- 1Division for Psychology, Department of Clinical Neuroscience, Karolinska Institutet, Solna, Sweden
- 2Stress Research Institute, Stockholm University, Stockholm, Sweden
- 3Department of Psychology, Stockholm University, Stockholm, Sweden
- 4Institute of Medical Psychology and Behavioral Immunobiology, University Hospital Essen, Essen, Germany
Fatigue is a highly disabling symptom in various medical conditions. While inflammation has been suggested as a potential contributor to the development of fatigue, underlying mechanisms remain poorly understood. In this review, we propose that a better assessment of central fatigue, taking into account its multidimensional features, could help elucidate the role and mechanisms of inflammation in fatigue development. A description of the features of central fatigue is provided, and the current evidence describing the association between inflammation and fatigue in various medical conditions is reviewed. Additionally, the effect of inflammation on specific neuronal processes that may be involved in distinct fatigue dimensions is described. We suggest that the multidimensional aspects of fatigue should be assessed in future studies of inflammation-induced fatigue and that this would benefit the development of effective therapeutic interventions.
Introduction
Fatigue is a highly disabling symptom that is common in various medical and psychiatric conditions (1–4). In some cases, the origin of fatigue can be explained by alterations in muscle metabolism or the cardiovascular system, but for most clinical populations, such as cancer survivors and patients suffering from multiple sclerosis (MS) or chronic fatigue syndrome/myalgic encephalomyelitis (CFS/ME), fatigue pathophysiology remains hard to establish. In these conditions, inflammation has been hypothesized as a possible contributor (5, 6), based on an extensive literature showing the capacity of inflammatory factors to act on the central nervous system (CNS) and induce behavioral changes, including fatigue (7–9). Furthermore, alterations in inflammatory processes are found in patients suffering from medical conditions also characterized by high rates of fatigue, such as cancer survivors and patients with MS or diabetes (6). However, despite reported associations between fatigue and circulating levels of inflammatory markers in populations of patients suffering from these diseases (10, 11), the specific role and biological mechanisms of inflammation in the development of fatigue symptoms remain elusive. One of the reasons may be that fatigue is rarely assessed from a multidimensional perspective.
Fatigue: A Multidimensional Perspective
Definitions of Fatigue
The multidimensional aspect of fatigue has been addressed in previous literature. One distinction relates to peripheral and central fatigue (12), with peripheral fatigue described as “the inability to sustain a specified force output or work rate during exercise,” and central fatigue as “the failure to initiate and/or sustain attentional tasks and physical activities requiring self-motivation” (13). This distinction thus lies mainly in the origin of the feeling, with peripheral fatigue developing from peripheral physiological and neuronal systems (e.g., neuromuscular transmission, muscular metabolism, or the cardiovascular system), whereas central fatigue results from changes in the CNS. Central fatigue is further comprised of several dimensions, namely physical fatigue, mental/cognitive fatigue, and motivational changes. Physical fatigue is characterized by a difficulty in performing physical activities, while mental/cognitive fatigue is described as difficulties concentrating and carrying out cognitive tasks (14). These distinctions reflect the behavioral outputs of central fatigue. For these behaviors, motivational changes appear to be central. Motivational inputs, such as expected rewards and benefits, modulate the effort exerted by the individual in any given situation (15). Hence, fatigue has been suggested to arise when the balance between the energy costs and the expected reward of an action is disrupted (16). Consequently, central fatigue may depend on flawed integrations of motivational inputs and/or energy expenditure (13, 15).
Another important aspect to take into account regarding central fatigue is the distinction between physiological and pathological fatigue. Biologically, fatigue is first and foremost an adaptive physiological process. It is the reduction of effort, resulting from perceived exertion (appraised by motor and sensory inputs) and motivational factors (15). Fatigue is a signal to rest, and it encourages energy preservation to prevent injuries, which may be beneficial after intense work or sleep loss, or when the bodily resources need to be redirected toward fighting pathogens during an infection (17). Fatigue also helps focus on more energy-efficient actions (16). As such, healthy, normal physiological processes of fatigue are denoted physiological fatigue in this review, as opposed to pathological fatigue, which is a state where the adaptive function has been lost. Although central fatigue is primarily a feeling, and usually assessed through subjective measurements (e.g., self-report questionnaires), it can also be measured objectively, using physical, cognitive, or motivational tasks.
Taken together, central fatigue appears to be not just “fatigue,” but a complex symptom that comprises several dimensions and concepts (Figure 1). In this review, we will focus on the effects of inflammation on central fatigue and illustrate the importance of multidimensional assessments in understanding the pathophysiology of inflammation-induced central fatigue.
Figure 1. Features of central fatigue. Central fatigue is a complex symptom including several dimensions and concepts. It can be divided into physiological fatigue, a signal to rest and encourage energy preservation to prevent injuries, and pathological fatigue, when the adaptive function has been lost. Central fatigue is also comprised of several dimensions, namely physical fatigue, mental fatigue, and lack of motivation. These dimensions can be assessed in subjective or objective ways. Finally, the study of fatigue is further complicated by the difficulty in separating fatigue from close but distinct concepts, such as sleepiness and boredom.
Issues to Consider When Studying Fatigue
Fatigue is a highly subjective experience that every human being experiences at some point. This intuitive everyday understanding of fatigue may complicate formal assessments. A subjective distinction between normal (physiological) but pronounced fatigue versus pathological fatigue may be difficult to describe and, as such, the nature and intensity of pathological fatigue may be difficult to understand for relatives and caregivers (18). Furthermore, in everyday speech, fatigue is often used interchangeably with tiredness, which in turn is used as a synonym for sleepiness, i.e., sleep propensity (19, 20). Although fatigue and sleepiness are generally considered different concepts in both research and clinical practice, some assessment scales use sleepiness as a dimension of fatigue (21), and some tasks that induce mental fatigue also cause sleepiness (22, 23). This relationship is further complicated by the concept of tiredness, which may be considered equal to fatigue or as a lesser version thereof (24). In addition, other feelings can also be interpreted as—and overlap with—fatigue, such as boredom (25). Evidently, there is a need for a clear characterization of fatigue, both physiological and pathological. The literature on diseases in which fatigue is one of the main causes of suffering for the patients, such as cancer, may help in this regard. The interdisciplinary workgroup Assessing the Symptoms of Cancer using Patient-Reported Outcomes highlights several characteristics of cancer-related fatigue (26), some of them appearing critical for distinguishing pathological from physiological fatigue. For example, as opposed to physiological fatigue, pathological fatigue is not alleviated by sleep or rest (18) and is not proportional to the degree of activity (27).
Beyond the distinction between physiological and pathological fatigue, the assessment of fatigue should be performed keeping in mind the several dimensions and conflicting or overlapping concepts, as discussed above (Figure 1). These dimensions may in fact involve distinct neuronal systems (see Part 3 of the current review). What is called “fatigue” may thus be driven by different underlying mechanisms from one patient to another and from one condition to another. While some self-assessment scales encompass several kinds of fatigue [e.g., the multidimensional fatigue inventory (MFI) (14)], others rely on single or non-specific aspects and, although having clinical relevance, may prevent the understanding of the pathophysiological processes. In addition, while the use of long or intense physical or cognitive tasks assesses fatigue in an objective way, these objective measures do not always correlate with subjective measures, indicating that they may actually assess distinct components (28) and may not be ecologically valid.
Given the large clinical overlap between pain and fatigue (29), the conceptualization of fatigue could be inspired by that of pain. In pain research, both central and peripheral biological components have been identified (30), as well as a fairly well-described neuronal network (31). There are clear mechanistic differences between acute and chronic pain (32), and, depending on the diagnosis, peripheral and central dysfunctions are involved to different degrees (33). However, some components are common for all pain diagnoses (34), and low-grade inflammation has recently been added to this list (35, 36). Following this rationale of pain research, we suggest that there are identifiable biological mechanisms that drive fatigue and that these include peripheral and central components, as well as identifiable neuronal networks. Moreover, the mechanisms may change if the fatigue becomes chronic and may vary for different conditions. Specifically, we argue that inflammation affects some of the biological systems underlying fatigue and that inflammation may therefore be one of the major driving forces for fatigue.
Inflammation and Fatigue
The Activated Immune System Induces Fatigue
During activation of the immune system, immune cells produce pro- and anti-inflammatory cytokines, which are signaling molecules that coordinate the fight against the pathogen. A less-known feature of cytokines is their capacity to act on the CNS, inducing behavioral alterations, including the development of fatigue [for reviews, see Ref. (7, 8)]. The cytokine signal reaches the brain via several immune-to-brain pathways, e.g., by a neuronal pathway via the vagus nerve or by a humoral pathway via brain locations with a weaker blood–brain barrier (37–39). Cytokines in the brain then induce modifications in neurotransmitter and neuroendocrine systems (Box 1), along with modifications in brain functions, which lead to behavioral changes.
