Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Aging Neurosci., 26 January 2023
Sec. Neurocognitive Aging and Behavior
This article is part of the Research Topic Oscillatory Brain Activity as a Marker of Brain Function and Dysfunction in Aging and in Neurodegenerative Disorders View all 10 articles

Patients with Alzheimer’s disease dementia show partially preserved parietal ‘hubs’ modeled from resting-state alpha electroencephalographic rhythms

  • 1Department of Physiology and Pharmacology “Vittorio Erspamer”, Sapienza University of Rome, Rome, Italy
  • 2IRCCS Synlab SDN, Naples, Italy
  • 3Neurology Unit, Department of Clinical and Experimental Sciences, University of Brescia, Brescia, Italy
  • 4Clinica Neurologica, IRCCS Ospedale Policlinico San Martino, Genova, Italy
  • 5Dipartimento di Neuroscienze, Oftalmologia, Genetica, Riabilitazione e Scienze Materno-infantili (DiNOGMI), Università di Genova, Genova, Italy
  • 6Alzheimer’s Disease Rehabilitation Unit, IRCCS Istituto Centro San Giovanni di Dio Fatebenefratelli, Brescia, Italy
  • 7Department of Neurosciences, University of Padua, Padova, Italy
  • 8Non-Invasive Brain Stimulation Unit/Department of Behavioral and Clinical Neurology, Santa Lucia Foundation IRCCS, Rome, Italy
  • 9Stroke Unit, Department of Neuroscience, Tor Vergata Policlinic, Rome, Italy
  • 10Department of Neuroscience Imaging and Clinical Sciences and CESI, University “G. D’Annunzio” of Chieti-Pescara, Chieti, Italy
  • 11Department of Motor Sciences and Healthiness, University of Naples Parthenope, Naples, Italy
  • 12Oasi Research Institute – IRCCS, Troina, Italy
  • 13Department of Neuroscience, Mental Health and Sensory Organs, Sapienza University of Rome, Rome, Italy
  • 14Department of Biophysics, School of Medicine, Istanbul Medipol University, Istanbul, Türkiye
  • 15Research Institute for Health Sciences and Technologies (SABITA), Istanbul Medipol University, Istanbul, Türkiye
  • 16Department of Neurology, Dokuz Eylül University Medical School, Izmir, Türkiye
  • 17Faculty of Medicine, Izmir University of Economics, Izmir, Türkiye
  • 18Institute for Research and Medical Care, IRCCS San Raffaele Roma, Rome, Italy
  • 19Telematic University San Raffaele, Rome, Italy
  • 20Department of Medicine and Aging Sciences, University G. D’Annunzio of Chieti-Pescara, Chieti, Italy
  • 21San Raffaele of Cassino, Cassino, Italy

Introduction: Graph theory models a network by its nodes (the fundamental unit by which graphs are formed) and connections. ‘Degree’ hubs reflect node centrality (the connection rate), while ‘connector’ hubs are those linked to several clusters of nodes (mainly long-range connections).

Methods: Here, we compared hubs modeled from measures of interdependencies of between-electrode resting-state eyes-closed electroencephalography (rsEEG) rhythms in normal elderly (Nold) and Alzheimer’s disease dementia (ADD) participants. At least 5 min of rsEEG was recorded and analyzed. As ADD is considered a ‘network disease’ and is typically associated with abnormal rsEEG delta (<4 Hz) and alpha rhythms (8–12 Hz) over associative posterior areas, we tested the hypothesis of abnormal posterior hubs from measures of interdependencies of rsEEG rhythms from delta to gamma bands (2–40 Hz) using eLORETA bivariate and multivariate-directional techniques in ADD participants versus Nold participants. Three different definitions of ‘connector’ hub were used.

Results: Convergent results showed that in both the Nold and ADD groups there were significant parietal ‘degree’ and ‘connector’ hubs derived from alpha rhythms. These hubs had a prominent outward ‘directionality’ in the two groups, but that ‘directionality’ was lower in ADD participants than in Nold participants.

Discussion: In conclusion, independent methodologies and hub definitions suggest that ADD patients may be characterized by low outward ‘directionality’ of partially preserved parietal ‘degree’ and ‘connector’ hubs derived from rsEEG alpha rhythms.

1. Introduction

Alzheimer’s disease (AD) is the most prevalent neurodegenerative disorder in the elderly and causes cognitive deficits (e.g., episodic and working memory, executive functions, visuospatial abilities, language, etc.) and disabilities in activities of daily living progressively (i.e., loss of autonomy) belonging to dementia as a clinical syndrome (Tahami Monfared et al., 2022). It is provoked by the abnormal accumulation in the brain of Ab-42 and tau proteins, so the neurobiological in vivo diagnosis can be made using techniques that measure that accumulation, such as analysis of cerebrospinal fluid and positron emission tomography (Jack et al., 2019).

AD is considered a pathology affecting functional brain connectivity (Teipel et al., 2016). In this area of research, previous structural and resting-state functional magnetic resonance imaging (sMRI and rs-fMRI) studies showed colocalized abnormalities in both interhemispheric and intrahemispheric cortical connectivity in ADD patients compared with healthy elderly people (Nold) with unimpaired cognition (Delbeuck et al., 2003; Busche and Konnerth, 2016; Nakamura et al., 2017). Thanks to the high spatial resolution of MRI techniques (i.e., millimeters), those abnormalities were mainly localized as follows: (1) in the posterior parietal (precuneus) and cingulate cortices of the cortical default mode network (DMN; Bokde et al., 2006; Sorg et al., 2007; Brier et al., 2012; Wang et al., 2013, 2015; Joo et al., 2016; Eyler et al., 2019; Talwar et al., 2021; Zhang et al., 2021); (2) in the occipital and inferior parietal gyrus (Wang et al., 2021); and (3) in the medial temporal lobe and other nodes of the limbic system (Talwar et al., 2021).

Another significant contribution made by the sMRI and rs-fMRI studies, together with other brain research techniques, was to unveil the abnormal topological organization underlying the above alterations in the functional brain connectivity observed in ADD patients [see reviews by Reijneveld et al. (2007), Xie and He (2012), Stam (2014)]. In this topological organization, a cortical neural network can be formally represented by a ‘graph’ constituted of ‘nodes’ interconnected by ‘edges’. Notably, the topology of ‘nodes’ and ‘graphs’ globally reflects the following properties of a network: (1) near cortical nodes can be highly interconnected to each other forming ‘clusters’, and the nodes with more edges may have a prominent central role and underpin the modularity and segregation of the information within a network; (2) a few cortical nodes, the ‘hubs’, can ensure long-range interconnections between ‘clusters’ and may reduce the path length between far nodes and underpin the integration of the information within a network; (3) the ‘degree centrality’ or ‘nodal degree’ can define the importance of the hub, the ‘hub centrality’, in the information transmission within a brain network; (4) a hub can be classified as ‘connector’, connecting several different network modules, or ‘provincial’, mostly connecting nodes in the same network module as measured by the hub ‘participation coefficient’; (5) the number of the shortest paths that pass through a cortical node defines the node importance, the ‘betweenness centrality’, in the information transmission within a brain network; (6) a few highly connected cortical nodes may show dense interconnections with each other and form a sort of ‘rich club’ structure with a particular importance in the network information processing; (7) the topological distance between nodes, i.e., the mean number of edges to connect them, the ‘global efficiency’, is inversely related to effective parallel information transfer and integrated processing; and (8) an optimal balance between the network modularity (segregation) and integration of the nodes defines the so-called ‘small worldness’ structure, which is a favorable for information processing and shows resilience to insults impairing cortical nodes (Bullmore and Sporns, 2009, 2012; He and Evans, 2010; van den Heuvel and Sporns, 2011; Sporns, 2013; Wang et al., 2015; Liao et al., 2017).

Previous rs-fMRI studies also showed that compared with Nold people, ADD patients were characterized by decreased network segregation, as revealed by lower clustering/modular structure of the network graphs (Supekar et al., 2008; Chen et al., 2013) and higher network integration structure, as revealed by lower characteristic path length among the cortical nodes (Sanz-Arigita et al., 2010). Furthermore, prodromal ADD patients with mild cognitive impairment (ADMCI) compared with controls showed a higher global ‘clustering coefficient’, while ADD patients presented a higher hub ‘participation coefficient’ in the inferior parietal cortex, prefrontal cortex, precuneus, and somatomotor cortex (Ng et al., 2021). By contrast, diffusion MRI showed the following opposite picture in ADD patients over Nold persons: (1) lower network segregation, as revealed by a higher ‘clustering coefficient’ (Yao et al., 2010; Daianu et al., 2013); (2) lower efficiency of the network structure in relation to memory and executive performances (Lo et al., 2010; Reijmer et al., 2013); and (3) lower network integration, as revealed by higher characteristic ‘path length’ (Lo et al., 2010; Yao et al., 2010).

It should be remarked that the rs-fMRI has a low temporal resolution of about 1 s, which is insufficient to investigate the interdependency between the emerging activity of neural brain populations at frequencies higher than 0.5 Hz. Therefore, electroencephalographic (EEG) techniques were used to explore that interdependency at a larger frequency spectrum, as they have a high temporal resolution of <1 ms, despite a moderate spatial resolution of centimeters (de Haan et al., 2012). Previous EEG studies showed abnormalities in several measures of the interrelatedness of rsEEG rhythms at electrode or source pairs. Compared with Nold persons, ADD patients presented lower ‘spectral coherence’ at alpha (8–12 Hz) and beta (13–20 Hz) rhythms, especially at temporo-parieto-occipital and fronto-parietooccipital electrode pairs; notably, ‘spectral coherence’ is the most popular linear measure of the interrelatedness of rsEEG activity (Leuchter et al., 1992; Dunkin et al., 1994; Locatelli et al., 1998; Jelic et al., 2000; Adler et al., 2003). Similarly, ADD and ADMCI patients exhibited lower interrelatedness of temporo-parieto-occipital and/or fronto-parietooccipital rsEEG alpha rhythms, as revealed by the following procedures: the ‘phase lag index’, a spectral measure of the phase difference distribution asymmetry (Stam et al., 2007b; Yu et al., 2016; Peraza et al., 2018), ‘synchronization likelihood’, a measure sensitive to both linear and non-linear interrelatedness of rsEEG activity (Babiloni et al., 2004b, 2006b), and ‘linear lagged connectivity’, a measure of the interrelatedness of rsEEG activity without the zero-lag component sensitive to the head volume conduction effects (Babiloni et al., 2018, 2019).

The above rsEEG findings were confirmed and extended by measures reflecting the directionality of the interrelatedness of rsEEG activity from one electrode/source to another, such as the ‘directed transfer function’ derived from Granger causality and autoregressive methods (Blinowska, 2011; Blinowska et al., 2017). ADD and ADMCI patients exhibited lower interrelatedness of the temporo-parieto-occipital and/or fronto-parietooccipital rsEEG alpha rhythms, as revealed by “directed transfer function” (Dauwels et al., 2009, 2010). Furthermore, there was a reduced prominence of the interrelatedness from parietal to frontal electrodes at the alpha and beta (13–35 Hz) rhythms (Babiloni et al., 2008, 2009a,b; Blinowska et al., 2017).

Concerning the rsEEG delta (<4 Hz) and/or theta (4–7 Hz) rhythms, most of the studies showed higher measures of the interrelatedness of topographically widespread rsEEG activity, intrahemispherically and inter-hemispherically; those measures were derived from the ‘spectral coherence’, ‘directed transfer function’, and ‘linear lagged connectivity’ (Babiloni et al., 2008, 2009a,b, 2010, 2018, 2019; Sankari et al., 2011; Canuet et al., 2012; Yu et al., 2016; Blinowska et al., 2017), with some exceptions (Knott et al., 2000; Adler et al., 2003).

Previous rsEEG studies also revealed the abnormal network topology of the interrelatedness of rsEEG rhythms at electrode/source pairs in ADD patients. Compared with Nold people, ADD and ADMCI patients showed a more random topology of the interrelatedness of rsEEG rhythms at an electrode or source pairs, possibly due to reduced ‘small worldness’ properties of brain networks (Reijneveld et al., 2007; Stam et al., 2007a; de Haan et al., 2009; Frantzidis et al., 2014; Vecchio et al., 2014, 2016; Hallett et al., 2020). This general effect was reported at the delta, alpha, and beta rhythms on the whole scalp (Stam et al., 2007a; de Haan et al., 2009; Vecchio et al., 2014, 2016) and in AD-vulnerable regions, such as the frontal and parietal regions (Frantzidis et al., 2014).

Moreover, beyond the ‘small worldness’ property, ADD patients were characterized by abnormalities in the following graph network indexes: (1) a shift of the ‘betweenness centrality’ center of mass from posterior to anterior alpha rhythms in relation to disease severity, as revealed by the ‘phase lag index’ (Engels et al., 2015); (2) a parietal and occipital loss of the network organization from theta and alpha rhythms, as revealed by the ‘phase lag index’ (Yu et al., 2016); (3) hub rearrangement and functioning at different rsEEG frequency bands, as revealed by several interrelatedness measures (Stam et al., 2007a; De Haan et al., 2009; Frantzidis et al., 2014; Engels et al., 2015; Song et al., 2019; Das and Puthankattil, 2022); (4) lower ‘global efficiency’, increased ‘local efficiency’, and lower resilience of cortical networks from the rsEEG alpha and beta rhythms, as revealed by the Granger ‘directed transfer function’ (Afshari and Jalili, 2017); and (5) reduced graph ‘local and global efficiency’ values from lower inward and outward directions of the interrelatedness derived from the whole-band rsEEG activity by another Granger measure based on a conditional multivariate vector autoregression model. Notably, the maximum abnormalities of the ‘hub degree’ were observed at parietal electrodes (Franciotti et al., 2019), whereas no changes in the global network organization from the whole-band rsEEG activity were found by ‘mutual information’ measures of that interrelatedness (Franciotti et al., 2022).

Considering the above rsEEG findings, both ADD and ADMCI patients showed reduced efficient information exchange in the cortical neural networks, as revealed by their more random topology. However, no previous study in those patients focused on the integrity of the parietal hubs derived from the rsEEG alpha rhythms, although it is well known that ADD patients show the following significant abnormalities: (1) impairment in the parietal nodes of the cortical DMN (Bokde et al., 2006; Sorg et al., 2007; Brier et al., 2012; Wang et al., 2015; Joo et al., 2016; Eyler et al., 2019; Talwar et al., 2021; Zhang et al., 2021); (2) reduced interrelatedness of the parietal rsEEG alpha rhythms electrode or source pairs (Leuchter et al., 1992; Locatelli et al., 1998; Jelic et al., 2000; Babiloni et al., 2004a, 2006a, 2018, 2019; Stam et al., 2007b; Yu et al., 2016; Peraza et al., 2018); and (3) reduced power density of the occipital and parietal rsEEG alpha rhythms (reviewed by Babiloni et al., 2021).

