Skip to main content

BRIEF RESEARCH REPORT article

Front. Psychiatry, 18 August 2021
Sec. Public Mental Health

Conspiratorial Thinking During COVID-19: The Roles of Paranoia, Delusion-Proneness, and Intolerance of Uncertainty

\nEmmett M. Larsen
Emmett M. Larsen*Kayla R. DonaldsonKayla R. DonaldsonMegan LiewMegan LiewAprajita MohantyAprajita Mohanty
  • Neuroscience of Emotion, Cognition, and Psychopathology Laboratory, Department of Psychology, Stony Brook University, Stony Brook, NY, United States

The COVID-19 global pandemic has left many feeling a sense of profound uncertainty about their world, safety, and livelihood. Sources espousing misinformation and conspiracy theories frequently offer information that can help make sense of this uncertainty. Individuals high in intolerance of uncertainty (IU) may be particularly impacted by the impoverished epistemic environment and may thus be more drawn to conspiratorial thinking (CT). In the present work, we show across 2 studies (N = 519) that COVID-19-specific CT is associated with higher levels of IU as well as delusion-proneness, and paranoia. Furthermore, delusion-proneness and paranoia explained the relationship between IU and CT and emerged as independent partial correlates of CT even when controlling for other facets of schizotypy. In contrast, anxiety did not explain the relationship between IU and CT. Overall, our findings highlight the importance of individual differences in IU, delusion-proneness and paranoia in the development of CT in the context of the acute uncertainty of a global crisis, in which conspiracy theories are more prevalent and salient. Informational intervention designs may benefit from leveraging the body of work demonstrating the efficacy of targeting IU to incite meaningful changes in thinking.

Introduction

The COVID-19 global pandemic has caused many to feel a sense of profound uncertainty about their world, safety, and livelihood. States of uncertainty are inherently aversive (1) and may provoke quick, less deliberative decisions. In the context of epistemic uncertainty–wherein the true state of the world is unclear and must be inferred–decision-makers must adjudicate between competing options. This epistemic uncertainty leaves decision-makers vulnerable to the influence of external sources which may vary in their degrees of veracity. Conspiracy theories are often present in this “marketplace” of frameworks for making sense of an uncertain world, and one key psychological appeal of conspiracy theories may be their promise of providing certainty (2). Perhaps unsurprisingly, conspiracy theories specifically regarding COVID-19 began to spread in the 1st weeks of the outbreak, proliferated quickly soon after (3), and have remained stably prevalent (4). The willingness to endorse conspiracy theories–or conspiratorial thinking (CT)–bears a causal link to diminished acceptance of science and decreased engagement in prosocial behaviors (5). Preliminary work has further shown that COVID-19-specific CT is associated with decreased engagement in recommended health behaviors (4, 6, 7). Importantly, the young adult demographic may be at greatest risk of being swayed by COVID-19 misinformation (8), less likely to adhere to health guidelines (9), and more likely to be asymptomatic spreaders of the virus (10). An important task of behavioral scientists in a global health crisis is to identify individual traits and predispositions that are associated with CT or susceptibility to misinformation in young adults in order to inform interventions and methods of science communication (11, 12).

CT arises frequently in times of crisis (13). However, individual differences leading to CT at these times are unclear, as most empirical research examining CT has been conducted outside the context of global crisis, making the COVID-19 pandemic a uniquely important circumstance in which to study these effects. Furthermore, the “epistemic motive” thought to drive CT may be particularly salient when available information is impoverished (2), such as during the COVID-19 pandemic. This affords a unique opportunity to examine these processes as they occur naturally, with COVID-19-related conspiracy theories being both prevalent and salient throughout the pandemic.

Intolerance of uncertainty (IU) is an individual difference in the degree to which uncertainty is experienced as aversive (14). Given the role of CT in resolving uncertainty, those high in IU may be more willing to accept conspiratorial explanations [e.g., (15)] due to their promise of explanatory power. Preliminary empirical work suggests that IU is indeed associated with CT (16). Additionally, experimental manipulation of uncertainty salience alters the way in which evidence for and against conspiracy theories is evaluated (17). The naturally-occurring state of heightened uncertainty due to the COVID-19 pandemic may produce an analogous modulation. So far, no study has directly tested the relationship between IU and COVID-19 CT, though evidence has begun to emerge for a relationship between IU and generic CT during the pandemic (18, 19). Furthermore, younger adults not only endorse greater COVID-19-related CT (8, 20) but also report higher levels of IU during the COVID-19 pandemic (21), underscoring the importance of examining their relationship in this demographic.

Another set of cognitive traits and predispositions that have been shown to constitute susceptibility to development of CT (2224) involve the need to favor/defend bizarre or elaborate beliefs in the face of contradictory evidence. Such a predisposition is also associated with delusion-proneness (25), which some have hypothesized functions as a protectant against undesired possible conclusions (2628). Similarly, one hypothesized function of CT is to protect cherished beliefs by discrediting contradictory evidence as the product of a conspiracy (2, 29). Indeed, empirical studies consistently show relationships between delusion-proneness and CT (3032). Delusion-proneness is one dimension in a cluster of traits known as schizotypy, which also includes proneness to unusual perceptual experience and social and experiential deficits [e.g., (33)]. Paranoia–a construct highly related to schizotypy (34) characterized by mistrust and beliefs that others harbor malintentions—is also associated with CT (23, 30, 35), including in the context of COVID-19-specific CT (36), and this relationship may in fact be separable from that between delusion-proneness and CT (23, 30, 35). Some evidence suggests that schizotypal traits are higher in younger adults (37), indicating the importance of examining whether these individual differences contribute to the higher prevalence of COVID-19-related CT's in this demographic.