Box 1. Cytokine effects in the brain.
During the activation of the immune system, immune cells produce cytokines that coordinate the immune response. In addition to their peripheral actions, cytokines are able to signal to the CNS. Several such immune-to-brain communication pathways have been described, including a neuronal and a humoral pathway. The former refers to the fact that cytokines can activate the vagus nerve at the periphery, which then modulates functions of the brain targets of vagal afferents (38). Cytokines can also activate brain immune cells (microglia) that are located alongside brain vessels where the blood–brain barrier is weaker (e.g., in the circumventricular organs) (39). These cells then produce cytokines locally. The cytokine signal propagates in the brain via diffusion, microglial activation, and neuronal projections (37).
A crucial mechanism by which cytokines modulate neuronal functions is through modifications of monoaminergic neurotransmission, specifically by activating enzymes interfering with dopamine and serotonin biosynthesis. One of these enzymes is the GTP-cyclohydrolase 1 (GTP-CH1), which is involved in the production of neopterin. The production of neopterin happens at the expense of the production of tetrahydrobiopterin (BH4), which is an essential cofactor for the biosynthesis of dopamine and serotonin (40, 41). In addition, cytokines activate the indoleamine 2,3-dioxygenase (IDO), the rate-limiting enzyme degrading tryptophan along the kynurenine pathway (7, 42). The degradation of tryptophan reduces its availability for serotonin biosynthesis. By activating GTP-CH1 and IDO, cytokines thus reduce the synthesis of dopamine and serotonin. Cytokines also modulate dopamine- and serotonin-transporter activity, reducing their synaptic availability (43, 44). In addition to neurotransmitter systems, cytokines modulate neuroendocrine systems, such as the hypothalamic–pituitary–adrenal axis, activating the release of corticotropin-releasing hormone, adrenocorticotropic hormone, and cortisol (45, 46).
Although cytokines induce a large array of behavioral changes, including changes in mood and cognitive functions, fatigue is, interestingly, one of the first and most common symptoms associated with an activated immune system (47). This has been demonstrated in patients suffering from cancer or hepatitis C, who undergo immunotherapy with the pro-inflammatory cytokine interferon-α, which activates the immune system and has neuropsychiatric side effects (48–50). Among these, fatigue develops very rapidly after instauration of the treatment in a large proportion (up to 80%) of patients, while other behavioral alterations, such as depressed mood and cognitive dysfunction, appear later and only in a subpopulation (30–60%) of patients (49, 51). This suggests that fatigue is very sensitive to the effects of cytokines, and underscores the biological connection between fatigue and inflammation.
Clinical Aspects of Inflammation and Fatigue
There is increasing evidence supporting the role of inflammation in fatigue in clinical populations, particularly from cancer and cancer-related fatigue research (11, 52). During cancer treatment, the increase in circulating concentrations of inflammatory markers, such as C-reactive protein (CRP) and interleukin (IL-6), was related to the development of an overall feeling of fatigue (53–55). Inflammation has also been associated with higher levels of post-cancer persistent fatigue. For example, breast cancer survivors who are fatigued, i.e., reporting lower levels of energy or vitality, show signs of activated inflammatory processes with increased concentrations of circulating inflammatory markers, as well as increased ex vivo inflammatory response to an immune challenge (56–58). In this population, higher levels of circulating CRP have shown a positive correlation with overall levels of fatigue even after adjusting for several confounders, such as obesity, self-rated health, depression, and insomnia symptoms (59). Although most of these studies used unidimensional assessments of overall fatigue, some have reported an association between inflammation and multidimensional fatigue, probing for different aspects, such as physical or mental fatigue, in cancer patients. These studies indicate that inflammation in cancer patients and survivors may affect particularly the physical rather than the mental aspects of fatigue (60–63). Further investigation, aiming specifically at assessing the role of inflammation in the different dimensions of fatigue, is needed to determine whether inflammation indeed leads mainly to the development of physical fatigue, or whether it also contributes to the cognitive/mental and motivational aspects of fatigue in cancer patients.
The extreme clinical form of fatigue in CFS/ME has been the subject of extensive study and provides a good model for assessing the potential role of inflammation in the development of fatigue (6). CFS/ME is a debilitating multisystem condition primarily defined by a disabling fatigue for more than 6 months, along with several other symptoms, including pain and cognitive changes (64). Due to the nature of the illness, a broad array of fatigue questionnaires are used for this group of patients, measuring several aspects of fatigue (65). One of the key symptoms is “postexertional fatigue” (64), which, interestingly, appears to be somewhat unique for this patient group (65). Although the underlying mechanisms of this disease are complex, a clear immunological component stands out; CFS/ME often appears following an infection, and some of the most promising treatments are immunomodulatory (64, 66). Furthermore, an extensive literature indicates that patients suffering from CFS/ME exhibit increased systemic production of pro-inflammatory cytokines [e.g., IL-6 or tumor necrosis factor (TNF)-α] and higher CRP at baseline as well as after immune stimulation, compared to non-fatigued individuals (67–72). Altered cytokine production is also associated with the intensity of fatigue symptoms in CFS/ME patients (73, 74). Regarding multidimensionality, CRP concentrations have been found to associate with physical health-related quality of life but not with mental health-related quality of life in a mixed sample of healthy individuals, individuals with high level of fatigue, and patients with CFS/ME (72). Although these measures of health-related quality of life do not specifically assess fatigue, this study highlights the fact that inflammation may be only related to certain dimensions of symptoms or symptom clusters. This has also been indicated in other clinical conditions, such as type 2 diabetes. Type 2 diabetes is characterized by low-grade but chronically increased concentrations of inflammatory markers, found to be closely associated with mental fatigue and lack of motivation, but not with physical fatigue (10). In other patient groups, inflammation has been found to relate to several dimensions of fatigue, both physical and mental. This is the case for patients suffering from MS, in which inflammation correlates with both physical and cognitive dimensions of fatigue, as well as with sleepiness (75).
Taken together, inflammation may be a key player in the development of pathological fatigue. However, the few studies assessing the role of inflammation in fatigue using a multidimensional perspective indicate that inflammation may not always relate to all dimensions of fatigue. In patient groups with long-term fatigue and comorbidity, other factors may thus be of greater importance for some aspects. Importantly, we do not advocate an “inflammation-specific type of fatigue,” but argue that the fatigue dimensions that are affected by inflammation may vary in different medical conditions. This is of high importance when considering the development of anti-inflammatory therapeutic interventions to improve fatigue in patients. Pharmacological treatments aiming at blocking the actions of cytokines, such as inhibitors of TNF-α, have been found to clinically reduce fatigue in patients suffering from rheumatoid arthritis or psoriasis (76, 77). However, if inflammation relates only to a specific aspect of fatigue in a certain population, the use of cytokine inhibitors may only improve certain types of fatigue. For instance, medication with a monoclonal antibody against IL-1β (XOMA052) was found to affect physical, but not cognitive fatigue in type 2 diabetes (78). A better understanding of the effect of inflammation on the multidimensional aspects of fatigue in medical conditions is therefore essential for long-term clinical applications.
Inflammation and Fatigue in the General Population
Inflammation does not only relate to fatigue in clinical populations, but there is also a connection between inflammatory activity and fatigue in the healthy population. Inflammation, as measured with CRP levels, has been found to predict the development of fatigue in healthy subjects 5 years later, even after adjusting for several confounders (79). In addition, a recent study has shown that CRP concentrations in the general population are associated with higher fatigue and reduced sleep quality, but not altered mood or concentration difficulties (80). An earlier study using a multidimensional assessment of fatigue in healthy individuals, however, contradicts these findings, showing that depressive symptoms and adiposity were better predictors of overall and physical fatigue than inflammation (81). Similar results were found in older individuals, for which the association of circulating concentrations of CRP and IL-6 with overall and physical fatigue was no longer significant when adjusting for depressive symptoms or adiposity (82). These results may at least partially be due to sex differences, as the authors also reported a significant relationship, independently of depressive symptoms and adiposity, between CRP levels and fatigue intensity in women but not in men (83).
Inflammation thus seems to contribute to the development of fatigue even in the general population, but a multidimensional assessment is generally lacking. This is unfortunate given that it may prevent the understanding of the pathophysiology of fatigue. Indeed, the different dimensions of fatigue may involve distinct underlying neurological processes. One illustrating example relies on motivational changes, which may drive, at least partially, inflammation-induced fatigue (84). Decreased motivation results from specific alterations in reward-related neuronal processes, involving notably the mesolimbic dopamine pathway (85).
