 30, and 60wo mice. Among the top 25 listed in Figure 9A, methionine and cysteine had the highest VIP scores. The top 7 had a bidirectional transition in relative levels when youngest mice having lowest or highest levels and late middle-aged mice having diametrically distinct levels. This relationship included methionine, cysteine, malate, fumarate, succinate, N-acetylglycine, alanine, and taurine. The differential prominence of these metabolites across age groups is supported by clustering in heatmap shown in Figure 9B. Results for PCA and PLS-DA for GC–MS data obtained from the mouse midline prefrontal cortex are shown in Supplementary Figure 12 and Figures 9C,D. VIP scores plot of the PLS-DA model for mouse prefrontal indicated differences in the top 25 metabolites between 10, 30 and 60wo mice. Among the top 25 listed in Figure 9C, proline, taurine, and methionine had the highest VIP scores. The differential prominence of these metabolites across age groups is supported by clustering in the heatmap in Figure 9D.
Figure 9. Mouse brain metabolomics identifies age variations in methionine, cysteine, and proline in prefrontal cortical and hippocampal regions. (A,B) Variable importance in projection (VIP) scores plot of the PLS-DA model for mouse hippocampus indicated differences in the top 25 metabolites between 10, 30, and 60wo mice. The differential prominence of these metabolites across age groups is supported by clustering in heatmap. (C,D) VIP scores plot of the PLS-DA model for mouse prefrontal indicated differences in the top 25 metabolites between 10, 30, and 60wo mice. (D) The differential prominence of these metabolites across age groups is supported by clustering in the heatmap. Group sizes are: 10wo n = 4, 30wo n = 5, 60wo n = 6.
Discussion
Our present results indicate that 10wo, 30wo and 60wo C57BL/6 J mice differed in their locomotor activity in a novel environment, social interaction time and speed, and fear conditioning. These differences were largely observed between the youngest and oldest age groups and may reflect differences in ‘emotionality’ between the two age groups. The youngest mice appear more active in response to novelty, such as a novel environment or a novel social interaction. The oldest group appeared less active and exhibited a more ‘cautious’ and variable pattern of behavior during fear conditioned training. Our fMRI results indicate that young 10wo mice had greater functional connectivity between a temporal cortical/auditory cortex network and subregions of the anterior cingulate cortex and ventral hippocampus, among other regions, and a greater network modularity and assortative mixing of nodes compared to older adult mice. We observed a close correspondence between behavioral measures of fear conditioning, locomotor activity in a novel environment and measures of network efficiency and segregation in young mice, which was no longer apparent in older adult mice. Finally, metabolome analyses revealed significantly different neurochemical profiles in prefrontal cortex and hippocampus between 10wo mice and the other age groups, with several essential amino acids differing the most between the groups (e.g., methionine, cysteine, proline, and taurine).
In mice, conditioned fear memory varies with age in both context (hippocampal-dependent) and cued (amygdala-dependent) paradigms. Compared to adult 6mo mice, late middle age 17mo C57BL/6 mice show deficits in renewal of extinguished freezing in a modified context (Sanders, 2011). Eighteen-month-old mice showed greater freezing in response to context but lower freezing in response to tone than 3mo mice (Evans et al., 2021). These results suggest dissociable aging changes in conditioned fear memory, perhaps involving distinct neural circuits involving amygdala and hippocampus (Evans et al., 2021). Twenty three month old mice showed higher cued fear compared to 2mo mice but impaired renewal of conditioned freezing following extinction than 12mo mice (Oh et al., 2018). This was associated with altered stress reactivity (Oh et al., 2018). Using a trace eye blinking conditioning protocol, Kishimoto showed significant impairments in conditioned responses in aged 20mo compared to 12 and 2mo mice (Kishimoto et al., 2001). The above studies support differences in conditioned fear in aged vs. young mice. Age differences in hippocampal/prefrontal structure and function at the neuronal and synaptic level, stress coping/stress molecular signaling changes and changes in neuroimmune activity are thought to account for the observed age differences in conditioned freezing in mice (von Bohlen und Halbach et al., 2006; Oh et al., 2018; Evans et al., 2021). Viewed from this perspective, human studies reporting lower amygdala or cingulate activation in response to negative valence stimuli in aged vs. young adults may be accounted for by deficits in these brain areas as well (as opposed to a greater reactivity in young adults; Chaudhary et al., 2023). Our present data reveals a close correspondence between conditioned fear recall and modularity, transitivity, and small world coefficient (Figure 8; Supplementary Figure 10). It should be noted that there is extensive literature on the role of the amygdala and its subregions in fear conditioning. One of the limitations of this study is the use of an fMRI sequence based on the gradient recalled echo that, while having signal to noise benefits, leads to signal loss in air-tissue interface regions where the amygdala is located. This effect can be appreciated from the representative cropped fMRI scan in Figure 2. Therefore, the lack of results in the amygdala should therefore be interpreted with caution.
