Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Aging Neurosci., 31 January 2022
Sec. Neurocognitive Aging and Behavior
This article is part of the Research Topic The Impact of Age-Related Changes in Brain Network Organization and Sleep on Memory View all 8 articles

The “Neural Shift” of Sleep Quality and Cognitive Aging: A Resting-State MEG Study of Transient Neural Dynamics

  • 1MRC Cognition and Brain Sciences Unit, University of Cambridge, Cambridge, United Kingdom
  • 2Department of Clinical Neurosciences, University of Cambridge, Cambridge, United Kingdom
  • 3Department of Psychology, University of Cambridge, Cambridge, United Kingdom

Sleep quality changes dramatically from young to old age, but its effects on brain dynamics and cognitive functions are not yet fully understood. We tested the hypothesis that a shift in brain networks dynamics relates to sleep quality and cognitive performance across the lifespan. Network dynamics were assessed using Hidden Markov Models (HMMs) in resting-state MEG data from a large cohort of population-based adults (N = 564, aged 18–88). Using multivariate analyses of brain-sleep profiles and brain-cognition profiles, we found an age-related “neural shift,” expressed as decreased occurrence of “lower-order” brain networks coupled with increased occurrence of “higher-order” networks. This “neural shift” was associated with both increased sleep dysfunction and decreased fluid intelligence, and this relationship was not explained by age, sex or other covariates. These results establish the link between poor sleep quality, as evident in aging, and a behavior-related shift in neural dynamics.

Introduction

With the increasing proportion of older adults in the worldwide population (Beard et al., 2016), there is a pressing need to understand the neurobiology of healthy aging. Sleep quality changes dramatically from young to older age, and might be both causative and indicative of brain changes in healthy aging (e.g., Feinberg et al., 1967; Scullin and Bliwise, 2015; Fjell et al., 2018). As people get older, sleep becomes more fragmented (e.g., Bliwise et al., 2009), less efficient (Gadie et al., 2017) and there is a decline in the quantity and quality of the “deep” stages of sleep, such as slow-wave sleep (SWS) and REM sleep (Ohayon et al., 2004; Mander et al., 2017). Among older adults, sleep problems have been associated with increased risk of developing cardiovascular disease (Wu et al., 2018), dementia (Shi et al., 2018), and mental health problems (Roberts et al., 2000). An outstanding question is how these variations in sleep quality are related to brain function and cognitive performance, which also change with age.

One way to investigate the relations between sleep quality and brain functioning is by relating sleep measures to resting-state functional connectivity. In recent decades, resting-state functional connectivity, measured mainly with functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI), have proved effective in distinguishing various patient groups from controls (e.g., Alzheimer’s disease, major depression, schizophrenia; see for example Lee et al., 2013). Substantial work has also used resting-state fMRI (rsfMRI) to examine the effects of age on functional connectivity (Andrews-Hanna et al., 2007; Grady, 2008; Ferreira and Busatto, 2013; Chan et al., 2014; Geerligs et al., 2015; Grady et al., 2016) with consequences for cognitive functioning (Tsvetanov et al., 2016; Bethlehem et al., 2020). Nevertheless, a recent attempt to relate resting-state functional connectivity to sleep quality, revealed no association between functional connectivity within or between resting-state networks and any objective or subjective sleep parameters (Lysen et al., 2020).

Decades-long investigation of sleep dysfunction and cognitive performance in healthy aging also yielded mixed results. Some studies suggest a link between poor sleep and reduced cognitive performance at older age (Feinberg et al., 1967; Hokett et al., 2021). For example, using a large cohort of healthy older adults (N∼1500, 65 + years old), Tsapanou et al. (2017) showed that poor sleep quality and longer sleep duration were linked to low memory performance. In another large-scale multicohort study, sleep problems were associated with subjective cognitive decline in multiple cognitive domains including memory, naming, orientation and calculations (Tsapanou et al., 2019). Furthermore, it has been shown that, for older adults, subjective sleep problems in later life were predictive of cognitive decline as indicated by their score in the Mini Mental Status Examination (MMSE; Folstein et al., 1975). Nevertheless, others have indicated that these relations might be more limited. In particular, following a comprehensive review of seven correlational and experimental domains, Scullin and Bliwise (2015) concluded that in older adults, variability in sleep often does not relate to cognitive functioning, and that solely improving sleep may not reverse cognitive impairments.

The discrepancy between studies may be in part due to methodological factors when investigating the effects of sleep on brain and cognitive function in healthy aging. First, the relations between sleep quality and the brain, as measured with fMRI, might be affected by age-dependent confounding factors like cerebrovascular reactivity and head motion (Power et al., 2012; Tsvetanov et al., 2015; Geerligs et al., 2017; Lehmann et al., 2017; Wu et al., 2021). While some of these confounds, like neurovascular coupling, can be addressed by more sophisticated modeling (Tsvetanov et al., 2021b), others like head-motion are notoriously difficult to correct (Maknojia et al., 2019). Moreover, the brain-sleep relations might not be fully captured by static measures of functional connectivity. In particular, dynamic fluctuations in activity and connectivity can support flexible reorganization and coordination of neural networks (e.g., Allen et al., 2014), prompting the extension of functional connectivity to further include dynamic measures (e.g., Cabral et al., 2017; Vidaurre et al., 2018). Nevertheless, the fundamentally limited temporal resolution of fMRI, owing to the sluggish hemodynamic response, precludes it from disclosing the potentially richer dynamics in brain connectivity. Finally, another important aspect to be considered is the multivariate nature of the relations between sleep patterns, cognitive patterns, and the associated neural mechanisms (Gadie et al., 2017).

In addressing these issues, we related transient resting-state neural dynamics to sleep quality, cognitive performance, and age, using a multi-level multivariate approach (Passamonti et al., 2019; Tsvetanov et al., 2021a; Tibon et al., 2021). We used magnetoencephalography (MEG) as a direct measure of neural activity sampled at 1 kHz and higher. Unlike fMRI, MEG is not affected by age-related changes in vascular factors (Tsvetanov et al., 2015), and allows simpler and more robust methods for correcting head-motion artifacts (e.g., Taulu and Simola, 2006). We used measures of resting-state MEG dynamic functional connectivity, inferred in our previous study (Tibon et al., 2021) from a population-based adult-lifespan cohort (18 to 88 years of age1), using Hidden Markov Models. These measures capture transient states of activity and connectivity signatures lasting a few hundred milliseconds (HMM; Baker et al., 2014; Vidaurre et al., 2016, 2017, 2018; Brookes et al., 2018; Hawkins et al., 2020). In addition to MEG scanning, these individuals also completed a self-report sleep questionnaire (PSQI: Pittsburgh Sleep Quality Index; Buysse et al., 1989) and a wide range of cognitive tasks. In the current study, we used Partial Least Squares (PLS) to relate patterns of neural dynamics to profiles of sleep quality and cognitive performance.

