Skip to main content

REVIEW article

Front. Behav. Neurosci., 23 January 2018
Sec. Individual and Social Behaviors
This article is part of the Research Topic Anger and Interpersonal Aggression View all 24 articles

Biological Clocks and Rhythms of Anger and Aggression

  • 1Department of Psychology, Bishop’s University, Sherbrooke, QC, Canada
  • 2Department of Psychology, Concordia University, Montreal, QC, Canada

The body’s internal timekeeping system is an under-recognized but highly influential force in behaviors and emotions including anger and reactive aggression. Predictable cycles or rhythms in behavior are expressed on several different time scales such as circadian (circa diem, or approximately 24-h rhythms) and infradian (exceeding 24 h, such as monthly or seasonal cycles). The circadian timekeeping system underlying rhythmic behaviors in mammals is constituted by a network of clocks distributed throughout the brain and body, the activity of which synchronizes to a central pacemaker, or master clock. Our daily experiences with the external environment including social activity strongly influence the exact timing of this network. In the present review, we examine evidence from a number of species and propose that anger and reactive aggression interact in multiple ways with circadian clocks. Specifically, we argue that: (i) there are predictable rhythms in the expression of aggression and anger; (ii) disruptions of the normal functioning of the circadian system increase the likelihood of aggressive behaviors; and (iii) conversely, chronic expression of anger can disrupt normal rhythmic cycles of physiological activities and create conditions for pathologies such as cardiovascular disease to develop. Taken together, these observations suggest that a comprehensive perspective on anger and reactive aggression must incorporate an understanding of the role of the circadian timing system in these intense affective states.

Introduction

In the writings of Galen and Aristotle, changes in human tempers were associated with the passage of time, where summer was the season of yellow bile, a humor responsible for a “nature that is angry, insolent, or fierce” (Grant, 2000, p.17). Although humorism has long since been abandoned as a medical perspective, the notion that states of anger and aggressive behavior nonetheless exhibit predictable cycles of waxing and waning across time holds some merit. Essentially all species on Earth possess internal timekeeping mechanisms that govern a multitude of cellular, physiological, and behavioral processes. An abundance of evidence demonstrates that these timekeeping mechanisms form a vital part of our physical and mental health, and that disruptions to their normal functioning can severely compromise emotional state and well-being.

Anger and aggressive behaviors are normal parts of the human behavioral repertoire, and an absence of these can be highly disadvantageous for survival (Green and Phillips, 2004; Waltes et al., 2016). At the same time, there is a maladaptive relationship between excessive anger and health, and this maladaptive relationship implicates the operation of the body’s internal timekeeping mechanisms. Here, we review evidence indicating that there are predictable cycles of anger and aggression in humans and non-human species, and offer critical insight into the mechanisms by which normal operation of the body’s circadian system influences these patterns of aggressive behavior across time. We also examine evidence that demonstrates a complex relationship between excessive anger in humans and disruption of circadian clocks: specifically, that disturbances of the body’s time keeping system increase the likelihood of aggression and irritability; and, reciprocally, that the physiological symptoms of anger and aggression perturb the normal functioning of this system.

Defining Anger and Aggression

The variety of operational definitions of anger and aggression presents a challenge for establishing whether there are predictable rhythms of these behaviors in humans and non-human species (Kempes et al., 2005; Mathias et al., 2007; Fung et al., 2009). It is important to consider these differences in terminology carefully, because some behaviors and emotional states appear to exhibit predictable rhythms whereas others do not. For example, reactive aggression in humans is argued to differ from proactive aggression, in that the former represents a response to a potential threat and is associated with high arousal and impulsivity, whereas the latter is a low arousal, calculated behavior intended to obtain instrumental ends such as a reward (Hubbard et al., 2002; Kempes et al., 2005). As discussed below, some evidence suggests that humans exhibit predictable cycles in displaying reactive aggression (e.g., Leggett et al., 2015; Hwang et al., 2016), but not proactive aggression. Distinguishing among types of aggressive behaviors is also important for identifying the environmental and physiological mechanisms that underlie each, and for determining how these mechanisms are linked to the internal timekeeping system. For example, in song sparrows, cyclic increases in androgen activity strongly influence territorial aggression in breeding season, but these hormone rhythms do not play the same role in regulating aggressive behavior outside of the breeding season (Wingfield, 2012). For these reasons, we have tried in the following sections to specify the type of aggression under analysis; state the context in which the behavior is displayed; and describe the features of the behavior, where possible.

The Circadian Oscillatory Network and Biological Rhythms

Endogenous biological clocks regulate patterns of physiological activity and behavior on several time scales. Cycles of change that complete within 24 h are known as circadian rhythms and include examples such as the sleep/wake cycle, body temperature change, and release of hormones such as melatonin and cortisol. Circadian rhythms provide an adaptive mechanism for organisms to coordinate physiological functions and behaviors with the predictable 24-h cycle of light and dark on Earth. In mammals, the suprachiasmatic nucleus (SCN) in the hypothalamus contains the master circadian clock, and exposure to daylight provides the dominant cue to synchronize this master clock to the external environment (RW.ERROR—Unable to find reference:249). Other powerful synchronizing cues, or zeitgebers, include food consumption and social interaction (Stephan, 2002; Mistlberger and Skene, 2004). Subordinate clocks, or oscillators, also exist throughout the body in tissues such as brain, heart, lungs, liver and endocrine glands (Schibler et al., 2015). By receiving time-of-day information from the SCN via synaptic and diffusible signals, these subordinate clocks coordinate the timing of rhythmic activities throughout the body to the external environment (Mohawk et al., 2012; Dibner and Schibler, 2015).

Although zeitgebers are vital for keeping the body’s network of oscillators in time with the 24-h day, they are not sufficient in and of themselves for circadian rhythms to occur. Rather, true clock-controlled functions persist even in the absence of environmental cues (known as “free running” rhythms), and will typically exhibit a period that deviates slightly from 24 h. Furthermore, certain zeitgebers such as daylight may be rendered ineffective if the SCN is destroyed. Among the criteria used to establish that a physiological process or behavior is truly under control of an endogenous clock, it must persist under constant (i.e., zeitgeber-free) conditions, and be able to synchronize anew (or re-entrain) to the re-introduction of an appropriate zeitgeber.

At a molecular level, biological circadian clocks are driven by a core group of genes that regulate their own transcription and translation over 24 h via a series of interacting negative feedback loops (Figure 1; for reviews, see Huang et al., 2011; Mohawk et al., 2012). “Clock” genes regulate their own levels of expression in a predictable cycle that completes in approximately 24 h. Beyond their self-regulation of expression across the day, clock genes play a vital role as transcription factors and control the timing of expression of a wide variety of other genes (referred to as “clock-controlled genes, CCGs”).

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. In mammals, the circadian molecular clock comprises a group of genes (“clock genes”) that regulate their own transcription and translation in a cycle that takes approximately 24 h to complete. This regulation is carried out through a series of interlocking negative feedback loops: the transcription factors BMAL1 and CLOCK heterodimerize and promote the expression of the Period (Per1/Per2) and Cryptochrome (Cry1/Cry2) genes, the nuclear receptors retinoid-related orphan receptor (RORα) and REV-ERBα, and a number of downstream genes referred to as clock-controlled genes (CCGs) including monoamine oxidase A (mao-a), excitatory amino acid transporter (eaat) and estrogen receptor beta (ERβ). In turn, the protein products of the Per and Cry genes dimerize and inhibit the transcriptional activity of CLOCK-BMAL1. The precise timing of this process is regulated by several kinases, such as casein kinase 1 epsilon/delta (CK1ε/δ), which regulate the post-transcriptional activity of PER-CRY dimers. RORα and REV-ERBα also regulate the transcription of BMAL1, whereby RORα promotes its expression, whereas REV-ERBα inhibits it (Huang et al., 2011; Mohawk et al., 2012). Sirtuin-1 (sirt1) is believed to regulate this feedback loop at several levels including through promoting RORα-mediated amplification of bmal expression (Asher et al., 2008) and acetylation of bmal (Nakahata et al., 2008). Figure adapted from Hood and Amir (2017) under a Creative Commons license.

Rhythms that complete over time periods exceeding 24 h, such as monthly, seasonally, or annually, are known collectively as infradian rhythms. Familiar examples of such rhythms include the menstrual cycle, seasonal breeding and migration behaviors. Although the circadian system is implicated in the expression of infradian rhythms (Oster et al., 2002), the precise mechanisms that regulate these long oscillations remain less clear compared to those underpinning circadian rhythms. A variety of environmental cues regulate the timing of infradian rhythms, depending on the specific rhythm and species under consideration. For example, day length (or photoperiod), ambient temperature and food availability play important roles in the regulation of seasonal breeding rhythms in mammalian and non-mammalian species (for a review, see Paul et al., 2008). These cues exert an impact at a cellular level by initiating changes in melatonin release from the pineal gland, and regulating other endocrine factors such as pituitary hormone signaling (e.g., thyroid, prolactin) and hypothalamic peptides. In turn, these factors ultimately trigger downstream physiological and behavioral changes over time, likely through epigenetic mechanisms (Dawson et al., 2001; Lincoln et al., 2006; Stevenson and Prendergast, 2015; Lynch et al., 2017). In contrast to the presence of a master circadian clock in the SCN, no single tissue appears to house a master infradian timekeeper: rather, evidence to date suggests that several tissues are involved, and that different infradian rhythms engage different networks of tissues (Paul et al., 2008).