Young 10wo mice displayed greater locomotor activity than 30 and 60wo mice, consistent with evidence of decreasing locomotion with age (Elias et al., 1975). Male B6 mice are generally more active at 1–3mo than at 7 or 13mo (Lhotellier and Cohen-Salmon, 1989; Serradj and Jamon, 2007), although others have reported greater locomotor activity in 17mo vs. 5 and 25mo B6NIA mice (Frick et al., 2000), greater locomotor activity in 3–8mo vs. 28mo B6NJ (Fahlstrom et al., 2011), or no difference between 2 and 18mo B6 mice (Barreto et al., 2010). In our present observations, locomotor activity in the younger B6 group was largely along the margins of a novel test cage. This suggests novelty associated exploratory activity along the safer walls of the cages in 10wo mice. Under novel exploratory conditions, however, 24mo B6 mice were observed to display greater exploratory activity than 2mo mice (Ammassari-Teule et al., 1994). Open field activity in our present work was greatest in 10mo mice and specifically female mice. Thus, sex differences also should be considered when assessing affective behaviors (Knight et al., 2021).
Our results showed no differences in sucrose preference between the studied adult age groups. All mice consumed preferred sucrose solution over water to the same degree. This result contrasts with previous reports. Eighteen-month-old (~72wo) C57BL/6 N mice reportedly show less sucrose reward preference than 3mo mice (Malatynska et al., 2012). This was correlated with other signs of depressive-like behaviors in aged vs. young mice (Malatynska et al., 2012). Similar reductions in sucrose and milk reward preference in 13–15mo vs. 3mo C57BL/6 were reported (Harb et al., 2014). Conversely, aged 24mo Balb/c mice did not show differences in sucrose preference, locomotor activity, immobility in a tail suspension test compared to 4–6mo mice (Kelley et al., 2013). Discrepancies between studies may arise from differences in the sucrose preference protocol, differences in mouse strains, and age groups. We also examined social interactions in mice. While all mice showed similar social interactions in terms of the number of approaches toward test mice, 10wo mice display faster and more brief interactions compared to the older mice. This is partly consistent with results from Shoji and Miyakawa, who reported similar levels of social interactions in B6 mice ages 2–23mo. However, they reported reduced preference for novel social interactions in 17 and 23mo relative to 11mo old mice. Although we observed no such preferences using a supervised learning approach to assess social interactions, 60wo mice did display slightly lower novelty preferences during a second novel social encounter relative to a first novel encounter (Figure 4).
We observed differences in functional connectivity within a temporal/auditory cortex network. Specifically, anterior cingulate cortex and ventral hippocampal connectivity within this network was strongest in 10wo compared to 60wo mice. Our results using functional MRI agree with previous structural neuroimaging of mice along the same age range, which identified maturational changes from young to middle age in auditory, cingulate, hippocampal formation, and somatosensory (parietal) cortices (Clifford et al., 2023). The results in our present study showing stronger temporal/auditory connectivity with the ACg and vHPC in the young age group is consistent with the robust cued fear conditioning these mice developed compared to older age groups. These structures have been studied for their roles in generalized fear and anxiety in murine models (Ortiz et al., 2019). Increased synaptic activity in projections from ACg to vHPC regulate the long-term generalization of fear memories (Bian et al., 2019). The vHPC in mice contains neurons that show higher levels of neuronal activity and conditioned freezing behavior in response to threat cues than safety or combined threat/safety cues (compound cues; Meyer et al., 2019). Conversely, a subset of prelimbic cortical projecting vHPC neurons show increased neuronal activity with safety cues and this was accompanied by lower levels of freezing relative to their response to threat cues (Meyer et al., 2019). Thus, the stronger functional connectivity observed in the present study in young adult mice may be associated with the fear and anxiety related functions of this vHPC-PFC circuit. Importantly, we observed the higher functional connectivity between temporal/auditory and vHPC-ACg regions in young vs. older mice prior to their exposure to fear conditioning, thus averting effects introduced by learned fear on functional connectivity. We should point out that the age differences in functional connectivity between ACg, vHPC and other regions within the temporal/auditory network (Figure 7) were lateralized, with a much greater functional connectivity observed in the right vs. left temporal cortical network. Fear-related anterior cingulate activity has been previously reported to be lateralized in mice (Kim et al., 2012). Inhibition of the right but not left ACg resulted in reduced observational fear learning relative to control mice (Kim et al., 2012).
We also observed a greater functional connectivity within a left dorsal striatal network in 10wo and 30wo compared to 60wo mice. The regions observed to show significantly greater functional connectivity with the dorsal striatum included a subregion of the HPC, superior colliculus, bed nucleus of stria terminalis. The role of the dorsal striatum in regards to fear and anxiety are supported by recent work indicating its synaptic connectivity arising from apical intercalated cells in the amygdala, which regulate fear conditioning in mice via inputs from thalamic, temporal association cortex and basolateral amygdala nuclei (Asede et al., 2021). Finally, we should note that age differences between young (1-2mo) and older adult (5–19mo) wildtype littermates of the transgenic Arcβ familial amyloidosis mouse have been reported (Grandjean et al., 2014b). Young adult mice around 2 months of age had weaker functional connectivity within subregions of the parietal, cingulate and dorsal striatal areas than older mice at 5mo and older (Grandjean et al., 2014b). These results identify similar regions to the ones reported here. However, whereas we observed decreases with age, Grandjean and colleagues (Grandjean et al., 2014b) observed increases in control aging mice. While this may arguably be due to differences in anesthesia protocol (isoflurane 1.4% in Grandjean et al. and 0.25% isoflurane and dexmedetomidine used here), it is more likely that differences are associated with the background strain of mice. Mouse strain-dependent genetics likely weighs heavily on brain microstructure and function (Neuner et al., 2019; Ashbrook et al., 2021; Murdy et al., 2022).