Using similar methods, we have previously observed a “neural shift,” expressed as increased occurrence of brain states involving “higher-order” networks and decreased occurrence of brain states that involve early visual networks. This neural shift was associated with both increased age and decreased fluid intelligence, suggesting that it likely reflects reduction in neural efficiency rather than compensation (Tibon et al., 2021). In light of the reduction in neural efficiency account, we predicted that the neural shift will be associated with the level of sleep dysfunction and that this association is distinguishable from the effects of age.

Materials and Methods

Participants

A flow diagram of the inclusion process is shown in Figure 1. A population-based sample of 708 healthy human adults (359 women and 349 men) was recruited as part of Stage 2 of the Cambridge Centre for Aging and Neuroscience (Cam-CAN; see text footnote 1; Shafto et al., 2014). Ethical approval for the study was obtained from the Cambridgeshire 2 (now East of England-Cambridge Central Research Ethics Committee), and participants gave full informed consent. Exclusion criteria included poor vision (below 20/50 on Snellen test; Snellen, 1862) and poor hearing (threshold 35 dB at 1,000 Hz in both ears), ongoing or serious past drug abuse as assessed by the Drug Abuse Screening Test (DAST-20; Skinner, 1982), significant psychiatric disorder (e.g., schizophrenia, bipolar disorder, personality disorder), neurological disease (e.g., known stroke, epilepsy, traumatic brain injury), low score in the Mini Mental State Exam (MMSE; 24 or lower; Folstein et al., 1975), or poor English knowledge (non-native or non-bilingual

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Flow diagram of the inclusion process for the study.

English speakers); a detailed description of the exclusion criteria can be found in Shafto et al. (2014), Table 1. Of these, only participants who were considered for our previous study (Tibon et al., 2021; N = 594, following the removal of 98 participants who did not have full neuroimaging data, 15 participants with poor MEG-MRI co-registration, and one participant who had no visits to one of the HMM states) were included. In addition, 30 participants who were included in Tibon et al. (2021), but did not have full PSQI data (in all seven measures of sleep quality), were excluded from the current study. Thus, the final sample included 564 participants (age range 18–88; see Table 1 for participants’ characteristics).

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Sample Characteristics.

Sleep Measures

Sleep quality was assessed using the PSQI (Buysse et al., 1989), a well-validated self-report questionnaire designed to assist in the diagnosis of sleep disorders. The questions are grouped into seven components including overall sleep quality, sleep latency, sleep duration, sleep efficiency, sleep disturbance, sleep medication use, and daytime dysfunction due to sleepiness. Participants’ scores in these components were used in our analyses. Each of the sleep components yields a score on an ordinal scale, ranging from 0 (good sleep/no problems) to 3 (poor sleep/severe problems), with higher scores reflecting greater dysfunction. Scores were obtained from Gadie et al. (2017).

Cognitive Tasks

Thirteen cognitive tasks, performed outside the scanner, were used to assess five broad cognitive domains, including executive function (fluid intelligence, multitasking), memory (visual short-term memory, story recall), language (spot the word, sentence comprehension, picture-picture priming, verbal fluency, proverb comprehension), processing speed (choice motor speed, choice motor coefficient of variation) and emotional processing (face recognition, emotion expression recognition). The tasks are fully detailed in (Shafto et al., 2014). Task scores, also used in our previous study (Tibon et al., 2021; see Table 1 for full details), were obtained from Borgeest et al. (2018), in which missing data (< 12% in all tasks) were interpolated using Full Information Maximum Likelihood (Enders and Bandalos, 2001) across the full Stage 2 sample (n = 708), as implemented in the Lavaan R package (Rosseel, 2012).

Magnetoencephalography Resting-State Data

MEG resting-state data included 4 temporal characteristics for 8 inferred brain states (i.e., 32 measures overall). The acquisition, preprocessing, and analysis pipeline are fully described in Tibon et al. (2021), and are summarized in Figure 2. In short, MEG data were recorded from each participant while resting in a 306-channel VectorView MEG system (Elekta Neuromag, Helsinki). Each resting-state scan lasted 8 min and 40 s, and the entire duration of the scan was included in the analysis. The data were preprocessed using MaxFilter 2.2.12 software (Elekta Neuromag Oy, Helsinki, Finland) SPM122, and the OHBA Software Library (OSL3), and co-registered to each participant’s structural T1-weighted MRI. Source space activity was then estimated for each participant at every point of an 8 mm whole-brain grid comprised of 3,559 grid points, using a single-shell lead-field model and a linearly constrained minimum variance (LCMV) scalar beamformer (Van Veen et al., 1997; Woolrich et al., 2011), parceled into 38 regions of interest (ROIs; as in Colclough et al., 2015), and summarized by the first principal component across grid points within that parcel. The amplitude envelope of each parcel’s time-course was then calculated using a Hilbert transform.

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Overview of processing and analysis pipeline, adapted from Tibon et al. (2021).

Group-level exploratory analysis of networks (GLEAN4; Vidaurre et al., 2017) was then applied to the temporally-concatenated envelope data across all participants, in order to infer 8 brain states via Hidden Markov Modeling (HMM). As reported in Tibon et al. (2021), we selected a priori eight states, as this number represents a reasonable trade-off between a sufficiently rich but not overly complex representation of network dynamics in resting-state MEG data (Baker et al., 2014). HMMs describe the dynamics of neural activity as a sequence of transient events, each of which corresponds to a visit to a particular brain state. Each state describes the data as coming from a unique 38-dimensional multivariate normal distribution, defined by a covariance matrix and a mean vector. Therefore, each state corresponds to a unique pattern of amplitude envelope variance and covariance that reoccurs at different time points. The HMM state time-courses then define the points in time at which each state was “active” or “visited.” These estimated state time-courses, represented by a binary sequence showing the points in time when that state was most probable, were obtained using the Viterbi algorithm (Rezek and Roberts, 2005). Using these time-courses, the temporal characteristics of each state were quantified according to four measures of interest: (1) Fractional Occupancy (FO): the proportion of time the state was active; (2) Mean Life Time (MLT): the average time spent in the state before transitioning to another state; (3) Number of Occurrences (NO): the number of times the state was active; and (4) Mean Interval Length (MIL): the average duration between recurring visits to that state. The spatial and temporal characteristics of the HMM states are fully described in Tibon et al., 2021; Figures 2, 3). The states include three distributed frontotemporoparietal networks (FTP1, FTP2, FTP3), a higher-order visual network (HOV), two early visual networks (EV1, EV2) and two sensorimotor networks (SM1, SM2). The spatial maps associated with the states are shown in Figure 3, Panel A.