Finally, delusion-proneness and IU may bear a relationship to one another. For example, The high conviction characteristic of delusion-like beliefs may serve to minimize uncertainty regarding future states (26), and findings in clinical samples support this, showing that severity of delusional thinking is associated with greater intolerance of uncertainty (38, 39). Given the possibly shared epistemic function of both CT and delusional thinking for reducing uncertainty and the hypothesized relationship of IU and delusion-proneness, the relationship between IU and CT may be mediated by delusion-proneness. It is also possible that the relationship of IU with CT operates via heightened affective distress (anxiety). For instance, van Prooijen and Jostmann (17) demonstrate that higher levels of distress following an uncertainty manipulation increased CT. In addition, although it is thought that CT typically fails to assuage anxiety created by unmet epistemic needs (2), preliminary work has suggested a “protective” relationship between CT and anxiety/depression during the COVID-19 pandemic (40). Finally, it has long been thought that IU directly causes and maintains anxious distress itself (14). Greater IU is associated with increased anxiety [e.g., (41)], and IU is considered a transdiagnostic target in anxiety treatment (42). Taken together, these relationships suggest that anxiety is a possible intermediary factor that explains the relationship of IU with CT.

Overall, theoretical and empirical work suggests a particular constellation of individual differences that predispose to CT. The present work seeks to characterize the interplay amongst individual differences in explaining COVID-19 CT and evaluate possible mediators in two samples of young adults. In Study 1 we aim to examine the hypothesized relationships between IU, delusion-proneness, paranoia, and CT. In Study 2 we assess whether these relationships replicate while also increasing statistical power to conduct mediation analysis across the two samples. We hypothesized that IU, anxiety, delusion-proneness, and paranoia would be positively associated with COVID-19 CT. Finally, we examined whether the relationship between IU and CT can be explained by schizotypy-related dimensions (delusion-proneness and paranoia) and/or by affective dimensions like anxiety, both of which are related to IU and CT.

Study 1

Method

Data were collected electronically from 261 participants between the dates of April 21st, 2020 and May 8th, 2020. Twenty-one individuals (8%) who spent <10 min completing surveys were excluded from analyses due to suspected inattentiveness, leaving 240 participants in the final sample. Median completion time for the remainder of the sample was 27.3 min. All study participants were Stony Brook University (SBU) undergraduates who received course credit for participation. The study measures were collected in the context of a larger study on attitudes during COVID-19 [see (43) for other variables assessed in the Study 1 sample]. Full participant demographics are presented in Table 1. Informed consent was obtained prior to study participation, and study procedures were approved by the SBU Institutional Review Board (IRB).

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Demographics and questionnaire descriptives for Study 1.

Individual Difference Measures

Questionnaires were administered via Qualtrics survey software. Demographic questions were first, followed by remaining questionnaires in randomized order.

Intolerance of Uncertainty

IU was measured using the short form Intolerance of Uncertainty Scale [IUS; (14, 44)]. The IUS assesses “excessive tendency of an individual to consider it unacceptable that a negative event may occur, however small the probability of its occurrence” (13, p. 932). It has been validated in young adult samples, in which it demonstrates very high internal consistency (44). The 12 short form items were summed for analysis (45).

Schizotypy

Schizotypal traits were assessed using the short Wisconsin Schizotypy Scales (33). Each scale contains 15 yes/no items with the sum of items comprising the scale score. These scales include two measures of “positive schizotypy,” assessing hallucination-proneness [Perceptual Aberration Scale (PAS)] and delusion-proneness [Magical Ideation Scale (MIS)], as well as two measures of “negative schizotypy” assessing physical and social anhedonia. The negative subscales were combined for the present analyses for use as a single index. The short-form Wisconsin scales have been well-validated in young adults and demonstrate even higher internal consistency than their long-form counterparts (33).

Paranoia

Paranoia was assessed using the Revised Green et al. Paranoid Thought Scale [GPTS; (46, 47)]. The GPTS contains two sub-scales of 10 and 8 items, yielding measures of ideas of social reference and persecution, respectively, over the past month. The Revised GPTS has excellent psychometric characteristics compared to its long-form counterpart and has been validated for diverse clinical and non-clinical sample-types (46). Because the present work does not hypothesize differential relationships with the two domains of paranoia, the two subscales were summed to form a single paranoia index utilized in analyses, which has been done in past work as well (48). Main analyses are also reproduced using the two subscales independently in Supplementary Tables 3, 4.

Conspiratorial Thinking

COVID-19 CT was evaluated using 16 items drawn from online news outlets reporting on common conspiracy theories (see Supplementary Table 1). Response options and instructions were adapted from the Generic Conspiracist Beliefs Scale (49), a broad measure of engagement with conspiracy theories. The score used in analyses represents mean endorsement across all items. In addition, we calculated the number of items rated as either Probably true or Definitely true for each participant to descriptively characterize overall CT.

Data Availability

Data and syntax used for analyses in both Study 1 and Study 2 are available at https://osf.io/dtzne/.