Rhythms of Anger and Aggressive Behavior

Infradian Cycles

Studies of non-human species provide the strongest evidence for seasonal rhythmicity in the expression of certain types of anger and aggression (for a visual summary of these rhythms, see Figure 2). Aggressive behaviors in many different species of mammals, birds, reptiles, fish and insects exhibit predictable peaks and valleys across the year, and the timing of these behavioral patterns typically exhibits a stable phase relationship with the expression of other seasonal behaviors, such as mating, territory selection, or challenges to social hierarchies (Wilson and Boelkins, 1970; Michael and Zumpe, 1978; Ruby, 1978; Klukowski and Nelson, 1998; Soma, 2006; Perrone et al., 2009; Muschett et al., 2017). For example, some species of non-human primates and ungulates that breed seasonally display more aggression towards others within the social group and sustain more physical injuries from intra-group member attacks (males vs. male) during the few weeks before and at the beginning of sexual partnering. This seasonal rise in aggression has been observed in animals living in laboratory conditions as well as in the wild (Wilson and Boelkins, 1970; Blank et al., 2015). Seasonality in aggressive behaviors is also well-documented in many species of birds and rodents, although the phase relationship of an aggression rhythm with other seasonally fluctuating behaviors such as mating varies. In contrast to the coincident rise of aggression with mating in primates, several species of hamsters and beach mice exhibit more vigorous attack responses towards territory intruders when reproductive activity is low during short photoperiods (Jasnow et al., 2000; Trainor et al., 2007).

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Overview of aggressive behaviors that have been observed to exhibit either seasonal (infradian) or daily (circadian) rhythmicity in humans or non-human species. In terms of infradian rhythms, seasonal changes in daylight length may trigger a variety of hormonal changes that alter the activity of brain structures implicated in aggression such as the ventromedial hypothalamus (VMH). For circadian rhythms, the genetic clockwork that underlies daily cycles of behavior regulates a variety of genes (CCGs, e.g., mao-a; eaat; serotonin receptors including 5htb). In turn, these genes influence the activity of neurotransmitter systems within brain networks involving structures such as the amygdala and striatum. These changes in activation patterns may increase the probability of expressing anger and hostility.

The physiological mechanisms that drive seasonal changes in aggression remain only partially understood; however, substantial research in mammals and birds species has identified complex relationships between photoperiod, thyroid hormone levels, and sex hormone levels. In rodents and songbirds, sex hormones play a fundamental role in the regulation of aggression (Ogawa et al., 2000; Sato et al., 2004; Sperry et al., 2010), yet the impact of androgens and estrogens on aggressive behaviors does not remain constant across the seasons. For example, in rodents, estrogens promote aggressive responses to territory intruders when animals are housed in short photoperiod conditions (i.e., winter-like, non-breeding season), but diminish aggression under long photoperiod conditions (Trainor et al., 2007; Laredo et al., 2013). In song sparrows, the role of androgens in modulating aggression also exhibits a seasonal shift, whereby circulating levels of luteinizing hormone and testosterone are necessary for territorial defense behaviors during long photoperiod conditions (the breeding season; Wingfield, 1994, 2012; Sato et al., 2004). Outside of the breeding season, however, displays of territorial aggression are not dependent on these hormones. The triggering of these seasonal differences in behavior appears to rely in part on a photoperiod-mediated endocrine cascade including melatonin and thyroid hormones that drive seasonal fluctuations in sex hormones, although there are significant inter-species differences in this process. For example, the daily period of melatonin release is an important intermediary driver between daylight length, hormone profile and seasonal aggression in rodents (Ono et al., 2008; Rendon et al., 2015), whereas deep brain photoreceptors directly mediate the impact of photoperiod changes, hypothalamic and pituitary hormones, and behavior in songbird species (Nakao et al., 2008; Mukai et al., 2009). Significant work remains to be done to further elucidate these mechanisms. Furthermore, much remains unknown regarding the mechanisms by which non-photoperiod–based seasonal zeitgebers, such as food availability, influence circannual cycles of aggressive behavior (Bailey et al., 2016).

In humans, epidemiological evidence indicates that rates of physically aggressive crime fluctuate in phase with seasonal changes in temperature and photoperiod. Analysis of violent crime statistics suggest that events involving personal physical attack (simple and aggravated assault, sexual assault, intimate partner violence) are more likely to occur during the summer season and less likely during the winter, whereas violent crimes not involving direct physical contact (e.g., robbery) do not exhibit seasonal trends (Michael and Zumpe, 1983, 1986; Lauritsen and White, 2014). Other multinational studies of criminally aggressive behavior suggest a similar pattern, whereby rates of physically violent crime (assault, sexual assault) increase during the geographic summer season and decline during the winter (e.g., an increase during July and August in northern nations such as Denmark, and during December and January in southern nations such as Australia); in contrast, non-physical, or property-based crime such as theft showed no seasonal variation (Schreiber et al., 1997). Perhaps surprisingly, however, these studies have not identified similar seasonal cycles in murder rates. Furthermore, global rates of suicide (conceptualized by some as a self-aggressing behavior) also appear to follow a seasonal rhythm, with completed suicides using either violent (e.g., use of firearms, drowning, hanging) or non-violent (e.g., overdose) means increasing slightly in the spring compared to other times of year (Hakko et al., 1998; Woo et al., 2012).

Seasonal trends in physically violent criminal acts have led some to propose a causal relationship between aggressive behavior, temperature and photoperiod (Anderson et al., 2000; Hsiang et al., 2013; discussed in Michael and Zumpe, 1983). Such proposals have not gone without challenge, and alternative interpretations of the relationship include the increased likelihood of social interaction during milder temperatures as compared to periods of cold or extreme heat (Rotton and Cohn, 1999). To date, there is no evidence to suggest that endogenous timekeeping mechanisms are responsible for seasonal changes in violent crime; for example, we are not aware of any demonstration that these patterns free-run in the absence of these environmental cues. Furthermore, there is limited evidence for seasonal variation in human hormone levels or receptor function that is comparable to the mechanisms in non-human species discussed above (Huhtaniemi et al., 1982; Smith et al., 2013).

Circadian Cycles

Rhythmic patterns in anger and aggressive behavior also have been documented on a circadian time scale (Figure 2). In humans, some evidence suggests that an individual’s chronotype (i.e., if one is a morning person or an evening person) is associated with expressions of anger and hostility. Specifically, young to middle-aged adults identifying as evening types tend to score higher on self-report scales of impulsivity, state and trait expressions of anger, and irritability (Park et al., 2015; Hwang et al., 2016; however, see Chrobak et al., 2017), and children and adolescents who are evening types are more likely to be rated by parents or teachers as displaying rule-breaking and externalizing behaviors such as conflict with others, lying, screaming, or swearing compared with individuals who identify as being morning types (reviewed in Schlarb et al., 2014). Interesting experiments also suggest that chronotype may interact with time of day to influence the likelihood of displaying socially transgressive behavior such as cheating: for example, young adults are more likely to lie about their success on monetarily rewarded tasks, like self-reported puzzle solving or dice throwing scores, if they are tested at a time of day that does not coincide with their chronotype (i.e., if morning-type people are tested in the late evening; Gunia et al., 2014; Kouchaki and Smith, 2014; Ingram et al., 2016).

Additionally, there is some evidence for a daily pattern in physically aggressive or agitated motor behaviors and verbal outbursts in individuals suffering from dementia-related disorders such as Alzheimer’s disease. An increase in these behaviors in the late afternoon and early evening has been described as “sundowning” (reviewed in Bachman and Rabins, 2006). Although it is debated in the literature whether sundowning is a real behavioral phenomenon or is instead a methodological artifact (e.g., due to reporting bias in caregivers; Yesavage et al., 2003), some studies indicate that an increase in late-afternoon agitation is likely not attributable to sleep disturbances, which are common in this population (Volicer et al., 2001).