In ADD patients, the abnormal reduction in rsEEG alpha rhythms may be related to disorders in the regulation of quiet vigilance. This functional interpretation is based on, among others, the following findings: (1) in healthy volunteers, posterior (eyes closed) rsEEG alpha rhythms were modulated in amplitude after transcranial magnetic stimulations over angular gyrus, a core region of the DMN, but not over control regions of the dorsal attention network (Capotosto et al., 2012); (2) those rsEEG alpha rhythms also reduced in amplitude 1 min before the onset of sleep stage 1 (Morikawa et al., 2002); (3) furthermore, they decreased in amplitude and theta rhythms increased in amplitude during the transition from quiet vigilance to drowsiness, behaviorally tested by both EEG spectral measures and reaction time and decision making to auditory stimuli (Jagannathan et al., 2018, 2022); and (4) moreover, a night of sleep deprivation reduced the posterior rsEEG alpha rhythms in healthy volunteers and visual attention performances (placebo condition), whereas an acute dose of an amphetamine (experimental condition) after sleep deprivation recovered both the posterior EEG alpha rhythms and those performances (Del Percio et al., 2019).

To fill the above literature gap, the present study explored the integrity of the parietal graph-based hubs derived from the rsEEG alpha rhythms in mild-to-moderate ADD patients compared with Nold people. In the present study, all methods for estimating the directional (isolated lagged effective coherence, iCoh) and non-directional (linear lagged connectivity, LLC) interrelatedness of the rsEEG activity at electrode pairs are implemented in the freeware platform called eLORETA.1 Along the same line, the methods for computing the Graph Theory indexes are implemented in the freeware platform called GraphVar.2 These methods were chosen to (1) use different mathematical approaches to measure that interrelatedness, (2) compare the results, (3) promote open science, and (4) allow easier cross-validation of the present results in the future. Notably, we did not want to provide a methodological standard for applying graph theory analysis. Rather, we provided a proof of concept of how the results may be affected by different thresholds and criteria.

2. Materials and methods

2.1. Subjects

For the present study, we used the clinical and rsEEG data of 40 ADD patients and 40 control Nold people carefully matched for age, gender, and education and enrolled by clinical units of our Consortium (data from three ADD patients were irremediable artifacts or relevant missing data, so were not considered further). The Local institutional Ethics Committees approved the present study. All experiments were performed with the informed and overt consent of each participant or caregiver, in line with the Code of Ethics of the World Medical Association (Declaration of Helsinki) and the standards established by the local Institutional Review Board.

Table 1 summarizes the most relevant demographic (i.e., age, gender, and education) and clinical (i.e., MMSE score) features of the Nold and ADD participants. Furthermore, it shows the results of the statistical comparisons (p < 0.05) of age (t-test), gender (Fisher test), education (t-test), and MMSE score (Mann–Whitney U test) between the two groups. As expected, a statistically significant difference was found for the MMSE score (p < 0.001), indicating a higher score in the Nold group than the ADD group. No difference was found for the age, gender, and education between the two groups (p > 0.05 uncorrected).

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Demographic, clinical, neuropsychological, and neurophysiological characteristics of the normal elderly (Nold) subject and Alzheimer’s Disease with Dementia (ADD) patients enrolled in the present study.

In Table 1, the mean values of TF and IAF for the Nold and ADD groups, together with the results of the statistical comparisons between them (t-test), are also reported. No statistically significant differences were observed for TF and IAF values (p > 0.05 uncorrected).

2.2. Diagnostic criteria

In all clinical units, probable ADD was diagnosed based on the criteria of the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, fourth edition (DSM-IV-TR; American Psychiatric Association), and the National Institute of Neurological Disorders and Stroke-Alzheimer Disease and Related Disorders (NINCDS-ADRDA) working group (McKhann et al., 1984, 2011). Diagnostic criteria refer to the time period when diagnoses were performed. All ADD individuals underwent medical, neuropsychological, neurological, psychiatric, and neuroimaging evaluations, according to standard procedures at each center and based on the expertise of each clinician. The procedures followed by all clinical units included the Instrumental Activities of Daily Living scale (IADL; Lawton and Brody, 1969), the Mini-Mental State Examination (MMSE; Folstein et al., 1975), the Clinical Dementia Rating scale (CDR; Hughes et al., 1982), the Geriatric Depression Scale (GDS; Yesavage et al., 1982), and the Hachinski Ischemic Score scale (HIS; Rosen et al., 1980).

Inclusion criteria included the clinical diagnosis of AD based on the above procedures and the determination of a worsening episodic memory in the last 6 months, thus referring to patients with typical ADD clinical presentation. According to the Alzheimer’s Disease Neuroimaging Initiative (ADNI),3 the MMSE score had to be 24 or lower. Additionally, inclusion criteria included the visual analysis of structural T1-weighted magnetic resonance images (MRIs) by local radiologists; those images had to be compatible with ADD diagnosis (Albert et al., 2011). Cognitive deficits were assessed by standard neuropsychological tests in the domains of episodic memory, language, executive function/attention, and visuoconstruction abilities (local normative reference thresholds). Only some of the patients received the CERAD-plus battery. In general, the tests assessing episodic memory included the delayed recall of Rey figures (Rey, 1959) and/or the delayed recall of a story (Spinnler and Tognoni, 1987). The tests assessing language included the 1-min verbal fluency for letters, fruits, animals, or car trades (Novelli et al., 1986), and/or the Token test (Spinnler and Tognoni, 1987). The tests assessing executive function and attention included the Trail Making Test Part A and B (Reitan, 1958). Finally, the tests assessing visuoconstruction abilities included the copy of Rey figures (Rey, 1959). This inhomogeneity derived from the retrospective nature of the study, with data collected during a clinical routine at each center.

Exclusion criteria included major neuropsychiatric disorders and other types or causes of dementia, such as frontotemporal dementia (Rascovsky et al., 2011), vascular dementia diagnosed based on the National Institute of Neurological Disorders and Stroke and Association Internationale pour la Recherché et l’Enseignement en Neurosciences (NINDS-AIREN) working group (Gorelick et al., 2011), Parkinson disease (Gelb et al., 1999), dementia with Lewy Bodies (McKeith et al., 2005), metabolic syndrome, nutritional deficits, tumors, epilepsy, etc. Exclusion criteria also included visual analysis of structural T2-weighted MRIs by local radiologists to exclude major cerebrovascular lesions, as well as the chronic use of psychoactive drugs except for acetylcholinesterase inhibitors (all patients chronically took them) and/or NMDA receptor antagonists.

The Nold participants received a cognitive, physical, and neurological examination to exclude the presence of cognitive deficits and psychiatric disorders. According to ADNI, the MMSE score had to be 27 or higher. Additionally, all Nold participants had a GDS score lower than the threshold of 5 (no depression) or were verified as not having depression after an interview with a physician or clinical psychologist. Those affected by chronic systemic illnesses (e.g., diabetes mellitus) were excluded, as well as participants receiving chronic psychoactive drugs. Assessed Nold people were also excluded if they had, currently or historically, neurological or psychiatric diseases and drug or alcohol abuse issues.

In ADD and Nold participants, pharmacological administration (when planned) of routine drugs was postponed until after the rsEEG recordings and performed in hospital settings in the morning. Although this procedure did not guarantee a full washout of the drugs, it synchronized the timing of drug administration. Longer periods of suspension would not have been valid for obvious ethical reasons.

2.3. Resting state eyes-closed electroencephalographic recordings

In all clinical units, the Nold and ADD participants were kindly asked to stay relaxed with their eyes closed during the experiments. They were also kindly asked not to move or talk and keep their mind wandering without focused mentalization. During the experimental recordings, the researchers controlled for the subject’s behavioral condition and ongoing rsEEG traces (specifically the amplitude of alpha waves on posterior regions and the onset of slow-wave activity in frontal regions), helping the participants to keep an adequate level of vigilance (i.e., avoiding drowsiness and sleep onset). These alarms were annotated in the protocol for the preliminary rsEEG data analysis phase. The above instructions and procedures were similar in all clinical units even if the respective protocols were not identical.

At least 5 min of electrophysiological data were recorded by professional digital EEG systems authorized for clinical applications (i.e., EB-Neuro Be-light, Micromed, Brain Product, etc.). For this purpose, 19 exploring scalp electrodes were placed according to the 10–20 montage system (i.e., Fp1, Fp2, F7, F3, Fz, F4, F8, T7, C3, Cz, C4, T8, P7, P3, Pz, P4, P8, O1, and O2; Figure 1).

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Scalp electrode positioning of the 19 electrodes according to the international standard 10–20.

The ground electrodes were placed in the posterior midline, while the reference electrodes were located in different positions across participating clinical units (i.e., linked earlobes, mastoids, vertex, etc.), in line with local standard protocols and clinical trials. During the rsEEG recordings, scalp electrode impedances were kept below 5 KOhm. The rsEEG recordings were performed using 128 Hz or a higher sampling rate (i.e., 128–1,024 Hz) with an adequate antialiasing band pass between 0.01 Hz and 60–100 Hz.

In addition to the rsEEG recording, bipolar vertical and horizontal electrooculographic (EOG) signals and one-channel electrocardiographic (ECG) signals were also acquired using the same sampling frequency adopted for recording the rsEEG data (128–1,024 Hz). Consequently, rsEEG, EOG, and ECG signals had the same sampling rate, so EOG and ECG signals could be used for artifact detection and their off-line correction when adequate.

As mentioned above, some rsEEG datasets were recorded using a relatively low sampling frequency of 128 Hz (i.e., 6 out of 40 rsEEG datasets collected for the Nold group and 4 out of 37 rsEEG datasets collected for the ADD group). It should be remarked that such a sampling frequency is suboptimal for an ideal reconstruction of rsEEG signal beyond 40 Hz without aliasing. Ideally, a factor of 3–4 between the low-band pass limit and the rsEEG sampling frequency should be set.

2.4. Preliminary rsEEG data analysis

Data analysis was centrally performed by the group located at the Department of Physiology and Pharmacology ‘Erspamer’ of Sapienza University of Rome, Italy. In the preliminary analysis, the rsEEG data were split into 2-s epochs and analyzed off-line. This segmentation allowed the use of standard toolboxes for the spectral analysis of rsEEG activity, such as fast Fourier transform (FFT) implemented in the official eLORETA platform. This analysis assumes the stationarity of rsEEG activity. Furthermore, it allowed for the minimization of the rejection of rsEEG data for artifactual activity. The use of those procedures allowed a better understanding of the present results in light of previous reference evidence of the PDWAVES Consortium (Babiloni et al., 2015, 2016; Lizio et al., 2016; more information can be found at www.pdwaves.eu), but it implied the focus on the linear components of rsEEG signals.

Two independent researchers (GN and RL) performed a visual analysis of EOG and rsEEG data blind to the clinical diagnosis associated with the electrophysiological datasets. They rejected those with artifacts due to instruments, electronic noise, head–neck movements, and face muscle tension. They also rejected rsEEG epochs with amplitude values exceeding 100 μV. Particular attention was given to the contamination of saccades and blinking on electrophysiological data recorded by frontal (i.e., F7, F3, Fz, F4, and F8) and frontopolar (Fp1 and Fp2) electrodes. This specific exam was based on the comparison of EOG and rsEEG traces. The rsEEG epochs with artifacts marked as eye movements and blinking were provided as inputs to a software toolbox based on an autoregressive model for their possible correction (MATLAB 6.5, MathWorks Inc.). Technical details and performances of this procedure have been reported elsewhere (Moretti et al., 2003) and validated in several previous studies by the present research group (Babiloni et al., 2004a, 2006a, 2008). Of note, the outcome of this procedure was visually revised by the two researchers (G. N. and R. L.). All Nold and ADD datasets showed less than 25% of artifact-free rsEEG epochs, without significant differences between the Nold and ADD groups (t-test, p > 0.05, two tails). More specifically, the total number of artifact-free epochs was as follows: 135 ± 11 (SE) epochs for the Nold group and 115 ± 8 (SE) epochs for the ADD group, with a total duration spanning between 3.5 and 4.5 min, respectively.

To harmonize rsEEG data recorded using different reference electrodes and sampling frequency rates, artifact-free rsEEG epochs were off-line frequency-band passed at 0.1–45 Hz and downsampled, when appropriate, to make the sampling rate of all artifact-free rsEEG datasets in the Nold and ADD participants equal to 128 Hz. For the sake of harmonization of all datasets, the recorded rsEEG data were re-referenced to the common average reference.

2.5. The spectral analysis of rsEEG epochs

A standard digital FFT-based analysis (Welch technique, Hanning windowing function, no phase shift) computed the power density of scalp rsEEG rhythms (0.5 Hz of frequency resolution). As mentioned above, only rsEEG epochs free from artifacts were used.

The EEG frequency bands of interest were individually identified based on the following frequency landmarks: the transition frequency (TF) and the individual alpha frequency (IAF) peak (Klimesch, 1999). In the EEG power density spectrum, the TF marks the transition frequency between the theta and alpha bands, defined as the minimum of the rsEEG power density between 3 and 8 Hz (between the delta and the alpha power peak). The IAF is defined as the maximum power density peak between 6 and 14 Hz. These frequency landmarks were previously well described by Dr. Wolfgang Klimesch (Klimesch, 1996, 1999; Klimesch et al., 1998). Specifically, the TF and IAF were measured on averaged rsEEG power density spectra at parietal and occipital electrodes.

The TF and IAF were computed for each subject involved in the study. Based on the TF and IAF, we estimated the individual delta, theta, and alpha bands as follows: delta from TF −4 Hz to TF −2 Hz, theta from TF −2 Hz to TF, low-frequency alpha (alpha 1 and alpha 2) from TF to IAF, and high-frequency alpha (or alpha 3) from IAF to IAF + 2 Hz. Specifically, the individual alpha 1 and alpha 2 bands were computed as follows: alpha 1 from TF to the frequency midpoint of the TF-IAF range and alpha 2 from that midpoint to IAF.

The other bands were defined based on the standard fixed frequency ranges used in the reference study series (reviewed by Babiloni et al., 2021): beta 1 from 14 to 20 Hz, beta 2 from 20 to 30 Hz, and gamma from 30 to 40 Hz. See Supplementary Figure 1 in the Supplementary materials for the graphical representation of the above-mentioned frequency bands.

Of note, important aspects of the procedure were as follows:

(1) The alpha band was divided into sub-bands because, in the rsEEG data, dominant low-frequency alpha rhythms (alpha 1 and alpha 2) may denote the synchronization of diffuse neural networks regulating the fluctuation of the subject’s global awake and conscious states, while high-frequency alpha rhythms (alpha 3) may denote the synchronization of more selective neural networks specialized in the processing of modal specific or semantic information (Pfurtscheller and Klimesch, 1992; Klimesch, 1999). When the subject is engaged in sensorimotor or cognitive tasks, alpha and low-frequency beta (beta 1) rhythms do reduce in power (i.e., desynchronization or blocking) and are replaced by fast EEG oscillations at high-frequency beta (beta 2) and gamma rhythms (Pfurtscheller and Klimesch, 1992).