Analyses

All analyses used SPSS 27. Bivariate Pearson correlations assessed the relationships between endorsement of COVID-19 CT and individual difference measures; spearman correlations were used in place of these for variables that demonstrated skewness values > 1.0. Linear regressions with CT as the outcome assessed specificity of schizotypy domains when controlling for shared variance. As a robustness check, linear regressions were repeated using log-transformed versions of variables in cases where significant skew was observed. Data visualizations were created using the ggplot2 package (50) of the R programming environment (51).

Results

Descriptive Characteristics and Correlations

Descriptive statistics for self-report measures are presented in Table 1 (interrelationships between scales are presented in Supplementary Table 2). Skewness values >1.0: were observed for MIS (1.19) and PAS (2.43); thus Spearman correlations were computed in lieu of Pearson correlations for these variables. Correlation analyses revealed significant associations (p < 0.05) of COVID-19 CT with all variables (Figure 1). These relationships remained significant after applying FDR correction for multiple comparisons.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Correlation coefficients for MIS and PAS represent Spearman's Rho while all others represent Pearson's R. CT, Conspiratorial thinking; IUS, Intolerance of Uncertainty Scale; MIS, Magical Ideation Scale; PAS, Perceptual Aberration Scale; GPTS, Green et al. Paranoid Thought Scale.

Specificity of Schizotypy Dimensions Associated With CT

A linear regression analysis with COVID-19 CT as outcome assessed the hypothesized relationships with MIS and GPTS when controlling for variance shared between them and with PAS and negative schizotypy. Significant independent coefficients emerged for both MIS (β = 0.173, p = 0.024) and GPTS (β = 0.148, p = 0.034); no other partial coefficients accounted for significant variance (ps > 0.30). In total, the adjusted R2 for this model was 0.079 [F(4,232) = 6.09, p < 0.001]. In addition, when conducting this analysis using log-transformed versions of MIS and PAS, we observed the same results: significant coefficients emerged for MIS (β = 0.218, p = 0.003) and GPTS (β = 0.147, p = 0.038), all other ps > 0.2.

Discussion

Study 1 found preliminary support for the hypotheses that individual differences in IU and schizotypy (which may be specific to delusion-proneness and paranoia) contribute to COVID-19-related CT. Given the rapidly-changing nature of both the COVID-19 pandemic as well as the availability of information to the public, we deemed it important to reexamine these relationships at a second time point within the pandemic when atmospheric uncertainty may be different. In addition, pooling data across Studies 1 and 2 provided statistical power to examine possible mediators of the relationship between IU and CT (52). Thus, Study 2 aimed to accomplish both the goals of testing replicability and obtaining power to test mediation.

Study 2

Method

Data were collected electronically from 258 participants between the dates of October 15th, 2020 and November 19th, 2020. Sixteen individuals (6%) who spent <10 min completing study surveys were excluded from analyses due to suspected inattentiveness, leaving 242 participants in the final sample. Median completion time for the remainder of the sample was 38.1 min. Missing data was handled by retaining participants for each analysis who answered all items for the relevant questionnaire(s). All study participants were SBU undergraduates who received course credit for participation. Participant demographics are presented in Table 2. Informed consent was obtained prior to study participation, and study procedures were approved by the SBU IRB.

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Demographics and questionnaire descriptives for Study 2.

Measures

Administration procedures as well as measures of IU, schizotypy, paranoia, and CT in Study 2 were identical to those in Study 1. In Study 2 we additionally assessed anxiety using the State-Trait Anxiety Inventory-State scale [STAIS; (53)], which contains 20 items measuring momentary experience of apprehension, tension, nervousness, and worry.

Analyses

Primary analyses were conducted identically to those in Study 1. In addition, we conducted mediation analysis using the PROCESS macro version 3.5 (54) to examine whether the relationship between IU and CT was explained by anxiety, delusion-proneness, or paranoia. Significance in these analyses at alpha = 0.05 is indicated by an indirect effect confidence interval that does not include zero.

To obtain appropriate statistical power for mediation analyses, we combined samples from studies 1 and 2. Additionally, to ensure that results were not affected by potential differences between the two study samples, we reran significant models with the inclusion of sample source (Study 1 or Study 2) as a covariate.

Results

Descriptive Characteristics and Correlations

Descriptive statistics for self-report measures are presented in Table 2 (interrelationships between scales are presented in Supplementary Table 2). Skewness values >1.0: were observed for PAS (1.91) and Negative schizotypy (1.11); thus Spearman correlations were computed in lieu of Pearson correlations for these variables. Correlation analyses revealed significant associations of COVID-19 CT with all variables (p < 0.05) except anxiety and negative schizotypy, thus replicating 4 of the 5 relationships identified in Study 1 (Figure 2A). These relationships remained significant after applying FDR correction.

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. (A) Relationships of individual difference measures with CT in Study 2. Correlation coefficients for PAS and Negative schizotypy represent Spearman's Rho while all others represent Pearson's R. (B) Mediation paths for three possible factors explaining the relationship between IU and CT. CT, Conspiratorial thinking; IUS, Intolerance of Uncertainty Scale; MIS, Magical Ideation Scale; PAS, Perceptual Aberration Scale; GPTS, Green et al. Paranoid Thought Scale. **p < 0.01, ***p < 0.001.