Our results indicate a higher functional network modularity and assortative mixing of nodes of young vs. older adult mice. The modularity changes we observed in young vs. older adult mice were of particular interest since it persisted across several different modularity algorithms. Graph analysis of brain structural covariance matrices of 2, 10, and 22mo mice showed reductions in node degree values from young to middle age in ACg, auditory and somatosensory cortices and an increase in hippocampus (Clifford et al., 2023). They also reported reduced modularity from 2 to 10mo (Clifford et al., 2023). In human subjects, young and early middle age groups have higher local nodal efficiency, clustering coefficients, and path lengths than late middle age and elderly groups (Wang et al., 2023). These graph measures, which suggest changes in network efficiency from young adulthood through aging, were considered in the context of cognitive changes and not affective behaviors. In the present work, we observed statistically meaningful associations between the multiple affective behavioral and connectomic variables. Although this does not confirm causation or even correlation between the variables, the results do point to interactions of interest that should be considered with other circuit specific approaches to determine the relationship between network configurations and affective behaviors.
Finally, we observed age related changes in several amino acid substrates in prefrontal and hippocampal tissue, which supports biochemical changes in these regions that might be linked to the observed maturational changes in brain function and behavior. Each of the substrates that differed the most between the groups are known to play important roles in neuronal function and development. Proline is important for glutamate homeostasis following methamphetamine challenge (Jones et al., 2021). Taurine is involved in developmental changes to neurons in the hippocampus, cerebellum, hypothalamus, and can act via GABAA receptors to modulate firing of GABA neurons (Curran and Marczinski, 2017). Methionine is associated with a host of distinct functions related to the control of gene expression in neurons and high methionine diets have been linked to neuronal damage and accelerated signs of senescence in animal models (Pi et al., 2021). The D enantiomer of cysteine is found in neuroprogenitor cells and is critical to early brain development (Semenza et al., 2021). Taken together, these results suggest ongoing maturational developmental changes in prefrontal cortex and hippocampus in young adult mice as well as senescence-related neurochemical changes in late middle-aged mice.
Data availability statement
The raw data supporting the conclusions of this article will be made available by the authors, without undue reservation.
Ethics statement
The animal study was approved by Institutional Animal Care and Use Committee. The study was conducted in accordance with the local legislation and institutional requirements.
Author contributions
MF: Conceptualization, Data curation, Formal analysis, Funding acquisition, Investigation, Methodology, Project administration, Resources, Software, Supervision, Validation, Visualization, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing. RM: Formal analysis, Investigation, Methodology, Writing – review & editing. NR: Data curation, Investigation, Methodology, Writing – review & editing. JB: Investigation, Methodology, Writing – review & editing. MP: Conceptualization, Investigation, Methodology, Supervision, Writing – review & editing. AP: Investigation, Writing – review & editing. MG: Investigation, Writing – review & editing. AB: Conceptualization, Supervision, Writing – review & editing. MM: Conceptualization, Formal analysis, Investigation, Methodology, Supervision, Writing – review & editing.
Funding
The author(s) declare that financial support was received for the research, authorship, and/or publication of this article. This study was funded by a National Institute on Aging grant (R21AG065819). Support was also provided by the McKnight Brain Institute of the University of Florida. JB was supported by the Summer Neuroscience Internship Program (SNIP) of the McKnight Brain Institute. The contents of this manuscript are solely the responsibility of the authors and do not necessarily represent the official views of the funding agencies. This work was performed in the McKnight Brain Institute at the National High Magnetic Field Laboratory’s AMRIS Facility, which is supported by National Science Foundation Cooperative Agreement No. DMR-1644779 and the State of Florida.
Conflict of interest
The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.
The author(s) declared that they were an editorial board member of Frontiers, at the time of submission. This had no impact on the peer review process and the final decision.
Publisher’s note
All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.