(2) We considered individual delta, theta, and alpha frequency bands because a clinical group may be characterized by a mean slowing in the peak frequency of the alpha power density without any substantial change in the magnitude of the power density. In that specific case, the use of fixed frequency bands would result in a statistical effect erroneously showing alpha power density values lower in the clinical group than in the control group. In some specific cases, the groups of AD patients and control participants may not show statistically significant differences in the mean values of TF and IAF. Nevertheless, we used those values as a research model to allow the identification of delta, theta, low-frequency alpha bands, and high-frequency alpha bands on an individual basis to ensure the spectral measures were accurate within those bands, in line with our reference rsEEG studies performed in patients with AD and related neurodegenerative disorders (Babiloni et al., 2017, 2018, 2019, 2020).

(3) Fixed frequency ranges were used for the beta and gamma bands because the individual beta and gamma frequency peaks were only evident in a few subjects (<10%).

(4) We selected the beginning of the beta frequency range at 14 Hz to avoid overlapping between individual alpha and fixed beta frequency ranges (i.e., the individual alpha frequency band ranged from TF to 14 Hz with an IAF of 12 Hz).

During rsEEG recording, very careful attention was paid to the amplitude of alpha rhythms on posterior regions and the abnormal slow wave on frontal regions. Overall, specific spectral features should be respected, namely:

(1) The physiological decrease of the EEG power density after the IAFp as a function of the increase of the frequencies in the range of 1–40 Hz (related to residual muscular activity);

(2) The absence of an offset of power density across all frequencies at some scalp electrodes (especially visible as big differences in gamma rsEEG power density among the ROI);

(3) The absence of several peaks of high-power density in the range of 1–40 Hz; and.

(4) Visible IAFp in the range between 6 Hz and 14 Hz, especially on posterior regions.

We carefully checked the presence of IAFp in the present cohort of AD patients, as in a mild-to-moderate dementia stage, AD neuropathology should not impair the neurophysiological synchronizing mechanism inducing a total disruption of IAFp. If an IAFp was not clearly present, we attributed the cause to substantial artifacts rather than AD neuropathology.

2.6. Estimation of linear lagged connectivity (LLC) and isolated lagged effective coherence (iCoh)

As mentioned above, LLC and iCoh are two complementary and mathematically independent approaches available at the freeware platform called LORETA (see technical details at https://www.uzh.ch/keyinst/loreta; Pascual-Marqui, 2007) for measuring the interrelatedness of rsEEG activity at electrode (source) pairs. Comparing the results with two techniques probed the intrinsic variability of this kind of readout and allowed us to select and discuss the one that was most consistent.

LLC belongs to the popular bivariate techniques that compute the non-directional interrelatedness of rsEEG activity at electrode pairs (e.g., spectral coherence, phase lag index, synchronization likelihood, etc.) without considering the interrelatedness of rsEEG activity across the other electrode (source) pairs. It has the conceptual advantage of not considering the interrelatedness of the rsEEG activity at the zero-lag phase, which may be affected by the instantaneous spread of the electric field to the well-known head volume conduction effects (Pascual-Marqui et al., 2011).

By contrast, iCoh belongs to a group of techniques based on an autoregressive model that computes the directional interrelatedness of rsEEG activity at electrode pairs (e.g., spectral coherence, phase lag index, synchronization likelihood, etc.), removing the linear component of the interrelatedness of rsEEG activity across the other electrode (source) pairs. It has the conceptual advantage of being multivariate (as opposed to bivariate) and exploring the directionality of that interrelatedness (Pascual-Marqui et al., 2014).

Using the iCoh procedure, we obtained not only a ‘directional’ measure of the interdependencies of rsEEG rhythms at electrode pairs but also a ‘non-directional’ measure. The directional measure was computed as the absolute difference of the iCoh values between the two ‘directions’, while the ‘non-directional’ measure was obtained by the mean of the two ‘directional’ values. The latter measure allowed cross-validation of (‘non-directional’) LLC measures.

For each participant, LLC, mean iCoh between the two ‘directions’, and the absolute difference of the iCoh values for the two ‘directions’ were calculated at each frequency bin between 0.5 and 45 Hz (matrix of 19 rows × 19 columns). LLC and iCoh values within the frequency bands individually identified based on the TF and IAF landmarks were averaged to obtain delta, theta, alpha 1, alpha 2, and alpha 3 bands. LLC and iCoh values for beta 1, beta 2, and gamma LLC were based on fixed frequency bands, as mentioned above.

To reduce statistical comparisons, we averaged the LLC or iCoh values calculated between scalp electrode pairs for regions of Interests (ROI). Specifically, we considered frontal, central, parietal, temporal, and occipital ROI. For each frequency band, LLC or iCoh values for interhemispheric comparisons were calculated as follows: (1) frontal ROI, mean values of Fp1-Fp2, F3-F4, and F7-F8 electrodes; (2) central ROI, the values of C3-C4 electrodes; (3) parietal ROI, the values of P3-P4 electrodes; (4) temporal ROI, mean values of T7-T8 and P7-P8 electrodes; and (5) occipital ROI, the values of O1-O2 electrodes.

Similarly, for each frequency band, LLC or iCoh values for intrahemispheric comparisons were calculated as follows: (1) left frontal ROI, mean values of electrode pairs between Fp1, F3, and F7 electrodes and all the left hemispheric electrodes; (2) right frontal ROI, mean values of electrode pairs between Fp2, F4, and F8 electrodes and all the right hemispheric electrodes; (3) left central ROI, mean values of electrode pairs of the left hemi-scalp involving the C3 electrode and all the left hemispheric electrodes; (4) right central ROI, mean values of electrode pairs of the right hemi-scalp involving the C4 electrode and all the right hemispheric electrodes; (5) left parietal ROI, mean values of electrode pairs of the left hemi-scalp involving the P3 electrode and all the left hemispheric electrodes; (6) right parietal ROI, mean values of electrode pairs of the right hemi-scalp involving the P4 electrode and all the right hemispheric electrodes; (7) left temporal ROI, mean values of electrode pairs of the left hemi-scalp involving the T7 and P7 electrodes and all the left hemispheric electrodes; (8) right temporal ROI, mean values of electrode pairs of the right hemi-scalp involving the T6 and P8 electrodes and all the right hemispheric electrodes; (9) left occipital ROI, mean values of electrode pairs of the left hemi-scalp involving the O1 electrode and all the left hemispheric electrodes; and (10) right parietal ROI, mean values of electrode pairs of the right hemi-scalp involving the O2 electrode and all the right hemispheric electrodes.

2.7. Graph theory analysis of LLC and iCoh values

For each participant, the LLC, mean iCoh, and absolute difference of iCoh values at the frequency bands showing statistically significant differences between the ADD and Nold groups were used as input for the graph theory analysis. This analysis was performed using the GraphVar 2.0 software platform (Waller et al., 2018).

For this purpose, matrices of LLC, mean iCoh, and absolute difference of iCoh values were converted into binary matrices having ‘0’ or ‘1’ in the cells. LLC or iCoh values associated with ‘1’ were considered as ‘significant’ and considered for the computation of the graph indexes of interest in the Nold and ADD groups. Notably, we converted the LLC and iCoh matrices into binary (‘1’ and ‘0’) graphs to (1) mitigate the inclusion of ‘spurious’ interdependencies of rsEEG rhythms at electrode pairs and (2) compare graphs with the same number of those interdependencies for the Nold and ADD groups.

For the identification of the ‘significant’ values of LLC or iCoh (‘1’ in the binary matrices), two arbitrary percentage thresholds were used, namely 10% (0.1) and 20% (0.2). For each frequency band and group of participants (Nold and ADD), the threshold at 10% did set to ‘1’ the 10% of the highest values of LLC (iCoh), considering all electrode pairs, and ‘0’ for the remaining ones. This procedure was repeated for LLC, mean iCoh, and the absolute difference of iCoh values. Specifically, 10% of the highest values of LLC corresponded to 17 electrode pairs. The same number of electrode pairs was true for (‘non-directional’) mean iCoh and the absolute difference of the two ‘directional’ iCoh values.

Following the same procedure, the threshold at 20% did set to ‘1’ the 20% of the highest values of LLC (iCoh), considering all electrode pairs, and ‘0’ for the remaining ones. Specifically, 20% of the highest values of (‘non-directional’) LLC corresponded to 34 electrode pairs. Again, this procedure was repeated for LLC, mean iCoh, and the absolute difference of iCoh values. As another step of the graph theory analysis, we arbitrarily used the nodal degree (ND), participation coefficient (PC), and local clustering coefficient (CC) graph indexes to scalp electrodes as degree hubs and then differentiate them into provincial and connector hubs.

For this purpose, ND was defined as the number of links (i.e., ‘significant’ interdependencies of rsEEG rhythms at electrode pairs represented as ‘1’ in the previously mentioned binary matrices) characterizing a given node (electrode). Among nodes with a high number of links (high-degree nodes = degree hubs), PC denoted the discriminant feature of their connection profile. In general, provincial hubs primarily link other nodes located within a single network region. By contrast, connector hubs predominantly link nodes located in several network regions (Sporns et al., 2007; Power et al., 2013). Here, this classification as provincial hub vs. connector hub was further confirmed by the CC index, which is a measure of the tendency of network nodes to form local clusters. High CC values mainly characterize provincial hubs rather than connector hubs.

In the present experimental context, we operationally defined degree hubs and then differentiated them into provincial and connector hubs using the following three approaches:

(1) Hubs were defined according to ND and classified into connector and provincial hubs according to the PC and CC calculated at 0.1 and 0.2 graph thresholds, in line with the Franciotti and Bonanni approach.

(2) Hubs were defined according to ND and classified into connector and provincial hubs according to the PC and betweenness centrality (BC) calculated at 0.1 and 0.2 graph thresholds, in line with the approach described by Cole et al. (2015).

(3) Hubs were defined according to the within-module degree z-score and classified into connector and provincial hubs according to the PC calculated at 0.1 and 0.2 graph thresholds, in line with the approach described by Power et al. (2013).

Furthermore, we used the four following alternative criteria for testing the consistency of the results (they were applied for each frequency band and each group of participants):

(1) According to the first criterion, degree hubs were defined as nodes (electrodes) with ND (number of links) higher than one standard deviation (SD) from the group mean of significant node links (i.e., ‘1’ in the previously mentioned binary matrices) within the network (electrode montage). Provincial hubs were then defined as degree hubs with an ND and a CC higher than one SD and PC lower than one SD from the group mean of significant node links within the network. Connector hubs were defined as degree hubs (electrodes) with an ND and a PC higher than one SD from the network mean and a CC lower than one SD from the group mean of significant node links within the network.

(2) According to the second criterion, degree hubs were defined as nodes with an ND higher than the 80th percentile from the group mean of significant node links within the network. Provincial hubs were then defined as degree hubs with an ND and a CC higher than the 80th percentile and a PC lower than the 20th percentile from the group mean of significant node links within the network. Connector hubs were defined as degree hubs with an ND and a PC higher than the 80th percentile from the network mean and a CC lower than the 20th percentile from the group mean of significant node links within the network.

(3) According to the third criterion, degree hubs were defined as nodes with an ND higher than the 70th percentile from the group mean of significant node links within the network. Provincial hubs were then defined as degree hubs with an ND and a CC higher than the 70th percentile and a PC lower than the 30th percentile from the group mean of significant node links within the network. Connector hubs were defined as degree hubs with an ND and a PC higher than the 70th percentile from the network mean and a CC lower than the 30th percentile from the group mean of significant node links within the network.

(4) According to the fourth criterion, degree hubs were defined as nodes with an ND higher than one standard error of the mean (SEM) from the group mean of significant node links within the network. Provincial hubs were then defined as degree hubs with an ND and a CC higher than one SEM and a PC lower than one SEM from the group mean of significant node links within the network. Connector hubs were defined as degree hubs with an ND and a PC higher than one SEM from the network mean and a CC lower than one SEM from the group mean of significant node links within the network.

All results obtained with the above criteria are reported in detail in the tables featured in the Supplementary materials (see Results). Of note, the selection of the criteria was performed to provide an index of the result variability using different mathematical threshold definitions.

2.8. Directionality of degree hubs by iCoh values

To evaluate the directionality of the interdependencies of rsEEG rhythms between degree hubs (electrodes), ‘directional’ iCoh values for pairs of those hubs were calculated at each frequency bin between 0.5 and 45 Hz and for each participant of the ADD and Nold groups. Then, these iCoh values were averaged according to individual frequency bands from delta to alpha 3. To limit the statistical comparisons, the subsequent analysis was focused on individual delta, alpha 2, and alpha 3 bands, which are typically abnormal in rsEEG rhythms recorded in ADD patients (Babiloni et al., 2021). For each frequency band and group of participants, the global output (outward) value of a given degree hub was obtained averaging all output iCoh values from it to the other degree hubs. The global input (inward) value of that degree hub was obtained averaging all input iCoh values to it coming from the other degree hubs.

2.9. Statistical analysis

To evaluate the study hypotheses, the following statistical sessions were performed by the commercial tool STATISTICA 10 (StatSoft Inc.).4 As analysis of variance (ANOVA) implies that dependent variables have Gaussian distributions, we tested this feature with the LLC and iCoh values using a Kolmogorov–Smirnov test (null hypothesis of non-Gaussian distributions tested at p > 0.05). Both the LLC and iCoh values showed non-Gaussian distributions, so we Log10 transformed them and retested Gaussian status. Such a transformation is a popular method for transforming a skewed data distribution with all positive values, such as LLC and iCoh values, to Gaussian distributions, as required when using ANOVA. Indeed, the outcome of the procedure did approximate the distributions of LLC and iCoh values to Gaussian distributions (p > 0.05), allowing the use of the ANOVA model.

For the session using ANOVAs, Mauchly’s test evaluated the sphericity assumption, and degrees of freedom were corrected using the Greenhouse–Geisser procedure when appropriate (p < 0.05). the Duncan test was used for post-hoc comparisons (p < 0.05, corrected for multiple comparisons).

The results of the following ANOVAs were controlled by the iterative (leave-one-out) Grubbs’ test detecting for the presence of one or more outliers in the distribution of the LLC and iCoh values showing the significant effects in relation to the study hypotheses. The null hypothesis of the non-outlier status was tested at the arbitrary threshold of p > 0.001 to remove only values with the highest probability of being outliers.

In the first statistical session, we evaluated whether the LLC, mean iCoh, and absolute difference of iCoh interhemispheric values may differ between the ADD and Nold groups at parietal delta and alpha rhythms. To this aim, we developed three ANOVA designs with the Log10-transformed LLC, mean iCoh, and absolute difference of iCoh values as dependent variables, respectively. The factors were group (Nold, ADD; independent variable), ROI (frontal, central, parietal, temporal, and occipital), and band (delta, theta, alpha 1, alpha 2, alpha 3, beta 1, beta 2, and gamma). The confirmation of the hypothesis would require (1) a statistically significant ANOVA interaction, including the factors group, ROI, and band (p < 0.05), and (2) a post-hoc Duncan test indicating statistically significant (p < 0.05) differences in the LLC values at parietal delta and alpha rhythms between the Nold and ADD groups (i.e., Nold ≠ ADD, p < 0.05).