Specificity of Schizotypy With CT

A linear regression analysis with COVID-19 CT as outcome assessed the hypothesized relationships with MIS and GPTS when controlling for variance shared with PAS and negative schizotypy. As in Study 1, a significant independent coefficient emerged for MIS (β = 0.350, p = 0.013). Contrary to Study 1, we did not observe an independent effect of GPTS (β = −0.005, p = 0.965). No other partial coefficients accounted for significant variance (ps > 0.30). The adjusted R2 for this model was 0.081 [F(4,80) = 2.845, p = 0.029]. In addition, when conducting this analysis using log-transformed versions of negative schizotypy and PAS, we observed the same results: significant coefficients emerged for MIS only (β = 0.333, p = 0.017), all other ps > 0.5.

Mediators of IU to CT

Mediational analyses examined whether relationships between IU and CT could be accounted for by anxiety (STAIS), delusion-proneness (MIS), or paranoia (GPTS). We first entered STAIS as the mediating variable and found a non-significant indirect effect (β = 0.005, SE = 0.027, 95% CI = −0.047, 0.060). With MIS as the mediating variable, we observed a significant indirect effect (β = 0.069, SE = 0.019, 95% CI = 0.035, 0.110). Likewise, with paranoia as the mediating variable, we observed a significant indirect effect (β = 0.089, SE = 0.026, 95% CI = 0.041, 0.142). Figure 2B depicts these pathways. To assess directionality of the two significant models, we entered IUS as the mediating variable, and did not observe a significant indirect effect for either the model from MIS or from GPTS to CT (95% CIs both included zero). Lastly, we reran the two significant models when controlling for source sample (Study 1 or Study 2), revealing that both models retained significant indirect effects of MIS and GPTS respectively.

Discussion

Results from Study 2 replicated those of Study 1 with two exceptions: a non-significant correlation of negative schizotypy with CT as well as slight difference in the result of the linear regression, such that delusion-proneness was the only significant factor associated with CT among schizotypy-related variables (whereas Study 1 found that paranoia was also significant). This divergent finding may point to a different relationship between paranoia and COVID-19-related CT in early 2020 as opposed to late 2020. Lastly, Study 2 provided evidence that the relationship between IU and CT is explained by delusion-proneness and paranoia, but not by anxiety.

General Discussion

Conspiracy theories have had heightened salience and prevalence during the COVID-19 pandemic (3, 8, 36). Ambient uncertainty has also been high due to constantly changing availability of information, and it is hypothesized that such uncertainty may activate the epistemic needs associated with CT (2). This work sought to shed light on the individual differences leading to increased COVID-19-CT amongst young adults, a demographic at heightened risk for CT (8, 20). Study 1 examined the hypothesized relationships between IU, delusion-proneness, paranoia, and CT; Study 2 assessed whether these relationships replicated and examined possible mediators of the IU-CT relationship. Study 1 showed that higher IU and schizotypy were related to increased COVID-19-CT. Examination of specific components of schizotypy showed that delusion-proneness and paranoia independently related to CT while other related facets did not. Study 2 replicated these findings (with one slight difference in the specificity analysis) and additionally showed that delusion-proneness and paranoia, but not anxiety, were significant mediators of the association between IU and CT.

Earlier theoretical [e.g., (15)] and empirical (16) work has identified the relationship between IU and CT in line with the present findings. It has also been strongly suggested that CT is most pronounced in historical times of crisis (13); our study provides evidence for the relationship of IU and CT in such a time of heightened uncertainty. While some preliminary work during the pandemic has shown an association between IU and generic CT (18, 19), the present findings are the first to establish the relationship of IU with COVID-19-specific CT. Some past work outside the context of a global crisis has suggested the relationship between IU and CT is less than clear (16), and it is possible that our sample exhibited an amplified relationship between these factors due to the particular salience of environmental uncertainty and uncontrollability caused by the COVID-19 pandemic. Indeed, the salience of uncertainty is thought to affect the relationship of IU to CT (17), and experimental evidence suggests that CT can be both increased and decreased by manipulating sense of control of outcomes (55).

Our findings are also in line with past work demonstrating relationships between schizotypy and CT (2224) and extend findings to context-specific CT in a major public health crisis. Furthermore, while past work has not often examined independent facets of schizotypy, here we show specificity in relationships with schizotypal symptoms. Neither negative schizotypy nor the perceptual aberration component of positive schizotypy were independently related to CT; rather, effects were specific to delusion-proneness and (in Study 1 only) paranoia. Past studies that have examined relationships with particular symptom/trait domains are consistent with the notion that delusion-proneness and paranoia may have unique contributions to CT (23, 30, 35). Our findings suggest that the independent effect of paranoia may be less stable and influenced by pandemic-related factors, as it did not replicate in the late-2020 sample. The effect of delusion-proneness is also consistent with work showing that CT relates to a tendency to perceive non-existent agency and intentionality (56, 57), an effect that is stronger in individuals with schizotypal traits (57). Future work should investigate the degree to which hypersensitive agency detection may explain the relationship between delusion-proneness and CT.