Potential Neuronal Mechanisms Underlying Dimensions of Inflammatory-Induced Central Fatigue
As described in Box 1, during the activation of the immune system, the inflammatory cytokine signals reach the brain (38, 39), inducing changes in neurotransmitter and neuroendocrine systems, and leading to behavioral changes (7, 8). For example, cytokines can inhibit the synthesis of neurotransmitters, such as dopamine or serotonin, by activating specific enzymes involved in the rate-limiting steps of their biosynthesis (86, 87). These alterations in neurotransmitter systems ultimately lead to modifications in neuronal functions, which in turn induce behavioral changes collectively called sickness behavior. Sickness behavior includes fatigue, reduced activity, altered mood state, changes in cognitive functions, and reduced appetite. Sickness behavior is an adaptive process allowing the body to rest and to redirect energy toward fighting infections (17, 88). Although most of the effects of cytokines on the CNS have been demonstrated with high levels of circulating cytokines (e.g., after an immune challenge or during immunotherapy), evidence also suggests that low-grade levels are enough to affect the brain (89, 90). Interestingly, the specific modifications of CNS functions during immune system activation can help infer some mechanisms that likely underlie inflammation-induced central fatigue. Notably, imaging studies of immune challenges highlight changes in activation of the anterior cingulate cortex (ACC), the anterior insula, and the ventral striatum (87, 91). As these areas have also been associated with fatigue in several medical conditions, they seem likely to underlie inflammation-induced fatigue symptoms (92). Here, we take this one step further and propose that specific functional brain alterations induced by inflammation may contribute to the development of the different dimensions of fatigue.
The Basal Ganglia
Given that motivation is a core feature of fatigue and that inflammation has been shown to modulate reward-related processes (88, 93, 94), it is possible that these reward-related processes are involved in the effect of inflammation on fatigue (84). The mesolimbic dopamine pathway, linking the ventral tegmental area to the nucleus accumbens (in the ventral striatum), is essential in the modulation of motivation (85, 95) and particularly in effort-related motivational behaviors (96). This “non-motor part” of the basal ganglia has been suggested as a critical mechanism for the development of central fatigue (13, 97). Altered dopamine processes in the ventral striatum can lead to an effort–reward imbalance, with increased perception of energy costs of actions and/or decreased expectation of reward or benefits (16). This can lead to the feeling of physical and/or mental fatigue, although the underlying issue is a reduced motivation to perform physical or cognitive tasks (98). Additionally, even though fatigue research has focused mainly on the motivation pathway of the basal ganglia, the motor pathway may be involved as well. Decreased volume and activation of the putamen, caudate, and pallidum have been described in fatigued patients with MS or CFS, and are associated with the intensity of fatigue symptoms (99, 100).
Several lines of research indicate that inflammation may induce the development of fatigue, specifically through reduced motivation via alterations in basal ganglia functions. Cytokines are known to affect dopamine function (see Box 1), which leads to modifications in basal ganglia activity, such as the mesolimbic dopamine pathway. A reduced activation of the ventral striatum in response to hedonic reward has indeed been observed after an immune challenge (93, 101). This functional change has been suggested to underlie the development of cytokine-induced fatigue (102, 103). Furthermore, fatigue, but also psychomotor slowing, that develops after the instauration of immunotherapy appears to relate to modifications in dopamine function (while mood and cognitive dimensions rather relate to serotonin function) (49–51, 104). After the commencement of cytokine therapy, patients also exhibit increased glucose metabolism in the basal ganglia, which is associated with symptoms of fatigue and reduced motivation as assessed with the MFI (105, 106). In addition, the reduction of ventral striatal activity in response to reward observed during immunotherapy is associated with reduced motivation, reduced activity, as well as depressive symptoms (101). Immunotherapy-induced physical fatigue, measured as decreased energy, was found to relate to increased basal activity both in the putamen and the ventral striatum (107), but motivational changes were not assessed in this study. Additionally, a recent study nicely illustrates the specific contribution of the ventral striatum in cytokine-induced fatigue (108). In this study, very early ventral striatal alterations induced by immunotherapy (4 h after the initiation of immunotherapy) significantly predicted the later development of fatigue (4 weeks follow-up). Importantly, these changes in striatal function did not predict mood symptoms, which supports the idea that inflammatory effects on the brain may be separated into distinct circuits that underlie the different parts of sickness behavior, some of which drive fatigue specifically. Nevertheless, fatigue was not assessed in a multidimensionality perspective in this study, and specific changes in motivation were not evaluated.
These studies highlight the potential contribution of the basal ganglia, particularly the mesolimbic pathway, in inflammation-induced fatigue. While these studies have been conducted in conditions of high-level activation of the immune system, some data on older adults suggest that inflammation at a low-grade state is sufficient to induce alterations in the dopamine system, contributing to the development of fatigue (41). However, although some of these studies have used multidimensional assessments of fatigue, including reduced motivation, those assessing the effect of inflammation on basal ganglia changes usually measure either fatigue or motivational changes, but rarely the two together.
The ACC
The ACC has been implicated in inflammation-driven processes in several studies (105, 109, 110), and we suggest that this area could be related to the cognitive aspects of fatigue. The ACC, in particular the dorsal part, is involved in conflict monitoring (111–113) and in cognitive control (114). Activation of the dorsal ACC seems to signal the adjustment of cognitive processes according to the difficulty or cognitive demand of the task (115). Interestingly, it has been suggested that the feeling of cognitive/mental fatigue may arise from an increased cerebral effort to maintain a satisfactory performance (116). An increased activation of the ACC during a motor or mental task has been shown in fatigued patients with CFS or MS (117, 118) and was associated with a feeling of having to exert more effort (118). Thus, it is possible that the stronger activation of the ACC signals a need for increased cognitive processing, leading to a feeling of mental fatigue.
Inflammation-induced sickness may represent a more demanding mental state for an individual than full health does, as indicated by a decline in cognitive abilities during immune activation (119). Altered activation of the ACC, mostly an increase in the dorsal part, has been repeatedly reported during activation of the immune system (105, 109, 110, 120, 121). Importantly, the dorsal ACC was the structure most strongly activated during an attentional task in patients treated with immunotherapy, in comparison to control subjects (109). This activation also correlated with number of errors, in line with the involvement of the dorsal ACC in conflict monitoring. Furthermore, inflammation-induced fatigue during a more acute model (typhoid vaccination) was found to significantly correlate with the activation of the ACC during a mental conflicting task (the Stroop task) (122). This was, however, not the case for those feeling fatigued after placebo, suggesting a specific mechanism of inflammation on ACC functions in the development of fatigue.
The Insula
There is a growing interest in the potential role of the insular cortex in inflammation-induced fatigue. This brain area is considered a main hub for the perception of the physiological condition of the body, so-called interoceptive signals, and it has been suggested as the central structure for “human awareness” (123, 124). Speculatively, a tiresome task would require insular involvement for the brain to interpret the associated bodily signals, and the behavioral output to restore homeostasis and promote rest would be the feeling of fatigue. An increased responsiveness of the insula to interoceptive signals would, therefore, make individuals more prone to feeling fatigued. Interestingly, it has repeatedly been shown that inflammation increases insular activity (121, 122, 125–127). Two studies even show a relationship between inflammation-induced insular function and fatigue development (122, 126). In addition, patients with MS, a condition characterized by both alterations of inflammatory processes and fatigue, exhibit increased activation of the insula during a motor task (128). To speculate further, inflammation may thus induce increased sensitivity to interoceptive signals, through stronger insula reactivity, leading to a more rapid development of an overall feeling of fatigue when performing tasks.
Other Central Processes
It is not our intention to reduce fatigue processes to the three brain structures above, and additional brain structures could very well contribute to fatigue in the situation of immune activation. For instance, the self-regulatory and cognitive functions of the pre-frontal cortices are likely to play an important role in the modulation of fatigue (129, 130). Nevertheless, our aim was to highlight that different neuronal functions may underlie different dimensions of fatigue and that more (multidimensional) studies are needed to comprehend the involvement of inflammation in its pathogenesis.
In addition, beyond the functions of specific brain areas, changes in the connections between structures may also underlie the development of fatigue (131–133). This research is still in its infancy, but bears great potential for understanding potential mechanisms. Inflammation has been shown to affect intrinsic connectivity (134–136) and, for instance, the connectivity between the insula and mid-cingulate cortex seems associated with the inflammation-induced state of malaise and discomfort, in line with the interoceptive role of insula (136). Although this has not yet been studied in relation to fatigue, it is probable that altered connections between structures, in addition to specific structural changes, also contribute to the development of fatigue (97, 137).