FIGURE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 3. (A) Loadings obtained via the PLS analysis relating neural dynamics (HMM) measures with sleep measures. Solid outlines represent loadings greater than | 0.2|, whereas dashed outlines represent loadings smaller than | 0.2| (see Smith et al., 2015 and Tibon et al., 2021, using the same cut-off value). Loadings for network measures are shown in different colors, representing different types of states. HMM measures are indicated as FO (fractional occupancy, MLT (mean lifetime), NO (number of occurrences), and MIL (mean interval length). The various states are indicated as FTP (frontotemporoparietal), HOV (higher-order visual), EV (early-visual) and SM (sensorimotor). Corresponding HMM state maps (obtained from Tibon et al., 2021) are inset. For clarity, loadings for each network are shown separately, although in practice all equally contributed to a single PLS analysis. Loadings for the sleep measures are shown in gray (bottom-left panel). Sleep measures are sleep quality, latency, duration, efficiency, disturbance, sleep medication use (Meds), and daytime dysfunction (DayDis). (B) Scatter plot of the bivariate association between the loadings for the HMM measures obtained via the brain-sleep PLS analysis and the HMM measures obtained via the brain-cognition PLS analysis. Different colors reflect different types of states (FTP, HOV, EV, or SM), and correspond to the same color coding used in Panel A. (C) Scatter plot of the bivariate association between subject scores for the HMM brain profile obtained via the brain-sleep PLS analysis and the HMM brain profile obtained via the brain-cognition PLS analysis. Each point represents the score for a given individual in the analysis. Age is color-coded such that darker colors represent younger age. Two outliers were removed for the purpose of this visualization. This removal did not change the results (i.e., the correlation between these measures slightly increased and remained highly significant, r = 0.81, p < 0.0001). In this plot, each data-point represents one participant, whereas in panel (B) above each data-point represents one measure.

Relating Brain States to Sleep Quality and Cognition

For the brain-sleep analysis, we adopted a two-level procedure (Passamonti et al., 2019; Tsvetanov et al., 2021a). In the first-level analysis, we assessed the multidimensional relationships between temporal characteristics of the HMM states and sleep quality using PLS implementation in Matlab, the Mathworks Inc. This analysis describes the linear relationships between the two multivariate data sets by providing pairs of latent factors, as linear combinations of the original variables that are optimized to maximize their covariance. It is similar to the canonical correlation analysis (CCA) used in our previous study (Tibon et al., 2021), which instead maximizes the correlation between the latent variables. Although both CCA and PLS are useful to characterize relationships between two datasets, PLS has been suggested as a more appropriate tool for mixed datasets (Grellmann et al., 2015; Beaton et al., 2019), as is in our case, with the continuous and ordinal nature of the HMM and sleep data, respectively. All variables were z-scored before being subjected to the PLS analysis. First, we used a permutation-based PLS with 10,000 permutations (by shuffling subjects in the sleep dataset) to relate the 4 temporal characteristics across all 8 HMM states (Set 1, 32 variables) to the 7 sleep measures (i.e., the seven components from the PSQI questionnaire: overall sleep quality, sleep latency, sleep duration, sleep efficiency, sleep disturbance, sleep medication use, and daytime dysfunction due to sleepiness; Set 2).

Once we established the relationships between the HMM brain measures and the sleep measures, we asked whether the relationship between the HMM profile and the sleep profile (i.e., the relations between the participants’ scores on the latent variables obtained by the PLS analysis) varied with age, using a moderation analysis (see Tsvetanov et al., 2016, 2018, for a similar approach with different measures). Specifically, we constructed a second-level multiple linear model where HMM scores (on the latent variable), age, and their interaction term (HMM scores × age) were used as independent variables, and sleep scores (for the paired latent variable) were used as the dependent variable (all statistical tests were two-sided).

We then conducted another PLS analysis to relate the 32 HMM measures (Set 1) to the 13 cognitive measures (Set 2). This analysis resembles that in our previous study (Tibon et al., 2021) but uses PLS (instead of CCA) and a somewhat different sample (due to the exclusion of additional participants for which sleep data were not available). Similarly to our previous report, this analysis revealed a neural “shift” expressed as decreased occurrence of “lower-order” brain networks, and increased occurrence of “higher-order” networks, which was associated with decreased fluid intelligence. The reason to repeat this analysis in the current study was to obtain PLS scores and loadings that would be comparable with those obtained by the abovementioned PLS analysis, relating the HMM states to the sleep measures. Using these comparable scores, we asked whether the neural shift that was observed in our previous study, is also related to the pattern of sleep quality observed in the current study. To this end, we correlated both the HMM loadings and the profile scores (in two separate analyses), obtained by the first PLS analysis (i.e., which related HMM states to sleep quality) with the HMM loadings and profile scores obtained by the second PLS analysis (which related HMM states to cognition). Significant correlations would indicate that the pattern that was obtained for sleep quality and the pattern that was obtained for cognition are associated with the same neural pattern.

Results

Relating Brain States to Sleep Measures

Our first step was to apply PLS to relate the 32 temporal characteristics of the HMM states (four metrics for each of the 8 states) to the seven sleep measures. This analysis identified one significant pair of latent factors (p = 0.01, based on a null distribution of 10,000 permutations). Figure 3A presents the loadings of this significant pair. For Set 1 (HMM data), the three frontotemporoparietal states (FTP1, FTP2, FTP3), and one of the sensorimotor states (SM1) showed positive loadings for the FO and NO measures, and negative loadings for the MIL measure. Furthermore, the first early visual state (EV1) showed negative loadings for FO, MLT, and NO and positive loadings for MIL. The second early visual state (EV2) displayed a similar pattern to that of EV1 for NO and for MIL, but not for NO (in which case the loadings for EV2 were positive). For the higher order visual state (HOV) and the second sensorimotor state (SM2) most loadings were lower than the threshold (±0.2), and those that exceeded the threshold—NO for HOV and MLT for SM2—were still relatively low. We therefore chose to exclude these states from further interpretations.

For Set 2 (sleep data), all of the components showed positive loadings (reflecting greater sleep dysfunction). The highest loadings were obtained for sleep efficiency, followed by sleep duration, and use of sleep medications. The lowest loadings were obtained for daytime dysfunction due to sleepiness. Taken together, poor sleep quality was associated with more and longer occurrences of states involving frontotemporoparietal regions and a state involving sensorimotor regions, and fewer, shorter occurrences of an early visual state.

