Skip to main content

BRIEF RESEARCH REPORT article

Front. Aging Neurosci., 10 May 2019
Sec. Neurocognitive Aging and Behavior
This article is part of the Research Topic Biomarkers to Disentangle the Physiological from Pathological Brain Aging View all 25 articles

Cognitive Profiles of Aging in Multiple Sclerosis

\r\nDejan Jakimovski,Dejan Jakimovski1,2Bianca Weinstock-GuttmanBianca Weinstock-Guttman1Shumita RoyShumita Roy1Michael Jaworski IIIMichael Jaworski III1Laura HancockLaura Hancock3Alissa NizinskiAlissa Nizinski1Pavitra SrinivasanPavitra Srinivasan1Tom A. Fuchs,Tom A. Fuchs1,2Kinga SzigetiKinga Szigeti1Robert Zivadinov,,Robert Zivadinov1,2,4Ralph H. B. Benedict*Ralph H. B. Benedict1*
  • 1Department of Neurology, Jacobs School of Medicine and Biomedical Sciences, University at Buffalo – The State University of New York, Buffalo, NY, United States
  • 2Buffalo Neuroimaging Analysis Center, Department of Neurology, Jacobs School of Medicine and Biomedical Sciences, University at Buffalo – The State University of New York, Buffalo, NY, United States
  • 3Department of Neurology, University of Wisconsin School of Medicine and Public Health, Madison, WI, United States
  • 4Clinical Translational Science Institute, Center for Biomedical Imaging, University at Buffalo – The State University of New York, Buffalo, NY, United States

Background: Increasingly favorable mortality prognosis in multiple sclerosis (MS) raises questions regarding MS-specific cognitive aging and the presence of comorbidities such as Alzheimer’s disease (AD).

Objective: To assess elderly with MS (EwMS) and age-matched healthy controls (HCs) using both MS- and AD-specific psychometrics.

Methods: EwMS (n = 104) and 56 HCs were assessed on a broad spectrum of language, visual-spatial processing, memory, processing speed, and executive function tests. Using logistic regression analysis, we examined cognitive performance differences between the EwMS and HC groups. Cognitive impairment (CI) was defined using a -1.5 SD threshold relative to age and education years-matched HCs, in two cognitive domains.

Results: CI was observed in 47.1% of EwMS with differences most often seen on tests emphasizing cognitive processing speed as measured by Symbol Digit Modalities Test (SDMT) (d = 0.9, p < 0.001) and verbal fluency (both category-based d = 0.87, p < 0.001; letter-based d = 0.67, p < 0.001). After adjusting for age, sex and years of education, MS/HC diagnosis was best predicted (R2 = 0.27) by differences in category-based verbal fluency (Wald = 9.935, p = 0.002) and SDMT (Wald = 13.937, p < 0.001).

Conclusion: This study confirms the common hallmark of slowed cognitive processing speed in MS among elderly patients. Defective verbal fluency, less often observed in younger cohorts, may represent emerging cognitive pathology due to other etiologies.

Introduction

Multiple sclerosis (MS) is a chronic demyelinating disease of the central nervous system (CNS) which presents with acute inflammatory episodes and co-occurring widespread neurodegeneration (Reich et al., 2018). These neuropathological processes are not only linked to physical disability progression, but also to cognitive impairment (Benedict et al., 2014). The increasingly favorable mortality prognosis with new disease modifying therapies raises new questions about quality of life, the influence of frailty, prevalence of comorbidities, and cognitive aging among the elderly with MS (EwMS) (Sanai et al., 2016; Koch-Henriksen et al., 2017).