In the second session, we evaluated whether the LLC, mean iCoh, and absolute difference of iCoh intrahemispheric values may differ between the ADD and Nold groups at delta and alpha rhythms within the two hemispheres. To this aim, we developed three ANOVA designs with the Log10-transformed LLC, mean iCoh, and absolute difference of iCoh values as dependent variables, respectively. The factors were group (Nold, ADD; independent variable), hemisphere (left, right), ROI (frontal, central, parietal, temporal, and occipital), and band (delta, theta, alpha 1, alpha 2, alpha 3, beta 1, beta 2, and gamma). The confirmation of the hypothesis would require (1) a statistically significant ANOVA interaction, including the factors group, ROI, and band (p < 0.05), and (2) a post-hoc Duncan test indicating statistically significant (p < 0.05) differences in the LLC values at parietal delta and alpha rhythms between the Nold and ADD groups (i.e., Nold ≠ ADD, p < 0.05).

In the third session, we evaluated whether the global output and input iCoh values of parietal degree hubs (identified by the previous graph theory analysis) may differ between the ADD and Nold groups at delta and alpha rhythms. We also evaluated whether, within each group, a difference between global output and input iCoh values for degree hubs could be observed. To these aims, for each frequency band, we developed an ANOVA design with the Log10-transformed global iCoh values as a dependent variable and the group (Nold, ADD; independent variable), hub (all degree hubs), and direction (output, input) as factors. The confirmation of the hypothesis would require (1) a statistically significant ANOVA interaction, including the factors group, hub, and direction (p < 0.05), (2) a post-hoc Duncan test indicating statistically significant (p < 0.05) between-group differences in the parietal iCoh values at delta and alpha rhythms between the Nold and ADD groups (i.e., Nold ≠ ADD, p < 0.05), and (3) a post-hoc Duncan test indicating statistically significant (p < 0.05) within-group differences between the global output and input iCoh values (i.e., output ≠ input, p < 0.05).

3. Results

3.1. Demographic, clinical, neuropsychological, and rsEEG features in the Nold and Add groups

Table 1 summarizes the most relevant demographic (i.e., age, gender, and education) and clinical (i.e., MMSE score) features of the groups of Nold (N = 40) and ADD (N = 37) participants. Furthermore, it reports results of the statistical comparison (p < 0.05) of age (t-test), gender (Fisher test), education (t-test), and MMSE score (Mann–Whitney U test) between the two groups. As expected, a statistically significant difference was found for the MMSE score (p < 0.001), indicating a higher score in the Nold group than in the ADD group. No difference was found for age, gender, and education between the two groups (p > 0.05 uncorrected).

In Table 1, the mean values of TF and IAF for the Nold and ADD groups, together with the results of the statistical comparisons between them (t-test), are also reported. No statistically significant differences were observed for TF and IAF values (p > 0.05 uncorrected).

3.2. Interdependencies of rsEEG rhythms at parietal electrode pairs as revealed by LLC and iCoh values

Results showed a statistically significant ANOVA interaction (F[28, 2,100] = 2.25, p < 0.05) in interhemispheric LLC values among the factors group (Nold and ADD; independent variable), ROI (frontal, central, parietal, temporal, and occipital), and band (delta, theta, alpha 1, alpha 2, alpha 3, beta 1, beta 2, and gamma; Supplementary Figure 2). Compared with the Nold group, the ADD group was mainly characterized by (1) lower alpha 2 and alpha 3 LLC values at parietal and temporal ROI and (2) higher delta LLC values at frontal, parietal, and occipital ROI (p < 0.05). No significant ANOVA effect was observed in the interhemispheric iCoh values (p > 0.05; see Supplementary Figures 3, 4).

Additionally, a statistically significant ANOVA interaction (F[28, 2,100] = 3.32, p < 0.05) in intrahemispheric (non-directional) LLC values among the factors group, ROI, and band (Supplementary Figure 5) was observed. Compared with the Nold group, the ADD group was mainly characterized by (1) lower LLC alpha 2 and alpha 3 values at the central, parietal, and occipital ROI and (2) higher LLC delta values at frontal, parietal, and occipital ROI (p < 0.05).

Another statistically significant ANOVA interaction (F[28, 2,100] = 1.91, p < 0.05) was found in intrahemispheric (non-directional) mean iCoh values among the factors group, hemisphere, ROI, and band (Supplementary Figure 6). No significant post-hoc effect was observed in the planned tests (p > 0.05). There was just a trend for lower left-parietal iCoh alpha 2 and alpha 3 values in the ADD group compared with the Nold group.

Finally, a statistically significant ANOVA interaction (F[28, 2,100] = 2.13, p < 0.05) was found in the intrahemispheric (directional) absolute difference of iCoh values among the factors group, hemisphere, ROI, and band (Supplementary Figure 7). Again, no significant post-hoc effect was observed in the planned tests (p > 0.05) but there was a trend showing lower left-parietal iCoh values in the ADD group compared with the Nold group.

Table 2 reports all the results from the planned post-hoc tests (p < 0.05) for the above significant ANOVA effects. Notably, the above findings based on LLC and iCoh values were not due to outliers, as shown by Grubbs’ test with an arbitrary threshold of p > 0.001 (see Supplementary Figures 8, 9, respectively).

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Post-hoc p-values (Duncan test) relative to the ANOVA interaction effects on the global output and input isolated lagged effective coherence (iCoh) in the alpha 2 and alpha 3 bands.

Globally, the above LLC and iCoh findings showed that interdependencies of rsEEG alpha 2 and alpha 3 rhythms at parietal electrode pairs were lower in the ADD group than in the Nold group. By contrast, results on interdependencies of rsEEG delta rhythms at scalp electrode pairs were inconsistent considering LLC and iCoh measures, so we did not use those measures for the graph hub analysis.

3.3. Parietal graph degree hubs from LLC and iCoh values at alpha rhythms

In the Supplementary material Tables 2–7 report detailed results about the graph degree hubs derived from LLC and iCoh alpha 2 and alpha 3 values computed in the Nold and ADD groups. As explained in the Materials and Methods section, those degree hubs were defined by the ND graph index using four different quantitative thresholds of qualification (i.e., mean + 1 SD, 80th percentile, 70th percentile, mean + 1 SEM).

Figure 2 illustrates those graph degree hubs computed from LLC and iCoh alpha 2 and alpha 3 values in the Nold and ADD groups. For sake of concision, the 0.1 and 0.2 thresholds are displayed in the same figure.

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Degree hubs for the alpha 2 and alpha 3 linear lagged connectivity (LLC, left column), mean (middle column), and absolute difference isolated lagged effective coherence (iCoh, right column) values in the Nold and ADD groups defined according to nodal degree (ND) calculated at 0.1 and 0.2 graph thresholds. Colors correspond to the different criteria adopted (mean and SD, 80th percentile, 70th percentile, mean and SEM).

Although there was a certain spread of degree hubs over the scalp at alpha 2 and alpha 3 bands, LLC and iCoh measures showed converging evidence of parietal degree hubs at these bands in both the Nold and ADD groups. Even using the most conservative criterion for the degree hub qualification (i.e., mean + 1 SD), consistent degree hubs from alpha 2 and alpha 3 rhythms were observed at parietal electrodes (i.e., P3, Pz, and P4). Notably, the iCoh values showed no substantial between-group differences in the topology of the parietal degree hubs at the alpha 2 and alpha 3 bands.

3.4. Parietal graph connector hubs from LLC and iCoh values at alpha 2 and alpha 3 bands

In the Supplementary material Tables 8–13 report detailed results about the graph connector and provincial hubs derived from LLC and iCoh alpha 2 and alpha 3 values computed in the Nold and ADD groups. Notably, the results showed no substantial graph provincial hub in the two groups.

Figures 3, 4 illustrate the localization of the graph connector hubs at the alpha 2 and alpha 3 bands in the Nold and ADD groups. For sake of concision, such a localization was computed considering the 0.1 and 0.2 thresholds together. There was a certain spread of connector hubs over the scalp at those bands. However, LLC and iCoh measures showed convergent evidence of parietal connector hubs at alpha 2 and alpha 3 bands in both the Nold and ADD groups. Even using the most conservative criterion for the connector hub qualification (i.e., mean + 1 SD), consistent connector hubs for alpha 2 and alpha 3 bands were observed at parietal electrodes (i.e., P3, Pz, and P4). Notably, the iCoh values showed no substantial between-group differences in the topology of the parietal connector hubs at the alpha 2 and alpha 3 bands.

FIGURE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 3. Connector and provincial hubs for the alpha 2 linear lagged connectivity (LLC, left column), mean (middle column), and absolute difference isolated lagged effective coherence (iCoh, right column) values in the Nold and ADD groups defined according to the three approaches used in the present study. Upper row: hubs were defined according to nodal degree (ND) and classified into connector and provincial hubs according to the participation coefficient (PC) and clustering coefficient (CC) calculated at 0.1 and 0.2 graph thresholds, in line with the approach described by Franciotti and Bonanni. Middle row: hubs were defined according to nodal degree (ND) and classified into connector and provincial hubs according to the participation coefficient (PC) and betweenness centrality (BC) calculated at 0.1 and 0.2 graph thresholds, in line with the approach described by Cole et al. (2015). Lower row: hubs were defined according to the within-module degree z-score and classified into connector and provincial hubs according to the participation coefficient (PC) calculated at 0.1 and 0.2 graph thresholds, in line with the approach described by Power et al. (2013). Colors correspond to the different criteria adopted (mean and SD, 80th/20th percentile, 70th/30th percentile, mean and SEM).

FIGURE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 4. Connector and provincial hubs for the alpha 3 linear lagged connectivity (LLC, left column), mean (middle column), and absolute difference isolated lagged effective coherence (iCoh, right column) values in the Nold and ADD groups defined according to the three approaches used in the present study. Upper row: hubs were defined according to nodal degree (ND) and classified into connector and provincial hubs according to the participation coefficient (PC) and clustering coefficient (CC) calculated at 0.1 and 0.2 graph thresholds, in line with the approach described by Franciotti and Bonanni. Middle row: hubs were defined according to nodal degree (ND) and classified in connector and provincial hubs according to the participation coefficient (PC) and betweenness centrality (BC) calculated at 0.1 and 0.2 graph thresholds, in line the approach described by Cole et al. (2015). Lower row: hubs were defined according to the within-module degree z-score and classified into connector and provincial hubs according to the participation coefficient (PC) calculated at 0.1 and 0.2 graph thresholds, in line with the approach described by Power et al. (2013). Colors correspond to the different criteria adopted (mean and SD, 80th/20th percentile, 70th/30th percentile, mean and SEM).

3.5. Directionality of hubs from LLC and iCoh values at alpha 2 and alpha 3 bands

Figures 5, 6 plot the global output (outward) and input (inward) iCoh alpha 2 and alpha 3 values at all electrodes denoted as a degree or a connector hub in the above analysis. Results showed a statistically significant ANOVA interaction of the iCoh alpha 2 values among group (Nold and ADD; independent variable), hub (electrodes with hub features), and direction (output and input) factors (F[10, 750] = 2.42, p < 0.05). Compared with the Nold group, the ADD group showed lower output global iCoh alpha 2 values at parietal electrodes (i.e., P3 and Pz; p < 0.05).

FIGURE 5
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 5. Global isolated lagged effective coherence (iCoh) values (mean across subjects ± SEM) in the alpha 2 frequency band within electrodes classified as substantial degree or connector hubs relative to a statistically significant ANOVA interaction (F [10, 750] = 2.42, p < 0.05) among the group (Nold and ADD), hubs (Fp2, F3, F4, F7, P3, P4, Pz, P7, P8, O1, and O2), and direction (output and input) factors. No statistically significant outliers were found according to Grubbs’ test (p < 0.0001).

FIGURE 6
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 6. Global isolated lagged effective coherence (iCoh) values (mean across subjects ± SEM) in the alpha 3 frequency band within electrodes classified as substantial degree or connector hubs relative to a statistically significant ANOVA interaction (F [10, 750] = 2.48, p < 0.05) among the group (Nold and ADD), hubs (Fp2, F3, F4, F7, P3, P4, Pz, P7, P8, O1, and O2), and direction (output and input) factors. No statistically significant outliers were found according to Grubbs’ test (p < 0.0001).

A statistically significant ANOVA interaction of the iCoh alpha 3 values was also observed among group, hub, and direction (F[10, 750] = 2.48, p < 0.05). Compared with the Nold group, the ADD group showed lower output global iCoh alpha 3 values at one parietal electrode (i.e., Pz; p < 0.05).

Additionally, a statistically significant ANOVA interaction of the iCoh alpha 3 values was observed among group, hub, and direction (F = 2.48, p < 0.05). Compared with the Nold group, the ADD group showed lower output global iCoh alpha 3 values at one parietal electrode (i.e., Pz; p < 0.05).

Supplementary material Table 1 reports the results of the Duncan planned post-hoc (p < 0.05) test relative to the ANOVA interaction effects. Of note, the above results were not caused by outliers, as shown by Grubbs’ test with an arbitrary threshold of p > 0.001 (see Supplementary Figure 10).

Overall, the above iCoh results showed no substantial between-group differences in the topology of the iCoh alpha 2 and alpha 3 values, with those values being maximized at parietal electrodes. However, the output iCoh alpha 2 and alpha 3 values at parietal electrodes were lower in the ADD group than in the Nold group.

3.6. Control analysis on parietal connector hubs identified at alpha 2 and alpha 3 bands by other graph theory measures

To control for the robustness of the present results about the parietal connector hubs computed at the alpha 2 and alpha 3 bands, we used the following additional graph measures and definitions of those hubs (GraphVar 2.0 platform). According to Cole et al. (2015), a connector hub can be associated with high values of ND, PC, and Betweenness Centrality (BC). Notably, BC of a node is defined as the number of shortest graph paths that goes through that node (Rubinov and Sporns, 2010). According to Power et al. (2013), a connector hub can be associated with high values of the within-module degree z-score and PC.

Figures 3, 4 illustrate the results of this control analysis. There was again a certain spread of connector hubs over the scalp at alpha 2 and alpha 3 bands. However, convergent results showed significant connector hubs located at the parietal electrodes (i.e., P3, Pz, and P4) in both the Nold and ADD groups. Supplementary material Tables 14–25 report detailed results of this control analysis.

3.7. Control analysis of the influence of normalized rsEEG spectral power density on iCoh values

To evaluate the influence of potential intergroup differences in the rsEEG spectral power density on iCoh (and LLC as the second interdependency measure) values, we included a control analysis for the comparison of the rsEEG power spectra for each frequency band of interest between the Nold and ADD groups. To this aim, we used the normalized rsEEG spectral power density calculated at each individual frequency band of interest (from delta to gamma) as the dependent variable in an ANOVA design, with group (Nold and ADD; independent variable), ROI (frontal, central, parietal, temporal, and occipital), and band (delta, theta, alpha 1, alpha 2, alpha 3, beta 1, beta 2, and gamma) as factors. The ROI were defined as those used for the interhemispheric iCoh analysis. We used the Log10-transformed rsEEG spectral power density values to meet the requirement of Gaussian distribution of the dependent ANOVA variable.