The results obtained with the PLS analysis identified a neural shift that resembles the one related to cognitive decline as observed in our previous study (Tibon et al., 2021). However, before exploring this association further, we asked whether the relationship between the HMM brain profile and the sleep profile differs across the lifespan, and whether it exists above and beyond the relation between the HMM brain profile and age. For this moderation analysis, we constructed a multiple linear regression model that included participants’ scores for the HMM profile, their age and the interaction (HMM profile × age) as predictors, and participants’ scores for the sleep profile as the dependent variable. The HMM scores were significantly associated with sleep scores after accounting for the main effect of age [β = 0.05, t(560) = 2.4, p = 0.017], demonstrating that the established brain-sleep relationship was not driven solely by age effects. The sleep scores were further associated with age [β = −0.29, t(560) = −5.5, p < 0.001], verifying previous findings of age-related decline in sleep quality (e.g., Gadie et al., 2017). The interaction between age and HMM profile was not significant [β = 0.01, t(560) = 0.63, p = 0.5]. We then repeated the moderation analysis after including additional variables as potential covariates. The results did not change in meaningful way (see full details in the Supplementary Material).

Relating Brain States to Cognition (Verification of Previous Findings)

Next, we applied PLS to relate the 32 temporal characteristics of the HMM states (four metrics for each of the 8 states) to the 13 cognitive measures. This verifies the results of our previous study (Tibon et al., 2021), but uses a comparable method and sample to those that were used here, to test the association between HMM states and sleep. This analysis revealed a significant pair of latent variables (p = 0.01), depicting a pattern that highly resembled the pattern obtained in our previous study (two additional significant pairs depicted a different pattern, and were not explored further). The loadings obtained for this pair are shown in Supplementary Figure 1, together with the loadings obtained via CCA as in our previous study, in order to allow direct comparison. Although some of the loadings changed slightly, they were highly comparable with our previous results. Thus, here too, we observed that greater involvement of frontotemporoparietal states and reduced involvement of early visual states are associated with decreased fluid intelligence.

Correspondence Between Brain Profiles

Our final step was to investigate whether the neural shift that was associated with sleep dysfunction is the same as the one that was associated with decreased fluid intelligence. To this end, we correlated the HMM loadings obtained from the PLS analysis that related the HMM states to sleep measures, with the HMM loadings obtained from the PLS analysis that related the HMM states to cognitive measures. The correlation between these measures was highly significant, r = 0.83, p < 0.0001. However, as shown in Figure 3B, some states were more similar across the two PLS analyses than others. In particular, whereas the loadings obtained for the frontotemporoparietal, high-order visual, and somatosensory states were highly similar (r = 0.98, r = 92, and r = 97, respectively), noticeable variations were observed for the early-visual states (r = 0.64). Finally, we correlated participants’ scores for the HMM profile obtained from the PLS analysis that related the HMM states to sleep measures with their scores for the HMM profile obtained from the PLS analysis that related the HMM states to cognitive measures. The correlation between these measures was also highly significant, r = 0.78, p < 0.0001.

Discussion

The results of our study show that transient neural dynamics, particularly those of frontotemporoparietal and early-visual states, are associated with sleep dysfunction. We found that increased sleep dysfunction is associated with increased occurrence of brain states involving “higher-order” networks and decreased occurrence of a brain state that involves an early visual network. Importantly, the same neural pattern was associated with decreased fluid intelligence and increased age (originally reported in Tibon et al., 2021, and verified in the current study). In our previous study (Tibon et al., 2021), borrowing from the approach that was applied to explain the posterior-to-anterior shift with aging (PASA), commonly observed with fMRI during task (Grady et al., 1994; West, 2000; Glisky et al., 2001; Park et al., 2004; Raz and Rodrigue, 2006; Davis et al., 2008; Park and Reuter-Lorenz, 2009; Grady, 2012; Nyberg et al., 2012; Morcom and Henson, 2018), we considered two competing accounts for the shift from “lower” to “higher” networks that we have observed. The functional compensation hypothesis suggests that greater activation of higher-order regions serves to compensate for impairments in posterior brain regions, in order to maintain levels of cognitive performance. Thus, under the compensation account, we expect greater activation to correlate with an attenuated age-related decrease in cognitive performance. Alternatively, the inefficiency account suggests instead that the increased activation in higher-order regions reflects reduced neural efficiency or specificity. The crucial difference between these two accounts is that, whereas the functional compensation hypothesis predicts that the shift would correlate with better cognitive performance, the inefficiency account predicts the opposite pattern. Our previous findings that the neural shift was associated with worse cognitive performance, suggested that it represents reduced neural efficiency, thereby supporting the latter account. The current study provides further support for the neural inefficiency account by verifying the relations between the neural shift and decreased cognition (observed in our previous study) with another analytical approach, and by showing that the neural shift is further related to another maladaptive pattern—increased sleep dysfunction.

The relationship between brain dynamics and sleep disturbance remained significant to other factors including chronological age. While the brain profile was associated with age (as was also demonstrated in our previous study, Tibon et al., 2021), it was further associated with the sleep profile above and beyond its relations to age. This suggests that the established relationship between brain and sleep cannot be solely explained by age. Notably, these results diverge from those obtained in a recent study by Lysen et al. (2020), in which sleep parameters were not associated with resting-state functional connectivity. Nevertheless, there exist important differences between the study by Lysen et al. (2020) and the current study. Indeed, by using dynamic measures of functional connectivity, the current study includes another dimension that goes beyond the static measures that were employed by Lysen et al. (2020). Another consideration is that the hemodynamically convolved signal measured by fMRI as in Lysen et al. (2020) is limited to low frequencies and may be unable to detect the transient neural dynamics that we observed here with MEG.

By using a novel data-driven method to infer brain states from MEG data, we were able to overcome some of the limitations of the more common use of fMRI to examine functional connectivity, such as confounding effects of vascular health, head motion, and the ability to examine only very slow dynamics owing to low-frequency fluctuations of the fMRI response. Moreover, by using multivariate analyses to relate patterns of neural dynamics to patterns of sleep quality and cognitive performance, we were able to investigate multifaceted profiles that are not captured well by summary measures and univariate analysis. Indeed, not all measures were equally associated with the neural pattern. Specifically, measures of fluid intelligence were highly associated with increased occurrence “higher-order” brain states and decreased occurrence of early visual brain states, whereas measures of crystallized intelligence were not. The pattern of findings was consistent with previous reports (Tibon et al., 2021). A novel finding of the current study, was the association between the observed neural pattern and overall increase in sleep dysfunction. The strength of the association with individual sleep measures varied: it was highly associated with sleep duration, efficiency, and sleep medication use (loadings > 0.5), moderately associated with sleep quality, latency, and disturbance (loadings > 0.2), and only loosely associated with daytime dysfunction due to sleepiness.