Supplementary material
The Supplementary material for this article can be found online at: https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fnagi.2024.1356086/full#supplementary-material
Footnotes
References
Ammassari-Teule, M., Fagioli, S., and Rossi-Arnaud, C. (1994). Radial maze performance and open-field behaviours in aged C57BL/6 mice: further evidence for preserved cognitive abilities during senescence. Physiol. Behav. 55, 341–345. doi: 10.1016/0031-9384(94)90144-9
Anagnostaras, S. G., Wood, S. C., Shuman, T., Cai, D. J., Leduc, A. D., Zurn, K. R., et al. (2010). Automated assessment of Pavlovian conditioned freezing and shock reactivity in mice using the VideoFreeze system. Front. Behav. Neurosci. 4:4. doi: 10.3389/fnbeh.2010.00158
Asede, D., Doddapaneni, D., Chavez, A., Okoh, J., Ali, S., Von-Walter, C., et al. (2021). Apical intercalated cell cluster: a distinct sensory regulator in the amygdala. Cell Rep. 35:109151. doi: 10.1016/j.celrep.2021.109151
Ashbrook, D. G., Arends, D., Prins, P., Mulligan, M. K., Roy, S., Williams, E. G., et al. (2021). A platform for experimental precision medicine: the extended BXD mouse family. Cell Syst 12:e239, 235–247.e9. doi: 10.1016/j.cels.2020.12.002
Barreto, G., Huang, T. T., and Giffard, R. G. (2010). Age-related defects in sensorimotor activity, spatial learning, and memory in C57BL/6 mice. J. Neurosurg. Anesthesiol. 22, 214–219. doi: 10.1097/ANA.0b013e3181d56c98
Baumgartner, N. E., Biraud, M. C., and Lucas, E. K. (2023). Sex differences in socioemotional behavior and changes in ventral hippocampal transcription across aging in C57Bl/6J mice. Neurobiol. Aging 130, 141–153. doi: 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2023.05.015
Beckmann, C. F., and Smith, S. M. (2004). Probabilistic independent component analysis for functional magnetic resonance imaging. IEEE Trans. Med. Imaging 23, 137–152. doi: 10.1109/TMI.2003.822821
Bian, X. L., Qin, C., Cai, C. Y., Zhou, Y., Tao, Y., Lin, Y. H., et al. (2019). Anterior cingulate cortex to ventral Hippocampus circuit mediates contextual fear generalization. J. Neurosci. 39, 5728–5739. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2739-18.2019
Blondel, V. D., Guillaume, J.-P., Lambiotte, R., and Lefebvre, E. (2008). Fast unfolding of communities in large networks. J. Statistical Mechanics: Theory and Experiment 2008, 2–12. doi: 10.1088/1742-5468/2008/10/P10008
Brenes, G. A. (2006). Age differences in the presentation of anxiety. Aging Ment. Health 10, 298–302. doi: 10.1080/13607860500409898
Bruijnzeel, A. W., Qi, X., Guzhva, L. V., Wall, S., Deng, J. V., Gold, M. S., et al. (2016). Behavioral characterization of the effects of Cannabis smoke and anandamide in rats. PLoS One 11:e0153327. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0153327
Bukhari, Q., Schroeter, A., Cole, D. M., and Rudin, M. (2017). Resting state fMRI in mice reveals anesthesia specific signatures of brain functional networks and their interactions. Front Neural Circuits 11:5. doi: 10.3389/fncir.2017.00005
Carstensen, L. L., Turan, B., Scheibe, S., Ram, N., Ersner-Hershfield, H., Samanez-Larkin, G. R., et al. (2011). Emotional experience improves with age: evidence based on over 10 years of experience sampling. Psychol. Aging 26, 21–33. doi: 10.1037/a0021285
Charles, S. T., Mather, M., and Carstensen, L. L. (2003). Aging and emotional memory: the forgettable nature of negative images for older adults. J. Exp. Psychol. Gen. 132, 310–324. doi: 10.1037/0096-3445.132.2.310
Chaudhary, S., Zhang, S., Zhornitsky, S., Chen, Y., Chao, H. H., and Li, C. R. (2023). Age-related reduction in trait anxiety: behavioral and neural evidence of automaticity in negative facial emotion processing. NeuroImage 276:120207. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2023.120207
Chellian, R., Behnood-Rod, A., Wilson, R., Wilks, I., Knight, P., Febo, M., et al. (2020). Exposure to smoke from high- but not low-nicotine cigarettes leads to signs of dependence in male rats and potentiates the effects of nicotine in female rats. Pharmacol. Biochem. Behav. 196:172998. doi: 10.1016/j.pbb.2020.172998
Clauset, A., Newman, M. E., and Moore, C. (2004). Finding community structure in very large networks. Phys. Rev. E Stat. Nonlinear Soft Matter Phys. 70:066111. doi: 10.1103/PhysRevE.70.066111
Clifford, K. P., Miles, A. E., Prevot, T. D., Misquitta, K. A., Ellegood, J., Lerch, J. P., et al. (2023). Brain structure and working memory adaptations associated with maturation and aging in mice. Front. Aging Neurosci. 15:1195748. doi: 10.3389/fnagi.2023.1195748
Colizza, V., Flammini, A., Serrano, M. A., and Vespignani, A. (2006). Detecting rich-club ordering in complex networks. Nat. Phys. 2, 110–115. doi: 10.1038/nphys209