In summary, inflammation appears to induce changes in neuronal functions that in turn may contribute to the development of fatigue (Figure 2). Although the specific involvement of the cerebral structures for the different dimensions of fatigue remains to be elucidated, we argue that a higher cognitive load during inflammation could lead to a feeling of mental fatigue and depend on changes in ACC function. The mesolimbic reward system on the other hand, may be involved in the dimension of fatigue that relates to lack of motivation, a feature that may be particularly prominent in inflammation-induced fatigue. Finally, higher sensitivity to interoceptive signals may induce an overall feeling of fatigue. While inflammation may be involved in all these processes, it is also possible that, in some medical conditions or patient subpopulations, inflammation contributes to only one or some of these processes. It is therefore important to characterize the specific dimensions of fatigue that develop in patient populations, and assess the role of inflammation. For instance, some aspects of fatigue may be derived from sleep alterations or changes in hormonal regulation in clinical populations, e.g., of insulin or cortisol, which can modulate brain functions including those in the above-mentioned areas (138–140).
Figure 2. Suggested mechanisms of inflammation-induced central fatigue. Inflammation is known to modulate several neuronal processes, including dorsal anterior cingulate cortex function, the mesolimbic pathway, and insula reactivity. During a physical or mental task, the inflammation-induced altered activation of these neuronal processes may contribute to the feeling of fatigue.
Concluding Remarks and Future Considerations
In this review, we have highlighted the potential role of inflammation in the development of pathological central fatigue. Importantly, we wanted to illustrate the need for multidimensional assessments of fatigue when assessing the role of inflammation, given that fatigue contains distinct features that may be explained by separate central mechanisms and may be specific to different medical conditions. Studying fatigue using a multidimensional perspective also appears highly relevant for the development of therapeutic interventions that target inflammation in order to improve fatigue. In cases where inflammation contributes to only some aspects of fatigue, the use of anti-inflammatory therapies may not be sufficient to improve the feeling of fatigue.
It is therefore important to disentangle the dimensions of fatigue if we are to understand the pathophysiological role of inflammation in this symptom. While the use of single, general measures of fatigue is sometimes preferable, depending on the researcher’s or clinician’s need (27), the choice of the fatigue measurement(s) should be carefully considered with regard to the study aims (141). This is especially true since no gold-standard measure exists at this point. However, some recommendations can be made for when the aim is to understand the underlying pathophysiological processes. Several self-report scales of multidimensional fatigue are available, such as the MFI (14), the Swedish Occupational Fatigue Inventory (SOFI) (21), the Checklist Individual Strength (CIS) (142), and the Multidimensional Fatigue Symptoms Inventory (MFSI) (143). It is also important to take into account the time span of fatigue. Hence, while one may assess the feeling of fatigue over a long period of time when referring to pathological fatigue (e.g., 1 or 2 weeks as assessed with the MFI, CIS, or MFSI), measuring acute changes in fatigue when assessing the effects of inflammation is also crucial. This can be done by repeated assessments of the level of fatigue that the subject feels at the time of scale completion, as measured with the SOFI (which, however, lacks a mental fatigue dimension) or visual analog scales, such as the Visual Analogue Scale for Fatigue (VAS-F) (144), which only focuses on physical fatigue. The use of an acute measurement of fatigue also allows for evaluating subjective fatigue induced by physical, mental, or motivational tasks. Using a fatigue-induced task can help define fatigue in a more precise way than when relying solely on reports that pertain to the past few weeks. Needless to say, the development of new scales would be beneficial to the field. These should include both chronic and acute fatigue, as well as the multidimensional features. In the meantime, we suggest using a combination of different scales. In addition, self-report scales could be combined with objective measures of fatigue, such as reduction of performance during a physical or mental task. Effort-related reward tasks, such as the Effort Expenditure for Reward Task (EEfRT) (145), in which subjects receive a monetary reward for effort, may also help in understanding the role of motivational changes in fatigue (146). Nevertheless, it is preferable to combine these objective assessments of fatigue with subjective measures, given that objective fatigue is not always associated with subjective reports of fatigue (28), and that fatigue is first and foremost a subjective experience.
In conclusion, although fatigue is increasingly taken into account by clinicians, the study of this symptom remains limited by being restricted to overall fatigue, which, as highlighted in this review, may encompass many different mechanisms. While inflammation may be involved in the development of fatigue, the specific underlying mechanisms remain poorly understood, perhaps partly because the different dimensions of fatigue are too rarely explored. The mechanisms underlying other inflammation-induced neuropsychiatric symptoms have been inferred thanks to multidimensional assessments (51), and this strategy should be pursued when studying inflammation-induced fatigue as well. Fatigue is a critical and highly disabling symptom for many patient groups and individuals. We argue for the need of adequate multidimensional assessments in order to increase the understanding of the mechanisms underlying inflammation-induced fatigue, as well as for the development of effective therapeutic interventions.
Author Contributions
JL, BK, and TS have written and approved the final version of this manuscript.
Conflict of Interest Statement
The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.
The reviewer AF-H declared a shared affiliation, though no other collaboration, with the authors to the handling Editor, who ensured that the process nevertheless met the standards of a fair and objective review.
Acknowledgments
The authors are very grateful to Prof. Manfred Schedlowski for backing this review. The authors also thank Prof. Mats Lekander and Prof. John Axelsson for the continuing support.
Funding
BK is funded by Swedish Council for Working Life and Social Research, Swedish Heart-Lung foundation, Swedish Asthma and Allergy Association, and Swedish Brain Foundation. JL is funded by the Alexander von Humboldt foundation (Germany, Humboldt fellowship for postdoctoral researchers). TS is funded by the Swedish Research Council (International Postdoc).
References
1. Wessely S, Chalder T, Hirsch S, Wallace P, Wright D. The prevalence and morbidity of chronic fatigue and chronic fatigue syndrome: a prospective primary care study. Am J Public Health (1997) 87(9):1449–55. doi: 10.2105/AJPH.87.9.1449