Using latent class analysis (LCA), Gadie et al. (2017) were able to classify individuals into four different classes representing “sleep types,” associated with distinct profiles of “sleep symptoms.” The probability of an individual showing the component profile associated with each class changed as a function of age for three of these classes labeled “good sleepers,” “inefficient sleepers,” and “delayed sleepers.” The component profile of the class labeled “inefficient sleepers” resembled the pattern observed in the current study (though notably, it was also associated with increased sleep latency). This suggests that while age is associated with a general reduction in sleep quality, the neural shift observed in our study represents a more specific pattern of reduction. Importantly, our study shows that the relations between the neural shift and the sleep pattern cannot be fully attributed to age as a moderating factor (i.e., in our moderation analysis, these relations remained significant when controlling for age). Thus, in addition to its relation to aging, the neural shift is further associated with inefficient sleep patterns that exist beyond age, throughout the entire adult lifespan.

An important factor that was associated with the neural shift observed in the current study was usage of sleep medications (3rd highest loadings). Interestingly, studies have shown that some sleep medications are associated with changes in functional connectivity. For example, in a study by Pflanz et al. (2015), resting-state functional connectivity was investigated with fMRI, following seven-day diazepam administration. The authors found increased connectivity in response to diazepam administration in the medial visual network and middle/inferior temporal network. Furthermore, in a recent study (Frölich et al., 2020) with a sample of older adults (aged 55–73), infusion of midazolam resulted in increased rsfMRI functional connectivity between the dorsal default mode network and the posterior salience network. In the current study additional data regarding the kind of sleep medications and/or the dosage that was used are not readily available (the sparse data that were obtained are reported in Table 1), nor can we infer any causal relations between sleep medications and functional connectivity. Therefore, potential associations between specific prescriptions and neural patterns cannot be explored further. It is important to note, however, that while it is unlikely that the neural shift is solely explained by usage of sleep medications (as other sleep measures also had substantial contribution), it is still associated with this factor to some extent. These relations between sleep medications and neural patterns should be explored further in future studies, especially given our finding that the neural pattern that is associated with increased use of medications is linked to reduced cognitive performance and neural inefficiency.

In the current study, we observed a strong correlation between the loadings of the HMM measures obtained via the brain-sleep PLS analysis, and the HMM measures obtained via the brain-cognition PLS analysis, suggesting that they represent a highly similar pattern. Nevertheless, not all HMM measures were similarly comparable. In particular, while the HMM measures describing the frontotempoparietal states were highly similar, those describing the early-visual states were not. This is mainly because the observed sleep pattern was only reliably related to one of the early-visual states, whereas the observed pattern of cognitive performance was associated with both. We speculate that the reason for this discrepancy is that cognitive performance depend more on the visual system than sleep quality, and are therefore more strongly associated with particular neural patterns that involve these regions.

One limitation of our study is that we used cross-sectional data, which precludes direct inferences about aging. However, we are not aware of any longitudinal MEG data on such a large, representative population, thus our results could be useful to generate hypotheses for future studies. The limitations of the MEG methods used in this study are thoroughly discussed in Tibon et al. (2021). In short, some properties of the assumptions and application of the HMM approach (e.g., group concatenation, Gaussian observation model, coarse percolation of ROIs, and a priori specification of 8 states) might result in oversimplification of the underlying neural dynamics. Nevertheless, some level of simplification is necessary for robust and interpretable modeling. Once the basic patterns are established, the parameters of the models can be adjusted to allow further optimization [see also discussion in Baker et al. (2014) and Quinn et al. (2018)]. Moreover, the current study used subjective reports of sleep quality. Although the PSQI questionnaire used for this purpose is well validated, it is yet to be determined whether the neural pattern that we have observed also correlates with objective measures of sleep quality (e.g., duration spent in Slow Wave Sleep; see review of various sleep measures in Hokett et al., 2021). Finally, the answers to the sleep questionnaire referred to the last month before the evaluation, and may not accurately represent long-term sleep patterns. Despite these limitations, our study verifies our previous findings, and further offers novel insights on the relationships between patterns of functional neural dynamics and sleep dysfunction in cognitive aging.

Members of Cam-CAN Group

The Cam-CAN group consists of the project principal personnel: Lorraine K. Tyler, Carol Brayne, Edward T. Bullmore, Andrew C. Calder, Rhodri Cusack, Tim Dalgleish, John Duncan, Richard N. Henson, Fiona E. Matthews, William D. Marslen-Wilson, James B. Rowe, Meredith A. Shafto; Research Associates: Karen Campbell, Teresa Cheung, Simon Davis, Linda Geerligs, Rogier Kievit, Anna McCarrey, Abdur Mustafa, Darren Price, David Samu, Jason R. Taylor, Matthias Treder, Kamen A. Tsvetanov, Janna van Belle, Nitin Williams; Research Assistants: Lauren Bates, Tina Emery, Sharon Erzinc, lioglu, Andrew Gadie, Sofia Gerbase, Stanimira Georgieva, Claire Hanley, Beth Parkin, David Troy; Research Interviewers: Jodie Allen, Gillian Amery, Liana Amunts, Anne Barcroft, Amanda Castle, Cheryl Dias, Jonathan Dowrick, Melissa Fair, Hayley Fisher, Anna Goulding, Adarsh Grewal, Geoff Hale, Andrew Hilton, Frances Johnson, Patricia Johnston, Thea Kavanagh-Williamson, Magdalena Kwasniewska, Alison McMinn, Kim Norman, Jessica Penrose, Fiona Roby, Diane Rowland, John Sargeant, Maggie Squire, Beth Stevens, Aldabra Stoddart, Cheryl Stone, Tracy Thompson, Ozlem Yazlik; and administrative staff: Dan Barnes, Marie Dixon, Jaya Hillman, Joanne Mitchell, and Laura Villis.

Data Availability Statement

Raw data from the Cam-CAN project are available from http://camcan-archive.mrc-cbu.cam.ac.uk, subject to conditions specified on that website. For a complete description of Cam-CAN data and pipelines, see Shafto et al. (2014) and Taylor et al. (2017). In addition, pre-processed mean data used for analyses and figures, together with all the analysis code, are available at https://osf.io/6nh5w/.

Ethics Statement

The studies involving human participants were reviewed and approved by Cambridgeshire 2 (now East of England-Cambridge Central Research Ethics Committee). The participants provided their written informed consent to participate in this study.

Author Contributions

RT designed, planned, carried out the analyses, and wrote the manuscript. KT consulted on the analyses and provided input on the manuscript. All authors contributed to the article and approved the submitted version.

Funding

The Cambridge Centre for Aging and Neuroscience (Cam-CAN) research was supported by the Biotechnology and Biological Sciences Research Council (BB/H008217/1). RT was supported by a British Academy Postdoctoral Fellowship (PF170046: SUAI/028 RG94188). KT was supported by the Guarantors of Brain (G101149). The funders had no role in study design, data collection and analysis, decision to publish, or preparation of the manuscript.