Colon-Perez, L. M., Ibanez, K. R., Suarez, M., Torroella, K., Acuna, K., Ofori, E., et al. (2019). Neurite orientation dispersion and density imaging reveals white matter and hippocampal microstructure changes produced by Interleukin-6 in the TgCRND8 mouse model of amyloidosis. NeuroImage 202:116138. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2019.116138
Cox, R. W. (1996). AFNI: software for analysis and visualization of functional magnetic resonance neuroimages. Comput. Biomed. Res. 29, 162–173. doi: 10.1006/cbmr.1996.0014
Crawley, J. N., Chen, T., Puri, A., Washburn, R., Sullivan, T. L., Hill, J. M., et al. (2007). Social approach behaviors in oxytocin knockout mice: comparison of two independent lines tested in different laboratory environments. Neuropeptides 41, 145–163. doi: 10.1016/j.npep.2007.02.002
Csardi, G., and Nepusz, T. (2006). The igraph software package for complex network research. InterJ. Complex Syst. 1695, 1–9. doi: 10.5281/zenodo.7682609
Curran, C. P., and Marczinski, C. A. (2017). Taurine, caffeine, and energy drinks: reviewing the risks to the adolescent brain. Birth Defects Res 109, 1640–1648. doi: 10.1002/bdr2.1177
D’aquila, P. S., Newton, J., and Willner, P. (1997). Diurnal variation in the effect of chronic mild stress on sucrose intake and preference. Physiol. Behav. 62, 421–426. doi: 10.1016/S0031-9384(97)00042-5
Elias, P. K., Elias, M. F., and Eleftheriou, B. E. (1975). Emotionality, exploratory behavior, and locomotion in aging inbred strains of mice. Gerontologia 21, 46–55. doi: 10.1159/000212029
Evans, A. K., Park, H. H., Saw, N. L., Singhal, K., Ogawa, G., Leib, R. D., et al. (2021). Age-related neuroinflammation and pathology in the locus coeruleus and hippocampus: beta-adrenergic antagonists exacerbate impairment of learning and memory in aged mice. Neurobiol. Aging 106, 241–256. doi: 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2021.06.012
Fahlstrom, A., Yu, Q., and Ulfhake, B. (2011). Behavioral changes in aging female C57BL/6 mice. Neurobiol. Aging 32, 1868–1880. doi: 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2009.11.003
Ferreira, A. C., Sousa, N., Sousa, J. C., and Marques, F. (2023). Age-related changes in mice behavior and the contribution of lipocalin-2. Front. Aging Neurosci. 15:1179302. doi: 10.3389/fnagi.2023.1179302
Fischer, H., Sandblom, J., Gavazzeni, J., Fransson, P., Wright, C. I., and Backman, L. (2005). Age-differential patterns of brain activation during perception of angry faces. Neurosci. Lett. 386, 99–104. doi: 10.1016/j.neulet.2005.06.002
Flurkey, K., Currer, J. M., and Harrison, D. E. (2007). Mouse models in aging research. US: Academic Press.
Frick, K. M., Burlingame, L. A., Arters, J. A., and Berger-Sweeney, J. (2000). Reference memory, anxiety and estrous cyclicity in C57BL/6NIA mice are affected by age and sex. Neuroscience 95, 293–307. doi: 10.1016/S0306-4522(99)00418-2
Grandjean, J., Schroeter, A., Batata, I., and Rudin, M. (2014a). Optimization of anesthesia protocol for resting-state fMRI in mice based on differential effects of anesthetics on functional connectivity patterns. NeuroImage 102, 838–847. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2014.08.043
Grandjean, J., Schroeter, A., He, P., Tanadini, M., Keist, R., Krstic, D., et al. (2014b). Early alterations in functional connectivity and white matter structure in a transgenic mouse model of cerebral amyloidosis. J. Neurosci. 34, 13780–13789. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4762-13.2014
Gross, J. J., Carstensen, L. L., Pasupathi, M., Tsai, J., Skorpen, C. G., and Hsu, A. Y. (1997). Emotion and aging: experience, expression, and control. Psychol. Aging 12, 590–599. doi: 10.1037/0882-7974.12.4.590
Harb, M. R., Sousa, N., Zihl, J., and Almeida, O. F. (2014). Reward components of feeding behavior are preserved during mouse aging. Front. Aging Neurosci. 6:242. doi: 10.3389/fnagi.2014.00242
Henry, K. R. (2004). Males lose hearing earlier in mouse models of late-onset age-related hearing loss; females lose hearing earlier in mouse models of early-onset hearing loss. Hear. Res. 190, 141–148. doi: 10.1016/S0378-5955(03)00401-5
Humphries, M. D., and Gurney, K. (2008). Network ‘small-world-ness’: a quantitative method for determining canonical network equivalence. PLoS One 3:e0002051. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0002051
Humphries, M. D., Gurney, K., and Prescott, T. J. (2006). The brainstem reticular formation is a small-world, not scale-free, network. Proc. Biol. Sci. 273, 503–511. doi: 10.1098/rspb.2005.3354
Hyvarinen, A. (1999). Fast and robust fixed-point algorithms for independent component analysis. IEEE Trans. Neural Netw. 10, 626–634. doi: 10.1109/72.761722
Iidaka, T., Okada, T., Murata, T., Omori, M., Kosaka, H., Sadato, N., et al. (2002). Age-related differences in the medial temporal lobe responses to emotional faces as revealed by fMRI. Hippocampus 12, 352–362. doi: 10.1002/hipo.1113