Results are illustrated in Supplementary Figure 11. Compared with the Nold group, the ADD group was characterized by (1) lower widespread alpha 2 and alpha 3 spectral power density values, especially at parietal and occipital ROI, and (2) higher widespread delta spectral power density values (p < 0.05).

Owing to the above-mentioned intergroup differences, we repeated the main statistical analyses by introducing the global alpha 2 or the alpha 3 regional normalized spectral power density as a covariate. Global values were calculated by averaging the regional values (as intergroup differences in the alpha 2 and alpha 3 bands were widespread). Results confirmed the previous main findings except for the intrahemispheric (non-directional) LLC values (no statistically significant interaction among the factors group, ROI, and band with global alpha 2 or alpha 3 spectral power densities as covariates; p > 0.05). In detail, the following results were obtained:

• intrahemispheric (non-directional) mean iCoh—covariate, global alpha 2 spectral power density: a statistically significant ANOVA interaction (F[28, 2072] = 3.55, p < 0.05) among the factors group, hemisphere, ROI, and band. Again, no significant post-hoc effect was observed in the planned tests (p > 0.05).

• intrahemispheric (non-directional) mean iCoh—covariate, global alpha 3 spectral power density: a statistically significant ANOVA interaction (F[28, 2072] = 3.02, p < 0.05) among the factors group, hemisphere, ROI, and band. Again, no significant post-hoc effect was observed in the planned tests (p > 0.05).

• intrahemispheric (non-directional) absolute difference iCoh—covariate, global alpha 2 spectral power density: a statistically significant ANOVA interaction (F[28, 2072] = 2.81, p < 0.05) among the factors group, hemisphere, ROI, and band. Again, no significant post-hoc effect was observed in the planned tests (p > 0.05).

• intrahemispheric (non-directional) absolute difference iCoh—covariate, global alpha 3 spectral power density: a statistically significant ANOVA interaction (F[28, 2072] = 2.02, p < 0.05) among the factors group, hemisphere, ROI, and band. Again, no significant post-hoc effect was observed in the planned tests (p > 0.05).

• interhemispheric LLC—covariate, global alpha 2 spectral power density: a statistically significant ANOVA interaction (F[28, 2072] = 2.22, p < 0.05) among the factors group, ROI, and band. A planned Duncan post-hoc test showed that, compared with the Nold group, the ADD group was mainly characterized by (1) lower LLC alpha 2 and alpha 3 values at the parietal and temporal ROI and (2) higher LLC delta values at frontal, parietal, and occipital ROI (p < 0.05).

• interhemispheric LLC—covariate, global alpha 3 spectral power density: a statistically significant ANOVA interaction (F[28, 2072] = 2.19, p < 0.05) among the factors group, ROI, and band. A planned Duncan post-hoc test showed that, compared with the Nold group, the ADD group was mainly characterized by (1) lower LLC alpha 2 and alpha 3 values at the parietal and temporal ROI and (2) higher LLC delta values at frontal, parietal, and occipital ROI (p < 0.05).

With regard to the directionality of hubs from iCoh values at alpha 2 and alpha 3 bands, we used the global alpha 2 and alpha 3 spectral power densities, respectively, as covariates in the main statistical analysis. Results with the global alpha 2 spectral power density as covariate confirmed a statistically significant ANOVA interaction of the iCoh alpha 2 values among group (Nold and ADD; independent variable), hub (electrodes with hub features), and direction (output and input) factors (F[10, 70] = 1.97, p < 0.05). Compared with the Nold group, the ADD group showed lower output global iCoh alpha 2 values at parietal electrodes (i.e., P3 and Pz; p < 0.05). No statistically significant ANOVA interaction (p > 0.05) was observed, including with global alpha 3 spectral power density as a covariate.

4. Discussion

4.1. Converging evidence of LLC and iCoh measures about the interdependencies of rsEEG rhythms at electrode pairs

In the present study, both bivariate LLC and multivariate iCoh measures showed that, compared with the Nold participants, the ADD patients were characterized by lower interdependencies of rsEEG alpha rhythms, especially at parietal electrode pairs. This effect was more spatially sharp with multivariate iCoh measures than with bivariate LLC measures. These results confirm the spatial variability of the effects derived from different techniques estimating interdependencies of rsEEG rhythms at electrode pairs and emphasize the importance of using more than one technique, including at least one multivariate approach (Blinowska, 2011; Blinowska et al., 2017).

The current results are globally in line with the bulk of previous rsEEG studies showing that ADD patients exhibit lower interrelatedness of rsEEG rhythms at alpha and higher frequencies at posterior electrode pairs (Leuchter et al., 1992, 1994; Besthorn et al., 1994; Dunkin et al., 1994; Sloan et al., 1994; Stam et al., 1995, 1996, 2003, 2009; Jelic et al., 1998, 2000; Locatelli et al., 1998; Anghinah et al., 2000; Knott et al., 2000; Adler et al., 2003; Babiloni et al., 2004a, 2006a, 2018; Pogarell et al., 2005; de Haan et al., 2009; Fonseca et al., 2011, 2013).

Additionally, the current results showed certain effects of ADD on the interdependencies of rsEEG delta rhythms at electrode pairs, but only when the bivariate LLC technique was used. Compared with the Nold group, the ADD group exhibited higher LLC values at frontal, parietal, and occipital delta rhythms. These effects were globally in agreement with previous rsEEG evidence obtained using bivariate techniques (e.g., FFT-based spectral coherence or LLC) to investigate the effects of ADD on the interdependencies of rsEEG rhythms (Locatelli et al., 1998; Babiloni et al., 2010, 2018; Hsiao et al., 2013, 2014). Given the lack of effects of ADD on interdependencies of rsEEG delta rhythms derived from multivariate iCoh measures, we did not estimate hubs at delta rhythms. Notably, the results at delta rhythms indicated that the present iCoh measures were not redundant compared with those of spectral power density, which showed greater widespread rsEEG delta power in the ADD group than the Nold group.

4.2. Converging evidence of LLC and iCoh measures about graph hubs at parietal electrodes and alpha rhythms

The present alpha LLC and iCoh measures showed converging evidence of prominent degree and connector hubs at parietal electrode pairs (i.e., P3, Pz, and P4). Furthermore, both groups were characterized by a prominent alpha iCoh outward direction (output or outflow) from parietal electrodes to other electrodes exhibiting degree hub properties. This effect was lower in the ADD group than in the Nold group and was consistent with the three definitions of connector hubs (Rubinov and Sporns, 2010; Power et al., 2013; Cole et al., 2015).

Taken together, it can be speculated that the present alpha LLC-iCoh and graph results may reflect abnormalities in parietal networks underpinning the regulation of quiet vigilance in ADD patients but with a global preservation of the parietal hub function, as revealed by the present analysis of the rsEEG alpha rhythms.

In line with this speculation, previous studies investigating directional interdependencies of rsEEG rhythms in cognitively unimpaired adults showed prominent outflow measures at alpha (and beta) rhythms from parietal electrodes, based on the computation of multivariate directed transfer function (Kuś et al., 2004; Blinowska and Kaminski, 2013). These measures were reduced in relation to a decrease in vigilance and an increase in errors during a continuous cognitive task in those adults (Liu et al., 2010). When applied to AD patients, directed transfer function measures at rsEEG alpha (and beta) rhythms exhibited lower outflow from parietal to frontal electrodes in ADD and ADMCI patients than in Nold participants (Babiloni et al., 2008, 2009a,b; Blinowska et al., 2017). These effects might be partially caused by abnormal ascending inputs coming from the thalamic and cholinergic basal forebrain regions (Hughes and Crunelli, 2005; Babiloni et al., 2006a, 2009a,b, 2020a, 2021; Wan et al., 2019).

4.3. What might graph hubs from rsEEG alpha rhythms tell us about add patients?

At this early stage of the research, we can only speculate about the neurophysiological significance of the present results. As novel and original neurophysiological findings, the present study showed evidence of prominent parietal hubs from interdependencies of the rsEEG alpha rhythms but with reduced outward iCoh measures in AD patients with mild-to-moderate dementia (mean MMSE score of approximately 19/30). An exciting hypothesis for future longitudinal studies is that the AD progression to severe dementia might be associated with (1) outward alpha iCoh measures that are even more reduced from parietal electrodes, (2) loss of degree and connector hubs from the present LLC and iCoh measures, and (3) increased disorders in the regulation and maintenance of quiet vigilance during the daytime, with frequent episodes of drowsiness, misperceptions, and light sleep. If confirmed, the present rsEEG evidence of partially preserved parietal hubs in mild-to-moderate ADD patients would reflect a sort of resilience or initial vulnerability of the brain networks underpinning quiet vigilance.

In line with this speculation, previous rsEEG evidence showed several signs of topographically widespread impairment of brain networks in ADD patients, as revealed by graph theory indexes. Multivariate directional techniques based on a Granger causality matrix from rsEEG alpha and beta rhythms unveiled that ADD patients were globally characterized by lower global efficiency, increased local efficiency, and lower resilience of cortical networks (Afshari and Jalili, 2017). Furthermore, these patients were characterized by lower inward and outward directions of interdependencies of the whole-band rsEEG activity recorded from posterior electrodes, with maximum abnormalities of degree hubs at parietal electrodes (Franciotti et al., 2019), but not replicated with a bivariate mutual information technique (Franciotti et al., 2022).

In the resting state condition, prominent outward directionality of the present hubs at parietal electrodes and alpha rhythms might reflect the synchronization and interdependence of neural activity into posterior thalamocortical and corticothalamic loops, which might maintain cortical arousal underpinning vigilance against sleep intrusion (Hughes and Crunelli, 2005; Lörincz et al., 2008, 2009; Crunelli et al., 2015). Indeed, the greater the posterior rsEEG alpha rhythms, the greater the cortical inhibition in quiet vigilance, the lower the attention to external stimuli (Pfurtscheller and Klimesch, 1992; Boksem et al., 2005; Babiloni et al., 2020b). Those thalamic and cortical reciprocal interactions might influence several cortical areas, including the nodes of the ‘default mode network’ (Raichle et al., 2001; Buckner et al., 2008). Overall, such traveling alpha rhythms may flow from higher-to lower-order areas in the visual and somatosensory cortices (Halgren et al., 2019). The effect may be to facilitate the scanning of internal and external environments (Liu et al., 2010; Al-Shargie et al., 2019), extract relevant features on demand (Ermentrout and Kleinfeld, 2001), and support communications within nodes of brain networks in relation to vigilance (Han et al., 2008; Crunelli et al., 2018).

4.4. Methodological limitations of the present study

This study was not performed within a unique multicentric clinical trial, so the present recording units did not follow the identical clinical, neuropsychological, and neuroimaging procedures during the enrollment of Nold and ADD participants. This makes the present study exploratory in nature.

Standard biomarkers of AD neuropathology (e.g., cerebrospinal diagnostic measures of Ab42/phospho tau or amyloid positron emission tomography) were not systematically measured in the present Nold and ADD participants, so only the strongest and most robust results could emerge at the group level. This limitation may explain some significant variability of graph indexes at rsEEG delta rhythms.

We used a low number of scalp electrodes to record rsEEG activity (i.e., 19 electrodes placed according to 10–20 system), two standard bivariate (LLC) and multivariate (iCoh) techniques estimating the interrelatedness of the rsEEG activity at electrode pairs, and well-known graph indexes in line with the general methodology of several previous successful studies; those studies investigated the graph-based rsEEG topology in ADD patients based on ‘synchronization likelihood’, ‘phase lag index’, ‘synchronization likelihood’, ‘generalized composite multiscale entropy vector’, and ‘mutual information’ techniques applied to rsEEG data recorded from ≤19 scalp electrodes (Stam et al., 2007a; De Haan et al., 2009; Engels et al., 2015; Yu et al., 2016; Song et al., 2019; Das and Puthankattil, 2022; Franciotti et al., 2022).

This intrinsic low resolution of the present rsEEG approach was partially considered by averaging the LLC and iCoh measures in large scalp ROI. Furthermore, head volume conduction effects may inflate LLC and iCoh measures. Indeed, electric fields can instantaneously spread from a brain source to several scalp electrodes, thus generating spurious (fake) interdependencies of rsEEG rhythms at electrode pairs. These effects of head volume conduction are partially mitigated by the fact that LLC and iCoh measures are insensitive to zero-lag interdependencies of rsEEG rhythms. However, the present application of those techniques at scalp electrodes ignores observational equations modeling confounding effects of head volume conduction and position/orientation of cortical sources of scalp EEG activity (Babiloni et al., 2020). Therefore, confounding non-zero-lag head volume conduction effects and false ‘interrelatedness’ cannot be excluded in the interpretation of the present results. In this framework, it should be remarked that bivariate techniques (including LLC) may be more prone to those confounds than multivariate techniques (including iCoh), as the latter typically remove common correlations of the rsEEG activity among the electrode pairs (Blinowska and Kaminski, 2013; Babiloni et al., 2020b).

The intrinsic methodological limits of all bivariate and multivariate techniques (including LLC and iCoh) were recently discussed by an Expert Panel of the International Federation of Clinical Neurophysiology (IFCN; Babiloni et al., 2020b). The Expert Panel agreed that all bivariate (e.g., LLC, synchronization likelihood, phase lag index, etc.) and multivariate (e.g., iCoh, directed transfer function, etc.) techniques estimating the interrelatedness of the rsEEG activity at scalp electrode pairs may be subject to unmodeled effects of (1) brain neural populations ‘invisible’ to EEG recordings and (2) head volume conduction. Furthermore, the Expert Panel shared the following recommendations to fruitfully tackle (Babiloni et al., 2020b): (1) the use of the locution ‘measures of the interrelatedness of rsEEG activity at scalp electrodes’ rather than locutions such as “measures of cortical functional connectivity from rsEEG activity” to emphasize that the head volume conduction effects cannot be entirely taken into account when those techniques are applied at scalp electrode pairs; (2) the development of exploratory rsEEG studies carried out by investigators belonging to independent research institutions, to ensure a significant intersubjectivity in the interpretation of the results; (3) the use of at least two independent techniques for estimating the interrelatedness of the rsEEG activity at scalp electrodes, to compare the results and represent their intrinsic variability dependent on the methodology used; and (4) the exploitation of open science to cross-validate the research results using, when possible, freeware techniques validated by independent research groups. We grounded the present study design on these recommendations.

