Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Vet. Sci., 15 August 2022
Sec. Animal Nutrition and Metabolism
This article is part of the Research Topic Early Life Programming in Poultry: Recent Insights and Interventional Approaches View all 10 articles

Traditional sexing methods and external egg characteristics combination allow highly accurate early sex determination in an endangered native turkey breed

\nJ. I. Salgado PardoJ. I. Salgado Pardo1Francisco Javier Navas Gonzlez,
Francisco Javier Navas González1,2*Antonio Gonzlez Ariza
Antonio González Ariza1*A. Arando Arbulu&#x;A. Arando Arbulu1J. M. Len JuradoJ. M. León Jurado3J. V. Delgado BermejoJ. V. Delgado Bermejo1M. E. Camacho VallejoM. E. Camacho Vallejo2
  • 1Department of Genetics, Faculty of Veterinary Sciences, University of Córdoba, Córdoba, Spain
  • 2Department of Agriculture and Ecological Husbandry, Area of Agriculture and Environment, Andalusian Institute of Agricultural and Fisheries Research and Training (IFAPA), Córdoba, Spain
  • 3Agropecuary Provincial Centre, Córdoba Provincial Government, Córdoba, Spain

Early sex determination methods are not only crucial in the worldwide massive poultry industry, but also for small-holder producers. The profitability of sexing techniques must be accounted for when aiming to boost management, nutrition, and conservation practices in endangered poultry breeds. This becomes pivotal when the local breed dealt with belongs to an understudied species, such as the turkey. So, the main objective of this study is to identify which method combination may report a higher likelihood of successful sex determination in poults across the three-pattern varieties of the Andalusian turkey breed. A total of 84 one to two days old Andalusian turkey poults (42 black, 28 black-roan, and 14 bronze-roan) were evaluated in this study. Sex determination was performed using 15 methods, which included testing external egg metrics and eggshell color, poult morphological appraisal and phaneroptics, and behavioral traits. Possible differences across plumage varieties and the interaction between sex and plumage were observed when external egg quality was measured. Sex determination through behavioral methods in black base feathered (black and black-roan) male sex individuals showed seven times higher sensitivity when compared to the rest of the studied individuals (χ2 = 7.14, df = 1, P < 0.01). In contrast, for the black-roan plumage females, the method based on the color of down feathers was approximately four times more sensitive (χ2 = 3.95, df = 1, P ≤ 0.05). For the bronze-roan pattern, none of the sexing techniques was reported to efficiently predict sex itself. However, the most proper method combination to determine sex, independent of plumage color, was physical external egg characteristics, the color of down feathers, and behavioral approaches (“English method” and “slap technique”). The specificity values were found to be 49.12, 93.33, and 100%, while the sensitivity values were observed to be 74.64, 91.03, and 100%, which translated into accuracy of 63.10, 92.26, and 100% in black, black-roan, and bronze-roan poults, respectively. Our results suggest that the method combination tested in this study could be considered a highly accurate, simple, and affordable alternative for sex determination in turkeys. This could mean a pivotal advance for small producers of turkeys, as early sex detection can help to plan timely conservational management strategies, which is of prominent importance in the context of endangered poultry breeds.

Introduction

Early sex determination plays a pivotal role in the turkey farming specialization, since two different lines are commonly used: a heavy line, which comprises males, and a laying line, which sources dams (1). The difference in body weights between these two strains is the basis for the differentiation of farms to ensure basic animal management and nutrition (2). Thus, hatching poults need to be separated by sexes to be raised independently, depending on the commercial strategy chosen by breeders (3). Apart from its critical economic impact, the possibility of sex detection before hatching is also interesting in terms of both animal welfare and ethical issues by the early separation of the different sexes (4).

Sexing chicks during the first day of life could be a critical step not only in the commercial poultry industry but also in the design of conservational and breeding programs for endangered native breeds, as described by (5). The use of reliable sexing techniques in endangered avian breeds is of special importance in breeding programs, since it could avoid lowing hatching rate problems or copulation problems due to side effects derived from high inbreeding in such minority populations (6). In these terms, native poultry breeds, such as the Andalusian turkey, could benefit from the early sex determination of poults. The Andalusian turkey breed is a Spanish endangered autochthonous population distributed around the Southeast Iberian Peninsula and might be the direct descendants of the first turkeys imported from Mexico to arrive in Seville's port during the early 16th century (7).

Andalusian turkey is raised in semi-grazing conditions by backyard producers in the Guadalquivir Basin and is characterized by great adaptability to the environment. However, during the 20th century, the number of individuals belonging to this native breed drastically decreased as a consequence of the introduction of commercial hybrid strains in Spain (8). This situation promoted local genotype displacement and hybridization, which suggested the need for urgent conservation measures to be taken.

The implementation of a standardized accurate method for the sex determination of 1-day-old poults could mean a crucial improvement for breeders, making it possible to take proper management decisions at hatching instead of waiting for 4–5 months, when animals start to display sexual dimorphism characters (9). As a consequence, Andalusian turkey males may be aimed toward the maximization of their meat production while letting hen for the laying aptitude (10). These sexing methods can also be a beneficial tool when management strategies to preserve genetic diversity are designed, since sex distribution across the population is possibly biased (5).