2. Visser MR, Smets EM. Fatigue, depression and quality of life in cancer patients: how are they related? Support Care Cancer (1998) 6(2):101–8. doi:10.1007/s005200050142
3. Fava M. Symptoms of fatigue and cognitive/executive dysfunction in major depressive disorder before and after antidepressant treatment. J Clin Psychiatry (2003) 64(Suppl 14):30–4.
4. Fritschi C, Quinn L. Fatigue in patients with diabetes: a review. J Psychosom Res (2010) 69(1):33–41. doi:10.1016/j.jpsychores.2010.01.021
5. Silverman MN, Heim CM, Nater UM, Marques AH, Sternberg EM. Neuroendocrine and immune contributors to fatigue. PM R (2010) 2(5):338–46. doi:10.1016/j.pmrj.2010.04.008
6. Klimas NG, Broderick G, Fletcher MA. Biomarkers for chronic fatigue. Brain Behav Immun (2012) 26(8):1202–10. doi:10.1016/j.bbi.2012.06.006
7. Dantzer R, O’Connor JC, Freund GG, Johnson RW, Kelley KW. From inflammation to sickness and depression: when the immune system subjugates the brain. Nat Rev Neurosci (2008) 9(1):46–56. doi:10.1038/nrn2297
8. Capuron L, Miller AH. Immune system to brain signaling: neuropsychopharmacological implications. Pharmacol Ther (2011) 130(2):226–38. doi:10.1016/j.pharmthera.2011.01.014
9. Schedlowski M, Engler H, Grigoleit JS. Endotoxin-induced experimental systemic inflammation in humans: a model to disentangle immune-to-brain communication. Brain Behav Immun (2014) 35:1–8. doi:10.1016/j.bbi.2013.09.015
10. Lasselin J, Laye S, Dexpert S, Aubert A, Gonzalez C, Gin H, et al. Fatigue symptoms relate to systemic inflammation in patients with type 2 diabetes. Brain Behav Immun (2012) 26(8):1211–9. doi:10.1016/j.bbi.2012.03.003
11. Bower JE, Lamkin DM. Inflammation and cancer-related fatigue: mechanisms, contributing factors, and treatment implications. Brain Behav Immun (2013) 30(Suppl):S48–57. doi:10.1016/j.bbi.2012.06.011
13. Chaudhuri A, Behan PO. Fatigue and basal ganglia. J Neurol Sci (2000) 179(S 1–2):34–42. doi:10.1016/S0022-510X(00)00411-1
14. Smets EM, Garssen B, Bonke B, De Haes JC. The multidimensional fatigue inventory (MFI) psychometric qualities of an instrument to assess fatigue. J Psychosom Res (1995) 39(3):315–25. doi:10.1016/0022-3999(94)00125-O
15. Chaudhuri A, Behan PO. Fatigue in neurological disorders. Lancet (2004) 363(9413):978–88. doi:10.1016/S0140-6736(04)15794-2
16. Boksem MA, Tops M. Mental fatigue: costs and benefits. Brain Res Rev (2008) 59(1):125–39. doi:10.1016/j.brainresrev.2008.07.001
17. Dantzer R. Cytokine-induced sickness behavior: where do we stand? Brain Behav Immun (2001) 15(1):7–24. doi:10.1006/brbi.2000.0613
19. Hossain JL, Ahmad P, Reinish LW, Kayumov L, Hossain NK, Shapiro CM. Subjective fatigue and subjective sleepiness: two independent consequences of sleep disorders? J Sleep Res (2005) 14(3):245–53. doi:10.1111/j.1365-2869.2005.00466.x
20. Shen J, Barbera J, Shapiro CM. Distinguishing sleepiness and fatigue: focus on definition and measurement. Sleep Med Rev (2006) 10(1):63–76. doi:10.1016/j.smrv.2005.05.004
21. Ahsberg E. Dimensions of fatigue in different working populations. Scand J Psychol (2000) 41(3):231–41. doi:10.1111/1467-9450.00192
22. Boksem MA, Meijman TF, Lorist MM. Effects of mental fatigue on attention: an ERP study. Brain Res Cogn Brain Res (2005) 25(1):107–16. doi:10.1016/j.cogbrainres.2005.04.011
23. Akerstedt T, Anund A, Axelsson J, Kecklund G. Subjective sleepiness is a sensitive indicator of insufficient sleep and impaired waking function. J Sleep Res (2014) 23(3):240–52. doi:10.1111/jsr.12158
24. Dittner AJ, Wessely SC, Brown RG. The assessment of fatigue: a practical guide for clinicians and researchers. J Psychosom Res (2004) 56(2):157–70. doi:10.1016/S0022-3999(03)00371-4
25. Pattyn N, Neyt X, Henderickx D, Soetens E. Psychophysiological investigation of vigilance decrement: boredom or cognitive fatigue? Physiol Behav (2008) 93(1–2):369–78. doi:10.1016/j.physbeh.2007.09.016
26. Barsevick AM, Cleeland CS, Manning DC, O’Mara AM, Reeve BB, Scott JA, et al. ASCPRO recommendations for the assessment of fatigue as an outcome in clinical trials. J Pain Symptom Manage (2010) 39(6):1086–99. doi:10.1016/j.jpainsymman.2010.02.006
27. Kirsh KL, Passik S, Holtsclaw E, Donaghy K, Theobald D. I get tired for no reason: a single item screening for cancer-related fatigue. J Pain Symptom Manage (2001) 22(5):931–7. doi:10.1016/S0885-3924(01)00350-5
28. Leavitt VM, DeLuca J. Central fatigue: issues related to cognition, mood and behavior, and psychiatric diagnoses. PM R (2010) 2(5):332–7. doi:10.1016/j.pmrj.2010.03.027
29. Almeida C, Choy EH, Hewlett S, Kirwan JR, Cramp F, Chalder T, et al. Biologic interventions for fatigue in rheumatoid arthritis. Cochrane Database Syst Rev (2016) (6):CD008334. doi:10.1002/14651858.CD008334.pub2
30. Tracey I, Mantyh PW. The cerebral signature for pain perception and its modulation. Neuron (2007) 55(3):377–91. doi:10.1016/j.neuron.2007.07.012
31. Jensen KB, Regenbogen C, Ohse MC, Frasnelli J, Freiherr J, Lundstrom JN. Brain activations during pain: a neuroimaging meta-analysis of patients with pain and healthy controls. Pain (2016) 157(6):1279–86. doi:10.1097/j.pain.0000000000000517
32. Martucci KT, Mackey SC. Imaging pain. Anesthesiol Clin (2016) 34(2):255–69. doi:10.1016/j.anclin.2016.01.001
33. Treede RD, Rief W, Barke A, Aziz Q, Bennett MI, Benoliel R, et al. A classification of chronic pain for ICD-11. Pain (2015) 156(6):1003–7. doi:10.1097/j.pain.0000000000000160
34. Williams AC, Eccleston C, Morley S. Psychological therapies for the management of chronic pain (excluding headache) in adults. Cochrane Database Syst Rev (2012) 11:CD007407. doi:10.1002/14651858.CD007407.pub3
35. DeVon HA, Piano MR, Rosenfeld AG, Hoppensteadt DA. The association of pain with protein inflammatory biomarkers: a review of the literature. Nurs Res (2014) 63(1):51–62. doi:10.1097/NNR.0000000000000013
36. Walker AK, Kavelaars A, Heijnen CJ, Dantzer R. Neuroinflammation and comorbidity of pain and depression. Pharmacol Rev (2014) 66(1):80–101. doi:10.1124/pr.113.008144
37. Vitkovic L, Konsman JP, Bockaert J, Dantzer R, Homburger V, Jacque C. Cytokine signals propagate through the brain. Mol Psychiatry (2000) 5(6):604–15. doi:10.1038/sj.mp.4000813
38. Konsman JP, Parnet P, Dantzer R. Cytokine-induced sickness behaviour: mechanisms and implications. Trends Neurosci (2002) 25(3):154–9. doi:10.1016/S0166-2236(00)02088-9
39. Quan N, Banks WA. Brain-immune communication pathways. Brain Behav Immun (2007) 21(6):727–35. doi:10.1016/j.bbi.2007.05.005
40. Murr C, Widner B, Wirleitner B, Fuchs D. Neopterin as a marker for immune system activation. Curr Drug Metab (2002) 3(2):175–87. doi:10.2174/1389200024605082
41. Capuron L, Schroecksnadel S, Feart C, Aubert A, Higueret D, Barberger-Gateau P, et al. Chronic low-grade inflammation in elderly persons is associated with altered tryptophan and tyrosine metabolism: role in neuropsychiatric symptoms. Biol Psychiatry (2011) 70(2):175–82. doi:10.1016/j.biopsych.2010.12.006
42. O’Connor JC, Andre C, Wang Y, Lawson MA, Szegedi SS, Lestage J, et al. Interferon-gamma and tumor necrosis factor-alpha mediate the upregulation of indoleamine 2,3-dioxygenase and the induction of depressive-like behavior in mice in response to Bacillus Calmette-Guerin. J Neurosci (2009) 29(13):4200–9. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5032-08.2009
43. Moron JA, Zakharova I, Ferrer JV, Merrill GA, Hope B, Lafer EM, et al. Mitogen-activated protein kinase regulates dopamine transporter surface expression and dopamine transport capacity. J Neurosci (2003) 23(24):8480–8.