Alzheimer’s disease (AD) and its prodromal state, amnestic mild cognitive impairment (aMCI), are among the many comorbidities expected to increase within the aging population. Furthermore, the progressive MS phase is associated with neurodegenerative features resembling an AD-like phenotype. Although both diseases may lead to dementia, their cognitive hallmarks are thought to differ. MS patients commonly present with slowed cognitive processing speed (CPS) and deficient learning, a cognitive profile frequently associated with lesion disruption of neural networks and atrophy of major hubs that facilitate information exchange (e.g., thalamus) (Van Schependom et al., 2015; Bergsland et al., 2018). Contrarily, AD is characterized by progressive decline in episodic memory, rapid forgetting, and impairments in semantic language. These defects are associated with neurodegeneration starting within the hippocampus, and entorhinal cortex (Hyman et al., 1984; McKhann et al., 2011; Reitz et al., 2011). The prevalence of MS in the United States is now estimated at one million persons, and the average age of the population is increasing (Nelson et al., 2019). Therefore, the possibility that an EwMS patient may develop AD should be considered with new onset cognitive complaints (Luczynski et al., 2018). Nevertheless, only a few case reports of MS and AD dual diagnosis are found in the literature (Luczynski et al., 2018). Therefore, understanding the cognitive profiles EwMS is highly warranted.

In a preliminary cross-sectional investigation, we compared EwMS (an average of 60 years old) with age and education-matched aMCI and AD patients (Roy et al., 2018). On a test of semantic verbal fluency, we found no significant difference between cognitively impaired MS and aMCI patients (Roy et al., 2018). Therefore, one may propose that as the MS cognitive decline progresses, additional cognitive features apart from processing speed are likely to emerge. Herein we endeavored to comprehensively examine the cognitive profile of a larger group of EwMS and age-matched healthy controls (HCs) using MS and AD-specific psychometric tests. We hypothesized that if EwMS have a true impairment in the language aspects of verbal fluency, this deficit should remain significant after controlling for the influence of cognitive processing speed.

Materials and Methods

Study Population

The study sample was composed of prospectively enrolled EwMS and HCs. The inclusion criteria for EwMS were (1) age ≥50 years old, (2) neuropsychological and clinical examination, and (3) MS diagnosis per 2010-revised McDonald criteria (Polman et al., 2011). The exclusion criteria for the EwMS consisted of (1) history of psychiatric and major depressive disorder prior to the onset of MS, (2) active relapse within 30 days of the examination, and (3) use of corticosteroids within 30 days of the study. Inclusion and exclusion criteria for HCs were (1) ≥50 years old, (2) neuropsychological examination, (3) self-reported intact cognition, (4) no history of neurological or medical illness that might impact cognitive function, and (5) no diagnosis of major depressive disorder. Furthermore, HCs with mini-mental state examination (MMSE) performance lower than 28 were excluded. Both the EwMS and HCs were additionally screened with the beck depression inventory-II fastscreen (BDI-FS).

Clinical examination was performed by a neurologist and physical disability was assessed using the expanded disability status scale (EDSS) (Kurtzke, 1983). The Timed 25-Foot Walk (T25FW) and 9-Hole Peg Test (9PHT) were additional measures of lower and upper extremity function, respectively. The EwMS were classified by disease course as relapsing-remitting MS (RRMS) and progressive MS (PMS). The study was approved by the Institutional Review Board (IRB) and all participants signed an informed consent form.

Neuropsychological Examination

The neuropsychological examination was performed by trained examiners under supervision of a board-certified neuropsychologist (RHBB) and included tests traditionally regarded as MS and AD-specific (Strauss et al., 2006). The cognitive domains, their participating tests, and corresponding references are shown in Table 1.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Cognitive domains and their respective tests.

The exam included the Minimal Assessment of Cognitive Function in Multiple Sclerosis (MACFIMS) battery (Benedict et al., 2002) which evaluates several domains, including: processing speed/working memory [Paced Auditory Serial Addition Test (PASAT) (Gronwall, 1977) and Symbol Digit Modalities Test (SDMT) (Smith, 1982)], learning and memory [learning trials for California Verbal Learning Test–second edition (CVLT-II) (Delis et al., 2000) and Brief Visuospatial Memory Test-Revised (BVMT-R) (Benedict, 1997)], language [Controlled Oral Word Association Test (COWAT) (Ruff et al., 1996)] and executive function [number of correct sorts and description scores from the Sorting subtest of the Delis-Kaplan Executive Function System (D-KEFS) (Delis et al., 2001)].