Conflict of Interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher’s Note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

Acknowledgments

We would like to thank Noham Wolpe for helping with classification of sleep medications. We are grateful to the Cam-CAN respondents and their primary care teams in Cambridge for their participation in this study. We would also like to thank colleagues at the MRC Cognition and Brain Sciences Unit MEG and MRI facilities for their assistance.

Supplementary Material

The Supplementary Material for this article can be found online at: https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fnagi.2021.746236/full#supplementary-material

Footnotes

  1. ^ www.cam-can.org
  2. ^ http://www.fil.ion.ucl.ac.uk/spm
  3. ^ https://ohba-analysis.github.io/osl-docs/
  4. ^ https://github.com/OHBA-analysis/GLEAN

References

Allen, E. A., Damaraju, E., Plis, S. M., Erhardt, E. B., Eichele, T., and Calhoun, V. D. (2014). Tracking whole-brain connectivity dynamics in the resting state. Cereb. Cortex 24, 663–676. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhs352

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Andrews-Hanna, J. R., Snyder, A. Z., Vincent, J. L., Lustig, C., Head, D., Raichle, M. E., et al. (2007). Disruption of large-scale brain systems in advanced aging. Neuron 56, 924–935. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2007.10.038

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Baker, A. P., Brookes, M. J., Rezek, I. A., Smith, S. M., Behrens, T., Probert Smith, P. J., et al. (2014). Fast transient networks in spontaneous human brain activity. eLife 3:e01867. doi: 10.7554/eLife.01867

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Beard, J. R., Officer, A. M., and Cassels, A. K. (2016). The world report on ageing and health. Gerontologist 56(Suppl. 2), S163–S166. doi: 10.1093/geront/gnw037

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Beaton, D., ADNI, Saporta, G., and Abdi, H. (2019). A generalization of partial least squares regression and correspondence analysis for categorical and mixed data: an application with the ADNI data. bioRxiv [Preprint]. doi: 10.1101/598888

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bethlehem, R. A. I., Paquola, C., Seidlitz, J., Ronan, L., Bernhardt, B., Consortium, C.-C., et al. (2020). Dispersion of functional gradients across the adult lifespan. Neuroimage 222:117299. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2020.117299

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bliwise, D. L., Foley, D. J., Vitiello, M. V., Ansari, F. P., Ancoli-Israel, S., and Walsh, J. K. (2009). Nocturia and disturbed sleep in the elderly. Sleep Med. 10, 540–548. doi: 10.1016/j.sleep.2008.04.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Borgeest, G. S., Henson, R., Shafto, M., Samu, D., and Cam-CAN, and Kievit, R. (2018). Greater lifestyle engagement is associated with better cognitive resilience. PsyArXiv [Preprint]. doi: 10.31234/osf.io/6pzve

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Brookes, M. J., Groom, M. J., Liuzzi, L., Hill, R. M., Smith, H. J. F., Briley, P. M., et al. (2018). Altered temporal stability in dynamic neural networks underlies connectivity changes in neurodevelopment. Neuroimage 174, 563–575. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2018.03.008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Buysse, D. J., Reynolds, C. F., Monk, T. H., Berman, S. R., and Kupfer, D. J. (1989). The Pittsburgh sleep quality index: a new instrument for psychiatric practice and research. Psychiatry Res. 28, 193–213. doi: 10.1016/0165-1781(89)90047-4

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cabral, J., Vidaurre, D., Marques, P., Magalhães, R., Silva Moreira, P., Miguel Soares, J., et al. (2017). Cognitive performance in healthy older adults relates to spontaneous switching between states of functional connectivity during rest. Sci. Rep. 7:5135. doi: 10.1038/s41598-017-05425-7

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chan, M. Y., Park, D. C., Savalia, N. K., Petersen, S. E., and Wig, G. S. (2014). Decreased segregation of brain systems across the healthy adult lifespan. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 111, E4997–E5006. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1415122111

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Colclough, G. L., Brookes, M. J., Smith, S. M., and Woolrich, M. W. (2015). A symmetric multivariate leakage correction for MEG connectomes. Neuroimage 117, 439–448. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2015.03.071

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Davis, S. W., Dennis, N. A., Daselaar, S. M., Fleck, M. S., and Cabeza, R. (2008). Qué PASA? The posterior–anterior shift in aging. Cereb. Cortex 18, 1201–1209. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhm155

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Enders, C. K., and Bandalos, D. L. (2001). The relative performance of full information maximum likelihood estimation for missing data in structural equation models. Struct. Equ. Model. Multidiscip. J. 8, 430–457. doi: 10.1207/S15328007SEM0803_5

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Feinberg, I., Koresko, R. L., and Heller, N. (1967). EEG sleep patterns as a function of normal and pathological aging in man. J. Psychiatr. Res. 5, 107–144. doi: 10.1016/0022-3956(67)90027-1

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ferreira, L. K., and Busatto, G. F. (2013). Resting-state functional connectivity in normal brain aging. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 37, 384–400. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2013.01.017

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fjell, A. M., Idland, A.-V., Sala-Llonch, R., Watne, L. O., Borza, T., Brækhus, A., et al. (2018). Neuroinflammation and tau interact with amyloid in predicting sleep problems in aging independently of atrophy. Cereb. Cortex 28, 2775–2785. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhx157

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Folstein, M. F., Folstein, S. E., and McHugh, P. R. (1975). “Mini-mental state”. A practical method for grading the cognitive state of patients for the clinician. J. Psychiatr. Res. 12, 189–198. doi: 10.1016/0022-3956(75)90026-6

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Frölich, M. A., White, D. M., Kraguljac, N. V., and Lahti, A. C. (2020). Baseline functional connectivity predicts connectivity changes due to a small dose of midazolam in older adults. Anesth. Analg. 130, 224–232. doi: 10.1213/ANE.0000000000004385

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gadie, A., Shafto, M., Leng, Y., and Cam-CAN, and Kievit, R. A. (2017). How are age-related differences in sleep quality associated with health outcomes? An epidemiological investigation in a UK cohort of 2406 adults. BMJ Open 7:e014920. doi: 10.1136/bmjopen-2016-014920

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Geerligs, L., Renken, R. J., Saliasi, E., Maurits, N. M., and Lorist, M. M. (2015). A brain-wide study of age-related changes in functional connectivity. Cereb. Cortex 25, 1987–1999. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhu012

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Geerligs, L., Tsvetanov, K. A., and Cam-Can, and Henson, R. N. (2017). Challenges in measuring individual differences in functional connectivity using fMRI: the case of healthy aging. Hum. Brain Mapp. 38, 4125–4156. doi: 10.1002/hbm.23653

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Glisky, E. L., Rubin, S. R., and Davidson, P. S. R. (2001). Source memory in older adults: an encoding or retrieval problem? J. Exp. Psychol. Learn. Mem. Cogn. 27, 1131–1146. doi: 10.1037/0278-7393.27.5.1131

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Grady, C. (2012). The cognitive neuroscience of ageing. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 13, 491–505. doi: 10.1038/nrn3256

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Grady, C., Sarraf, S., Saverino, C., and Campbell, K. (2016). Age differences in the functional interactions among the default, frontoparietal control, and dorsal attention networks. Neurobiol. Aging 41, 159–172. doi: 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2016.02.020

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Grady, C. L. (2008). Cognitive neuroscience of aging. Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 1124, 127–144. doi: 10.1196/annals.1440.009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Grady, C. L., Maisog, J. M., Horwitz, B., Ungerleider, L. G., Mentis, M. J., Salerno, J. A., et al. (1994). Age-related changes in cortical blood flow activation during visual processing of faces and location. J. Neurosci. 14(3 Pt 2), 1450–1462.