44. Malynn S, Campos-Torres A, Moynagh P, Haase J. The pro-inflammatory cytokine TNF-alpha regulates the activity and expression of the serotonin transporter (SERT) in astrocytes. Neurochem Res (2013) 38(4):694–704. doi:10.1007/s11064-012-0967-y
45. Besedovsky HO, del Rey A. Immune-neuro-endocrine interactions: facts and hypotheses. Endocr Rev (1996) 17(1):64–102. doi:10.1210/edrv-17-1-64
46. Pace TW, Miller AH. Cytokines and glucocorticoid receptor signaling. Relevance to major depression. Ann N Y Acad Sci (2009) 1179:86–105. doi:10.1111/j.1749-6632.2009.04984.x
47. DellaGioia N, Devine L, Pittman B, Hannestad J. Bupropion pre-treatment of endotoxin-induced depressive symptoms. Brain Behav Immun (2013) 31:197–204. doi:10.1016/j.bbi.2012.10.008
48. Musselman DL, Lawson DH, Gumnick JF, Manatunga AK, Penna S, Goodkin RS, et al. Paroxetine for the prevention of depression induced by high-dose interferon alfa. N Engl J Med (2001) 344(13):961–6. doi:10.1056/NEJM200103293441303
49. Capuron L, Gumnick JF, Musselman DL, Lawson DH, Reemsnyder A, Nemeroff CB, et al. Neurobehavioral effects of interferon-alpha in cancer patients: phenomenology and paroxetine responsiveness of symptom dimensions. Neuropsychopharmacology (2002) 26(5):643–52. doi:10.1016/S0893-133X(01)00407-9
50. Capuron L, Miller AH. Cytokines and psychopathology: lessons from interferon-alpha. Biol Psychiatry (2004) 56(11):819–24. doi:10.1016/j.biopsych.2004.02.009
51. Capuron L, Castanon N. Role of inflammation in the development of neuropsychiatric symptom domains: evidence and mechanisms. Curr Top Behav Neurosci (2017) 31:31–44. doi:10.1007/7854_2016_14
52. Schubert C, Hong S, Natarajan L, Mills PJ, Dimsdale JE. The association between fatigue and inflammatory marker levels in cancer patients: a quantitative review. Brain Behav Immun (2007) 21(4):413–27. doi:10.1016/j.bbi.2006.11.004
53. Bower JE, Ganz PA, Tao ML, Hu W, Belin TR, Sepah S, et al. Inflammatory biomarkers and fatigue during radiation therapy for breast and prostate cancer. Clin Cancer Res (2009) 15(17):5534–40. doi:10.1158/1078-0432.CCR-08-2584
54. Liu L, Mills PJ, Rissling M, Fiorentino L, Natarajan L, Dimsdale JE, et al. Fatigue and sleep quality are associated with changes in inflammatory markers in breast cancer patients undergoing chemotherapy. Brain Behav Immun (2012) 26(5):706–13. doi:10.1016/j.bbi.2012.02.001
55. Xiao C, Beitler JJ, Higgins KA, Conneely K, Dwivedi B, Felger J, et al. Fatigue is associated with inflammation in patients with head and neck cancer before and after intensity-modulated radiation therapy. Brain Behav Immun (2016) 52:145–52. doi:10.1016/j.bbi.2015.10.016
56. Bower JE, Ganz PA, Aziz N, Fahey JL. Fatigue and proinflammatory cytokine activity in breast cancer survivors. Psychosom Med (2002) 64(4):604–11. doi:10.1097/00006842-200207000-00010
57. Collado-Hidalgo A, Bower JE, Ganz PA, Cole SW, Irwin MR. Inflammatory biomarkers for persistent fatigue in breast cancer survivors. Clin Cancer Res (2006) 12(9):2759–66. doi:10.1158/1078-0432.CCR-05-2398
58. Zick SM, Zwickey H, Wood L, Foerster B, Khabir T, Wright B, et al. Preliminary differences in peripheral immune markers and brain metabolites between fatigued and non-fatigued breast cancer survivors: a pilot study. Brain Imaging Behav (2014) 8(4):506–16. doi:10.1007/s11682-013-9270-z
59. Orre IJ, Reinertsen KV, Aukrust P, Dahl AA, Fossa SD, Ueland T, et al. Higher levels of fatigue are associated with higher CRP levels in disease-free breast cancer survivors. J Psychosom Res (2011) 71(3):136–41. doi:10.1016/j.jpsychores.2011.04.003
60. Inagaki M, Isono M, Okuyama T, Sugawara Y, Akechi T, Akizuki N, et al. Plasma interleukin-6 and fatigue in terminally ill cancer patients. J Pain Symptom Manage (2008) 35(2):153–61. doi:10.1016/j.jpainsymman.2007.03.009
61. Orre IJ, Murison R, Dahl AA, Ueland T, Aukrust P, Fossa SD. Levels of circulating interleukin-1 receptor antagonist and C-reactive protein in long-term survivors of testicular cancer with chronic cancer-related fatigue. Brain Behav Immun (2009) 23(6):868–74. doi:10.1016/j.bbi.2009.04.003
62. Alfano CM, Imayama I, Neuhouser ML, Kiecolt-Glaser JK, Smith AW, Meeske K, et al. Fatigue, inflammation, and omega-3 and omega-6 fatty acid intake among breast cancer survivors. J Clin Oncol (2012) 30(12):1280–7. doi:10.1200/JCO.2011.36.4109
63. de Raaf PJ, Sleijfer S, Lamers CH, Jager A, Gratama JW, van der Rijt CC. Inflammation and fatigue dimensions in advanced cancer patients and cancer survivors: an explorative study. Cancer (2012) 118(23):6005–11. doi:10.1002/cncr.27613
64. Smith ME, Haney E, McDonagh M, Pappas M, Daeges M, Wasson N, et al. Treatment of myalgic encephalomyelitis/chronic fatigue syndrome: a systematic review for a National Institutes of Health pathways to prevention workshop. Ann Intern Med (2015) 162(12):841–50. doi:10.7326/M15-0114
65. Jason LA, Evans M, Brown M, Porter N, Brown A, Hunnell J, et al. Fatigue scales and chronic fatigue syndrome: issues of sensitivity and specificity. Disabil Stud Q (2011) 31(1):1375. doi:10.18061/dsq.v31i1.1375
66. Collatz A, Johnston SC, Staines DR, Marshall-Gradisnik SM. A systematic review of drug therapies for chronic fatigue syndrome/myalgic encephalomyelitis. Clin Ther (2016) 38(6):1263.e–71.e. doi:10.1016/j.clinthera.2016.04.038
67. Chao CC, Janoff EN, Hu SX, Thomas K, Gallagher M, Tsang M, et al. Altered cytokine release in peripheral blood mononuclear cell cultures from patients with the chronic fatigue syndrome. Cytokine (1991) 3(4):292–8. doi:10.1016/1043-4666(91)90497-2
68. Patarca R, Klimas NG, Lugtendorf S, Antoni M, Fletcher MA. Dysregulated expression of tumor necrosis factor in chronic fatigue syndrome: interrelations with cellular sources and patterns of soluble immune mediator expression. Clin Infect Dis (1994) 18(Suppl 1):S147–53. doi:10.1093/clinids/18.Supplement_1.S147
69. Buchwald D, Wener MH, Pearlman T, Kith P. Markers of inflammation and immune activation in chronic fatigue and chronic fatigue syndrome. J Rheumatol (1997) 24(2):372–6.
70. Moss RB, Mercandetti A, Vojdani A. TNF-alpha and chronic fatigue syndrome. J Clin Immunol (1999) 19(5):314–6. doi:10.1023/A:1020595709352
71. Fletcher MA, Zeng XR, Barnes Z, Levis S, Klimas NG. Plasma cytokines in women with chronic fatigue syndrome. J Transl Med (2009) 7:96. doi:10.1186/1479-5876-7-96
72. Raison CL, Lin JM, Reeves WC. Association of peripheral inflammatory markers with chronic fatigue in a population-based sample. Brain Behav Immun (2009) 23(3):327–37. doi:10.1016/j.bbi.2008.11.005
73. Gupta S, Aggarwal S, Starr A. Increased production of interleukin-6 by adherent and non-adherent mononuclear cells during ‘natural fatigue’ but not following ‘experimental fatigue’ in patients with chronic fatigue syndrome. Int J Mol Med (1999) 3(2):209–13.
74. Gaab J, Rohleder N, Heitz V, Engert V, Schad T, Schurmeyer TH, et al. Stress-induced changes in LPS-induced pro-inflammatory cytokine production in chronic fatigue syndrome. Psychoneuroendocrinology (2005) 30(2):188–98. doi:10.1016/j.psyneuen.2004.06.008
75. Heesen C, Nawrath L, Reich C, Bauer N, Schulz KH, Gold SM. Fatigue in multiple sclerosis: an example of cytokine mediated sickness behaviour? J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry (2006) 77(1):34–9. doi:10.1136/jnnp.2005.065805
76. Tyring S, Gottlieb A, Papp K, Gordon K, Leonardi C, Wang A, et al. Etanercept and clinical outcomes, fatigue, and depression in psoriasis: double-blind placebo-controlled randomised phase III trial. Lancet (2006) 367(9504):29–35. doi:10.1016/S0140-6736(05)67763-X
77. Druce KL, Jones GT, Macfarlane GJ, Basu N. Patients receiving anti-TNF therapies experience clinically important improvements in RA-related fatigue: results from the British Society for Rheumatology biologics register for rheumatoid arthritis. Rheumatology (Oxford) (2015) 54(6):964–71. doi:10.1093/rheumatology/keu390
78. Cavelti-Weder C, Furrer R, Keller C, Babians-Brunner A, Solinger AM, Gast H, et al. Inhibition of IL-1{beta} improves fatigue in type 2 diabetes. Diabetes Care (2011) 34(10):e158. doi:10.2337/dc11-1196