Jenkinson, M., Bannister, P., Brady, M., and Smith, S. (2002). Improved optimization for the robust and accurate linear registration and motion correction of brain images. NeuroImage 17, 825–841. doi: 10.1006/nimg.2002.1132
Jonckers, E., Van Audekerke, J., De Visscher, G., Van Der Linden, A., and Verhoye, M. (2011). Functional connectivity fMRI of the rodent brain: comparison of functional connectivity networks in rat and mouse. PLoS One 6:e18876. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0018876
Jones, B., Balasubramaniam, M., Lebowitz, J. J., Taylor, A., Villalta, F., Khoshbouei, H., et al. (2021). Activation of proline biosynthesis is critical to maintain glutamate homeostasis during acute methamphetamine exposure. Sci. Rep. 11:1422. doi: 10.1038/s41598-020-80917-7
Kelley, K. W., O’connor, J. C., Lawson, M. A., Dantzer, R., Rodriguez-Zas, S. L., and Mccusker, R. H. (2013). Aging leads to prolonged duration of inflammation-induced depression-like behavior caused by Bacillus Calmette-Guerin. Brain Behav. Immun. 32, 63–69. doi: 10.1016/j.bbi.2013.02.003
Kessler, R. C., Petukhova, M., Sampson, N. A., Zaslavsky, A. M., and Wittchen, H. U. (2012). Twelve-month and lifetime prevalence and lifetime morbid risk of anxiety and mood disorders in the United States. Int. J. Methods Psychiatr. Res. 21, 169–184. doi: 10.1002/mpr.1359
Kim, S., Matyas, F., Lee, S., Acsady, L., and Shin, H. S. (2012). Lateralization of observational fear learning at the cortical but not thalamic level in mice. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 109, 15497–15501. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1213903109
Kishimoto, Y., Suzuki, M., Kawahara, S., and Kirino, Y. (2001). Age-dependent impairment of delay and trace eyeblink conditioning in mice. Neuroreport 12, 3349–3352. doi: 10.1097/00001756-200110290-00040
Klein, A., Andersson, J., Ardekani, B. A., Ashburner, J., Avants, B., Chiang, M. C., et al. (2009). Evaluation of 14 nonlinear deformation algorithms applied to human brain MRI registration. NeuroImage 46, 786–802. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2008.12.037
Knight, P., Chellian, R., Wilson, R., Behnood-Rod, A., Panunzio, S., and Bruijnzeel, A. W. (2021). Sex differences in the elevated plus-maze test and large open field test in adult Wistar rats. Pharmacol. Biochem. Behav. 204:173168. doi: 10.1016/j.pbb.2021.173168
Kotlarz, P., Nino, J. C., and Febo, M. (2022). Connectomic analysis of Alzheimer’s disease using percolation theory. Netw Neurosci 6, 213–233. doi: 10.1162/netn_a_00221
Lê, S., Josse, J., and Husson, F. (2008). FactoMineR: an R package for multivariate analysis. J. Stat. Softw. 25, 1–18. doi: 10.18637/jss.v025.i01
Lein, E. S., Hawrylycz, M. J., Ao, N., Ayres, M., Bensinger, A., Bernard, A., et al. (2007). Genome-wide atlas of gene expression in the adult mouse brain. Nature 445, 168–176. doi: 10.1038/nature05453
Lhotellier, L., and Cohen-Salmon, C. (1989). Genetics and senescence. I. Age-related changes in activity and exploration in three inbred strains of mice. Physiol. Behav. 45, 491–493. doi: 10.1016/0031-9384(89)90063-2
Machado, L., Thompson, L. M., and Brett, C. H. R. (2019). Visual analogue mood scale scores in healthy young versus older adults. Int. Psychogeriatr. 31, 417–424. doi: 10.1017/S1041610218000996
Mahar, R., Chang, M. C., and Merritt, M. E. (2021). Measuring NQO1 bioactivation using [(2)H(7)]glucose. Cancers (Basel) 13, 4165–4183. doi: 10.3390/cancers13164165
Malatynska, E., Steinbusch, H. W., Redkozubova, O., Bolkunov, A., Kubatiev, A., Yeritsyan, N. B., et al. (2012). Anhedonic-like traits and lack of affective deficits in 18-month-old C57BL/6 mice: implications for modeling elderly depression. Exp. Gerontol. 47, 552–564. doi: 10.1016/j.exger.2012.04.010
Malezieux, M., Klein, A. S., and Gogolla, N. (2023). Neural circuits for emotion. Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 46, 211–231. doi: 10.1146/annurev-neuro-111020-103314
Mathis, A., Mamidanna, P., Cury, K. M., Abe, T., Murthy, V. N., Mathis, M. W., et al. (2018). DeepLabCut: markerless pose estimation of user-defined body parts with deep learning. Nat. Neurosci. 21, 1281–1289. doi: 10.1038/s41593-018-0209-y
Mccall, N. M., Sprow, G. M., Delpire, E., Thiele, T. E., Kash, T. L., and Pleil, K. E. (2013). Effects of sex and deletion of neuropeptide Y2 receptors from GABAergic neurons on affective and alcohol drinking behaviors in mice. Front. Integr. Neurosci. 7:100. doi: 10.3389/fnint.2013.00100
McLean, D. J., and Skowron Volponi, M. A. (2018). Trajr: an R package for characterisation of animal trajectories. Ethology 124, 440–448. doi: 10.1111/eth.12739