Google Scholar

Grellmann, C., Bitzer, S., Neumann, J., Westlye, L. T., Andreassen, O. A., Villringer, A., et al. (2015). Comparison of variants of canonical correlation analysis and partial least squares for combined analysis of MRI and genetic data. Neuroimage 107, 289–310. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2014.12.025

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hawkins, E., Akarca, D., Zhang, M., Brkiæ, D., Woolrich, M., Baker, K., et al. (2020). Functional network dynamics in a neurodevelopmental disorder of known genetic origin. Hum. Brain Mapp. 41, 530–544. doi: 10.1002/hbm.24820

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hokett, E., Arunmozhi, A., Campbell, J., Verhaeghen, P., and Duarte, A. (2021). A systematic review and meta-analysis of individual differences in naturalistic sleep quality and episodic memory performance in young and older adults. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 127, 675–688. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2021.05.010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lee, M. H., Smyser, C. D., and Shimony, J. S. (2013). Resting-state fMRI: a review of methods and clinical applications. Am. J. Neuroradiol. 34, 1866–1872. doi: 10.3174/ajnr.A3263

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lehmann, B. C. L., White, S. R., Henson, R. N., and Cam-CAN, and Geerligs, L. (2017). Assessing dynamic functional connectivity in heterogeneous samples. Neuroimage 157, 635–647. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2017.05.065

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lysen, T. S., Zonneveld, H. I., Muetzel, R. L., Ikram, M. A., Luik, A. I., Vernooij, M. W., et al. (2020). Sleep and resting-state functional magnetic resonance imaging connectivity in middle-aged adults and the elderly: a population-based study. J. Sleep Res. 29:e12999. doi: 10.1111/jsr.12999

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Maknojia, S., Churchill, N. W., Schweizer, T. A., and Graham, S. J. (2019). Resting state fMRI: going through the motions. Front. Neurosci. 13:825. doi: 10.3389/fnins.2019.00825

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mander, B. A., Winer, J. R., and Walker, M. P. (2017). Sleep and human aging. Neuron 94, 19–36. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2017.02.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Morcom, A. M., and Henson, R. N. A. (2018). Increased prefrontal activity with aging reflects nonspecific neural responses rather than compensation. J. Neurosci. 38, 7303–7313. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1701-17.2018

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nyberg, L., Lövdén, M., Riklund, K., Lindenberger, U., and Bäckman, L. (2012). Memory aging and brain maintenance. Trends Cogn. Sci. 16, 292–305. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2012.04.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ohayon, M. M., Carskadon, M. A., Guilleminault, C., and Vitiello, M. V. (2004). Meta-analysis of quantitative sleep parameters from childhood to old age in healthy individuals: developing normative sleep values across the human lifespan. Sleep 27, 1255–1273. doi: 10.1093/sleep/27.7.1255

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Park, D. C., Polk, T. A., Park, R., Minear, M., Savage, A., and Smith, M. R. (2004). Aging reduces neural specialization in ventral visual cortex. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 101, 13091–13095. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0405148101

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Park, D. C., and Reuter-Lorenz, P. (2009). The adaptive brain: aging and neurocognitive scaffolding. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 60, 173–196. doi: 10.1146/annurev.psych.59.103006.093656

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Passamonti, L., Tsvetanov, K. A., Jones, P. S., Bevan-Jones, W. R., Arnold, R., Borchert, R. J., et al. (2019). Neuroinflammation and functional connectivity in Alzheimer’s disease: interactive influences on cognitive performance. J. Neurosci. 39, 7218–7226. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2574-18.2019

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pflanz, C. P., Pringle, A., Filippini, N., Warren, M., Gottwald, J., Cowen, P. J., et al. (2015). Effects of seven-day diazepam administration on resting-state functional connectivity in healthy volunteers: a randomized, double-blind study. Psychopharmacology 232, 2139–2147. doi: 10.1007/s00213-014-3844-3

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Power, J. D., Barnes, K. A., Snyder, A. Z., Schlaggar, B. L., and Petersen, S. E. (2012). Spurious but systematic correlations in functional connectivity MRI networks arise from subject motion. Neuroimage 59, 2142–2154. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.10.018

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Quinn, A. J., Vidaurre, D., Abeysuriya, R., Becker, R., Nobre, A. C., and Woolrich, M. W. (2018). Task-evoked dynamic network analysis through hidden Markov modeling. Front. Neurosci. 12:603. doi: 10.3389/fnins.2018.00603

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Raz, N., and Rodrigue, K. M. (2006). Differential aging of the brain: patterns, cognitive correlates and modifiers. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 30, 730–748. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2006.07.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rezek, I., and Roberts, S. (2005). “Ensemble hidden Markov models with extended observation densities for biosignal analysis,” in Probabilistic Modeling in Bioinformatics and Medical Informatics, eds D. Husmeier, R. Dybowski, and S. Roberts (London: Springer), 419–450. doi: 10.1007/1-84628-119-9_14

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Roberts, R. E., Shema, S. J., Kaplan, G. A., and Strawbridge, W. J. (2000). Sleep complaints and depression in an aging cohort: a prospective perspective. Am. J. Psychiatry 157, 81–88. doi: 10.1176/ajp.157.1.81

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rosseel, Y. (2012). Lavaan: an R package for structural equation modeling. J. Stat. Softw. 48, 1–36.

Google Scholar

Scullin, M. K., and Bliwise, D. L. (2015). Sleep, cognition, and normal aging: integrating a half century of multidisciplinary research. Perspect. Psychol. Sci. 10, 97–137. doi: 10.1177/1745691614556680

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Shafto, M. A., Tyler, L. K., Dixon, M., Taylor, J. R., Rowe, J. B., Cusack, R., et al. (2014). The Cambridge Centre for Ageing and Neuroscience (Cam-CAN) study protocol: a cross-sectional, lifespan, multidisciplinary examination of healthy cognitive ageing. BMC Neurol. 14:204. doi: 10.1186/s12883-014-0204-1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Shi, L., Chen, S.-J., Ma, M.-Y., Bao, Y.-P., Han, Y., Wang, Y.-M., et al. (2018). Sleep disturbances increase the risk of dementia: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Sleep Med. Rev. 40, 4–16. doi: 10.1016/j.smrv.2017.06.010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Skinner, H. A. (1982). The drug abuse screening test. Addict. Behav. 7, 363–371. doi: 10.1016/0306-4603(82)90005-3

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Smith, S. M., Nichols, T. E., Vidaurre, D., Winkler, A. M., Behrens, T. E., Glasser, M. F., et al. (2015). A positive-negative mode of population covariation links brain connectivity, demographics and behavior. Nat. Neurosci. 18, 1565–1567.