The foregoing examples of daily fluctuations in aggressive behaviors are suggestive of behavioral rhythms; however, these patterns have not yet been shown to meet the criteria of being controlled by an endogenous timekeeper. Nevertheless, several interesting hypotheses could explain the physiological mechanisms by which the circadian system could regulate 24-h susceptibility to anger and aggression. Studies of both human and non-human species indicate that there is a significant genetic component to aggression and anger (for reviews see Takahashi and Miczek, 2014; Waltes et al., 2016), and some of the candidate genes identified to date appear to be clock controlled (Duffield, 2003). For example, individual differences in the activity of several monoamine neurotransmitter systems such as dopamine (DA), norepinephrine (NE) and serotonin (5-HT) are clearly associated with the likelihood of aggressive behaviors in both human and non-human species (Figure 1; Marino et al., 2005; Miczek et al., 2007; Alia-Klein et al., 2008), and some regulatory elements of each of these systems such as catalytic and metabolic enzymes, and receptor proteins are indeed clock controlled (Aston-Jones et al., 2001; Ueda et al., 2002; Weber et al., 2004; Malek et al., 2005). The metabolizing enzyme monoamine oxidase A (MAO-A), which degrades DA, NE and 5-HT, provides a compelling illustration of this. Brain levels of MAO-A are inversely associated with self-reported trait anger in humans (Alia-Klein et al., 2008), and pharmacological blockade of MAO-A activity in rodents increases aggressive responses to intruders (Shih, 2004). The clock proteins BMAL and PER2 positively regulate mao-a gene expression in regions of the rodent brain including the striatum and ventral tegmental area, and daily fluctuations in levels of MAO-A in these tissues could presumably influence the likelihood of responding in a more or less aggressive fashion to events happening at different times of day (Hampp et al., 2008). A role for brain glutamatergic and gamma aminobutyric acid (GABA) signaling in aggression has also been demonstrated in non-human species. PER2 positively regulates the expression of the excitatory amino acid transporter (eaat) in astrocytes, which contributes to the re-uptake of glutamate from the extracellular space (Abarca et al., 2002; Spanagel et al., 2005; Hampp and Albrecht, 2008; Takahashi and Miczek, 2014).

Clock-controlled regulation of these genes may be particularly important for influencing the activity of brain structures known to mediate the expression of aggression and hostility in mammals. For example, activity in the amygdala is closely linked with aggressive behavior and trait anger, and blunted serotonin activity in this region is associated with elevated aggression and impulsivity in both non-human species and humans (Rosell and Siever, 2015; Suzuki and Lucas, 2015; da Cunha-Bang et al., 2018). Notably, circadian oscillators have been identified within the central and basolateral nuclei of the amygdala (Lamont et al., 2005), and the timing of these oscillators can be shifted by events such as acute glucocorticoid hormone release and exposure to psychological stressors (Segall and Amir, 2010; Al-Safadi et al., 2015). Such events are also known to increase displays of reactive aggression (Mikics et al., 2007).

The genetic circadian clock also plays an important role in mediating the activity of signaling systems such as sex hormones in the ventromedial hypothalamus (VMH), a region strongly implicated in the expression of aggressive behaviors (Cai et al., 2008; Falkner and Lin, 2014). A number of studies demonstrate that changes in the expression of sex hormone receptors in the VMH and in the firing rates of neurons in this region contribute to the display of aggressive behaviors by rodents and songbirds (Spiteri et al., 2010; Lee et al., 2014; Falkner et al., 2016; Wacker et al., 2016). Evidence suggests that the VMH houses a circadian oscillator that is under control of signals from the SCN master clock (Inouye, 1983; Egawa et al., 1993; Ono et al., 1987).

More recent evidence has implicated the role of the nicotinamide adenine dinucleotide-dependent sirtuin proteins (SIRT)—specifically SIRT1—in both the regulation of the genetic circadian clock and the risk for diagnosis of antisocial personality disorder in humans (Chang et al., 2017). SIRT1 regulates the expression of bmal1 and per2 through deacetylation (Asher et al., 2008; Nakahata et al., 2008), and overexpression of sirt1 in rodents leads to elevated levels of bmal1 and per2, and shortens the period of the circadian clocks in the brain controlling locomotor activity (Chang and Guarente, 2013). In a sample of young men, some of whom were juvenile offenders and had received a diagnosis of antisocial personality disorder, a particular single nucleotide polymorphism (SNP) in the sirt1 gene was associated with a lower risk of antisocial personality disorder diagnosis, whereas a second SNP was found to be more frequent among youth who had received a diagnosis. Although these associations were modest, these observations, when combined with additional evidence linking SIRT1 to the development of midbrain dopaminergic activity, raise interesting questions regarding a link between SIRT1, the circadian clock, monoamine neurotransmitter systems and aggressive behaviors (Lee et al., 2009; Kishi et al., 2011).

Individual genetic differences leading to chronotype may also underlie the association of daily fluctuations in anger and aggression and the circadian timekeeping system. Recent genome-wide association studies in humans have identified multiple loci associated with a morningness type, and many of these loci are near clock genes or genes implicated in the phototransduction process mediating the transfer of daylight information to neurons in the SCN. Interestingly, loci were also identified near genes implicated in the regulation of serotonin activity (5htr6) as well as GABAergic activity in brain (plcl1; nol4), suggesting that the genetic profile that influences morningness or eveningness preference could also involve differences in the activation or sensitivity of these neurotransmitter systems (Hu et al., 2016; Jones et al., 2016). At a behavioral level, chronotype influences changes in cognitive alertness across the day, whereby performances on tests of memory function, reaction time and decision making are worse if one is tested at a time of day that does not align with self-reported morningness/eveningness preference or phase of the genetic circadian clock (May, 1999; Ingram et al., 2016). This worsening of cognitive performance outside of one’s self-identified “optimal” time may increase the likelihood of poorer decision making and impulsiveness, and in turn predispose the expression of anger or hostility in the face of challenging or frustrating circumstances.

Disruption of the Circadian System and Aggression

In addition to the forgoing evidence suggesting a role for biological clocks in regulating anger and aggressive behaviors, a significant body of research suggests that disruptions of normal biological rhythms also influence these behaviors. The exact nature of this relation remains unclear, as much of the evidence collected on this topic to date in human populations is correlational. As such, it is an open question as to whether one particular direction of relationship is more influential than the other—that is, whether disruptions of rhythmic behaviors promote aggression, or whether heightened arousal and anger disrupt biological clocks, or both (Kamphuis et al., 2012). Below, we examine the evidence that disruptions of circadian rhythms such as the sleep/wake cycle do in fact modulate the expression of anger and hostility in several species. The inverse relation—whether intense or chronic expressions of anger and aggression can impact the functioning of the circadian system itself—will be discussed in the final section of this review.

A common example of a perturbation of the circadian system is the disruption of the normal sleep/wake cycle. Although sleep is not exclusively governed by biological clocks, even short-term periods of sleep deprivation can negatively affect a number of other physiological and behavioral rhythms, and transiently alter the genetic clock in a variety of tissues (for review, see Archer and Oster, 2015; Cedernaes et al., 2015; Gil-Lozano et al., 2016). A number of studies have explored the link between the disruption of sleep and the expression of aggressive behavior. To date, this literature has revealed a complex relationship, in that the impact of sleep impairment on aggression varies depending on the species and the nature of the aggressive behavior under consideration (for review see Kamphuis et al., 2012). In humans, poor sleep quality and loss of sleep correlate with higher self-reported ratings of irritability and experience of anger, and, in the case of studies of children and adolescents, greater instance of aggressive physical activity and hyperactivity based on observer ratings (Waters et al., 1993; Gregory et al., 2004; Grano et al., 2008; Coulombe et al., 2011). Interventions to improve sleep quality appear to lessen these behaviors in some cases (Haynes et al., 2006; Mitchell and Kelly, 2006). It has been noted that many of the studies investigating the relation between sleep loss and aggression in adults have used self-report measures of irritability or responses to hypothetical social scenarios as a measure of aggression, rather than objective assessments of external behavior. However, an interesting exception to this includes observations of on-the-job behavior in shift workers. For example, a large-scale study of police officers in North America revealed that approximately 40% experienced some form of chronic sleep disorder such as apnea or insomnia, and that affected individuals were more likely to have demonstrated adverse work-related behavior such as displaying uncontrolled anger towards suspects or citizens (Rajaratnam et al., 2011). Studies in animal models, particularly rodents, are consistent with the view that sleep deprivation is associated with an increased likelihood of physically aggressive behaviors (Licklider and Bunch, 1946; Webb, 1962; Hicks et al., 1979).

Complicating this picture of the relation between sleep and aggression are several studies that have failed to demonstrate an impact of sleep disruption on aggression, and still others that suggest sleep deprivation may actually diminish aggressive behaviors. For example, in humans, an acute period of sleep deprivation (33 h) decreased the likelihood in male participants of displaying retaliatory behavior towards an opponent in a computer game (Cote et al., 2013). In Drosophila, a 12-h period of sleep deprivation similarly reduced physically aggressive behaviors towards other males, and following a sleep recovery period these behaviors were restored (Kayser et al., 2015). Given these differences in the literature, it is difficult to determine conclusively how sleep interacts with aggression, although there is clearly some kind of modulatory influence.

Several mechanisms by which sleep disruption might drive the likelihood of aggressive behavior have been proposed. Sleep loss is known to negatively impact both simple and more complex aspects of cognitive performance including executive function skills and inhibitory regulation (as reviewed in Killgore, 2010). Sleep loss also worsens mood state; increases sensitivity to negatively valenced emotional stimuli; impairs the accuracy of judgment of emotion expressed by human faces; and is associated with poorer inhibitory emotion regulation and decreased empathy towards others (Pilcher and Huffcutt, 1996; Yoo et al., 2007; Killgore et al., 2008; Hisler and Krizan, 2017). This pattern of behavioral change has been linked to impaired functioning of prefrontal cortex, and indeed metabolic activity in this brain region has been observed to decrease following a brief period of sleep deprivation (Harrison and Horne, 2000; Drummond and Brown, 2001). Together, a reduction in executive control, emotion regulation, and weakened perception of emotional states in others could create conditions whereby sleep-deprived individuals are at greater risk of displaying poorly controlled emotional reactions in situations of interpersonal conflict.