Meyer, H. C., Odriozola, P., Cohodes, E. M., Mandell, J. D., Li, A., Yang, R., et al. (2019). Ventral hippocampus interacts with prelimbic cortex during inhibition of threat response via learned safety in both mice and humans. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 116, 26970–26979. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1910481116
Murdy, T. J., Dunn, A. R., Singh, S., Telpoukhovskaia, M. A., Zhang, S., White, J. K., et al. (2022). Leveraging genetic diversity in mice to inform individual differences in brain microstructure and memory. Front. Behav. Neurosci. 16:1033975. doi: 10.3389/fnbeh.2022.1033975
Nath, T., Mathis, A., Chen, A. C., Patel, A., Bethge, M., and Mathis, M. W. (2019). Using DeepLabCut for 3D markerless pose estimation across species and behaviors. Nat. Protoc. 14, 2152–2176. doi: 10.1038/s41596-019-0176-0
Neuner, S. M., Heuer, S. E., Huentelman, M. J., O’connell, K. M. S., and Kaczorowski, C. C. (2019). Harnessing genetic complexity to enhance translatability of Alzheimer’s disease mouse models: a path toward precision medicine. Neuron 101:e395, 399–411.e5. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2018.11.040
Newman, M. E. (2002). Assortative mixing in networks. Phys. Rev. Lett. 89:208701. doi: 10.1103/PhysRevLett.89.208701
Newman, M. E. (2006). Finding community structure in networks using the eigenvectors of matrices. Phys. Rev. E Stat. Nonlinear Soft Matter Phys. 74:036104. doi: 10.1103/PhysRevE.74.036104
Newman, M. E., and Girvan, M. (2004). Finding and evaluating community structure in networks. Phys. Rev. E 69:026113. doi: 10.1103/PhysRevE.69.026113
Oh, H. J., Song, M., Kim, Y. K., Bae, J. R., Cha, S. Y., Bae, J. Y., et al. (2018). Age-related decrease in stress responsiveness and proactive coping in male mice. Front. Aging Neurosci. 10:128. doi: 10.3389/fnagi.2018.00128
Onnela, J. P., Saramaki, J., Kertesz, J., and Kaski, K. (2005). Intensity and coherence of motifs in weighted complex networks. Phys. Rev. E Stat. Nonlinear Soft Matter Phys. 71:065103. doi: 10.1103/PhysRevE.71.065103
Ortiz, S., Latsko, M. S., Fouty, J. L., Dutta, S., Adkins, J. M., and Jasnow, A. M. (2019). Anterior cingulate cortex and ventral hippocampal inputs to the basolateral amygdala selectively control generalized fear. J. Neurosci. 39, 6526–6539. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0810-19.2019
Pang, Z., Chong, J., Zhou, G., De Lima Morais, D. A., Chang, L., Barrette, M., et al. (2021). MetaboAnalyst 5.0: narrowing the gap between raw spectra and functional insights. Nucleic Acids Res. 49, W388–W396. doi: 10.1093/nar/gkab382
Paxinos, G., and Franklin, K. B. J. (2001). The mouse brain in stereotaxic coordinates. San Diego: Academic Press.
Pi, T., Wei, S., Jiang, Y., and Shi, J. S. (2021). High methionine diet-induced Alzheimer’s disease like symptoms are accompanied by 5-Methylcytosine elevated levels in the brain. Behav. Neurol. 2021, 1–16. doi: 10.1155/2021/6683318
Pompilus, M., Colon-Perez, L. M., Grudny, M. M., and Febo, M. (2020). Contextual experience modifies functional connectome indices of topological strength and efficiency. Sci. Rep. 10:19843. doi: 10.1038/s41598-020-76935-0
Pons, P., and Latapy, M. (2005). Computing communities in large networks using random walks. eds. p. Yolum, T. Güngör, F. Güngör, and C. Özturan. Computer and Information Sciences - ISCIS 2005 Berlin, Heidelberg: Springer Berlin Heidelberg, 3733, 284–293. doi: 10.1007/11569596_31
Ravindranath, O., Ordaz, S. J., Padmanabhan, A., Foran, W., Jalbrzikowski, M., Calabro, F. J., et al. (2020). Influences of affective context on amygdala functional connectivity during cognitive control from adolescence through adulthood. Dev. Cogn. Neurosci. 45:100836. doi: 10.1016/j.dcn.2020.100836
Rubinov, M., and Sporns, O. (2010). Complex network measures of brain connectivity: uses and interpretations. NeuroImage 52, 1059–1069. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.10.003
Sadaka, A. H., Canuel, J., Febo, M., Johnson, C. T., Bradshaw, H. B., Ortiz, R., et al. (2023). Effects of inhaled cannabis high in Delta9-THC or CBD on the aging brain: a translational MRI and behavioral study. Front. Aging Neurosci. 15:1055433. doi: 10.3389/fnagi.2023.1055433
Sakthivel, R., Criado-Marrero, M., Barroso, D., Braga, I. M., Bolen, M., Rubinovich, U., et al. (2023). Fixed time-point analysis reveals repetitive mild traumatic brain injury effects on resting state functional magnetic resonance Imaging connectivity and neuro-spatial protein profiles. J. Neurotrauma 40, 2037–2049. doi: 10.1089/neu.2022.0464
Samhsa (2022). “Key substance use and mental health indicators in the United States: Results from the 2021 National Survey on drug use and health ”. Center for Behavioral Health Statistics and Quality, Substance Abuse and Mental Health Services Administration, US.