Google Scholar

Snellen, H. (1862). Letterproeven, tot Bepaling der Gezigtsscherpte. Utrecht: J. Greven.

Google Scholar

Taulu, S., and Simola, J. (2006). Spatiotemporal signal space separation method for rejecting nearby interference in MEG measurements. Phys. Med. Biol. 51, 1759–1768. doi: 10.1088/0031-9155/51/7/008

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Taylor, J. R., Williams, N., Cusack, R., Auer, T., Shafto, M. A., Dixon, M., et al. (2017). The Cambridge Centre for Ageing and Neuroscience (Cam-CAN) data repository: structural and functional MRI, MEG, and cognitive data from a cross-sectional adult lifespan sample. Neuroimage 144, 262–269. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2015.09.018

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tibon, R., Tsvetanov, K. A., Price, D., Nesbitt, D., Can, C., and Henson, R. (2021). Transient neural network dynamics in cognitive ageing. Neurobiol. Aging 105, 217–228. doi: 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2021.01.035

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tsapanou, A., Gu, Y., O’Shea, D. M., Yannakoulia, M., Kosmidis, M., Dardiotis, E., et al. (2017). Sleep quality and duration in relation to memory in the elderly: initial results from the Hellenic Longitudinal Investigation of Aging and Diet. Neurobiol. Learn. Mem. 141, 217–225. doi: 10.1016/j.nlm.2017.04.011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tsapanou, A., Vlachos, G. S., Cosentino, S., Gu, Y., Manly, J. J., Brickman, A. M., et al. (2019). Sleep and subjective cognitive decline in cognitively healthy elderly: results from two cohorts. J. Sleep Res. 28:e12759. doi: 10.1111/jsr.12759

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tsvetanov, K. A., Henson, R. N. A., and Rowe, J. B. (2021b). Separating vascular and neuronal effects of age on fMRI BOLD signals. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. B Biol. Sci. 376:20190631. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2019.0631

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tsvetanov, K. A., Gazzina, S., Jones, P. S., van Swieten, J., Borroni, B., Sanchez-Valle, R., et al. (2021a). Brain functional network integrity sustains cognitive function despite atrophy in presymptomatic genetic frontotemporal dementia. Alzheimers Dement. 17, 500–514. doi: 10.1002/alz.12209

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tsvetanov, K. A., Henson, R. N. A., Tyler, L. K., Davis, S. W., Shafto, M. A., Taylor, J. R., et al. (2015). The effect of ageing on fMRI: correction for the confounding effects of vascular reactivity evaluated by joint fMRI and MEG in 335 adults. Hum. Brain Mapp. 36, 2248–2269. doi: 10.1002/hbm.22768

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tsvetanov, K. A., Henson, R. N. A., Tyler, L. K., Razi, A., Geerligs, L., Ham, T. E., et al. (2016). Extrinsic and intrinsic brain network connectivity maintains cognition across the lifespan despite accelerated decay of regional brain activation. J. Neurosci. 36, 3115–3126. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2733-15.2016

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tsvetanov, K. A., Ye, Z., Hughes, L., Samu, D., Treder, M. S., Wolpe, N., et al. (2018). Activity and connectivity differences underlying inhibitory control across the adult life span. J. Neurosci. 38, 7887–7900. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2919-17.2018

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Van Veen, B. D., Van Drongelen, W., Yuchtman, M., and Suzuki, A. (1997). Localization of brain electrical activity via linearly constrained minimum variance spatial filtering. IEEE Trans. Biomed. Eng. 44, 867–880. doi: 10.1109/10.623056

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vidaurre, D., Abeysuriya, R., Becker, R., Quinn, A. J., Alfaro-Almagro, F., Smith, S. M., et al. (2018). Discovering dynamic brain networks from big data in rest and task. Neuroimage 180, 646–656. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2017.06.077

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vidaurre, D., Quinn, A. J., Baker, A. P., Dupret, D., Tejero-Cantero, A., and Woolrich, M. W. (2016). Spectrally resolved fast transient brain states in electrophysiological data. Neuroimage 126, 81–95. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2015.11.047

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vidaurre, D., Smith, S. M., and Woolrich, M. W. (2017). Brain network dynamics are hierarchically organized in time. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U. S. A. 114, 12827–12832. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1705120114

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

West, R. (2000). In defense of the frontal lobe hypothesis of cognitive aging. J. Int. Neuropsychol. Soc. 6, 727–729. doi: 10.1017/S1355617700666109

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Woolrich, M., Hunt, L., Groves, A., and Barnes, G. (2011). MEG beamforming using Bayesian PCA for adaptive data covariance matrix regularization. Neuroimage 57, 1466–1479. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.04.041

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wu, S., Tyler, L. K., Henson, R. N. A., Rowe, J. B., and Cam-CAN, and Tsvetanov, K. A. (2021). Cerebral blood flow predicts multiple demand network activity and fluid intelligence across the lifespan. bioRxiv [Preprint]. doi: 10.1101/2021.11.10.468042

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wu, Z., Chen, F., Yu, F., Wang, Y., and Guo, Z. (2018). A meta-analysis of obstructive sleep apnea in patients with cerebrovascular disease. Sleep Breath. 22, 729–742. doi: 10.1007/s11325-017-1604-4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: magnetoencephalography, Hidden Markov Model, partial least squares, aging, sleep, cognition

Citation: Tibon R and Tsvetanov KA (2022) The “Neural Shift” of Sleep Quality and Cognitive Aging: A Resting-State MEG Study of Transient Neural Dynamics. Front. Aging Neurosci. 13:746236. doi: 10.3389/fnagi.2021.746236

Received: 23 July 2021; Accepted: 21 December 2021;
Published: 31 January 2022.

Edited by:

Géraldine Rauchs, Institut National de la Santé et de la Recherche Médicale (INSERM), France

Reviewed by:

Guohao Wang, National Institutes of Health (NIH), United States
Georgios D. Mitsis, McGill University, Canada

Copyright © 2022 Tibon and Tsvetanov. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Roni Tibon, cm9uaS50aWJvbkBtcmMtY2J1LmNhbS5hYy51aw==

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.