Mutations of the genetic clock itself in animal models have also been documented to increase the likelihood of aggression and associated behaviors such as hyperactivity and impulsivity. For example, mice with a knockout of the clock gene rev-erb alpha display more aggression towards a territory intruder compared to wild type mice, as well as exhibit greater locomotor activity and exploration behavior (Chung et al., 2014; Jager et al., 2014). The clock delta 19 mutant mouse, which lacks a CLOCK protein capable of transcription regulation, also exhibits hyperactivity and impulsivity (Roybal et al., 2007; Coque et al., 2011). Associated with the behavioral features of both of these models appears to be the dysregulation of midbrain DA systems, whereby the corresponding gene mutation has been found to create a hyperdopaminergic state through the disinhibition of DA synthesis in midbrain cell groups (in the case of the rev-erb alpha KO model) or hyperexcitability of midbrain DA neuron firing (in the case of the clock delta 19 mutant). Correspondingly, manipulations that diminish this hyperdopaminergic activity appear to attenuate these aggressive phenotypes. Catecholamine dysregulation also appears to be implicated in the effects of sleep deprivation on aggression in drosophila, whereby the administration of an octopamine agonist in sleep deprived flies restores the display of physically aggressive behaviors towards another male (octopamine in insects is akin to NE in mammals; Kayser et al., 2015). Taken together, these findings suggest that the dysregulation of midbrain dopaminergic systems by an atypically functioning clock may predispose aggressive and hyperactive behaviors in these models.

Impact of Anger and Aggression on Endogenous Clocks

Because the circadian network is a physiological system that is exquisitely sensitive to feedback signals arising from both within the body and outside it, it is perhaps not surprising that intense emotional states themselves have been found to influence the pattern of several rhythms acutely. For example, during the night after an episode of work-related or interpersonal conflict, individuals report poorer sleep quality and more sleep disruptions. The strength of this effect appears to be mediated by the extent to which individuals exhibit cynical hostility (a personality construct characterized by negative attitudes towards others, mistrust and defensiveness) and their tendency to ruminate on distressing events (Brissette and Cohen, 2002; Radstaak et al., 2014). In chronically stressed individuals, previous day experiences of anger have been found to predict a blunted amplitude of the cortisol rhythm on the following day (Leggett et al., 2015; Oberle et al., 2017; although see Adam et al., 2006). Acute, anger-provoking stressors have also been observed to blunt the diurnal rhythm of reactive oxygen species production, often used as a marker of immune system activity, in young adults completing a computerized visual judgment task (Atanackovic et al., 2003). Additional evidence suggests that personality disposition towards anger and hostility may perturb biological rhythms over longer time scales. Higher trait anger is predictive of greater sleep fragmentation, poorer self-reported sleep quality, and increased risk for sleep disorders such as insomnia (Waters et al., 1993; Grano et al., 2008; Taylor et al., 2013; Hisler and Krizan, 2017).

The experience of anger is often characterized by physiological symptoms of intense autonomic nervous system activation including transient elevations in blood pressure, heart rate, and sympathetic neurochemical tone, and changes in each of these activities have been found to influence the timing of biological rhythms and clock gene expression in several respects (for a review, see Buijs et al., 2016; Sasaki et al., 2016). For example, substantial research in cell cultures and animal models has demonstrated convincingly that acute surges in corticosteroids can act as a zeitgeber for tissue-specific clocks such as within the limbic system of the brain, liver, kidney and heart (Balsalobre et al., 2000; Al-Safadi et al., 2015). As such, it is possible that frequent and/or intense spikes in sympathetic activation in highly emotional or aggressive individuals may create “noisy” internal feedback conditions that impair the daily entrainment of biological clocks to other important signals.

It should be noted that the effect sizes reported in several of the studies of humans discussed above are modest; nevertheless, even small changes in rhythms such as sleep, when accrued across time, may exert a negative effect on physical health and cognitive performance (Vgontzas et al., 2004; Wang et al., 2016). Given the delicate relationship between biological rhythms and vital physiological functions, anger-induced disruptions of the biological clock may be a key mechanism underlying the increased risk of high trait-anger individuals to physical illness such as cardiovascular disease and inflammatory disorders (Gouin et al., 2008; Haukkala et al., 2010; Boylan and Ryff, 2013). Individuals predisposed towards trait anger or aggression exhibit larger, more prolonged changes in heart rate, galvanic skin responses and blood pressure to stressful events (for review, see Waters et al., 1993; Smith et al., 2004), and this exaggerated reactivity may underlie the attenuated nighttime declines in heart rate and blood pressure observed in these individuals (Linden et al., 2008; Beatty and Matthews, 2009; Pavek and Taube, 2009). Given the evidence that chronic variability in blood pressure and heart rate are risk factors for cardiovascular disease (Kikuya et al., 2000), it is possible that anger-induced perturbation in the biological rhythms of vascular activity may be an important contributor towards exacerbating this risk. If so, the circadian system could represent an important target for therapeutic interventions intended to reduce the risk of illness in individuals who have poorly controlled anger or hostility.

Conclusion

Taken together, the findings reviewed here demonstrate a meaningful role for biological clocks in anger and aggression. Seasonal changes in the behaviors of several non-human species provide some of the most convincing demonstrations of predictable cycles in the expression of certain types of aggression. It is less clear that true, clock-controlled seasonal cycles exist in human aggression; however, stronger evidence suggests that circadian patterns are present, and that clock-related individual differences such as chronotype are associated with the propensity for anger and aggression. There are a number of hypothesized mechanisms that could account for how the biological timekeeping system at a genetic level influences the cellular and physiological factors that regulate aggressive behaviors, and further research into the role of clock genes in controlling catecholaminergic midbrain systems may be particularly useful in understanding these mechanisms more deeply. Further studies at this level of analysis may also provide more definitive insight into how disruptions of endogenous clocks increase the likelihood of hostile behaviors. Advancements in our understanding of the circadian system’s role in aggression and anger may perhaps be most valuable for improving our ability to prevent and treat the health complications that individuals high in trait anger are at higher risk of developing.

Author Contributions

SH and SA wrote the manuscript.

Conflict of Interest Statement

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Acknowledgments

This work was supported by the Natural Sciences and Engineering Research Council of Canada, les Fonds de la Recherché en Santé Québec, and the Canadian Institutes for Health Research (grant no. #MOP142458) to SA.

References

Abarca, C., Albrecht, U., and Spanagel, R. (2002). Cocaine sensitization and reward are under the influence of circadian genes and rhythm. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U S A 99, 9026–9030. doi: 10.1073/pnas.142039099

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Adam, E. K., Hawkley, L. C., Kudielka, B. M., and Cacioppo, J. T. (2006). Day-to-day dynamics of experience—cortisol associations in a population-based sample of older adults. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U S A 103, 17058–17063. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0605053103

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Alia-Klein, N., Goldstein, R. Z., Kriplani, A., Logan, J., Tomasi, D., Williams, B., et al. (2008). Brain monoamine oxidase A activity predicts trait aggression. J. Neurosci. 28, 5099–5104. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0925-08.2008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Al-Safadi, S., Branchaud, M., Rutherford, S., and Amir, S. (2015). Glucocorticoids and stress-induced changes in the expression of PERIOD1 in the rat forebrain. PLoS One 10:e0130085. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0130085

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Anderson, C. A., Anderson, K. B., Dorr, N., DeNeve, K. M., and Flanagan, M. (2000). Temperature and aggression. Adv. Exp. Soc. Psychol. 32, 63–133. doi: 10.1016/S0065-2601(00)80004-0

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Archer, S. N., and Oster, H. (2015). How sleep and wakefulness influence circadian rhythmicity: effects of insufficient and mistimed sleep on the animal and human transcriptome. J. Sleep Res. 24, 476–493. doi: 10.1111/jsr.12307

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Asher, G., Gatfield, D., Stratmann, M., Reinke, H., Dibner, C., Kreppel, F., et al. (2008). SIRT1 regulates circadian clock gene expression through PER2 deacetylation. Cell 134, 317–328. doi: 10.1016/j.cell.2008.06.050

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Aston-Jones, G., Chen, S., Zhu, Y., and Oshinsky, M. L. (2001). A neural circuit for circadian regulation of arousal. Nat. Neurosci. 4, 732–738. doi: 10.1038/89522

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Atanackovic, D., Schulze, J., Kröger, H., Brunner-Weinzierl, M. C., and Deter, H. C. (2003). Acute psychological stress induces a prolonged suppression of the production of reactive oxygen species by phagocytes. J. Neuroimmunol. 142, 159–165. doi: 10.1016/s0165-5728(03)00267-4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bachman, D., and Rabins, P. (2006). “Sundowning” and other temporally associated agitation states in dementia patients. Annu. Rev. Med. 57, 499–511. doi: 10.1146/annurev.med.57.071604.141451