Participants also completed neuropsychological measures that are traditionally utilized for detecting AD-specific cognitive impairments (Strauss et al., 2006). These tests included those measuring the domains of memory [Logical Memory from the Wechsler Memory Scale-Revised (WMS-R) (Wechsler, 1945)], language [Boston Naming Test (BNT) (Goodglass et al., 1983)], two categories of semantic fluency (total number of categorical items generated in one minute; animals and supermarket items), and visuospatial skill [Beery Visual-Motor Integration (VMI) (Beery, 2010) and clock drawing test (Agrell and Dehlin, 1998)]. The MMSE was also performed as an additional assessment of descriptive global cognitive functioning. For all performed psychometric tests, higher scores indicate better cognitive performance.

Statistical Analysis

All statistical tests were performed using SPSS version 25.0 (IBM, Armonk, NY, United States). The distribution of the variables was examined using both Kolmogorov-Smirnov and Shapiro-Wilk tests of normality. The differences in demographics and cognitive raw scores between EwMS and HCs were determined by χ2–test for categorical variables, ANOVA or t-test for continuous, normally distributed variables, and Mann Whitney U-test for continuous, not normally distributed variables. To control for the multiple comparisons, Benjamini-Hochberg procedure was employed and adjusted p < 0.05 were considered statistically significant.

In order to delineate the degree of cognitive impairment across various cognitive domains while controlling for age, z-scores derived from the age-matched HCs were calculated. EwMS with z-score performance lower than -1.5 on at least two different cognitive domains were classified as cognitively impaired. The number of tests within each corresponding domain are shown in Table 1.

The impact of CPS on differences in cognitive performance between EwMS and HCs was additionally determined by analysis of covariance (ANCOVA) adjusted for SDMT scores.

Lastly, a multivariate logistic regression model determining the specific cognitive differences between the EwMS and the HCs was constructed. The model was composed of an initial force-entered block which corrected for differences in sex, age, and years of education. Furthermore, a second step-wise inclusion block determined the strength and order of cognitive variables which provided the greatest explanatory value in differentiating EwMS and HCs. For each derived step, the respective R2, Wald coefficient, and p-values were reported.

Results

Demographic and Clinical Characteristics

The EwMS (n = 104) and HCs (n = 56) were similar in age (62.1 vs. 62.3 years, p = 0.919), years of education (15.2 vs. 15.6 years, p = 0.213) and sex ratio (75.0% vs. 62.5% females, p = 0.09). The MS group had a mean disease duration of 20.5 years, median EDSS scores of 3.5 (IQR 3.0–6.0), and ratio of RRMS/PMS of 73/31. Regarding disease modifying therapy (DMT), most EwMS were prescribed interferon-β (32, 30.8%), followed by glatiramer acetate (28, 17.3%), oral DMT medications (15, 14.4%), natalizumab (6, 5.8%), off-label medication (1, 0.9%), and the rest of the EwMS were not on any DMT (32, 30.8%).

The EwMS were more disabled when compared to the HCs in both lower and upper extremity function (T25FW, 6.7s vs. 4.7s, p < 0.001; and 9PHT, 24.7s vs. 21.5s, p < 0.001). The MS group also had higher depression scores (BDI-FS of 1.0 vs. 0.0, p = 0.005). All demographic and clinical information are shown in Table 2.

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Demographic and clinical characteristics of the EwMS and HCs samples.

Cognitive Performance, and Z-Score-Derived Domain-Specific Impairment Rates in the EwMS

The raw scores from the neuropsychological examination of the EwMS and HCs on both the MACFIMS and AD-specific batteries are shown in Table 3. EwMS had lower raw scores when compared to the age-matched controls on both total learned and short delay recall in CVLT-II (49.7 vs. 54.9, d = 0.45, p = 0.018 and 9.8 vs. 11.2, d = 0.38, p = 0.045, respectively), SDMT (46.3 vs. 55.4, d = 0.91, p < 0.001), letter verbal fluency (35.8 vs. 43.0, d = 0.67, p = 0.001) and category verbal fluency (17.3 vs. 20.3, d = 0.67, p = 0.001; 20.9 vs. 25.7, d = 0.84, p < 0.001; and 38.2 vs. 45.9, d = 0.87, p < 0.001, for animal category, supermarket category and total score, respectively). Furthermore, the EwMS had lower immediate recall and delayed recall scores on WMS-R logical memory performance (23.7 vs. 26.4, d = 0.39, p = 0.045 and 18.8 vs. 21.7, d = 0.38, p = 0.048). No other group differences were observed.