Keeping in mind the previously mentioned low spatial resolution and head volume conduction effects, we included a relatively low number of network nodes (corresponding to the standard 10–20 electrode montage) in the graph analysis. This low-resolution EEG method could not allow the disentanglement of the contribution of the nodes of the default mode network or associate parietal cortex. Therefore, future studies may improve the methodological approach with the following solutions: (1) large samples of the enrolled ADD, ADMCI, and Nold participants and a longitudinal design to enhance the statistical power of the study and test the impact of disease severity and progression on the topology of the interrelatedness of rsEEG activity; (2) harmonized protocols in the multicentric studies; (3) >48 scalp electrodes for the rsEEG recordings; (4) mathematical source and head volume conduction models for an rsEEG source estimation probing the activity of more cortical nodes, including those located in the default mode and other relevant cortical networks; (5) a multimodal approach, including the rs-fMRI recordings, to correlate the AD-related abnormal topology of the cortical functional connectivity, as revealed by rs-fMRI and rsEEG data; and (6) a more systematic variation of statistical thresholds to qualify the significant associations between sensors and the criteria used to define and describe the present hubs with those thresholds.

5. Conclusion

In the present exploratory study, we compared hubs modeled from measures of interdependencies of between-electrode rsEEG alpha rhythms in Nold and mild-to-moderate ADD participants. We tested the hypothesis of abnormal posterior hubs from those measures in ADD versus Nold participants. To report robust results, we measured interdependencies of rsEEG rhythms using both bivariate LLC and multivariate (directional) iCoh measures. Furthermore, we used three different definitions of ‘connector’ hub.

Convergent results of LLC and iCoh measures showed that in both Nold and ADD groups there were significant ‘degree’ and ‘connector’ hubs at parietal electrodes derived from rsEEG alpha rhythms. Furthermore, these hubs showed a prominent outward directionality in both groups of participants. As a main difference between the two groups, the outward ‘directionality’ of the hubs at parietal electrodes was lower in the ADD group than in the Nold group.

Future longitudinal high-resolution rsEEG studies in ADD patients will have to test hypotheses about the resilience or vulnerability of those parietal hubs derived from rsEEG alpha rhythms and their relationships with the neuropathological burden, derangement in the DMN, and the neurophysiological regulation and maintenance of quiet vigilance during daytime.

Data availability statement

The datasets presented in this article are not readily available because they may be made available by CB through a formal data sharing agreement and approval from the requesting researcher’s local ethics committee. The presentation of a formal collaboration project outline is also suggested. Requests to access the datasets should be directed to SL, susanna.lopez@uniroma1.it.

Ethics statement

The studies involving human participants were reviewed and approved by Sapienza University of Rome. The patients/participants provided their written informed consent to participate in this study.

Author contributions

CBa and SL: conceptualization, methodology, formal analysis, investigation, data curation, validation, project administration, writing—original draft, supervision, writing—review and editing. CP, GN, and RL: formal analysis, software, writing—review and editing. AP, FN, DA, FF, DM, AC, GK, AB, MO, BB, AS, RF, CBu, FG, BG, GY, FS, LV, and LB: methodology, project administration, writing—review and editing. All authors contributed to the article and approved the submitted version.

Funding

In this study, the electroencephalographic data analysis was partially supported by the funds of “Ricerca Corrente 2021–2022” attributed by the Ministry of Health, Italy to the IRCCS Synlab SDN of Naples, IRCCS OASI Maria SS of Troina, IRCCS San Giovanni di Dio “Fatebenefratelli” of Brescia, and IRCCS San Raffaele Rome.

Acknowledgments

The research activities were partly developed in the framework of the ‘PDWAVES’ Consortium (https://www.pdwaves.eu/) founded by CB (Sapienza University of Rome, Italy) and Peter Fuhr (University of Basel, Basel, Switzerland).

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher’s note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

Supplementary material

The Supplementary material for this article can be found online at: https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fnagi.2023.780014/full#supplementary-material

Footnotes

References

Adler, G., Brassen, S., and Jajcevic, A. (2003). EEG coherence in Alzheimer’s dementia. J. Neural Transm. 110, 1051–1058. doi: 10.1007/s00702-003-0024-8

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Afshari, S., and Jalili, M. (2017). Directed functional networks in Alzheimer’s disease: disruption of global and local connectivity measures. IEEE J. Biomed. Health Inform. 21, 949–955. doi: 10.1109/JBHI.2016.2578954

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Albert, M. S., DeKosky, S. T., Dickson, D., Dubois, B., Feldman, H. H., Fox, N. C., et al. (2011). The diagnosis of mild cognitive impairment due to Alzheimer’s disease: recommendations from the National Institute on Aging-Alzheimer’s Association workgroups on diagnostic guidelines for Alzheimer’s disease. Alzheimers Dement. 7, 270–279. doi: 10.1016/j.jalz.2011.03.008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Al-Shargie, F., Tariq, U., Hassanin, O., Mir, H., Babiloni, F., and Al-Nashash, H. (2019). Brain connectivity analysis under semantic vigilance and enhanced mental states. Brain Sci. 9:363. doi: 10.3390/brainsci9120363

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Anghinah, R., Kanda, P. A., Jorge, M. S., Lima, E. E., Pascuzzi, L., and Melo, A. C. (2000). Alpha band coherence analysis of EEG in healthy adult’s and Alzheimer’s type dementia patients. Arq. Neuropsiquiatr. 58, 272–275. doi: 10.1590/S0004-282X2000000200011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Babiloni, C., Arakaki, X., Azami, H., Bennys, K., Blinowska, K., Bonanni, L., et al. (2021). Measures of resting state EEG rhythms for clinical trials in Alzheimer’s disease: recommendations of an expert panel. Alzheimers Dement. 17, 1528–1553. doi: 10.1002/alz.12311

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Babiloni, C., Barry, R. J., Başar, E., Blinowska, K. J., Cichocki, A., Drinkenburg, W. H. I. M., et al. (2020b). International Federation of Clinical Neurophysiology (IFCN) – EEG research workgroup: recommendations on frequency and topographic analysis of resting state EEG rhythms. Part 1: applications in clinical research studies. Clin. Neurophysiol. 131, 285–307. doi: 10.1016/j.clinph.2019.06.234

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Babiloni, C., Blinowska, K., Bonanni, L., Cichocki, A., De Haan, W., Del Percio, C., et al. (2020a). What electrophysiology tells us about Alzheimer’s disease: a window into the synchronization and connectivity of brain neurons. Neurobiol. Aging 85, 58–73.

Google Scholar

Babiloni, C., Cassetta, E., Dal Forno, G., Del Percio, C., Ferreri, F., Ferri, R., et al. (2006a). Donepezil effects on sources of cortical rhythms in mild Alzheimer’s disease: responders vs. non-responders. Neuroimage 31, 1650–1665. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2006.02.015

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Babiloni, C., Del Percio, C., Boccardi, M., Lizio, R., Lopez, S., Carducci, F., et al. (2015). Occipital sources of resting-state alpha rhythms are related to local gray matter density in subjects with amnesic mild cognitive impairment and Alzheimer’s disease. Neurobiol. Aging 36, 556–570. doi: 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2014.09.011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Babiloni, C., Del Percio, C., Lizio, R., Noce, G., Cordone, S., Lopez, S., et al. (2017). Abnormalities of cortical neural synchronization mechanisms in patients with dementia due to Alzheimer’s and Lewy body diseases: an EEG study. Neurobiol. Aging 55, 143–158. doi: 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2017.03.030

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Babiloni, C., Del Percio, C., Lizio, R., Noce, G., Lopez, S., Soricelli, A., et al. (2018). Abnormalities of resting-state functional cortical connectivity in patients with dementia due to Alzheimer’s and Lewy body diseases: an EEG study. Neurobiol. Aging 65, 18–40.

Google Scholar

Babiloni, C., Del Percio, C., Pascarelli, M. T., Lizio, R., Noce, G., Lopez, S., et al. (2019). Abnormalities of functional cortical source connectivity of resting-state electroencephalographic alpha rhythms are similar in patients with mild cognitive impairment due to Alzheimer’s and Lewy body diseases. Neurobiol. Aging. 77, 112–127. doi: 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2019.01.013

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Babiloni, C., Ferri, R., Binetti, G., Vecchio, F., Frisoni, G. B., Lanuzza, B., et al. (2009a). Directionality of EEG synchronization in Alzheimer’s disease subjects. Neurobiol. Aging 30, 93–102.

Google Scholar

Babiloni, C., Ferri, R., Moretti, D. V., Strambi, A., Binetti, G., Dal Forno, G., et al. (2004b). Abnormal fronto-parietal coupling of brain rhythms in mild Alzheimer’s disease: a multicentric EEG study. Eur J Neurosci. 19, 2583–2590. doi: 10.1111/j.0953-816X.2004.03333.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Babiloni, C., Ferri, R., Noce, G., Lizio, R., Lopez, S., Soricelli, A., et al. (2020). Resting-state electroencephalographic delta rhythms may reflect global cortical arousal in healthy old seniors and patients with Alzheimer’s disease dementia. Int. J. Psychophysiol. 158, 259–270. doi: 10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2020.08.012

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Babiloni, C., Frisoni, G. B., Pievani, M., Vecchio, F., Infarinato, F., Geroldi, C., et al. (2008). White matter vascular lesions are related to parietal-to-frontal coupling of EEG rhythms in mild cognitive impairment. Hum. Brain Mapp. 29, 1355–1367. doi: 10.1002/hbm.20467

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Babiloni, C., Frisoni, G. B., Vecchio, F., Pievani, M., Geroldi, C., De Carli, C., et al. (2010). Global functional coupling of resting EEG rhythms is related to white-matter lesions along the cholinergic tracts in subjects with amnesic mild cognitive impairment. J. Alzheimers Dis. 19, 859–871.

Google Scholar

Babiloni, C., Lizio, R., Marzano, N., Capotosto, P., Soricelli, A., Triggiani, A. I., et al. (2016). Brain neural synchronization and functional coupling in Alzheimer’s disease as revealed by resting state EEG rhythms. Int. J. Psychophysiol. 103, 88–102. doi: 10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2015.02.008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Babiloni, C., Miniussi, C., Moretti, D. V., Vecchio, F., Salinari, S., Frisoni, G., et al. (2004a). Cortical networks generating movement-related EEG rhythms in Alzheimer’s disease: an EEG coherence study. Behav. Neurosci. 118, 698–706. doi: 10.1037/0735-7044.118.4.698

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Babiloni, C., Pievani, M., Vecchio, F., Geroldi, C., Eusebi, F., Fracassi, C., et al. (2009b). White-matter lesions along the cholinergic tracts are related to cortical sources of EEG rhythms in amnesic mild cognitive impairment. Hum. Brain Mapp. 30, 1431–1443. doi: 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2007.05.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Babiloni, C., Vecchio, F., Bultrini, A., Luca Romani, G., and Rossini, P. M. (2006b). Pre-and poststimulus alpha rhythms are related to conscious visual perception: a high-resolution EEG study. Cereb. Cortex 16, 1690–1700. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhj104

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Besthorn, C., Förstl, H., Geiger-Kabisch, C., Sattel, H., Gasser, T., and Schreiter-Gasser, U. (1994). EEG coherence in Alzheimer disease. Electroencephalogr. Clin. Neurophysiol. 90, 242–245. doi: 10.1016/0013-4694(94)90095-7

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Blinowska, K. J. (2011). Review of the methods of determination of directed connectivity from multichannel data. Med. Biol. Engin. Comput. 49, 521–529. doi: 10.1007/s11517-011-0739-x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Blinowska, K. J., and Kaminski, M. (2013). Functional brain networks: random, “small world” or deterministic? PLoS One. 8:e78763. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0078763

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Blinowska, K. J., Rakowski, F., Kaminski, M., De Vico, F. F., Del Percio, C., Lizio, R., et al. (2017). Functional and effective brain connectivity for discrimination between Alzheimer’s patients and healthy individuals: a study on resting state EEG rhythms. Clin. Neurophysiol. 128, 667–680. doi: 10.1016/j.clinph.2016.10.002

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bokde, A. L., Lopez-Bayo, P., Meindl, T., Pechler, S., Born, C., Faltraco, F., et al. (2006). Functional connectivity of the fusiform gyrus during a face-matching task in subjects with mild cognitive impairment. Brain 129, 1113–1124. doi: 10.1093/brain/awl051

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Boksem, M. A., Meijman, T. F., and Lorist, M. M. (2005). Effects of mental fatigue on attention: an ERP study. Brain Res. Cogn. Brain Res. 25, 107–116. doi: 10.1016/j.cogbrainres.2005.04.011

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Brier, M. R., Thomas, J. B., Snyder, A. Z., Benzinger, T. L., Zhang, D., Raichle, M. E., et al. (2012). Loss of intranetwork and internetwork resting state functional connections with Alzheimer’s disease progression. J Neurosci. 32, 8890–8899. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5698-11.2012

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Buckner, R. L., Andrews-Hanna, J. R., and Schacter, D. L. (2008). The brain’s default network: anatomy, function, and relevance to disease. Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci. 1124, 1–38. doi: 10.1196/annals.1440.011

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bullmore, E., and Sporns, O. (2009). Complex brain networks: graph theoretical analysis of structural and functional systems Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 10, 186–198. doi: 10.1038/nrn2575

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bullmore, E., and Sporns, O. (2012). The economy of brain network organization. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 13, 336–349. doi: 10.1038/nrn3214

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Busche, M. A., and Konnerth, A. (2016). Impairments of neural circuit function in Alzheimer’s disease. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. Ser. B Biol. Sci. 371:20150429. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2015.0429

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Canuet, L., Tellado, I., Couceiro, V., Fraile, C., Fernandez-Novoa, L., Ishii, R., et al. (2012). Resting-state network disruption and APOE genotype in Alzheimer’s disease: a lagged functional connectivity study. PLoS One 7:e46289. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0046289

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Capotosto, P., Babiloni, C., Romani, G. L., and Corbetta, M. (2012). Differential contribution of right and left parietal cortex to the control of spatial attention: a simultaneous EEG-rTMS study. Cereb. Cortex 22, 446–454. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhr127

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chen, G., Zhang, H. Y., Xie, C., Chen, G., Zhang, Z. J., Teng, G. J., et al. (2013). Modular reorganization of brain resting state networks and its independent validation in Alzheimer’s disease patients. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 7:456. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2013.00456

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cole, M. W., Ito, T., and Braver, T. S. (2015). Lateral prefrontal cortex contributes to fluid intelligence through multinetwork connectivity. Brain Connect. 5, 497–504. doi: 10.1089/brain.2015.0357

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Crunelli, V., David, F., Lőrincz, M. L., and Hughes, S. W. (2015). The thalamocortical network as a single slow wave-generating unit. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 31, 72–80. doi: 10.1016/j.conb.2014.09.001

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Crunelli, V., Lőrincz, M. L., Connelly, W. M., David, F., Hughes, S. W., Lambert, R. C., et al. (2018). Dual function of thalamic low-vigilance state oscillations: rhythm-regulation and plasticity. Nat Rev Neurosci. 19, 107–118. doi: 10.1038/nrn.2017.151