79. Cho HJ, Seeman TE, Bower JE, Kiefe CI, Irwin MR. Prospective association between C-reactive protein and fatigue in the coronary artery risk development in young adults study. Biol Psychiatry (2009) 66(9):871–8. doi:10.1016/j.biopsych.2009.06.008
80. Jokela M, Virtanen M, Batty GD, Kivimaki M. Inflammation and specific symptoms of depression. JAMA Psychiatry (2016) 73(1):87–8. doi:10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2015.1977
81. Lim W, Hong S, Nelesen R, Dimsdale JE. The association of obesity, cytokine levels, and depressive symptoms with diverse measures of fatigue in healthy subjects. Arch Intern Med (2005) 165(8):910–5. doi:10.1001/archinte.165.8.910
82. Valentine RJ, Woods JA, McAuley E, Dantzer R, Evans EM. The associations of adiposity, physical activity and inflammation with fatigue in older adults. Brain Behav Immun (2011) 25(7):1482–90. doi:10.1016/j.bbi.2011.06.002
83. Valentine RJ, McAuley E, Vieira VJ, Baynard T, Hu L, Evans EM, et al. Sex differences in the relationship between obesity, C-reactive protein, physical activity, depression, sleep quality and fatigue in older adults. Brain Behav Immun (2009) 23(5):643–8. doi:10.1016/j.bbi.2008.12.003
84. Dantzer R, Heijnen CJ, Kavelaars A, Laye S, Capuron L. The neuroimmune basis of fatigue. Trends Neurosci (2014) 37(1):39–46. doi:10.1016/j.tins.2013.10.003
85. Salamone JD, Correa M. The mysterious motivational functions of mesolimbic dopamine. Neuron (2012) 76(3):470–85. doi:10.1016/j.neuron.2012.10.021
86. Dantzer R, O’Connor JC, Lawson MA, Kelley KW. Inflammation-associated depression: from serotonin to kynurenine. Psychoneuroendocrinology (2011) 36(3):426–36. doi:10.1016/j.psyneuen.2010.09.012
87. Miller AH, Haroon E, Raison CL, Felger JC. Cytokine targets in the brain: impact on neurotransmitters and neurocircuits. Depress Anxiety (2013) 30(4):297–306. doi:10.1002/da.22084
88. Hart BL. Biological basis of the behavior of sick animals. Neurosci Biobehav Rev (1988) 12(2):123–37. doi:10.1016/S0149-7634(88)80004-6
89. Pollmacher T, Haack M, Schuld A, Reichenberg A, Yirmiya R. Low levels of circulating inflammatory cytokines – do they affect human brain functions? Brain Behav Immun (2002) 16(5):525–32. doi:10.1016/S0889-1591(02)00004-1
90. Lasselin J, Capuron L. Chronic low-grade inflammation in metabolic disorders: relevance for behavioral symptoms. Neuroimmunomodulation (2014) 21(2–3):95–101. doi:10.1159/000356535
91. Harrison NA. Brain structures implicated in inflammation-associated depression. Curr Top Behav Neurosci (2017) 31:221–48. doi:10.1007/7854_2016_30
92. Hanken K, Eling P, Hildebrandt H. The representation of inflammatory signals in the brain – a model for subjective fatigue in multiple sclerosis. Front Neurol (2014) 5:264. doi:10.3389/fneur.2014.00264
93. Eisenberger NI, Berkman ET, Inagaki TK, Rameson LT, Mashal NM, Irwin MR. Inflammation-induced anhedonia: endotoxin reduces ventral striatum responses to reward. Biol Psychiatry (2010) 68(8):748–54. doi:10.1016/j.biopsych.2010.06.010
94. Harrison NA, Voon V, Cercignani M, Cooper EA, Pessiglione M, Critchley HD. A neurocomputational account of how inflammation enhances sensitivity to punishments versus rewards. Biol Psychiatry (2016) 80(1):73–81. doi:10.1016/j.biopsych.2015.07.018
95. Salamone JD, Pardo M, Yohn SE, Lopez-Cruz L, SanMiguel N, Correa M. Mesolimbic dopamine and the regulation of motivated behavior. Curr Top Behav Neurosci (2016) 27:231–57. doi:10.1007/7854_2015_383
96. Salamone JD, Yohn SE, Lopez-Cruz L, San Miguel N, Correa M. Activational and effort-related aspects of motivation: neural mechanisms and implications for psychopathology. Brain (2016) 139(Pt 5):1325–47. doi:10.1093/brain/aww050
97. Dobryakova E, Genova HM, DeLuca J, Wylie GR. The dopamine imbalance hypothesis of fatigue in multiple sclerosis and other neurological disorders. Front Neurol (2015) 6:52. doi:10.3389/fneur.2015.00052
98. Salamone JD, Correa M, Farrar A, Mingote SM. Effort-related functions of nucleus accumbens dopamine and associated forebrain circuits. Psychopharmacology (Berl) (2007) 191(3):461–82. doi:10.1007/s00213-006-0668-9
99. Calabrese M, Rinaldi F, Grossi P, Mattisi I, Bernardi V, Favaretto A, et al. Basal ganglia and frontal/parietal cortical atrophy is associated with fatigue in relapsing-remitting multiple sclerosis. Mult Scler (2010) 16(10):1220–8. doi:10.1177/1352458510376405
100. Miller AH, Jones JF, Drake DF, Tian H, Unger ER, Pagnoni G. Decreased basal ganglia activation in subjects with chronic fatigue syndrome: association with symptoms of fatigue. PLoS One (2014) 9(5):e98156. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0098156
101. Capuron L, Pagnoni G, Drake DF, Woolwine BJ, Spivey JR, Crowe RJ, et al. Dopaminergic mechanisms of reduced basal ganglia responses to hedonic reward during interferon alfa administration. Arch Gen Psychiatry (2012) 69(10):1044–53. doi:10.1001/archgenpsychiatry.2011.2094
102. Felger JC, Miller AH. Cytokine effects on the basal ganglia and dopamine function: the subcortical source of inflammatory malaise. Front Neuroendocrinol (2012) 33(3):315–27. doi:10.1016/j.yfrne.2012.09.003
103. Felger JC. The role of dopamine in inflammation-associated depression: mechanisms and therapeutic implications. Curr Top Behav Neurosci (2017) 31:199–219. doi:10.1007/7854_2016_13
104. Felger JC, Li L, Marvar PJ, Woolwine BJ, Harrison DG, Raison CL, et al. Tyrosine metabolism during interferon-alpha administration: association with fatigue and CSF dopamine concentrations. Brain Behav Immun (2013) 31:153–60. doi:10.1016/j.bbi.2012.10.010
105. Haroon E, Woolwine BJ, Chen X, Pace TW, Parekh S, Spivey JR, et al. IFN-alpha-induced cortical and subcortical glutamate changes assessed by magnetic resonance spectroscopy. Neuropsychopharmacology (2014) 39(7):1777–85. doi:10.1038/npp.2014.25
106. Haroon E, Felger JC, Woolwine BJ, Chen X, Parekh S, Spivey JR, et al. Age-related increases in basal ganglia glutamate are associated with TNF, reduced motivation and decreased psychomotor speed during IFN-alpha treatment: preliminary findings. Brain Behav Immun (2015) 46:17–22. doi:10.1016/j.bbi.2014.12.004
107. Capuron L, Pagnoni G, Demetrashvili MF, Lawson DH, Fornwalt FB, Woolwine B, et al. Basal ganglia hypermetabolism and symptoms of fatigue during interferon-alpha therapy. Neuropsychopharmacology (2007) 32(11):2384–92. doi:10.1038/sj.npp.1301362
108. Dowell NG, Cooper EA, Tibble J, Voon V, Critchley HD, Cercignani M, et al. Acute changes in striatal microstructure predict the development of interferon-alpha induced fatigue. Biol Psychiatry (2016) 79(4):320–8. doi:10.1016/j.biopsych.2015.05.015
109. Capuron L, Pagnoni G, Demetrashvili M, Woolwine BJ, Nemeroff CB, Berns GS, et al. Anterior cingulate activation and error processing during interferon-alpha treatment. Biol Psychiatry (2005) 58(3):190–6. doi:10.1016/j.biopsych.2005.03.033
110. Harrison NA, Brydon L, Walker C, Gray MA, Steptoe A, Critchley HD. Inflammation causes mood changes through alterations in subgenual cingulate activity and mesolimbic connectivity. Biol Psychiatry (2009) 66(5):407–14. doi:10.1016/j.biopsych.2009.03.015
111. Carter CS, Braver TS, Barch DM, Botvinick MM, Noll D, Cohen JD. Anterior cingulate cortex, error detection, and the online monitoring of performance. Science (1998) 280(5364):747–9. doi:10.1126/science.280.5364.747
112. Duncan J, Owen AM. Common regions of the human frontal lobe recruited by diverse cognitive demands. Trends Neurosci (2000) 23(10):475–83. doi:10.1016/S0166-2236(00)01633-7
113. Botvinick MM, Cohen JD, Carter CS. Conflict monitoring and anterior cingulate cortex: an update. Trends Cogn Sci (2004) 8(12):539–46. doi:10.1016/j.tics.2004.10.003
114. Ridderinkhof KR, Ullsperger M, Crone EA, Nieuwenhuis S. The role of the medial frontal cortex in cognitive control. Science (2004) 306(5695):443–7. doi:10.1126/science.1100301
115. Shenhav A, Botvinick MM, Cohen JD. The expected value of control: an integrative theory of anterior cingulate cortex function. Neuron (2013) 79(2):217–40. doi:10.1016/j.neuron.2013.07.007
116. DeLuca J. Fatigue, cognition, and mental effort. Fatigue as a Window to the Brain. London: The MIT Press (2005). p. 31–57.