TABLE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. Cognitive performance of the EwMS and HCs populations on the minimal assessment for cognitive function in multiple sclerosis (MACFIMS) battery.

To investigate the influence of processing speed on our findings, we performed an analysis of covariance in which the COWAT differences (letter-based and category-based fluency) were adjusted by CPS performance (SDMT score as covariate). The performance on SDMT had a significant influence on letter-based verbal fluency performance (F1,156 = 39.5 r = 0.49, p < 0.001). In the final model, EwMS and HCs were not significantly different in letter-based fluency scores when CPS was controlled (F1,156 = 3.9, p = 0.057). Processing speed also influenced the performance on total categorical verbal fluency (F1,156 = 34.2, r = 0.425, p < 0.001) and on both animal-specific (F1,156 = 12.2, r = 0.242, p = 0.001), and supermarket items-specific category (F1,156 = 39.7, r = 0.476, p < 0.001). Despite the large SDMT effect, categorical verbal fluency remained significantly different between the EwMS and HCs (adjusted p = 0.036, adjusted p < 0.001, and adjusted p = 0.001, for animals, supermarket items, and total categorical verbal fluency, respectively).

Based on the HC-derived z-scores, the differences in processing speed and verbal fluency represented the most commonly affected domains in the EwMS. More than a quarter (27.9%) of EwMS had categorical verbal fluency impairment relative to HCs (22.1 and 24.0% for individual animal and supermarket tests, respectively). 25.0% were impaired on the letter-based verbal fluency, and 25.9% as impaired on the SDMT. Overall, 47.1% of the EwMS were classified as cognitively impaired on at least two different domain-specific tests. Table 4 portrays the impairment rate observed for each cognitive test.

TABLE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Table 4. The ratio and percentage of cognitive impairment in the EwMS patients.

Both the multivariate logistic regression model blocks and derived step-wise predictors are shown in Table 5. The diagnosis of MS was best predicted by differences in category-based verbal fluency (Wald = 9.935, p = 0.002) and SDMT (Wald = 13.937, p < 0.001). Together with the force-entered block containing age, sex and years of education, the final step-wise model was able to explain 27% of the diagnosis variance (Nagelkerke R2 = 0.27). In terms of odds ratio (OR), for every point decrease in categorical verbal fluency, the chance of MS diagnosis increased for 8.1% [Exp(B) = 0.919]. Similarly, for every point decrease in SDMT, the change of MS diagnosis is increased for 9.3% [Exp(B) = 0.907].

TABLE 5
www.frontiersin.org

Table 5. Logistic regression model determining the cognitive differences between the EwMS and HCs.

Lastly, we investigated the associations between verbal fluency and cognitive processing speed with the overall disability scores as depicted by EDSS. Within the total sample of EwMS, EDSS scores were associated with both total verbal fluency (Spearman’s ranked correlation, rs = -0.292, p = 0.003) and SDMT performance (rs = -0.398, p < 0.001). Due to the nature of EDSS scoring system, we further delineated the EwMS into groups within the lower range of the score (EDSS scores between 0 and 4.0 which depend and reflect the neurological examination) and within the higher range of the score (EDSS scores ≥4.0 which solemnly depends on the walking ability) (van Munster and Uitdehaag, 2017; Jakimovski et al., 2018a). As expected, the associations between cognitive performance and EDSS scores were mainly present within the subset of EwMS with lower EDSS scores (total verbal fluency rs = -0.341, p = 0.001 and SDMT rs = -0.398, p < 0.001).

Discussion

The main findings from this comprehensive neuropsychological evaluation of EwMS and age-matched HCs are grounded in two main themes. First, in addition to the previously established processing speed impairment, EwMS present with poorer verbal fluency (for both letter and category). Second, these findings were further extended by the logistic regression model which highlighted verbal fluency performance as second major differentiator between EwMS and HCs.