The finding that delusion-proneness and paranoia mediated the IU-CT relationship is a novel empirical contribution to the literature. It is consistent with theoretical work on cognitive functions played by CT and delusion-like thinking (2, 26, 27) as well as empirical evidence of the relationship between delusions and IU in psychosis (38, 39). However, no previous study to our knowledge has assessed dynamics of these variables with relation to CT. Furthermore, the mediation effect sizes were substantial: delusion-proneness and paranoia explained 40.6 and 52.4%, respectively, of the relationship between IU and CT. Our results provide preliminary evidence that for individuals experiencing IU and with proneness to delusion-like thinking or mistrust, conspiracies represent an appealing option for belief. Conversely, our finding that anxiety did not explain the relationship between IU and CT provides an important contrast and clue about the psychological dynamics that result in CT in times of uncertainty. That is, while those who experience uncertainty as particularly aversive also tend to experience higher levels of anxiety and distress (particularly in the context of the global pandemic), this distress may not be the mechanism by which conspiracy theories come to be an appealing epistemic anecdote for uncertainty. Rather, our results suggest that prior tendencies for delusion-proneness and paranoid ideation are more likely to drive those experiencing aversive uncertainty to settle on conspiratorial conclusions. Overall, this constitutes evidence for processes involved in reasoning and belief formation as opposed to emotional responses to uncertain threats in explaining the relation between IU and CT in young adults, which may have implications for intervention.

Our cross-sectional design is a limitation of the present work. While we posit that the personality constructs evaluated were pre-existing and constitute vulnerability factors for CT, such an inference can only be truly supported via longitudinal measurements. Additionally, little work has assessed measurement invariance in the time of COVID-19, and it is possible that self-report instruments do not operate identically in a context of environmental uncertainty posed by the pandemic (58). One final limitation of this work is that it took a relatively broad approach to assessing endorsement of CT. There is considerable variation in the degree to which conspiracy theories are divorced from reality, and some types of CT may originate from socio-political, demographic and economic realities (59, 60). It is thus likely that future work would benefit from taking a more nuanced approach to assessment of types of CT and potentially separable factors associated with them. Such examinations would be crucial to quantifying the portions of CT phenomena that are and are not explained by the psychopathology-related factors examined here–a point that is further highlighted by noting the relatively small percent of variance accounted for by the variables we examined here (~8%).

Future work should attempt to identify cognitive mechanisms of the relationships shown here to better understand the nature of traits that predispose to CT during a global health crisis. It remains unclear whether processes such as cognitive insight and analytical thinking, which are diminished in individuals with schizotypal traits and CT proneness (61), play significant roles in heightened threat and uncertainty. Additionally, given the potentially-dissociable relationships of CT with delusion-proneness and paranoia, further work is needed to identify whether they differ in their pathways to CT, cognitive mediators, and behavioral correlates. Such work would also clarify the precise targets for informational interventions to protect vulnerable populations from the allure of conspiracy theories. Importantly, the present conclusions are drawn from a sample of young adults, a population that is both more likely to endorse COVID-19-specific CT and to be asymptomatic spreaders of the COVID-19 virus. Given the predisposition amongst this demographic to both IU and schizotypal traits, the present findings represent highly relevant targets for future interventions. It is possible that additional factors may be contributory to COVID-19-specific CT in other populations–including other populations of young adults with different demographic compositions; future studies should seek to explore potential effects of demographic group on the effectiveness of intervention strategies by recruiting larger and more diverse samples.

Despite the scientific community condemning conspiratorial thinking and misinformation spread early in the COVID-19 pandemic (62, 63), it is likely that more concerted efforts are needed to stymie the proliferation of misinformation that contributes to CT during the remainder of the pandemic and beyond. While the optimal strategy for fighting misinformation is unclear, recent attempts as well as cumulative efforts made by the scientific community to correct misinformation show promise in combating the COVID-19 “infodemic” (64, 65). These findings suggest that targeting cognitive factors (rather than affective responses), such as engaging analytic thinking for COVID-19 related information, will improve intervention strategies since such thinking is less habitual for some with a predilection for CT (61). In addition, efforts might benefit from consulting the growing body of research that has supported the efficacy of targeting IU itself to incite meaningful changes in thinking (66, 67).

Data Availability Statement

The datasets presented in this study can be found in online repositories. The names of the repository/repositories and accession number(s) can be found at: https://osf.io/dtzne/.

Ethics Statement

The studies involving human participants were reviewed and approved by Stony Brook University Institutional Review Board. All participants indicated informed consent for study procedures.

Author Contributions

EL and AM conceived the study and conducted the analyses. EL and ML collected and curated the data. EL, KD, ML, and AM wrote and revised the manuscript. All authors contributed to the article and approved the submitted version.

Conflict of Interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher's Note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

Acknowledgments

The authors are grateful to Nic Weitermann, Clare McDonald, Viveka Hall-Holt, Akaisha An, Dmitro Gubenko, and Kristina Pidvirny for logistical support, to Frances Jin and Howard Huang for helpful conversations, and to reviewers for comments on earlier drafts.

Supplementary Material

The Supplementary Material for this article can be found online at: https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fpsyt.2021.698147/full#supplementary-material

References

1. Webster DM, Kruglanski AW. Individual differences in need for cognitive closure. J Pers Soc Psychol. (1994) 67:1049–62. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.67.6.1049

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

2. Douglas KM, Sutton RM, Cichocka A. The psychology of conspiracy theories: current directions in psychological. Science. 26:538–42. doi: 10.1177/0963721417718261

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

3. Grey Ellis E. The Coronavirus Outbreak Is a Petri Dish for Conspiracy Theories. Wired. (2020). Available online at: https://www.wired.com/story/coronavirus-conspiracy-theories/ (accessed June 1, 2020).