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Daianu, M., Jahanshad, N., Nir, T. M., Toga, A., Jack, C. R., Weiner, M. W., et al. (2013). Breakdown of brain connectivity between normal aging and Alzheimer’s disease: a structural k-core network analysis. Brain Connect. 3, 407–422. doi: 10.1089/brain.2012.0137

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Das, S., and Puthankattil, S. D. (2022). Functional connectivity and complexity in the phenomenological model of mild cognitive-impaired Alzheimer’s disease. Front. Comput. Neurosci. 6:877912. doi: 10.3389/fncom.2022.877912

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dauwels, J., Vialatte, F., Latchoumane, C., Jeong, J., and Cichocki, A. (2009). EEG synchrony analysis for early diagnosis of Alzheimer’s disease: a study with several synchrony measures and EEG data sets. Ann. Int. Conf. IEEE Eng. Med. Biol. Soc., 2224–2227. doi: 10.1109/IEMBS.2009.5334862

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dauwels, J., Vialatte, F., Musha, T., and Cichocki, A. (2010). A comparative study of synchrony measures for the early diagnosis of Alzheimer’s disease based on EEG. NeuroImage 49, 668–693. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.06.056

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

de Haan, W., Mott, K., van Straaten, E. C., Scheltens, P., and Stam, C. J. (2012). Activity dependent degeneration explains hub vulnerability in Alzheimer’s disease. PLoS Comput Biol. 8:e1002582. doi: 10.1371/journal.pcbi.1002582

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

de Haan, W., Pijnenburg, Y. A., Strijers, R. L., van der Made, Y., van der Flier, W. M., Scheltens, P., et al. (2009). Functional neural network analysis in frontotemporal dementia and Alzheimer’s disease using EEG and graph theory. BMC Neurosci. 10:101. doi: 10.1186/1471-2202-10-101

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Del Percio, C., Derambure, P., Noce, G., Lizio, R., Bartrés-Faz, D., Blin, O., et al. (2019). Sleep deprivation and Modafinil affect cortical sources of resting state electroencephalographic rhythms in healthy young adults. Clin. Neurophysiol. 130, 1488–1498. doi: 10.1016/j.clinph.2019.06.007

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Delbeuck, X., Van der Linden, M., and Collette, F. (2003). Alzheimer’s disease as a disconnection syndrome? Neuropsychol. Rev. 13, 79–92. doi: 10.1023/a:1023832305702

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dunkin, J. J., Leuchter, A. F., Newton, T. F., and Cook, I. A. (1994). Reduced EEG coherence in dementia: state or trait marker? Biol. Psychiatry 35, 870–879.

Google Scholar

Engels, M. M., Stam, C. J., van der Flier, W. M., Scheltens, P., de Waal, H., and van Straaten, E. C. (2015). Declining functional connectivity and changing hub locations in Alzheimer’s disease: an EEG study. BMC Neurol. 15:145. doi: 10.1186/s12883-015-0400-7

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ermentrout, G. B., and Kleinfeld, D. (2001). Traveling electrical waves in cortex: insights from phase dynamics and speculation on a computational role. Neuron. 29, 33–44. doi: 10.1016/s0896-6273(01)00178-7

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Eyler, L. T., Elman, J. A., Hatton, S. N., Gough, S., Mischel, A. K., Hagler, D. J., et al. (2019). Resting state abnormalities of the default mode network in mild cognitive impairment: a systematic review and meta-analysis. J Alzheimers Dis. 70, 107–120. doi: 10.3233/JAD-180847

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Folstein, M. F., Folstein, S. E., and McHugh, P. R. (1975). ‘Mini mental state’: a practical method for grading the cognitive state of patients for clinician. J. Psychiatr. Res. 12, 189–198.

Google Scholar

Fonseca, L. C., Tedrus, G. M., Carvas, P. N., and Machado, E. C. (2013). Comparison of quantitative EEG between patients with Alzheimer’s disease and those with Parkinson’s disease dementia. Clin. Neurophysiol. 124, 1970–1974. doi: 10.1016/j.clinph.2013.05.001

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fonseca, L. C., Tedrus, G. M., Fondello, M. A., Reis, I. N., and Fontoura, D. S. (2011). EEG theta and alpha reactivity on opening the eyes in the diagnosis of Alzheimer’s disease. Clin. EEG Neurosci. 42, 185–189. doi: 10.1177/155005941104200308

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Franciotti, R., Falasca, N. W., Arnaldi, D., Famà, F., Babiloni, C., Onofrj, M., et al. (2019). Cortical network topology in prodromal and mild dementia due to Alzheimer’s disease: graph theory applied to resting state EEG. Brain Topogr. 32, 127–141. doi: 10.1007/s10548-018-0674-3

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Franciotti, R., Moretti, D. V., Benussi, A., Ferri, L., Russo, M., Carrarini, C., et al. (2022). Cortical network modularity changes along the course of frontotemporal and Alzheimer’s dementing diseases. Neurobiol. Aging 110, 37–46. doi: 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2021.10.016

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Frantzidis, C. A., Vivas, A. B., Tsolaki, A., Klados, M. A., Tsolaki, M., and Bamidis, P. D. (2014). Functional disorganization of small-world brain networks in mild Alzheimer’s disease and amnestic mild cognitive impairment: an EEG study using relative wavelet entropy (RWE). Front. Aging Neurosci. 6:224. doi: 10.3389/fnagi.2014.00224

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gelb, D. J., Oliver, E., and Gilman, S. (1999). Diagnostic criteria for Parkinson disease. Arch Neurol. 56, 33–39. doi: 10.1001/archneur.56.1.33

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gorelick, P. B., Scuteri, A., Black, S. E., Decarli, C., Greenberg, S. M., Iadecola, C., et al. (2011). American Heart Association stroke council, council on epidemiology and prevention, council on cardiovascular nursing, council on cardiovascular radiology and intervention, and council on cardiovascular surgery and anesthesia. Vascular contributions to cognitive impairment and dementia: a statement for healthcare professionals from the american heart association/american stroke association. Stroke 42, 2672–2713. doi: 10.1161/STR.0b013e3182299496

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Halgren, M., Ulbert, I., Bastuji, H., Fabó, D., Erőss, L., Rey, M., et al. (2019). The generation and propagation of the human alpha rhythm. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 116, 23772–23782. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1913092116

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hallett, M., de Haan, W., Deco, G., Dengler, R., Di Iorio, R., Gallea, C., et al. (2020). Human brain connectivity: clinical applications for clinical neurophysiology. Clin. Neurophysiol. 131, 1621–1651. doi: 10.1016/j.clinph.2020.03.031

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Han, S., Fan, Y., and Mao, L. (2008). Gender difference in empathy for pain: an electrophysiological investigation. Brain Res. 1196, 85–93. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2007.12.062

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

He, Y., and Evans, A. (2010). Graph theoretical modeling of brain connectivity. Curr. Opin. Neurol. 23, 341–350. doi: 10.1097/WCO.0b013e32833aa567

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hsiao, F. J., Chen, W. T., Wang, Y. J., Yan, S. H., and Lin, Y. Y. (2014). Altered source-based EEG coherence of resting-state sensorimotor network in early-stage Alzheimer’s disease compared to mild cognitive impairment. Neurosci. Lett. 558, 47–52. doi: 10.1016/j.neulet.2013.10.056

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hsiao, F. J., Wang, Y. J., Yan, S. H., Chen, W. T., and Lin, Y. Y. (2013). Altered oscillation and synchronization of default-mode network activity in mild Alzheimer’s disease compared to mild cognitive impairment: an electrophysiological study. PLoS One 8:e68792. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0068792

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hughes, C. P., Berg, L., Danziger, W. L., Coben, L. A., and Martin, R. L. (1982). A new clinical scale for the staging of dementia. Br. J. Psychiatry 140, 566–572. doi: 10.1192/bjp.140.6.566

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hughes, S. W., and Crunelli, V. (2005). Thalamic mechanisms of EEG alpha rhythms and their pathological implications. Neuroscientist 11, 357–372. doi: 10.1177/1073858405277450

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jack, C. R. Jr., Therneau, T. M., Weigand, S. D., Wiste, H. J., Knopman, D. S., Vemuri, P., et al. (2019). Prevalence of Biologically vs Clinically Defined Alzheimer Spectrum Entities Using the National Institute on Aging-Alzheimer’s Association Research Framework. JAMA Neurol. 76, 1174–1183. doi: 10.1001/jamaneurol.2019.1971

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jagannathan, S. R., Bareham, C. A., and Bekinschtein, T. A. (2022). Decreasing alertness modulates perceptual decision-making. J. Neurosci. 42, 454–473.

Google Scholar

Jagannathan, S. R., Ezquerro-Nassar, A., Jachs, B., Pustovaya, O. V., Bareham, C. A., and Bekinschtein, T. A. (2018). Tracking wakefulness as it fades: micro-measures of alertness. NeuroImage 1, 138–151. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2018.04.046

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jelic, V., Dierks, T., Amberla, K., Almkvist, O., Winblad, B., and Nordberg, A. (1998). Longitudinal changes in quantitative EEG during long-term tacrine treatment of patients with Alzheimer’s disease. Neurosci. Lett. 254, 85–88. doi: 10.1016/S0304-3940(98)00669-7

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jelic, V., Johansson, S. E., Almkvist, O., Shigeta, M., Julin, P., Nordberg, A., et al. (2000). Quantitative electroencephalography in mild cognitive impairment: longitudinal changes and possible prediction of Alzheimer’s disease. Neurobiol. Aging 21, 533–540. doi: 10.1016/s0197-4580(00)00153-6

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Joo, S. H., Lee, C. U., and Lim, H. K. (2016). Apathy and intrinsic functional connectivity networks in amnestic mild cognitive impairment. Neuropsychiatr Dis Treat. 13, 61–67. doi: 10.2147/NDT.S123338

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Klimesch, W. (1996). Memory processes, brain oscillations and EEG synchronization. Int. J. Psychophysiol. 24, 61–100.

Google Scholar

Klimesch, W. (1999). EEG alpha and theta oscillations reflect cognitive and memory performance: a review and analysis. Brain Res. Rev. 29, 1169–1195.

Google Scholar

Klimesch, W., Russegger, H., Doppelmayr, M., and Pachinger, T. (1998). A method for the calculation of induced band power: implications for the significance of brain oscillations. Electroencephalogr. Clin. Neurophysiol. 108, 123–130. doi: 10.1016/s0168-5597(97)00078-6

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Knott, V., Engeland, C., Mohr, E., Mahoney, C., and Ilivitsky, V. (2000). Acute nicotine administration in Alzheimer’s disease: an exploratory EEG study. Neuropsychobiology 41, 210–220. doi: 10.1159/000026662

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kuś, R., Kamiński, M., and Blinowska, K. J. (2004). Determination of EEG activity propagation: pair-wise versus multichannel estimate. I.E.E.E. Trans. Biomed. Eng. 51, 1501–1510. doi: 10.1109/TBME.2004.827929

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lawton, M. P., and Brody, E. M. (1969). Assessment of older people: self-maintaining and instrumental activities of daily living. Gerontologist 9, 179–186.

Google Scholar

Leuchter, A. F., Dunkin, J. J., Lufkin, R. B., Anzai, Y., Cook, I. A., and Newton, T. F. (1994). Effect of white matter disease on functional connections in the aging brain. J. Neurol. Neurosurg. Psychiatry 57, 1347–1354. doi: 10.1136/jnnp.57.11.1347

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Leuchter, A. F., Newton, T. F., Cook, I. A., Walter, D. O., Rosenberg-Thompson, S., and Lachenbruch, P. A. (1992). Changes in brain functional connectivity in alzheimer-type and multi-infarct dementia. Brain: a. J. Neurol. 115, 1543–1561.

Google Scholar

Liao, X., Vasilakos, A. V., and He, Y. (2017). Small-world human brain networks: perspectives and challenges. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 77, 286–300. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2017.03.018

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Liu, J. P., Zhang, C., and Zheng, C. X. (2010). Estimation of the cortical functional connectivity by directed transfer function during mental fatigue. Appl. Ergon. 42, 114–121. doi: 10.1016/j.apergo.2010.05.008

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lizio, R., Del Percio, C., Marzano, N., Soricelli, A., Yener, G. G., Başar, E., et al. (2016). Neurophysiological assessment of Alzheimer’s disease individuals by a single electroencephalographic marker. J. Alzheimers Dis. 49, 159–177. doi: 10.3233/JAD-143042

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lo, C. Y., Wang, P. N., Chou, K. H., Wang, J., He, Y., and Lin, C. P. (2010). Diffusion tensor tractography reveals abnormal topological organization in structural cortical networks in Alzheimer’s disease. J. Neurosci. 30, 16876–16885. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4136-10.2010

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Locatelli, T., Cursi, M., Liberati, D., Franceschi, M., and Comi, G. (1998). EEG coherence in Alzheimer’s disease. Electroencephalogr. Clin. Neurophysiol. 106, 229–237. doi: 10.1016/S0013-4694(97)00129-6

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lörincz, M. L., Crunelli, V., and Hughes, S. W. (2008). Cellular dynamics of cholinergically induced alpha (8-13 Hz) rhythms in sensory thalamic nuclei in vitro. J. Neurosci. 28, 660–671. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4468-07.2008

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lörincz, M. L., Kékesi, K. A., Juhász, G., Crunelli, V., and Hughes, S. W. (2009). Temporal framing of thalamic relay-mode firing by phasic inhibition during the alpha rhythm. Neuron 63, 683–696. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2009.08.012

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

McKeith, I. G., Dickson, D. W., Lowe, J., Emre, M., O’Brien, J. T., Feldman, H., et al. (2005). Diagnosis and management of dementia with Lewy bodies: third report of the DLB consortium. Neurology 65, 1863–1872. doi: 10.1212/01.wnl.0000187889.17253.b1

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

McKhann, G., Drachman, D., Folstein, M., Katzman, R., Price, D., and Stadlan, E. M. (1984). Clinical diagnosis of Alzheimer’s disease: report of the NINCDS-ADRDA work group under the auspices of Department of Health and Human Services Task Force on Alzheimer’s disease. Neurology 34, 939–944.