Currently there are few published analyses examining the question of comorbid aMCI/AD and cognitive aging in EwMS. Muller et al. (2013) used an AD–specific battery to examine 120 total age-, sex-, and education-matched SPMS, aMCI, and HCs and found word list recognition as the single metric distinguishing SPMS from aMCI – recognition memory was preserved in SPMS and defective in aMCI. Furthermore, they additionally showed that SPMS patients had similar performance on both letter-based and category-based verbal fluency when compared to the aMCI patients and significantly lower than HCs (Muller et al., 2013). In a similar study, EwMS were impaired on both processing speed and categorical fluency (Cohen’s d = 0.68) when compared to HCs (Roth et al., 2018). Therefore, both studies are conforming to the present results showing a high rate of processing speed impairment and diminished verbal fluency performance in EwMS, but no differences in the recognition indices of both verbal memory and visuospatial processing (Roy et al., 2018). Although language impairment is classically associated with an AD cognitive profile, the deficits in processing speed and verbal fluency were not accompanied by deficits in naming and memory consolidation. Therefore, the verbal fluency deficits found here are likely attributable to MS rather than co-morbid aMCI/AD.

Verbal fluency tasks allow the participants only 1 min to produce as many words that start with a certain letter of the alphabet or are part of a specific category (e.g., animals or supermarket items). Given this predetermined time limitation, verbal fluency performance may potentially be moderated by processing speed, as we found in our adjusted model. Report by Roth et al. (2018) showed that the EwMS have poorer categorical fluency when compared to HCs; however, after adjusting for CPS performance, these differences became non-significant. The discrepancy between our results and theirs is, however, minuscule (d 0.87 vs. 0.68 in Roth et al., 2018) – the partial correlation survived in our study with p = 0.036 with more statistical power by virtue of a larger sample. Devising new language tests that are less dependent on CPS would be of value in this area of investigation. Finally, determining the nature of the language impairment and its association with specific structural MS changes will allow better understanding of this particular cognitive ability. For example, while examining neurodegenerative MS patterns, studies demonstrated accelerated atrophy of the entorhinal cortex and the bilateral temporal lobes in a population of older (on average of 57 years old) SPMS patients (Steenwijk et al., 2016). These neurodegenerative changes located within AD-associated areas may provide explanation for the emerging semantic and logical memory deficit seen in our EwMS.

One limitation of this study is its cross-sectional research design. In order to better evaluate cognitive aging trajectories, ideally, one would evaluate these same cognitive functions over a period of three or more years, and include not only EwMS, but also aMCI patients. MRI-derived neurodegenerative outcome measures, amyloid PET imaging, assessment of known AD risk factors and the effect of other age-related comorbidities may further determine the nature of the semantic fluency impairment seen in EwMS (Jakimovski et al., 2018b). Lastly, global disability scores which are currently used in the MS field are not suitable in depicting the increasingly prevalent cognitive impairment within the aging MS population.

Conclusion

In conclusion, roughly half of EwMS are impaired on tests of cognitive processing speed or verbal fluency. The deficit in verbal fluency is not a hallmark feature of MS associated cognitive disorder for the wider MS population, and may represent a unique quality in the EwMS neurocognitive profile. Further studies investigating the additional disease-related factors which influence cognitive aging in EwMS are warranted.

Data Availability

The datasets generated for this study are available on request to the corresponding author.

Ethics Statement

This study was carried out in accordance with the Institutional Review Board (IRB) with written informed consent from all subjects. All subjects gave written informed consent in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki. The protocol was approved by the University at Buffalo Institutional Review Board.

Author Contributions

RB and DJ conceived and designed the study. All authors analyzed and interpreted the data and critically revised the manuscript for important intellectual content. RB, RZ, and BW-G supervised the study.

Funding

This study was supported in part by a grant from the Biogen IIS Aging, US-mSG-15-10855 grant to BW-G.

Conflict of Interest Statement

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

References

Agrell, B., and Dehlin, O. (1998). The clock-drawing test. Age Ageing 27, 399–403.

Google Scholar

Beery, K. (2010). Beery VMI Administration, Scoring, and Teaching Manual. Bloomington: Pearson.

Google Scholar

Benedict, R. H. (1997). Brief Visuospatial Memory Test–Revised: Professional Manual. Lutz, FL: PAR.