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bailey, A. M., Rendon, N. M., O’Malley, K. J., and Demas, G. E. (2016). Food as a supplementary cue triggers seasonal changes in aggression, but not reproduction, in siberian hamsters. Physiol. Behav. 167, 298–308. doi: 10.1016/j.physbeh.2016.09.023

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Balsalobre, A., Brown, S. A., Marcacci, L., Tronche, F., Kellendonk, C., Reichardt, H. M., et al. (2000). Resetting of circadian time in peripheral tissues by glucocorticoid signaling. Science 289, 2344–2347. doi: 10.1126/science.289.5488.2344

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Beatty, D. L., and Matthews, K. A. (2009). Unfair treatment and trait anger in relation to nighttime ambulatory blood pressure in african american and white adolescents. Psychosom. Med. 71, 813–820. doi: 10.1097/PSY.0b013e3181b3b6f8

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Blank, D. A., Ruckstuhl, K., and Yang, W. (2015). Seasonal dynamics of agonistic displays in territorial and non-territorial males of goitered gazelle. Zoology 118, 63–68. doi: 10.1016/j.zool.2014.08.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Boylan, J. M., and Ryff, C. D. (2013). Varieties of anger and the inverse link between education and inflammation: toward an integrative framework. Psychosom. Med. 75, 566–574. doi: 10.1097/PSY.0b013e31829683bd

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Brissette, I., and Cohen, S. (2002). The contribution of individual differences in hostility to the associations between daily interpersonal conflict, affect, and sleep. Pers. Soc. Psychol. Bull. 28, 1265–1274. doi: 10.1177/01461672022812011

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Buijs, F. N., León-Mercado, L., Guzmán-Ruiz, M., Guerrero-Vargas, N. N., Romo-Nava, F., and Buijs, R. M. (2016). The circadian system: a regulatory feedback network of periphery and brain. Physiology 31, 170–181. doi: 10.1152/physiol.00037.2015

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cai, W., Rambaud, J., Teboul, M., Masse, I., Benoit, G., Gustafsson, J. A., et al. (2008). Expression levels of estrogen receptor beta are modulated by components of the molecular clock. Mol. Cell. Biol. 28, 784–793. doi: 10.1128/mcb.00233-07

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cedernaes, J., Osler, M. E., Voisin, S., Broman, J. E., Vogel, H., Dickson, S. L., et al. (2015). Acute sleep loss induces tissue-specific epigenetic and transcriptional alterations to circadian clock genes in men. J. Clin. Endocrinol. Metab. 100, E1255–E1261. doi: 10.1210/JC.2015-2284

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chang, H. C., and Guarente, L. (2013). SIRT1 mediates central circadian control in the SCN by a mechanism that decays with aging. Cell 153, 1448–1460. doi: 10.1016/j.cell.2013.05.027

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chang, H., Yan, Q., Tang, J., Huang, J., Zhang, Y., Ma, Y., et al. (2017). Possible association between SIRT1 single nucleotide polymorphisms and predisposition to antisocial personality traits in chinese adolescents. Sci. Rep. 7:1099. doi: 10.1038/s41598-017-01208-2

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chrobak, A. A., Tereszko, A., Dembinska-Krajewska, D., Arciszewska, A., Siwek, M., Dudek, D., et al. (2017). Morningness-eveningness and affective temperaments assessed by the temperament evaluation of memphis, pisa and san diego-autoquestionnaire (TEMPS-A). Chronobiol. Int. 34, 57–65. doi: 10.1080/07420528.2016.1236806

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chung, S., Lee, E. J., Yun, S., Choe, H. K., Park, S. B., Son, H. J., et al. (2014). Impact of circadian nuclear receptor REV-ERBα on midbrain dopamine production and mood regulation. Cell 157, 858–868. doi: 10.1016/j.cell.2014.03.039

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Coque, L., Mukherjee, S., Cao, J. L., Spencer, S., Marvin, M., Falcon, E., et al. (2011). Specific role of VTA dopamine neuronal firing rates and morphology in the reversal of anxiety-related, but not depression-related behavior in the ClockDelta19 mouse model of mania. Neuropsychopharmacology 36, 1478–1488. doi: 10.1038/npp.2011.33

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cote, K. A., McCormick, C. M., Geniole, S. N., Renn, R. P., and MacAulay, S. D. (2013). Sleep deprivation lowers reactive aggression and testosterone in men. Biol. Psychol. 92, 249–256. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2012.09.011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Coulombe, J. A., Reid, G. J., Boyle, M. H., and Racine, Y. (2011). Sleep problems, tiredness, and psychological symptoms among healthy adolescents. J. Pediatr. Psychol. 36, 25–35. doi: 10.1093/jpepsy/jsq028

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

da Cunha-Bang, S., Fisher, P. M., Hjordt, L. V., Perfalk, E., Beliveau, V., Holst, K., et al. (2018). Men with high serotonin 1B receptor binding respond to provocations with heightened amygdala reactivity. Neuroimage 166, 79–85. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2017.10.032

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dawson, A., King, V. M., Bentley, G. E., and Ball, G. F. (2001). Photoperiodic control of seasonality in birds. J. Biol. Rhythms 16, 365–380. doi: 10.1177/074873001129002079

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dibner, C., and Schibler, U. (2015). Circadian timing of metabolism in animal models and humans. J. Intern. Med. 277, 513–527. doi: 10.1111/joim.12347

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Drummond, S. P., and Brown, G. G. (2001). The effects of total sleep deprivation on cerebral responses to cognitive performance. Neuropsychopharmacology 25, S68–S73. doi: 10.1016/s0893-133x(01)00325-6

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Duffield, G. E. (2003). DNA microarray analyses of circadian timing: the genomic basis of biological time. J. Neuroendocrinol. 15, 991–1002. doi: 10.1046/j.1365-2826.2003.01082.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Egawa, M., Inoue, S., Satoh, S., Takamura, Y., Nagai, K., and Nakagawa, H. (1993). Acute and chronic effects of VMH lesions on circadian rhythms in food intake and metabolites. Brain Res. Bull. 31, 293–299. doi: 10.1016/0361-9230(93)90220-6

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Falkner, A. L., Grosenick, L., Davidson, T. J., Deisseroth, K., and Lin, D. (2016). Hypothalamic control of male aggression-seeking behavior. Nat. Neurosci. 19, 596–604. doi: 10.1038/nn.4264

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Falkner, A. L., and Lin, D. (2014). Recent advances in understanding the role of the hypothalamic circuit during aggression. Front. Syst. Neurosci. 8:168. doi: 10.3389/fnsys.2014.00168

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fung, A. L., Raine, A., and Gao, Y. (2009). Cross-cultural generalizability of the reactive-proactive aggression questionnaire (RPQ). J. Pers. Assess. 91, 473–479. doi: 10.1080/00223890903088420

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gil-Lozano, M., Hunter, P. M., Behan, L. A., Gladanac, B., Casper, R. F., and Brubaker, P. L. (2016). Short-term sleep deprivation with nocturnal light exposure alters time-dependent glucagon-like peptide-1 and insulin secretion in male volunteers. Am. J. Physiol. Endocrinol. Metab. 310, E41–E50. doi: 10.1152/ajpendo.00298.2015

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gouin, J. P., Kiecolt-Glaser, J. K., Malarkey, W. B., and Glaser, R. (2008). The influence of anger expression on wound healing. Brain Behav. Immun. 22, 699–708. doi: 10.1016/j.bbi.2007.10.013

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Grano, N., Vahtera, J., Virtanen, M., Keltikangas-Jarvinen, L., and Kivimaki, M. (2008). Association of hostility with sleep duration and sleep disturbances in an employee population. Int. J. Behav. Med. 15, 73–80. doi: 10.1080/10705500801929510

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Grant, M. (2000). On The Humors. New York, NY: Routledge.