4. Romer D, Jamieson KH. Conspiracy theories as barriers to controlling the spread of COVID-19 in the US. Soc Sci Med. (2020) 263:113356. doi: 10.1016/j.socscimed.2020.113356

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

5. van der Linden S. The conspiracy-effect: Exposure to conspiracy theories (about global warming) decreases pro-social behavior and science acceptance. Pers Individ Dif. (2015) 87:171–3. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2015.07.045

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

6. Imhoff R, Lamberty P. A Bioweapon or a Hoax? The link between distinct conspiracy beliefs about the coronavirus disease (COVID-19) outbreak and pandemic behavior. Soc Psychol Personal. Sci. (2020) 11:1110–8. doi: 10.1177/1948550620934692

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

7. Swami V, Barron D. Analytic thinking, rejection of coronavirus (COVID-19) conspiracy theories, and compliance with mandated social-distancing: direct and indirect relationships in a nationally representative sample of adults in the United Kingdom. Preprints. (2020). doi: 10.31219/osf.io/nmx9w. [Epub ahead of print].

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

8. Lazer D, Baum MA, Ognyanova K, Della Volpe J, Quintana A, Chwe H, et al. The State of The Nation: A 50-State Covid-19 Survey. Boston, MA: Northeastern University (2020). doi: 10.31219/osf.io/arwh3

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

9. Park CL, Russell BS, Fendrich M, Finkelstein-Fox L, Hutchison M, Becker J. Americans' COVID-19 stress, coping, and adherence to CDC guidelines. J Gene Internal Med. (2020) 35:2296–303. doi: 10.1007/s11606-020-05898-9

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

10. Moghadas SM, Fitzpatrick MC, Sah P, Pandey A, Shoukat A, Singer BH, et al. The implications of silent transmission for the control of COVID-19 outbreaks. Proc Nat Acad Sci USA. (2020) 117:17513–5. doi: 10.1073/pnas.2008373117

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

11. Van Bavel JJ, Baicker K, Boggio PS, Capraro V, Cichocka A, Cikara M, et al. Using social and behavioural science to support COVID-19 pandemic response. Nat Human Behav. (2020) 4:460–71. doi: 10.1038/s41562-020-0884-z

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

12. Jaiswal J, LoSchiavo C, Perlman DC. Disinformation, misinformation and inequality-driven mistrust in the time of COVID-19: lessons unlearned from AIDS denialism. AIDS Behav. (2020). doi: 10.1007/s10461-020-02925-y

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

13. van Prooijen JW, Douglas KM. Conspiracy theories as part of history: the role of societal crisis situations. Memory Stud. (2017) 10:323–33. doi: 10.1177/1750698017701615

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

14. Buhr K, Dugas MJ. The intolerance of uncertainty scale: psychometric properties of the English version. Behav Res Ther. (2002) 40:931–45. doi: 10.1016/S0005-7967(01)00092-4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

15. Abalakina-Paap M, Stephan WG, Craig T, Gregory WL. Beliefs in conspiracies. Polit Psychol. (1999) 20:637–47. doi: 10.1111/0162-895X.00160

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

16. Moulding R, Nix-Carnell S, Schnabel A, Nedeljkovic M, Burnside EE, Lentini AF, et al. Better the devil you know than a world you don't? Intolerance of uncertainty and worldview explanations for belief in conspiracy theories. Personal Individ Differ. (2016) 98:345–54. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2016.04.060

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

17. van Prooijen JW, Jostmann NB. Belief in conspiracy theories: The influence of uncertainty and perceived morality. Eur J Soc Psychol. (2013) 43:109–15. doi: 10.1002/ejsp.1922

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

18. Farias J, Pilati R. COVID-19 as an undesirable political issue: Conspiracy beliefs and intolerance of uncertainty predict adhesion to prevention measures. Curr Psychol. (2021). doi: 10.1007/s12144-021-01416-0. [Epub ahead of print].

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

19. Maftei A, Holman AC. Beliefs in conspiracy theories, intolerance of uncertainty, and moral disengagement during the coronavirus crisis. Ethics Behav. (2020). doi: 10.1080/10508422.2020.1843171. [Epub ahead of print].

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

20. Duplaga M. The determinants of conspiracy beliefs related to the COVID-19 pandemic in a nationally representative sample of internet users. Int J Environ Res Public Health. (2020) 17:7818. doi: 10.3390/ijerph17217818

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

21. Glowacz F, Schmits E. Psychological distress during the COVID-19 lockdown: the young adults most at risk. Psychiatry Res. (2020) 293:113486. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2020.113486

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

22. Bruder M, Haffke P, Neave N, Nouripanah N, Imhoff R. Measuring individual differences in generic beliefs in conspiracy theories across cultures: conspiracy mentality questionnaire. Front. Psychol. (2013) 4:225. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00225

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

23. Darwin H, Neave N, Holmes J. Belief in conspiracy theories. The role of paranormal belief, paranoid ideation and schizotypy. Personal Individ Differ. (2011) 50:1289–93. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2011.02.027

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

24. Hart J, Graether M. Something's going on here. J Individ Differ. (2018) 39:229–37. doi: 10.1027/1614-0001/a000268

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

25. Woodward TS, Buchy L, Moritz S, Liotti M. A bias against disconfirmatory evidence is associated with delusion proneness in a nonclinical sample. Schizophr Bull. (2007) 33:1023–8. doi: 10.1093/schbul/sbm013

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

26. Feeney EJ, Groman SM, Taylor JR, Corlett PR. Explaining delusions: reducing uncertainty through basic and computational neuroscience. Schizophr Bull. (2017) 43:263–72. doi: 10.1093/schbul/sbw194