Google Scholar

Benedict, R. H., Fischer, J. S., Archibald, C. J., Arnett, P. A., Beatty, W. W., Bobholz, J., et al. (2002). Minimal neuropsychological assessment of MS patients: a consensus approach. Clin. Neuropsychol. 16, 381–397. doi: 10.1076/clin.16.3.381.13859

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Benedict, R. H., Morrow, S., Rodgers, J., Hojnacki, D., Bucello, M. A., Zivadinov, R., et al. (2014). Characterizing cognitive function during relapse in multiple sclerosis. Mult. Scler. 20, 1745–1752. doi: 10.1177/1352458514533229

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bergsland, N., Schweser, F., Dwyer, M. G., Weinstock-Guttman, B., Benedict, R. H. B., and Zivadinov, R. (2018). Thalamic white matter in multiple sclerosis: a combined diffusion-tensor imaging and quantitative susceptibility mapping study. Hum. Brain Mapp. 39, 4007–4017. doi: 10.1002/hbm.24227

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Delis, D. C., Kaplan, E., and Kramer, J. H. (2001). Delis-Kaplan Executive Function System®(D-KEFS®): Examiner’s Manual: Flexibility of Thinking, Concept Formation, Problem Solving, Planning, Creativity, Impluse Control, Inhibition. Bloomington: Pearson.

Google Scholar

Delis, D. C., Kramer, J., Kaplan, E., and Ober, B. A. (2000). CVLT-II: California Verbal Learning Test: Adult Version. New York, NY: The Psychological Corporation.

Google Scholar

Goodglass, H., Kaplan, E., and Weintraub, S. (1983). Boston Naming Test. Philadelphia, PA: Lea & Febiger.

Google Scholar

Gronwall, D. (1977). Paced auditory serial-addition task: a measure of recovery from concussion. Percept. Motor Skills 44, 367–373. doi: 10.2466/pms.1977.44.2.367

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hyman, B. T., Van Hoesen, G. W., Damasio, A. R., and Barnes, C. L. (1984). Alzheimer’s disease: cell-specific pathology isolates the hippocampal formation. Science 225, 1168–1170. doi: 10.1126/science.6474172

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jakimovski, D., Benedict, R., Marr, K., Gandhi, S., Bergsland, N., Weinstock-Guttmam, B., et al. (2018a). Lower total cerebral arterial flow contributes to cognitive perofrmance in multiple sclerosis patients. Mult. Scler. doi: 10.1177/1352458518819608 [Epub ahead of print].

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jakimovski, D., Weinstock-Guttman, B., Hagemeier, J., Vaughn, C. B., Kavak, K. S., Gandhi, S., et al. (2018b). Walking disability measures in multiple sclerosis patients: correlations with MRI-derived global and microstructural damage. J. Neurol. Sci. 393, 128–134. doi: 10.1016/j.jns.2018.08.020

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Koch-Henriksen, N., Laursen, B., Stenager, E., and Magyari, M. (2017). Excess mortality among patients with multiple sclerosis in Denmark has dropped significantly over the past six decades: a population based study. J. Neurol. Neurosurg. Psychiatry 88, 626–631. doi: 10.1136/jnnp-2017-315907

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kurtzke, J. F. (1983). Rating neurologic impairment in multiple sclerosis: an expanded disability status scale (EDSS). Neurology 33, 1444–1452.

Google Scholar

Luczynski, P., Laule, C., Hsiung, G. R., Moore, G. R. W., and Tremlett, H. (2018). Coexistence of multiple sclerosis and Alzheimer’s disease: a review. Mult. Scler. Relat. Disord. 27, 232–238. doi: 10.1016/j.msard.2018.10.109

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

McKhann, G. M., Knopman, D. S., Chertkow, H., Hyman, B. T., Jack, C. R. Jr., Kawas, C. H., et al. (2011). The diagnosis of dementia due to Alzheimer’s disease: recommendations from the national institute on aging-Alzheimer’s association workgroups on diagnostic guidelines for Alzheimer’s disease. Alzheimers Dement. 7, 263–269. doi: 10.1016/j.jalz.2011.03.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Muller, S., Saur, R., Greve, B., Melms, A., Hautzinger, M., Fallgatter, A. J., et al. (2013). Recognition performance differentiates between elderly patients in the long term course of secondary progressive multiple sclerosis and amnestic mild cognitive impairment. Mult. Scler. 19, 799–805. doi: 10.1177/1352458512461392