Green, M. J., and Phillips, M. L. (2004). Social threat perception and the evolution of paranoia. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 28, 333–342. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2004.03.006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gregory, A. M., Eley, T. C., O’Connor, T. G., and Plomin, R. (2004). Etiologies of associations between childhood sleep and behavioral problems in a large twin sample. J. Am. Acad. Child Adolesc. Psychiatry 43, 744–751. doi: 10.1097/01.chi/0000122798.47863.a5

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gunia, B. C., Barnes, C. M., and Sah, S. (2014). The morality of larks and owls: unethical behavior depends on chronotype as well as time of day. Psychol. Sci. 25, 2272–2274. doi: 10.1177/0956797614541989

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hakko, H., Räsänen, P., and Tiihonen, J. (1998). Seasonal variation in suicide occurrence in finland. Acta Psychiatr. Scand. 98, 92–97. doi: 10.1111/j.1600-0447.1998.tb10048.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hampp, G., and Albrecht, U. (2008). The circadian clock and mood-related behavior. Commun. Integr. Biol. 1, 1–3. doi: 10.4161/cib.1.1.6286

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hampp, G., Ripperger, J. A., Houben, T., Schmutz, I., Blex, C., Perreau-Lenz, S., et al. (2008). Regulation of monoamine oxidase A by circadian-clock components implies clock influence on mood. Curr. Biol. 18, 678–683. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2008.04.012

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Harrison, Y., and Horne, J. A. (2000). The impact of sleep deprivation on decision making: a review. J. Exp. Psychol. Appl. 6, 236–249. doi: 10.1037//1076-898x.6.3.236

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Haukkala, A., Konttinen, H., Laatikainen, T., Kawachi, I., and Uutela, A. (2010). Hostility, anger control, and anger expression as predictors of cardiovascular disease. Psychosom. Med. 72, 556–562. doi: 10.1097/PSY.0b013e3181dbab87

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Haynes, P. L., Bootzin, R. R., Smith, L., Cousins, J., Cameron, M., and Stevens, S. (2006). Sleep and aggression in substance-abusing adolescents: results from an integrative behavioral sleep-treatment pilot program. Sleep 29, 512–520.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Hicks, R. A., Moore, J. D., Hayes, C., Phillips, N., and Hawkins, J. (1979). REM sleep deprivation increases aggressiveness in male rats. Physiol. Behav. 22, 1097–1100. doi: 10.1016/0031-9384(79)90263-4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hisler, G., and Krizan, Z. (2017). Sleepiness and behavioral risk-taking: do sleepy people take more or less risk? Behav. Sleep Med. 26, 1–14. doi: 10.1080/15402002.2017.1357122

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hood, S., and Amir, S. (2017). Neurodegeneration and the circadian clock. Front. Aging Neurosci. 9:170. doi: 10.3389/fnagi.2017.00170

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hsiang, S. M., Burke, M., and Miguel, E. (2013). Quantifying the influence of climate on human conflict. Science 341:1235367. doi: 10.1126/science.1235367

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hu, Y., Shmygelska, A., Tran, D., Eriksson, N., Tung, J. Y., and Hinds, D. A. (2016). GWAS of 89,283 individuals identifies genetic variants associated with self-reporting of being a morning person. Nat. Commun. 7:10448. doi: 10.1038/ncomms10448

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Huang, W., Ramsey, K. M., Marcheva, B., and Bass, J. (2011). Circadian rhythms, sleep, and metabolism. J. Clin. Invest. 121, 2133–2141. doi: 10.1172/JCI46043

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hubbard, J. A., Smithmyer, C. M., Ramsden, S. R., Parker, E. H., Flanagan, K. D., Dearing, K. F., et al. (2002). Observational, physiological, and self-report measures of children’s anger: relations to reactive versus proactive aggression. Child Dev. 73, 1101–1118. doi: 10.1111/1467-8624.00460

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Huhtaniemi, I., Martikainen, H., and Tapanainen, J. (1982). Large annual variation in photoperiodicity does not affect testicular endocrine function in man. Acta Endocrinol. (Copenh) 101, 105–107. doi: 10.1530/acta.0.1010105

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hwang, J. Y., Kang, S. G., Gwak, A. R., Park, J., and Lee, Y. J. (2016). The associations of morningness-eveningness with anger and impulsivity in the general population. Chronobiol. Int. 33, 200–209. doi: 10.3109/07420528.2015.1128947

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ingram, K. K., Ay, A., Kwon, S. B., Woods, K., Escobar, S., Gordon, M., et al. (2016). Molecular insights into chronotype and time-of-day effects on decision-making. Sci. Rep. 6:29392. doi: 10.1038/srep29392

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Inouye, S. T. (1983). Does the ventromedial hypothalamic nucleus contain a self-sustained circadian oscillator associated with periodic feedings? Brain Res. 279, 53–63. doi: 10.1016/0006-8993(83)90162-2

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jager, J., O’Brien, W. T., Manlove, J., Krizman, E. N., Fang, B., Gerhart-Hines, Z., et al. (2014). Behavioral changes and dopaminergic dysregulation in mice lacking the nuclear receptor rev-erbalpha. Mol. Endocrinol. 28, 490–498. doi: 10.1210/me.2013-1351

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jasnow, A. M., Huhman, K. L., Bartness, T. J., and Demas, G. E. (2000). Short-day increases in aggression are inversely related to circulating testosterone concentrations in male siberian hamsters (Phodopus sungorus). Horm. Behav. 38, 102–110. doi: 10.1006/hbeh.2000.1604

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jones, S. E., Tyrrell, J., Wood, A. R., Beaumont, R. N., Ruth, K. S., Tuke, M. A., et al. (2016). Genome-wide association analyses in 128,266 individuals identifies new morningness and sleep duration loci. PLoS Genet. 12:e1006125. doi: 10.1371/journal.pgen.1006125

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kamphuis, J., Meerlo, P., Koolhaas, J. M., and Lancel, M. (2012). Poor sleep as a potential causal factor in aggression and violence. Sleep Med. 13, 327–334. doi: 10.1016/j.sleep.2011.12.006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kayser, M. S., Mainwaring, B., Yue, Z., and Sehgal, A. (2015). Sleep deprivation suppresses aggression in Drosophila. Elife 4:e07643. doi: 10.7554/elife.07643

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kempes, M., Matthys, W., de Vries, H., and van Engeland, H. (2005). Reactive and proactive aggression in children–a review of theory, findings and the relevance for child and adolescent psychiatry. Eur. Child Adolesc. Psychiatry 14, 11–19. doi: 10.1007/s00787-005-0432-4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kikuya, M., Hozawa, A., Ohokubo, T., Tsuji, I., Michimata, M., Matsubara, M., et al. (2000). Prognostic significance of blood pressure and heart rate variabilities: the ohasama study. Hypertension 36, 901–906. doi: 10.1161/01.hyp.36.5.901

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Killgore, W. D. (2010). Effects of sleep deprivation on cognition. Prog. Brain Res. 185, 105–129. doi: 10.1016/B978-0-444-53702-7.00007-5

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Killgore, W. D., Kahn-Greene, E. T., Lipizzi, E. L., Newman, R. A., Kamimori, G. H., and Balkin, T. J. (2008). Sleep deprivation reduces perceived emotional intelligence and constructive thinking skills. Sleep Med. 9, 517–526. doi: 10.1016/j.sleep.2007.07.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kishi, T., Fukuo, Y., Kitajima, T., Okochi, T., Yamanouchi, Y., Kinoshita, Y., et al. (2011). SIRT1 gene, schizophrenia and bipolar disorder in the japanese population: an association study. Genes Brain Behav. 10, 257–263. doi: 10.1111/j.1601-183x.2010.00661.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Klukowski, M., and Nelson, C. E. (1998). The challenge hypothesis and seasonal changes in aggression and steroids in male northern fence lizards (Sceloporus undulatus hyacinthinus). Horm. Behav. 33, 197–204. doi: 10.1006/hbeh.1998.1449

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kouchaki, M., and Smith, I. H. (2014). The morning morality effect: the influence of time of day on unethical behavior. Psychol. Sci. 25, 95–102. doi: 10.1177/0956797613498099

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lamont, E. W., Robinson, B., Stewart, J., and Amir, S. (2005). The central and basolateral nuclei of the amygdala exhibit opposite diurnal rhythms of expression of the clock protein Period2. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U S A 102, 4180–4184. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0500901102

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Laredo, S. A., Villalon Landeros, R., Dooley, J. C., Steinman, M. Q., Orr, V., Silva, A. L., et al. (2013). Nongenomic effects of estradiol on aggression under short day photoperiods. Horm. Behav. 64, 557–565. doi: 10.1016/j.yhbeh.2013.06.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lauritsen, J. L., and White, N. (2014). Seasonal Patterns in Criminal Victimization Trends. (No. NCJ245959). Washington, DC: U.S. Department of Justice.

Google Scholar

Lee, J. Y., Ahn, K., Jang, B. G., Park, S. H., Kang, H. J., Heo, J. I., et al. (2009). Nicotinamide reduces dopamine in postnatal hypothalamus and causes dopamine-deficient phenotype. Neurosci. Lett. 461, 163–166. doi: 10.1016/j.neulet.2009.06.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lee, H., Kim, D. W., Remedios, R., Anthony, T. E., Chang, A., Madisen, L., et al. (2014). Scalable control of mounting and attack by Esr1+ neurons in the ventromedial hypothalamus. Nature 509, 627–632. doi: 10.1038/nature13169

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Leggett, A. N., Zarit, S. H., Kim, K., Almeida, D. M., and Klein, L. C. (2015). Depressive mood, anger and daily cortisol of caregivers on high- and low-stress days. J. Gerontol. B Psychol. Sci. Soc. Sci. 70, 820–829. doi: 10.1093/geronb/gbu070

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Licklider, J. C., and Bunch, M. E. (1946). Effects of enforced wakefulness upon the growth and the maze-learning performance of white rats. J. Comp. Psychol. 39, 339–350. doi: 10.1037/h0060623