Sanders, M. J. (2011). Context processing in aging: older mice are impaired in renewal of extinguished fear. Exp. Aging Res. 37, 572–594. doi: 10.1080/0361073X.2011.619874
Semenza, E. R., Harraz, M. M., Abramson, E., Malla, A. P., Vasavda, C., Gadalla, M. M., et al. (2021). D-cysteine is an endogenous regulator of neural progenitor cell dynamics in the mammalian brain. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 118, 1–10. doi: 10.1073/pnas.2110610118
Serradj, N., and Jamon, M. (2007). Age-related changes in the motricity of the inbred mice strains 129/sv and C57BL/6j. Behav. Brain Res. 177, 80–89. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2006.11.001
Sforazzini, F., Schwarz, A. J., Galbusera, A., Bifone, A., and Gozzi, A. (2014). Distributed BOLD and CBV-weighted resting-state networks in the mouse brain. NeuroImage 87, 403–415. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.09.050
Shoji, H., and Miyakawa, T. (2019). Age-related behavioral changes from young to old age in male mice of a C57BL/6J strain maintained under a genetic stability program. Neuropsychopharmacol Rep 39, 100–118. doi: 10.1002/npr2.12052
Sturman, O., Von Ziegler, L., Schlappi, C., Akyol, F., Privitera, M., Slominski, D., et al. (2020). Deep learning-based behavioral analysis reaches human accuracy and is capable of outperforming commercial solutions. Neuropsychopharmacology 45, 1942–1952. doi: 10.1038/s41386-020-0776-y
Tan, Y., Singhal, S. M., Harden, S. W., Cahill, K. M., Nguyen, D. M., Colon-Perez, L. M., et al. (2019). Oxytocin receptors are expressed by glutamatergic prefrontal cortical neurons that selectively modulate social recognition. J. Neurosci. 39, 3249–3263. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2944-18.2019
Telesford, Q. K., Joyce, K. E., Hayasaka, S., Burdette, J. H., and Laurienti, P. J. (2011). The ubiquity of small-world networks. Brain Connect. 1, 367–375. doi: 10.1089/brain.2011.0038
Terlizzi, E. P., and Villarroel, M. A. (2020). “Symptoms of generalized anxiety disorder among adults: United States, 2019” in NCHS Data Brief United States Department of Health and Human Services. (Hyattsville, MD: National Center for Health Statistics)
Tessitore, A., Hariri, A. R., Fera, F., Smith, W. G., Das, S., Weinberger, D. R., et al. (2005). Functional changes in the activity of brain regions underlying emotion processing in the elderly. Psychiatry Res. 139, 9–18. doi: 10.1016/j.pscychresns.2005.02.009
Van Den Heuvel, M. P., and Sporns, O. (2011). Rich-club organization of the human connectome. J. Neurosci. 31, 15775–15786. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3539-11.2011
von Bohlen und Halbach, O., Zacher, C., Gass, P., and Unsicker, K. (2006). Age-related alterations in hippocampal spines and deficiencies in spatial memory in mice. J. Neurosci. Res. 83, 525–531. doi: 10.1002/jnr.20759
Wang, Y., Ghumare, E., Vandenberghe, R., and Dupont, P. (2017). Comparison of different generalizations of clustering coefficient and local efficiency for weighted undirected graphs. Neural Comput. 29, 313–331. doi: 10.1162/NECO_a_00914
Wang, Q., Qi, L., He, C., Feng, H., Xie, C., and Depression Imaging, R. C. (2023). Age- and gender-related dispersion of brain networks across the lifespan. Geroscience. 46, 1303–1318. doi: 10.1007/s11357-023-00900-8
Wright, C. I., Wedig, M. M., Williams, D., Rauch, S. L., and Albert, M. S. (2006). Novel fearful faces activate the amygdala in healthy young and elderly adults. Neurobiol. Aging 27, 361–374. doi: 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2005.01.014
Xia, M., Wang, J., and He, Y. (2013). BrainNet viewer: a network visualization tool for human brain connectomics. PLoS One 8:e68910. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0068910
Youn, C. K., Jun, Y., Jo, E. R., and Cho, S. I. (2020). Age-related hearing loss in C57BL/6J mice is associated with Mitophagy impairment in the central auditory system. Int. J. Mol. Sci. 21, 7202–7217. doi: 10.3390/ijms21197202
Yu, Y. F., Zhai, F., Dai, C. F., and Hu, J. J. (2011). The relationship between age-related hearing loss and synaptic changes in the hippocampus of C57BL/6J mice. Exp. Gerontol. 46, 716–722. doi: 10.1016/j.exger.2011.04.007
Keywords: aging, brain maturation, fMRI, fear conditioning, locomotor activity, functional connectivity, metabolomics
Citation: Febo M, Mahar R, Rodriguez NA, Buraima J, Pompilus M, Pinto AM, Grudny MM, Bruijnzeel AW and Merritt ME (2024) Age-related differences in affective behaviors in mice: possible role of prefrontal cortical-hippocampal functional connectivity and metabolomic profiles. Front. Aging Neurosci. 16:1356086. doi: 10.3389/fnagi.2024.1356086
Edited by:
Ignacio Torres-Aleman, Achucarro Basque Center for Neuroscience, SpainReviewed by:
Caesar Miguel Hernandez, University of Alabama at Birmingham, United StatesRuixiang Wang, The University of Iowa, United States
Copyright © 2024 Febo, Mahar, Rodriguez, Buraima, Pompilus, Pinto, Grudny, Bruijnzeel and Merritt. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.
*Correspondence: Marcelo Febo, febo@ufl.edu