Google Scholar

McKhann, G. M., Knopman, D. S., Chertkow, H., Hyman, B. T., Jack, C. R. Jr., Kawas, C. H., et al. (2011). The diagnosis of dementia due to Alzheimer’s disease: recommendations from the National Institute on Aging-Alzheimer’s Association workgroups on diagnostic guidelines for Alzheimer’s disease. Alzheimers Dement. 7, 263–269. doi: 10.1016/j.jalz.2011.03.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Moretti, D. V., Babiloni, F., Carducci, F., Cincotti, F., Remondini, E., Rossini, P. M., et al. (2003). Computerized processing of EEG–EOG–EMG artifacts for multicentric studies in EEG oscillations and event-related potentials. Int. J. Pshycophysiol. 47, 199–216. doi: 10.1016/s0167-8760(02)00153-8

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Morikawa, T., Hayashi, M., and Hori, T. (2002). Spatiotemporal variations of alpha and sigma band EEG in the waking-sleeping transition period. Percept. Mot. Skills 95, 131–154. doi: 10.2466/pms.2002.95.1.131

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nakamura, T., Goverdovsky, V., Morrell, M. J., and Mandic, D. P. (2017). Automatic sleep monitoring using ear-EEG. IEEE J Transl Eng. Health Med. 5:2800108. doi: 10.1109/JTEHM.2017.2702558

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ng, A. S. L., Wang, J., Ng, K. K., Chong, J. S. X., Qian, X., Lim, J. K. W., et al. (2021). Distinct network topology in Alzheimer’s disease and behavioral variant frontotemporal dementia. Alzheimers Res Ther. 13:13. doi: 10.1186/s13195-020-00752-w

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Novelli, G., Papagno, C., Capitani, E., Laiacona, M., Vallar, G., and Cappa, S. F. (1986). Tre test clinici di ricerca e produzione lessicale. Taratura su sogetti normali. Arch. Psicol. Neurol. Psichiatr. 47, 477–506.

Google Scholar

Pascual-Marqui, R. D. (2007). Discrete, 3D distributed, linear imaging methods of electric neuronal activity. Part 1: exact, zero error localization. Math. Phys., 1–16.

Google Scholar

Pascual-Marqui, R. D., Biscay, R. J., Bosch-Bayard, J., Lehmann, D., Kochi, K., Kinoshita, T., et al. (2014). Assessing direct paths of intracortical causal information flow of oscillatory activity with the isolated effective coherence (iCoh). Front. Hum. Neurosci. 8:448. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2014.00448

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pascual-Marqui, R. D., Lehmann, D., Koukkou, M., Kochi, K., Anderer, P., Saletu, B., et al. (2011). Assessing interactions in the brain with exact low-resolution electromagnetic tomography. Philos Trans A Math Phys Eng Sci. 369, 3768–3784. doi: 10.1098/rsta.2011.0081

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Peraza, L. R., Cromarty, R., Kobeleva, X., Firbank, M. J., Killen, A., Graziadio, S., et al. (2018). Electroencephalographic derived network differences in Lewy body dementia compared to Alzheimer’s disease patients. Sci. Rep. 8:4637. doi: 10.1038/s41598-018-22984-5

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pfurtscheller, G., and Klimesch, W. (1992). Functional topography during a visuoverbal judgment task studied with event-related desynchronization mapping. J Clin Neurophysiol. 9, 120–131. doi: 10.1097/00004691-199201000-00013

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pogarell, O., Teipel, S. J., Juckel, G., Gootjes, L., Möller, T., Bürger, K., et al. (2005). EEG coherence reflects regional corpus callosum area in Alzheimer’s disease. J. Neurol. Neurosurg. Psychiatry 76, 109–111. doi: 10.1136/jnnp.2004.036566

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Power, J. D., Schlaggar, B. L., Lessov-Schlaggar, C. N., and Petersen, S. E. (2013). Evidence for hubs in human functional brain networks. Neuron 79, 798–813. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2013.07.035

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Raichle, M. E., MacLeod, A. M., Snyder, A. Z., Powers, W. J., Gusnard, D. A., and Shulman, G. L. (2001). A default mode of brain function. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U. S. A. 98, 676–682. doi: 10.1073/pnas.98.2.676

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rascovsky, K., Hodges, J. R., Knopman, D., Mendez, M. F., Kramer, J. H., Neuhaus, J., et al. (2011). Miller BL. Sensitivity of revised diagnostic criteria for the behavioural variant of frontotemporal dementia. Brain 134, 2456–2477. doi: 10.1093/brain/awr179

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Reijmer, Y. D., Leemans, A., Caeyenberghs, K., Heringa, S. M., Koek, H. L., Biessels, G. J., et al. (2013). Disruption of cerebral networks and cognitive impairment in Alzheimer disease. Neurology. 80, 1370–1377. doi: 10.1212/WNL.0b013e31828c2ee5

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Reijneveld, J. C., Ponten, S. C., Berendse, H. W., and Stam, C. J. (2007). The application of graph theoretical analysis to complex networks in the brain. Clin. Neurophysiol. 118, 2317–2331. doi: 10.1016/j.clinph.2007.08.010

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Reitan, R. M. (1958). Validity of the trail making test as an indicator of organic brain damage. Percept. Mot. Skills 8, 271–276. doi: 10.2466/PMS.8.7.271-276

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rey, A. (1959). Reattivo della figura complessa. Manuale del Centre de Psychologie Appliquee, Parigi.

Google Scholar

Rosen, W. G., Terry, R. D., Fuld, P. A., Katzman, R., and Peck, A. (1980). Pathological verification of ischemic score in differentiation of dementias. Ann. Neurol. 7, 486–488. doi: 10.1002/ana.410070516

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rubinov, M., and Sporns, O. (2010). Complex network measures of brain connectivity: uses and interpretations. NeuroImage 52, 1059–1069. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.10.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sankari, Z., Adeli, H., and Adeli, A. (2011). Intrahemispheric, interhemispheric, and distal EEG coherence in Alzheimer’s disease. Clin. Neurophysiol. 122, 897–906. doi: 10.1016/j.clinph.2010.09.008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sanz-Arigita, E. J., Schoonheim, M. M., Damoiseaux, J. S., Rombouts, S. A., Maris, E., Barkhof, F., et al. (2010). Loss of ‘small-world’ networks in Alzheimer’s disease: graph analysis of FMRI resting-state functional connectivity. PLoS One 5:e13788. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0013788

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sloan, E. P., Fenton, G. W., Kennedy, N. S., and MacLennan, J. M. (1994). Neurophysiology and SPECT cerebral blood flow patterns in dementia. Electroencephalogr. Clin. Neurophysiol. 91, 163–170. doi: 10.1016/0013-4694(94)90066-3

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Song, Z., Deng, B., Wang, J., and Wang, R. (2019). Biomarkers for Alzheimer’s disease defined by a novel brain functional network measure. I.E.E.E. Trans. Biomed. Eng. 66, 41–49. doi: 10.1109/TBME.2018.2834546

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sorg, C., Riedl, V., Mühlau, M., Calhoun, V. D., Eichele, T., Läer, L., et al. (2007). Selective changes of resting-state networks in individuals at risk for Alzheimer’s disease. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U. S. A. 104, 18760–18765. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0708803104

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Spinnler, H., and Tognoni, G. (1987). Standardizzazione e taratura italiana di test neuropsicologici. Ital. J. Neurol. Sci. 8, 1–120.

Google Scholar

Sporns, O. (2013). Structure and function of complex brain networks. Dialogues Clin Neurosci. 15, 247–262. doi: 10.31887/DCNS.2013.15.3/osporns

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sporns, O., Honey, C. J., and Kötter, R. (2007). Identification and classification of hubs in brain networks. PLoS One 2:e1049. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0001049

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Stam, C. J. (2014). Modern network science of neurological disorders. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 15, 683–695. doi: 10.1038/nrn3801

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Stam, C. J., de Haan, W., Daffertshofer, A., Jones, B. F., Manshanden, I., Cappellen Van Wlsum, A. M., et al. (2009). Graph theoretical analysis of magnetoencephalographic functional connectivity in Alzheimer’s disease. Brain 132, 213–224. doi: 10.1093/brain/awn262

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Stam, C., Jelles, B., Achtereekte, H., Rombouts, S., Slaets, J., and Keunen, R. (1995). Investigation of EEG non-linearity in dementia and Parkinson’s disease. Electroencephalogr. Clin. Neurophysiol. 95, 309–317. doi: 10.1016/0013-4694(95)00147-Q

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Stam, C. J., Jelles, B., Achtereekte, H. A., van Birgelen, J. H., and Slaets, J. P. (1996). Diagnostic usefulness of linear and nonlinear quantitative EEG analysis in Alzheimer’s disease. Clin. Electroencephalogr. 27, 69–77. doi: 10.1177/155005949602700205

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Stam, C. J., Jones, B. F., Nolte, G., Breakspear, M., and Scheltens, P. (2007a). Small-world networks and functional connectivity in Alzheimer’s disease. Cereb. Cortex 17, 92–99. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhj127

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Stam, C. J., Nolte, G., and Daffertshofer, A. (2007b). Phase lag index: assessment of functional connectivity from multi-channel EEG and MEG with diminished bias from common sources. Hum. Brain Mapp. 28, 1178–1193. doi: 10.1002/hbm.20346

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Stam, C. J., van der Made, Y., Pijnenburg, Y. A., and Scheltens, P. (2003). EEG synchronization in mild cognitive impairment and Alzheimer’s disease. Acta Neurol. Scand. 108, 90–96. doi: 10.1034/j.1600-0404.2003.02067.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Supekar, K., Menon, V., Rubin, D., Musen, M., and Greicius, M. D. (2008). Network analysis of intrinsic functional brain connectivity in Alzheimer’s disease. PLoS Comput. Biol. 4:e1000100. doi: 10.1371/journal.pcbi.1000100

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tahami Monfared, A. A., Byrnes, M. J., White, L. A., and Zhang, Q. (2022). We Alzheimer’s disease: epidemiology and clinical progression. Neurol. Ther. 11, 553–569. doi: 10.1007/s40120-022-00338-8

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Talwar, P., Kushwaha, S., Chaturvedi, M., and Mahajan, V. (2021). Systematic review of different neuroimaging correlates in mild cognitive impairment and Alzheimer’s Disease. Clin Neuroradiol. 31, 953–967. doi: 10.1007/s00062-021-01057-7

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Teipel, S., Grothe, M. J., Zhou, J., Sepulcre, J., Dyrba, M., Sorg, C., et al. (2016). Measuring cortical connectivity in Alzheimer’s disease as a brain neural network pathology: toward clinical applications. J. Int. Neuropsychol. Soc. 22, 138–163. doi: 10.1017/S1355617715000995

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

van den Heuvel, M. P., and Sporns, O. (2011). Rich-club organization of the human connectome. J Neurosci. 31, 15775–15786. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3539-11.2011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vecchio, F., Miraglia, F., Bramanti, P., and Rossini, P. M. (2014). Human brain networks in physiological aging: a graph theoretical analysis of cortical connectivity from EEG data. J. Alzheimers Dis. 41, 1239–1249. doi: 10.3233/JAD-140090

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vecchio, F., Miraglia, F., Quaranta, D., Granata, G., Romanello, R., Marra, C., et al. (2016). Cortical connectivity and memory performance in cognitive decline: a study via graph theory from EEG data. Neuroscience 316, 143–150. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2015.12.036

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Waller, L., Brovkin, A., Dorfschmidt, L., Bzdok, D., Walter, H., and Kruschwitz, J. D. (2018). GraphVar 2.0: a user-friendly toolbox for machine learning on functional connectivity measures. J. Neurosci. Methods 308, 21–33. doi: 10.1016/j.jneumeth.2018.07.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wan, L., Huang, H., Schwab, N., Tanner, J., Rajan, A., Lam, N. B., et al. (2019). From eyes-closed to eyes-open: Role of cholinergic projections in EC-to-EO alpha reactivity revealed by combining EEG and MRI. Hum Brain Mapp. 40, 566–577. doi: 10.1002/hbm.24395

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wang, Y., Lou, F., Li, Y., Liu, F., Wang, Y., Cai, L., et al. (2021). Clinical, neuropsychological, and neuroimaging characteristics of amyloid- positive vs. amyloid-negative patients with clinically diagnosed alzheimer’s disease and amnestic mild cognitive impairment. Curr Alzheimer Res. 18, 523–532. doi: 10.2174/1567205018666211001113349

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wang, P., Zhou, B., Yao, H., Zhan, Y., Zhang, Z., Cui, Y., et al. (2015). Aberrant intra- and inter-network connectivity architectures in Alzheimer’s disease and mild cognitive impairment. Sci Rep. 5:14824. doi: 10.1038/srep14824

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wang, J., Zuo, X., Dai, Z., Xia, M., Zhao, Z., Zhao, X., et al. (2013). Disrupted functional brain connectome in individuals at risk for Alzheimer’s disease. Biol. Psychiatry 73, 472–481. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2012.03.026

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Xie, T., and He, Y. (2012). Mapping the Alzheimer’s brain with connectomics. Front. Psych. 2:77. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2011.00077

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Yao, Z., Zhang, Y., Lin, L., Zhou, Y., Xu, C., and Jiang, T. (2010). Alzheimer’s Disease Neuroimaging Initiative. Abnormal cortical networks in mild cognitive impairment and Alzheimer’s disease. PLoS Comput Biol. 6:e1001006. doi: 10.1371/journal.pcbi.1001006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Yesavage, J. A., Brink, T. L., Rose, T. L., Lum, O., Huang, V., Adey, M., et al. (1982). Development and validation of a geriatric depression screening scale: a preliminary report. J. Psychiatr. Res. 17, 37–49.

Google Scholar

Yu, M., Gouw, A. A., Hillebrand, A., Tijms, B. M., Stam, C. J., Van Straaten, E. C., et al. (2016). Different functional connectivity and network topology in behavioral variant of frontotemporal dementia and Alzheimer’s disease: an EEG study. Neurobiol. Aging 42, 150–162.

Google Scholar

Zhang, L., Biessels, G. J., Hilal, S., Chong, J. S. X., Liu, S., Shim, H. Y., et al. (2021). Cerebral microinfarcts affect brain structural network topology in cognitively impaired patients. J Cereb Blood Flow Metab. 41, 105–115. doi: 10.1177/0271678X20902187

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: resting-state eyes closed electroencephalographic (rseeg) rhythms, alzheimer’s disease with dementia (add), interdependencies of rseeg rhythms, linear lagged connectivity, graph theory, hub topology

Citation: Lopez S, Del Percio C, Lizio R, Noce G, Padovani A, Nobili F, Arnaldi D, Famà F, Moretti DV, Cagnin A, Koch G, Benussi A, Onofrj M, Borroni B, Soricelli A, Ferri R, Buttinelli C, Giubilei F, Güntekin B, Yener G, Stocchi F, Vacca L, Bonanni L and Babiloni C (2023) Patients with Alzheimer’s disease dementia show partially preserved parietal ‘hubs’ modeled from resting-state alpha electroencephalographic rhythms. Front. Aging Neurosci. 15:780014. doi: 10.3389/fnagi.2023.780014

Received: 20 September 2021; Accepted: 05 January 2023;
Published: 26 January 2023.

Edited by:

Aneta Kielar, University of Arizona, United States

Reviewed by:

Jonathan Cole, Bournemouth University, United Kingdom
Lars Meyer, Max Planck Society, Germany

Copyright © 2023 Lopez, Del Percio, Lizio, Noce, Padovani, Nobili, Arnaldi, Famà, Moretti, Cagnin, Koch, Benussi, Onofrj, Borroni, Soricelli, Ferri, Buttinelli, Giubilei, Güntekin, Yener, Stocchi, Vacca, Bonanni and Babiloni. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Susanna Lopez, ✉ susanna.lopez@uniroma1.it

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.