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lincoln, G. A., Clarke, I. J., Hut, R. A., and Hazlerigg, D. G. (2006). Characterizing a mammalian circannual pacemaker. Science 314, 1941–1944. doi: 10.1126/science.1132009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Linden, W., Klassen, K., and Phillips, M. (2008). Can psychological factors account for a lack of nocturnal blood pressure dipping? Ann. Behav. Med. 36, 253–258. doi: 10.1007/s12160-008-9069-0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lynch, E. W., Coyle, C. S., and Stevenson, T. J. (2017). Photoperiodic and ovarian steroid regulation of histone deacetylase 1, 2, and 3 in siberian hamster (Phodopus sungorus) reproductive tissues. Gen. Comp. Endocrinol. 246, 194–199. doi: 10.1016/j.ygcen.2016.12.008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Malek, Z. S., Dardente, H., Pevet, P., and Raison, S. (2005). Tissue-specific expression of tryptophan hydroxylase mRNAs in the rat midbrain: anatomical evidence and daily profiles. Eur. J. Neurosci. 22, 895–901. doi: 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2005.04264.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Marino, M. D., Bourdélat-Parks, B. N., Cameron Liles, L., and Weinshenker, D. (2005). Genetic reduction of noradrenergic function alters social memory and reduces aggression in mice. Behav. Brain Res. 161, 197–203. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2005.02.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mathias, C. W., Stanford, M. S., Marsh, D. M., Frick, P. J., Moeller, F. G., Swann, A. C., et al. (2007). Characterizing aggressive behavior with the impulsive/premeditated aggression scale among adolescents with conduct disorder. Psychiatry Res. 151, 231–242. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2006.11.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

May, C. P. (1999). Synchrony effects in cognition: the costs and a benefit. Psychon. Bull. Rev. 6, 142–147. doi: 10.3758/bf03210822

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Michael, R. P., and Zumpe, D. (1978). Annual cycles of aggression and plasma testosterone in captive male rhesus monkeys. Psychoneuroendocrinology 3, 217–220. doi: 10.1016/0306-4530(78)90011-2

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Michael, R. P., and Zumpe, D. (1983). Annual rhythms in human violence and sexual aggression in the united states and the role of temperature. Soc. Biol. 30, 263–278. doi: 10.1080/19485565.1983.9988541

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Michael, R. P., and Zumpe, D. (1986). An annual rhythm in the battering of women. Am. J. Psychiatry 143, 637–640. doi: 10.1176/ajp.143.5.637

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Miczek, K. A., de Almeida, R. M., Kravitz, E. A., Rissman, E. F., de Boer, S. F., and Raine, A. (2007). Neurobiology of escalated aggression and violence. J. Neurosci. 27, 11803–11806. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3500-07.2007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mikics, E., Barsy, B., and Haller, J. (2007). The effect glucocorticoids on aggressiveness in established colonies of rats. Psychoneuroendocrinology 32, 160–170. doi: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2006.12.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mistlberger, R. E., and Skene, D. J. (2004). Social influences on mammalian circadian rhythms: animal and human studies. Biol. Rev. Camb. Philos. Soc. 79, 533–556. doi: 10.1017/s1464793103006353

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mitchell, R. B., and Kelly, J. (2006). Behavior, neurocognition and quality-of-life in children with sleep-disordered breathing. Int. J. Pediatr. Otorhinolaryngol. 70, 395–406. doi: 10.1016/j.ijporl.2005.10.020

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mohawk, J. A., Green, C. B., and Takahashi, J. S. (2012). Central and peripheral circadian clocks in mammals. Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 35, 445–462. doi: 10.1146/annurev-neuro-060909-153128

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mukai, M., Replogle, K., Drnevich, J., Wang, G., Wacker, D., Band, M., et al. (2009). Seasonal differences of gene expression profiles in song sparrow (melospiza melodia) hypothalamus in relation to territorial aggression. PLoS One 4:e8182. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0008182

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Muschett, G., Umbers, K. D., and Herberstein, M. E. (2017). Within-season variability of fighting behaviour in an australian alpine grasshopper. PLoS One 12:e0171697. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0171697

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nakahata, Y., Kaluzova, M., Grimaldi, B., Sahar, S., Hirayama, J., Chen, D., et al. (2008). The NAD+-dependent deacetylase SIRT1 modulates CLOCK-mediated chromatin remodeling and circadian control. Cell 134, 329–340. doi: 10.1016/j.cell.2008.07.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nakao, N., Ono, H., Yamamura, T., Anraku, T., Takagi, T., Higashi, K., et al. (2008). Thyrotrophin in the pars tuberalis triggers photoperiodic response. Nature 452, 317–322. doi: 10.1038/nature06738

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Oberle, E., McLachlan, K., Catherine, N. L. A., Brain, U., Schonert-Reichl, K. A., Weinberg, J., et al. (2017). Afternoon cortisol provides a link between self-regulated anger and peer-reported aggression in typically developing children in the school context. Dev. Psychobiol. 59, 688–695. doi: 10.1002/dev.21522

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ogawa, S., Chester, A. E., Hewitt, S. C., Walker, V. R., Gustafsson, J. A., Smithies, O., et al. (2000). Abolition of male sexual behaviors in mice lacking estrogen receptors alpha and beta (alpha beta ERKO). Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U S A 97, 14737–14741. doi: 10.1073/pnas.250473597

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ono, H., Hoshino, Y., Yasuo, S., Watanabe, M., Nakane, Y., Murai, A., et al. (2008). Involvement of thyrotropin in photoperiodic signal transduction in mice. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U S A 105, 18238–18242. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0808952105

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ono, T., Sasaki, K., and Shibata, R. (1987). Diurnal- and behaviour-related activity of ventromedial hypothalamic neurones in freely behaving rats. J. Physiol. 394, 201–220. doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.1987.sp016866

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Oster, H., Maronde, E., and Albrecht, U. (2002). The circadian clock as a molecular calendar. Chronobiol. Int. 19, 507–516. doi: 10.1081/cbi-120004210

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Park, C. I., An, S. K., Kim, H. W., Koh, M. J., Namkoong, K., Kang, J. I., et al. (2015). Relationships between chronotypes and affective temperaments in healthy young adults. J. Affect. Disord. 175, 256–259. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2015.01.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Paul, M. J., Zucker, I., and Schwartz, W. J. (2008). Tracking the seasons: the internal calendars of vertebrates. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. B Biol. Sci. 363, 341–361. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2007.2143

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pavek, K., and Taube, A. (2009). Personality characteristics influencing determinacy of day and night blood pressure and heart rate. Blood Press. 18, 30–35. doi: 10.1080/08037050902812648

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Perrone, R., Macadar, O., and Silva, A. (2009). Social electric signals in freely moving dyads of brachyhypopomus pinnicaudatus. J. Comp. Physiol. A Neuroethol. Sens. Neural Behav. Physiol. 195, 501–514. doi: 10.1007/s00359-009-0427-6

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pilcher, J. J., and Huffcutt, A. I. (1996). Effects of sleep deprivation on performance: a meta-analysis. Sleep 19, 318–326. doi: 10.1093/sleep/19.4.318

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Radstaak, M., Geurts, S. A., Beckers, D. G., Brosschot, J. F., and Kompier, M. A. (2014). Work stressors, perseverative cognition and objective sleep quality: a longitudinal study among dutch helicopter emergency medical service (HEMS) pilots. J. Occup. Health 56, 469–477. doi: 10.1539/joh.14-0118-OA

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rajaratnam, S. M., Barger, L. K., Lockley, S. W., Shea, S. A., Wang, W., Landrigan, C. P., et al. (2011). Sleep disorders, health, and safety in police officers. JAMA 306, 2567–2578. doi: 10.1001/jama.2011.1851

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rendon, N. M., Rudolph, L. M., Sengelaub, D. R., and Demas, G. E. (2015). The agonistic adrenal: melatonin elicits female aggression via regulation of adrenal androgens. Proc. Biol. Sci. 282:20152080. doi: 10.1098/rspb.2015.2080

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rosell, D. R., and Siever, L. J. (2015). The neurobiology of aggression and violence. CNS Spectr. 20, 254–279. doi: 10.1017/S109285291500019X

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rotton, J., and Cohn, E. G. (1999). Errors of commission and omission: comment on anderson anderson’s (1998) “temperature and aggression”. Psychol. Rep. 85, 611–620. doi: 10.2466/pr0.85.6.611-620

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Roybal, K., Theobold, D., Graham, A., DiNieri, J. A., Russo, S. J., Krishnan, V., et al. (2007). Mania-like behavior induced by disruption of CLOCK. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U S A 104, 6406–6411. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0609625104

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ruby, D. E. (1978). Seasonal changes in the territorial behavior of the iguanid lizard sceloperus jarrovi. Copeia 1978, 430–438. doi: 10.2307/1443607

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sasaki, H., Hattori, Y., Ikeda, Y., Kamagata, M., Iwami, S., Yasuda, S., et al. (2016). Forced rather than voluntary exercise entrains peripheral clocks via a corticosterone/noradrenaline increase in PER2::LUC mice. Sci. Rep. 6:27607. doi: 10.1038/srep27607

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sato, T., Matsumoto, T., Kawano, H., Watanabe, T., Uematsu, Y., Sekine, K., et al. (2004). Brain masculinization requires androgen receptor function. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U S A 101, 1673–1678. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0305303101