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

27. McKay R, Kinsbourne M. Confabulation, delusion, and anosognosia: Motivational factors and false claims. Cogn Neuropsychiatry. (2010) 15:288–318. doi: 10.1080/13546800903374871

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

28. McKay R, Langdon R, Coltheart M. “Sleights of mind”: Delusions, defences, and self-deception. Cognit Neuropsychiatry. (2005) 10:305–26. doi: 10.1080/13546800444000074

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

29. Lewandowsky S, Oberauer K, Gignac GE. NASA faked the moon landing-therefore, (climate) science is a hoax: an anatomy of the motivated rejection of science. Psychol Sci. (2013) 24:622–33. doi: 10.1177/0956797612457686

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

30. Barron D, Morgan K, Towell T, Altemeyer B, Swami V. Associations between schizotypy and belief in conspiracist ideation. Pers Individ Dif. (2014) 70:156–9. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2014.06.040

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

31. Dagnall N, Drinkwater K, Parker A, Denovan A, Parton M. Conspiracy theory and cognitive style: A worldview. Front Psychol. (2015) 6:206. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2015.00206

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

32. March E, Springer J. Belief in conspiracy theories: the predictive role of schizotypy, Machiavellianism, primary psychopathy. PLoS ONE. (2019) 14:225964. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0225964

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

33. Winterstein BP, Silvia PJ, Kwapil TR, Kaufman JC, Reiter-Palmon R, Wigert B. Brief assessment of schizotypy: developing short forms of the wisconsin schizotypy scales. Pers Individ Dif. (2011) 51:920–4. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2011.07.027

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

34. Kwapil TR, Barrantes-Vidal N, Silvia PJ. The dimensional structure of the wisconsin schizotypy scales: factor identification and construct validity. Schizophr Bull. (2008) 34:444–57. doi: 10.1093/schbul/sbm098

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

35. Swami V, Weis L, Lay A, Barron D, Furnham A. Associations between belief in conspiracy theories and the maladaptive personality traits of the personality inventory for DSM-5. Psychiatry Res. (2016) 236:86–90. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2015.12.027

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

36. Freeman D, Waite F, Rosebrock L, Petit A, Causier C, East A, et al. Coronavirus conspiracy beliefs, mistrust, and compliance with government guidelines in England. Psychol Med. (2020) 1–13. doi: 10.1017/S0033291720001890

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

37. Bora E, Baysan Arabaci L. Effect of age and gender on schizotypal personality traits in the normal population. Psychiatry Clin Neurosci. (2009) 63:663–9. doi: 10.1111/j.1440-1819.2009.02011.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

38. Bredemeier K, McCole K, Luther L, Beck AT, Grant PM. Reliability and validity of a brief version of the intolerance of uncertainty scale in outpatients with psychosis. J Psychopathol Behav Assess. (2019) 41:221–34. doi: 10.1007/s10862-018-9714-2

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

39. Startup H, Pugh K, Dunn G, Cordwell J, Mander H, Cernis E, et al. Worry processes in patients with persecutory delusions. Br J Clin Psychol. (2016) 55:387–400. doi: 10.1111/bjc.12109

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

40. Fountoulakis KN, Apostolidou MK, Atsiova MB, Filippidou AK, Florou AK, Gousiou DS, et al. Self-reported changes in anxiety, depression and suicidality during the COVID-19 lockdown in Greece. J Affect Disord. (2020) 279:624–9. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2020.10.061

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

41. McEvoy PM, Mahoney AE. To be sure, to be sure: intolerance of uncertainty mediates symptoms of various anxiety disorders and depression. Behav Ther. (2012) 43:533–45. doi: 10.1016/j.beth.2011.02.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

42. McEvoy PM, Erceg-Hurn DM. The search for universal transdiagnostic and trans-therapy change processes: Evidence for intolerance of uncertainty. J Anxiety Disord. (2016) 41:96–107. doi: 10.1016/j.janxdis.2016.02.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

43. Imbriano G, Larsen EM, Mackin DM, An AK, Luhmann CC, Mohanty A, et al. Online survey of the impact of COVID-19 risk and cost estimates on worry and health behavior compliance in young adults. Front Public Health. (2021) 9:157. doi: 10.3389/fpubh.2021.612725

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

44. Carleton RN, Norton MAPJ, Asmundson GJG. Fearing the unknown: a short version of the intolerance of uncertainty scale. J Anxiety Disord. (2007) 21:105–17. doi: 10.1016/j.janxdis.2006.03.014

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

45. Hale W, Richmond M, Bennett J, Berzins T, Fields A, Weber D, et al. Resolving uncertainty about the intolerance of uncertainty scale-12: application of modern psychometric strategies. J Pers Assess. (2016) 98:200–8. doi: 10.1080/00223891.2015.1070355

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

46. Freeman D, Loe BS, Kingdon D, Startup H, Molodynski A, Bird JC. Paranoid Thoughts Scale (R-GPTS): Psychometric properties, severity ranges, clinical cut-offs. Psychol Med. (2019) 51:244–53. doi: 10.1017/S0033291719003155

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

47. Green CEL, Freeman D, Kuipers E, Bebbington P, Fowler D, Dunn G, et al. Measuring ideas of persecution and social reference: The Green et al. Paranoid Thought Scales (GPTS). Psychol Med. (2008) 38:101–11. doi: 10.1017/S0033291707001638

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

48. Lee HS, Hong SJJ, Baxter T, Scott J, Shenoy S, Buck L, et al. Altered peripersonal space and the bodily self in schizophrenia: a virtual reality study. Schizophr Bull. (2021) 47:927–37. doi: 10.1093/schbul/sbab024

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

49. Brotherton R, French CC, Pickering AD. Measuring belief in conspiracy theories: the generic conspiracist beliefs scale. Front Psychol. (2013) 4:279. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00279

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

50. Wickham H. ggplot2-Elegant Graphics for Data Analysis. Cham: Springer International Publishing. (2016). doi: 10.1007/978-3-319-24277-4

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

51. R Core Team. R: A Language and Environment for Statistical Computing. R Foundation for Statistical Computing, Vienna (2020).