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nelson, L. M., Wallin, M. T., Marrie, R. A., Culpepper, W. J., Langer-Gould, A., Campbell, J., et al. (2019). A new way to estimate neurologic disease prevalence in the United States: Illustrated with MS. Neurology 92, 1–13. doi: 10.1212/WNL.0000000000007044

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Polman, C. H., Reingold, S. C., Banwell, B., Clanet, M., Cohen, J. A., Filippi, M., et al. (2011). Diagnostic criteria for multiple sclerosis: 2010 revisions to the McDonald criteria. Ann. Neurol. 69, 292–302. doi: 10.1002/ana.22366

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Reich, D. S., Lucchinetti, C. F., and Calabresi, P. A. (2018). Multiple sclerosis. N. Engl. J. Med. 378, 169–180.

Google Scholar

Reitz, C., Brayne, C., and Mayeux, R. (2011). Epidemiology of Alzheimer disease. Nat. Rev. Neurol. 7, 137–152. doi: 10.1038/nrneurol.2011.2

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Roth, A. K., Denney, D. R., Burns, J. M., and Lynch, S. G. (2018). Cognition in older patients with multiple sclerosis compared to patients with amnestic mild cognitive impairment and healthy older adults. Neuropsychology 32, 654–663. doi: 10.1037/neu0000453

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Roy, S., Drake, A., Snyder, S., Cline, B., Khan, A., Fuchs, T., et al. (2018). Preliminary investigation of cognitive function in aged multiple sclerosis patients: challenges in detecting comorbid Alzheimer’s disease. Mult. Scler. Relat. Disord. 22, 52–56. doi: 10.1016/j.msard.2018.03.008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ruff, R., Light, R., Parker, S., and Levin, H. (1996). Benton controlled oral word association test: reliability and updated norms. Arch. Clin. Neuropsychol. 11, 329–338. doi: 10.1016/0887-6177(95)00033-x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sanai, S. A., Saini, V., Benedict, R. H., Zivadinov, R., Teter, B. E., Ramanathan, M., et al. (2016). Aging and multiple sclerosis. Mult. Scler. 22, 717–725.

Google Scholar

Smith, A. (1982). Symbol Digit Modalities Test. Los Angeles, CA: Western Psychological Services.

Google Scholar

Steenwijk, M. D., Geurts, J. J., Daams, M., Tijms, B. M., Wink, A. M., Balk, L. J., et al. (2016). Cortical atrophy patterns in multiple sclerosis are non-random and clinically relevant. Brain 139, 115–126. doi: 10.1093/brain/awv337

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Strauss, E., Sherman, E., and Spreen, O. (2006). A Compendium of Neuropsychological Tests: Administration, Norms, and Commentary. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

Google Scholar

van Munster, C. E., and Uitdehaag, B. M. (2017). Outcome measures in clinical trials for multiple sclerosis. CNS Drugs 31, 217–236. doi: 10.1007/s40263-017-0412-5

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Van Schependom, J., D’hooghe, M. B., Cleynhens, K., D’hooge, M., Haelewyck, M. C., De Keyser, J., et al. (2015). Reduced information processing speed as primum movens for cognitive decline in MS. Mult. Scler. 21, 83–91. doi: 10.1177/1352458514537012

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wechsler, D. (1945). Wechsler Memory Scale. Bloomington: Pearson.

Google Scholar

Keywords: aging, MS, cognition, processing speed, verbal fluency

Citation: Jakimovski D, Weinstock-Guttman B, Roy S, Jaworski M III, Hancock L, Nizinski A, Srinivasan P, Fuchs TA, Szigeti K, Zivadinov R and Benedict RHB (2019) Cognitive Profiles of Aging in Multiple Sclerosis. Front. Aging Neurosci. 11:105. doi: 10.3389/fnagi.2019.00105

Received: 01 March 2019; Accepted: 18 April 2019;
Published: 10 May 2019.

Reviewed by:

Laura Ghezzi, University of Milan, Italy
Maurizio Gallucci, Unità Locale Socio Sanitaria 9, Italy

Copyright © 2019 Jakimovski, Weinstock-Guttman, Roy, Jaworski, Hancock, Nizinski, Srinivasan, Fuchs, Szigeti, Zivadinov and Benedict. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Ralph H. B. Benedict, YmVuZWRpY3RAYnVmZmFsby5lZHU=

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.