117. Filippi M, Rocca MA, Colombo B, Falini A, Codella M, Scotti G, et al. Functional magnetic resonance imaging correlates of fatigue in multiple sclerosis. Neuroimage (2002) 15(3):559–67. doi:10.1006/nimg.2001.1011
118. Schmaling KB, Lewis DH, Fiedelak JI, Mahurin R, Buchwald DS. Single-photon emission computerized tomography and neurocognitive function in patients with chronic fatigue syndrome. Psychosom Med (2003) 65(1):129–36. doi:10.1097/01.PSY.0000038942.33335.9B
119. Reichenberg A, Yirmiya R, Schuld A, Kraus T, Haack M, Morag A, et al. Cytokine-associated emotional and cognitive disturbances in humans. Arch Gen Psychiatry (2001) 58(5):445–52. doi:10.1001/archpsyc.58.5.445
120. Eisenberger NI, Inagaki TK, Rameson LT, Mashal NM, Irwin MR. An fMRI study of cytokine-induced depressed mood and social pain: the role of sex differences. Neuroimage (2009) 47(3):881–90. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.04.040
121. Hannestad J, Subramanyam K, Dellagioia N, Planeta-Wilson B, Weinzimmer D, Pittman B, et al. Glucose metabolism in the insula and cingulate is affected by systemic inflammation in humans. J Nucl Med (2012) 53(4):601–7. doi:10.2967/jnumed.111.097014
122. Harrison NA, Brydon L, Walker C, Gray MA, Steptoe A, Dolan RJ, et al. Neural origins of human sickness in interoceptive responses to inflammation. Biol Psychiatry (2009) 66(5):415–22. doi:10.1016/j.biopsych.2009.03.007
123. Craig AD. How do you feel now? the anterior insula and human awareness. Nat Rev Neurosci (2009) 10(1):59–70. doi:10.1038/nrn2555
124. Craig AD. Significance of the insula for the evolution of human awareness of feelings from the body. Ann N Y Acad Sci (2011) 1225:72–82. doi:10.1111/j.1749-6632.2011.05990.x
125. Benson S, Rebernik L, Wegner A, Kleine-Borgmann J, Engler H, Schlamann M, et al. Neural circuitry mediating inflammation-induced central pain amplification in human experimental endotoxemia. Brain Behav Immun (2015) 48:222–31. doi:10.1016/j.bbi.2015.03.017
126. Harrison NA, Cooper E, Dowell NG, Keramida G, Voon V, Critchley HD, et al. Quantitative magnetization transfer imaging as a biomarker for effects of systemic inflammation on the brain. Biol Psychiatry (2015) 78(1):49–57. doi:10.1016/j.biopsych.2014.09.023
127. Karshikoff B, Jensen KB, Kosek E, Kalpouzos G, Soop A, Ingvar M, et al. Why sickness hurts: a central mechanism for pain induced by peripheral inflammation. Brain Behav Immun (2016) 57:38–46. doi:10.1016/j.bbi.2016.04.001
128. White AT, Lee JN, Light AR, Light KC. Brain activation in multiple sclerosis: a BOLD fMRI study of the effects of fatiguing hand exercise. Mult Scler (2009) 15(5):580–6. doi:10.1177/1352458508100034
129. Sepulcre J, Masdeu JC, Goni J, Arrondo G, Velez de Mendizabal N, Bejarano B, et al. Fatigue in multiple sclerosis is associated with the disruption of frontal and parietal pathways. Mult Scler (2009) 15(3):337–44. doi:10.1177/1352458508098373
130. Pardini M, Bonzano L, Mancardi GL, Roccatagliata L. Frontal networks play a role in fatigue perception in multiple sclerosis. Behav Neurosci (2010) 124(3):329–36. doi:10.1037/a0019585
131. Hampson JP, Zick SM, Khabir T, Wright BD, Harris RE. Altered resting brain connectivity in persistent cancer related fatigue. Neuroimage Clin (2015) 8:305–13. doi:10.1016/j.nicl.2015.04.022
132. Nordin LE, Moller MC, Julin P, Bartfai A, Hashim F, Li TQ. Post mTBI fatigue is associated with abnormal brain functional connectivity. Sci Rep (2016) 6:21183. doi:10.1038/srep21183
133. Wortinger LA, Endestad T, Melinder AM, Oie MG, Sevenius A, Bruun Wyller V. Aberrant resting-state functional connectivity in the salience network of adolescent chronic fatigue syndrome. PLoS One (2016) 11(7):e0159351. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0159351
134. Dipasquale O, Cooper EA, Tibble J, Voon V, Baglio F, Baselli G, et al. Interferon-alpha acutely impairs whole-brain functional connectivity network architecture – a preliminary study. Brain Behav Immun (2016) 58:31–9. doi:10.1016/j.bbi.2015.12.011
135. Labrenz F, Wrede K, Forsting M, Engler H, Schedlowski M, Elsenbruch S, et al. Alterations in functional connectivity of resting state networks during experimental endotoxemia – an exploratory study in healthy men. Brain Behav Immun (2016) 54:17–26. doi:10.1016/j.bbi.2015.11.010
136. Lekander M, Karshikoff B, Johansson E, Soop A, Fransson P, Lundstrom JN, et al. Intrinsic functional connectivity of insular cortex and symptoms of sickness during acute experimental inflammation. Brain Behav Immun (2016) 56:34–41. doi:10.1016/j.bbi.2015.12.018
137. Finke C, Schlichting J, Papazoglou S, Scheel M, Freing A, Soemmer C, et al. Altered basal ganglia functional connectivity in multiple sclerosis patients with fatigue. Mult Scler (2015) 21(7):925–34. doi:10.1177/1352458514555784
138. Blazquez E, Velazquez E, Hurtado-Carneiro V, Ruiz-Albusac JM. Insulin in the brain: its pathophysiological implications for states related with central insulin resistance, type 2 diabetes and Alzheimer’s disease. Front Endocrinol (Lausanne) (2014) 5:161. doi:10.3389/fendo.2014.00161
139. Montoya ER, Bos PA, Terburg D, Rosenberger LA, van Honk J. Cortisol administration induces global down-regulation of the brain’s reward circuitry. Psychoneuroendocrinology (2014) 47:31–42. doi:10.1016/j.psyneuen.2014.04.022
140. Schilling TM, Ferreira de Sa DS, Westerhausen R, Strelzyk F, Larra MF, Hallschmid M, et al. Intranasal insulin increases regional cerebral blood flow in the insular cortex in men independently of cortisol manipulation. Hum Brain Mapp (2014) 35(5):1944–56. doi:10.1002/hbm.22304
141. Elbers RG, Rietberg MB, van Wegen EE, Verhoef J, Kramer SF, Terwee CB, et al. Self-report fatigue questionnaires in multiple sclerosis, Parkinson’s disease and stroke: a systematic review of measurement properties. Qual Life Res (2012) 21(6):925–44. doi:10.1007/s11136-011-0009-2
142. Vercoulen JH, Swanink CM, Fennis JF, Galama JM, van der Meer JW, Bleijenberg G. Dimensional assessment of chronic fatigue syndrome. J Psychosom Res (1994) 38(5):383–92. doi:10.1016/0022-3999(94)90099-X
143. Stein KD, Martin SC, Hann DM, Jacobsen PB. A multidimensional measure of fatigue for use with cancer patients. Cancer Pract (1998) 6(3):143–52. doi:10.1046/j.1523-5394.1998.006003143.x
144. Lee KA, Hicks G, Nino-Murcia G. Validity and reliability of a scale to assess fatigue. Psychiatry Res (1991) 36(3):291–8. doi:10.1016/0165-1781(91)90027-M
145. Treadway MT, Buckholtz JW, Schwartzman AN, Lambert WE, Zald DH. Worth the ‘EEfRT’? The effort expenditure for rewards task as an objective measure of motivation and anhedonia. PLoS One (2009) 4(8):e6598. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0006598
Keywords: central fatigue, inflammation, immune system, multidimensional assessments, motivation, ventral striatum, anterior cingulate cortex, insula
Citation: Karshikoff B, Sundelin T and Lasselin J (2017) Role of Inflammation in Human Fatigue: Relevance of Multidimensional Assessments and Potential Neuronal Mechanisms. Front. Immunol. 8:21. doi: 10.3389/fimmu.2017.00021
Received: 09 September 2016; Accepted: 05 January 2017;
Published: 20 January 2017
Edited by:
Sanae Hasegawa-Ishii, Pennsylvania State University, USAReviewed by:
Urs Nater, University of Marburg, GermanyAnna Fogdell-Hahn, Karolinska Institutet, Sweden
Copyright: © 2017 Karshikoff, Sundelin and Lasselin. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) or licensor are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.
*Correspondence: Julie Lasselin, anVsaWUubGFzc2VsaW4mI3gwMDA0MDt1ay1lc3Nlbi5kZQ==