Google Scholar

52. Fritz MS, MacKinnon DP. Required sample size to detect the mediated effect. Psychol Sci. (2007) 18:233–9. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2007.01882.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

53. Spielberger CD, Gorsuch RL, Lushene RE, Vagg PR, Jacobs GA. State-Trait Anxiety Inventory. Palo Alto, CA: Mind Garden (1983).

Google Scholar

54. Hayes AF. Introduction to Mediation, Moderation, and Conditional Process Analysis, Second Edition: A Regression-Based Approach. New York, NY: Guilford Publications (2017).

Google Scholar

55. van Prooijen JW, Acker M. The influence of control on belief in conspiracy theories: conceptual and applied extensions. Appl Cogn Psychol. (2015) 29:753–61. doi: 10.1002/acp.3161

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

56. Douglas KM, Sutton RM, Callan MJ, Dawtry RJ, Harvey AJ. Someone is pulling the strings: hypersensitive agency detection and belief in conspiracy theories. Think Reason. (2016) 22:57–77. doi: 10.1080/13546783.2015.1051586

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

57. van der Tempel J, Alcock JE. Relationships between conspiracy mentality, hyperactive agency detection, and schizotypy: Supernatural forces at work? Pers Individ Dif. (2015) 82:136–41. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2015.03.010

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

58. Olino TM, Case JA, Hawes MT, Szenczy A, Nelson B, Klein DN. Testing invariance of measures of internalizing symptoms before and after a major life stressor: the impact of COVID-19 in an adolescent and young adult sample. Assessment. (2021). doi: 10.1177/10731911211015315. [Epub ahead of print].

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

59. Bale JM. Political paranoia v. political realism: On distinguishing between bogus conspiracy theories and genuine conspiratorial politics. Patterns Prejudice. (2007) 41:45–60. doi: 10.1080/00313220601118751

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

60. Oleksy T, Wnuk A, Maison D, Łyś A. Content matters. Different predictors and social consequences of general and government-related conspiracy theories on COVID-19. Personal Individ Differ. (2021) 168:110289. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2020.110289

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

61. Barron D, Furnham A, Weis L, Morgan KD, Towell T, Swami V. The relationship between schizotypal facets and conspiracist beliefs via cognitive processes. Psychiatry Res. (2018) 259:15–20. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2017.10.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

62. Calisher C, Carroll D, Colwell R, Corley RB, Daszak P, Drosten C, et al. Statement in support of the scientists, public health professionals, and medical professionals of China combatting COVID-19. Lancet. (2020) 395:e42–3. doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736(20)30418-9

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

63. Mian A, Khan S. Coronavirus: The spread of misinformation. BMC Med. (2020) 18:89. doi: 10.1186/s12916-020-01556-3

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

64. Caulfield T. Does debunking work? Correcting COVID-19 Misinformation on Social Media. In: Flood CM, editor. Vulnerable: The Law, Policy and Ethics of COVID-19. Ottawa, ON: University of Ottawa Press (2020). doi: 10.31219/osf.io/5uy2f

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

65. Pennycook G, McPhetres J, Zhang Y, Lu JG, Rand DG. Fighting COVID-19 misinformation on social media: experimental evidence for a scalable accuracy-nudge intervention. Psychol Sci. (2020) 31:770–80. doi: 10.1177/0956797620939054

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

66. Dugas MJ, Ladouceur R. Treatment of GAD: Targeting intolerance of uncertainty in two types of worry. Behav Modif. (2000) 24:635–57. doi: 10.1177/0145445500245002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

67. Mahoney AE, McEvoy PM. Changes in intolerance of uncertainty during cognitive behavior group therapy for social phobia. J Behav Ther Exp Psychiatry. (2012) 43:849–54. doi: 10.1016/j.jbtep.2011.12.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: conspiracy, conspiracy theory, schizotypy, anxiety, individual differences, epistemic

Citation: Larsen EM, Donaldson KR, Liew M and Mohanty A (2021) Conspiratorial Thinking During COVID-19: The Roles of Paranoia, Delusion-Proneness, and Intolerance of Uncertainty. Front. Psychiatry 12:698147. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2021.698147

Received: 20 April 2021; Accepted: 29 June 2021;
Published: 18 August 2021.

Edited by:

Wulf Rössler, Charité–Universitätsmedizin Berlin, Germany

Reviewed by:

Joachim Kowalski, Polish Academy of Sciences, Poland
Wan-jun Guo, Sichuan University, China

Copyright © 2021 Larsen, Donaldson, Liew and Mohanty. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Emmett M. Larsen, ZW1tZXR0LmxhcnNlbiYjeDAwMDQwO3N0b255YnJvb2suZWR1; ZW10bGFyc2VuJiN4MDAwNDA7Z21haWwuY29t

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.