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schibler, U., Gotic, I., Saini, C., Gos, P., Curie, T., Emmenegger, Y., et al. (2015). Clock-talk: interactions between central and peripheral circadian oscillators in mammals. Cold Spring Harb. Symp. Quant. Biol. 80, 223–232. doi: 10.1101/sqb.2015.80.027490

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schlarb, A. A., Sopp, R., Ambiel, D., and Grunwald, J. (2014). Chronotype-related differences in childhood and adolescent aggression and antisocial behavior–a review of the literature. Chronobiol. Int. 31, 1–16. doi: 10.3109/07420528.2013.829846

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schreiber, G., Avissar, S., Tzahor, Z., Barak-Glantz, I., and Grisaru, N. (1997). Photoperiodicity and annual rhythms of wars and violent crimes. Med. Hypotheses 48, 89–96. doi: 10.1016/s0306-9877(97)90029-3

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Segall, L. A., and Amir, S. (2010). Exogenous corticosterone induces the expression of the clock protein, PERIOD2, in the oval nucleus of the bed nucleus of the stria terminalis and the central nucleus of the amygdala of adrenalectomized and intact rats. J. Mol. Neurosci. 42, 176–182. doi: 10.1007/s12031-010-9375-4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Shih, J. C. (2004). Cloning, after cloning, knock-out mice, and physiological functions of MAO A and B. Neurotoxicology 25, 21–30. doi: 10.1016/s0161-813x(03)00112-8

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Smith, R. P., Coward, R. M., Kovac, J. R., and Lipshultz, L. I. (2013). The evidence for seasonal variations of testosterone in men. Maturitas 74, 208–212. doi: 10.1016/j.maturitas.2012.12.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Smith, T. W., Glazer, K., Ruiz, J. M., and Gallo, L. C. (2004). Hostility, anger, aggressiveness and, coronary heart disease: an interpersonal perspective on personality, emotion and health. J. Pers. 72, 1217–1270. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-6494.2004.00296.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Soma, K. K. (2006). Testosterone and aggression: berthold, birds and beyond. J. Neuroendocrinol. 18, 543–551. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2826.2006.01440.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Spanagel, R., Pendyala, G., Abarca, C., Zghoul, T., Sanchis-Segura, C., Magnone, M. C., et al. (2005). The clock gene Per2 influences the glutamatergic system and modulates alcohol consumption. Nat. Med. 11, 35–42. doi: 10.1038/nm1163

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sperry, T. S., Wacker, D. W., and Wingfield, J. C. (2010). The role of androgen receptors in regulating territorial aggression in male song sparrows. Horm. Behav. 57, 86–95. doi: 10.1016/j.yhbeh.2009.09.015

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Spiteri, T., Musatov, S., Ogawa, S., Ribeiro, A., Pfaff, D. W., and Agmo, A. (2010). The role of the estrogen receptor alpha in the medial amygdala and ventromedial nucleus of the hypothalamus in social recognition, anxiety and aggression. Behav. Brain Res. 210, 211–220. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2010.02.033

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Stephan, F. K. (2002). The “other” circadian system: food as a Zeitgeber. J. Biol. Rhythms 17, 284–292. doi: 10.1177/074873040201700402

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Stevenson, T. J., and Prendergast, B. J. (2015). Photoperiodic time measurement and seasonal immunological plasticity. Front. Neuroendocrinol. 37, 76–88. doi: 10.1016/j.yfrne.2014.10.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Suzuki, H., and Lucas, L. R. (2015). Neurochemical correlates of accumbal dopamine D2 and amygdaloid 5-HT1B receptor densities on observational learning of aggression. Cogn. Affect. Behav. Neurosci. 15, 460–474. doi: 10.3758/s13415-015-0337-8

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Takahashi, A., and Miczek, K. A. (2014). Neurogenetics of aggressive behavior: studies in rodents. Curr. Top. Behav. Neurosci. 17, 3–44. doi: 10.1007/7854_2013_263

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Taylor, N. D., Fireman, G. D., and Levin, R. (2013). Trait hostility, perceived stress, and sleep quality in a sample of normal sleepers. Sleep Disord. 2013:735812. doi: 10.1155/2013/735812

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Trainor, B. C., Lin, S., Finy, M. S., Rowland, M. R., and Nelson, R. J. (2007). Photoperiod reverses the effects of estrogens on male aggression via genomic and nongenomic pathways. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U S A 104, 9840–9845. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0701819104

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ueda, H. R., Matsumoto, A., Kawamura, M., Iino, M., Tanimura, T., and Hashimoto, S. (2002). Genome-wide transcriptional orchestration of circadian rhythms in Drosophila. J. Biol. Chem. 277, 14048–14052. doi: 10.1074/jbc.C100765200

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vgontzas, A. N., Zoumakis, E., Bixler, E. O., Lin, H. M., Follett, H., Kales, A., et al. (2004). Adverse effects of modest sleep restriction on sleepiness, performance, and inflammatory cytokines. J. Clin. Endocrinol. Metab. 89, 2119–2126. doi: 10.1210/jc.2003-031562

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Volicer, L., Harper, D. G., Manning, B. C., Goldstein, R., and Satlin, A. (2001). Sundowning and circadian rhythms in Alzheimer’s disease. Am. J. Psychiatry 158, 704–711. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.158.5.704

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wacker, D. W., Khalaj, S., Jones, L. J., Champion, T. L., Davis, J. E., Meddle, S. L., et al. (2016). Dehydroepiandrosterone heightens aggression and increases androgen receptor and aromatase mRNA expression in the brain of a male songbird. J. Neuroendocrinol. 28:12. doi: 10.1111/jne.12443

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Waltes, R., Chiocchetti, A. G., and Freitag, C. M. (2016). The neurobiological basis of human aggression: a review on genetic and epigenetic mechanisms. Am. J. Med. Genet. B Neuropsychiatr. Genet. 171, 650–675. doi: 10.1002/ajmg.b.32388

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wang, X., Greer, J., Porter, R. R., Kaur, K., and Youngstedt, S. D. (2016). Short-term moderate sleep restriction decreases insulin sensitivity in young healthy adults. Sleep Health 2, 63–68. doi: 10.1016/j.sleh.2015.11.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Waters, W. F., Adams, S. G. Jr., Binks, P., and Varnado, P. (1993). Attention, stress and negative emotion in persistent sleep-onset and sleep-maintenance insomnia. Sleep 16, 128–136. doi: 10.1093/sleep/16.2.128

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Webb, W. B. (1962). Some effects of prolonged sleep deprivation on the hooded rat. J. Comp. Physiol. Psychol. 55, 791–793. doi: 10.1037/h0042196

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Weber, M., Lauterburg, T., Tobler, I., and Burgunder, J. M. (2004). Circadian patterns of neurotransmitter related gene expression in motor regions of the rat brain. Neurosci. Lett. 358, 17–20. doi: 10.1016/j.neulet.2003.12.053

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wilson, A. P., and Boelkins, R. C. (1970). Evidence for seasonal variation in aggressive behaviour by macaca mulatta. Anim. Behav. 18, 719–724. doi: 10.1016/0003-3472(70)90017-5

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wingfield, J. C. (1994). Regulation of territorial behavior in the sedentary song sparrow, melospiza melodia morphna. Horm. Behav. 28, 1–15. doi: 10.1006/hbeh.1994.1001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wingfield, J. C. (2012). The challenge hypothesis: behavioral economics to neurogenomics. J. Ornithol. 153, 85–96. doi: 10.1007/s10336-012-0857-8

CrossRef Full Text

Woo, J. M., Okusaga, O., and Postolache, T. T. (2012). Seasonality of suicidal behavior. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 9, 531–547. doi: 10.3390/ijerph9020531

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Yesavage, J. A., Friedman, L., Ancoli-Israel, S., Bliwise, D., Singer, C., Vitiello, M. V., et al. (2003). Development of diagnostic criteria for defining sleep disturbance in alzheimer’s disease. J. Geriatr. Psychiatry Neurol. 16, 131–139. doi: 10.1177/0891988703255684

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Yoo, S. S., Gujar, N., Hu, P., Jolesz, F. A., and Walker, M. P. (2007). The human emotional brain without sleep—a prefrontal amygdala disconnect. Curr. Biol. 17, R877–R878. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2007.08.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: anger, aggression, circadian rhythm, infradian rhythm, clock genes

Citation: Hood S and Amir S (2018) Biological Clocks and Rhythms of Anger and Aggression. Front. Behav. Neurosci. 12:4. doi: 10.3389/fnbeh.2018.00004

Received: 22 September 2017; Accepted: 09 January 2018;
Published: 23 January 2018.

Edited by:

Nelly Alia-Klein, Icahn School of Medicine at Mount Sinai, United States

Reviewed by:

Jorge Mendoza, UPR3212 Institut des Neurosciences Cellulaires et Intégratives (INCI), France
Etienne Challet, Centre National de la Recherche Scientifique (CNRS), France

Copyright © 2018 Hood and Amir. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) or licensor are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Shimon Amir, shimon.amir@concordia.ca

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.