Sexual dimorphism is defined as the differences in external appearance, among other traits, between the two genders of one species and is influenced by both genetic and environmental factors (11). Generally, males and females differ in size, color, shape, and appendage development (such as feathers, wattle or appendage, caruncles, beard, and spurs). On the other hand, sexual dimorphism can also be manifested by scent or courtship vocalizations, behavior, and cognition (12). Recent advances in poultry genetics have made it possible to obtain, based on the crosses of given parental strains, offspring showing specific phenotypic traits that make both sexes distinguishable in the early stages of life (13). Genes that modify feather growth have also been described and reported to permit early sex determination (14). However, its implementation in breeding programs was discarded due to a negative impact on the production traits (15). More recent technologies have developed new tools, in which algorithm wing edge detection is used. For this, computational imaging of external wing feathers growth is employed (16). Again, these methods may be difficult to implement in local poultry populations, as morphological and phaneroptic traits may broadly vary across different breeds and varieties.

In avian species, sexual dimorphism is caused by several secondary phenotypical traits that can be recognizable even in the laid egg until the 1-day-old poult (5). Several sex-influenced phenotypical traits in these early stages have been reported, including egg size (17, 18), the opacity of the eggshell (19), feather color, morphology and distribution (20), appendicular skeleton dimensions, focusing on tarsus-metatarsus length (21), head length and size (22, 23), tail inclination (24), and the behavioral performance of the individuals (22, 25).

Considering the aforementioned premises, this study aims to establish which method combination may offer the most efficient and accurate method to determine sex at the early stages of life across the three plumage varieties of the Andalusian turkey breed. This information will be processed to tailor specific non-invasive sexing methods for poult from local turkey populations. The identification of the proportions of individuals belonging to each sex when working with endangered populations can contribute to the improvement and progress of the genetic management tasks carried out in these genotypes. Thus, the tool developed in this study can be a complement to the more commonly used techniques, which have been widely tested on a commercial scale but are sometimes inefficient due to the implicit diversity found in local populations.

Materials and methods

Animals and sample size

The present research was conducted in a public hatchery located at the Agropecuary Provincial Center of Diputación of Córdoba (Andalusian, Spain). A total of 18 turkey hens and 3 toms, aged between 12 and 16 months, coming from the base population of the Andalusian turkey breed were reared in three different groups according to plumage color (black: 1M/6F; roan-black: 1M/6F; and bronze-roan: 1M/6F), and were involved in the egg production.

Taking advance of the breeding season (from February to April 2019), eggs were collected daily and stored at 17–18 °C and 70–75 % humidity in incubating platters until their incubation. All eggs were individually numbered, and external egg metrics and eggshell color were determined before incubation.

Eggs intended for incubation were kept for a maximum of 7 days since oviposition. A total of 311 eggs were incubated and divided into seven different incubation periods to ensure sufficient birds are included in the study. An incubator with automatic egg turning (Masalles, M240-I, Barcelona, Spain) was used for 26 days at 37.2 °C and 55 % RH. On the 26th day of incubation, eggs were transferred to a hatchery cabinet (Masalles, 25-N HLC, Barcelona, Spain) maintained at 36.7 °C and 60 % RH until hatching (2 more days). A total of 162 poults hatched, and then were wing-banded and placed in a room with an electric stove to help them regulate their body temperature until performing the sex determination tests.

A random sample of 162 turkey poults (76 black, 58 black-roan, and 28 bronze-roan) was used for sexing. Finally, of the total animals subjected to the sexing tests, we were able to determine the sex of 84 individuals (42 black, 28 black-roan, and 14 bronze-roan). This was due to the fact that some animals died, and others were donated to local farms (as part of an Andalusian turkey breed recovery program) before the sexual dimorphism of the individuals became evident. In the literature, it has been reported that samples of around 100 or even fewer individuals report 95% sexing accuracy in other local poultry species (5, 9, 26). Therefore, of the total of 972 observations that were obtained, only 506 observations were used in the analyzed database, of which the individual sex was confirmed and a complete sexing determination procedure of three appraisers was collected.

Bird management was directed under the European Union Direction regulations (2010/63/EU) as transposed to Spanish Royal Decree-Law 53/2013. This study did not need to be subjected to evaluation by the Ethics Committee of Animal of the University of Córdoba, since it is not part of the legislation for the protection of animals used for scientific purposes.

External egg metrics and eggshell color

Before incubation, external egg quality was determined in each egg:

• M1 and M2 (major and minor diameters). These measurements were determined using a digital caliper (precision, ±0.01 mm; Electro DH M 60.205, Barcelona, Spain).

• M3 [shape index (SI)]. This index was computed using the following formula (27):

SI=(Mm ) 100

where ØM is the major diameter and Øm is the minor diameter.

If the egg is long and pointed, the individual will be taken as a female, while wide and flat eggs are assigned to males (28). To establish the limits to consider an egg long or flat and wide or broad, the shape index and the median of the diameters were calculated (non-normal distribution, p > 0.05), to set over and below the median categories.

• M4 (egg weight). Eggs were weighed individually using an electronic scale (precision ±0.01 g; Cobos, CSB-600C, Barcelona, Spain).

• M5, M6, and M7 (eggshell L*, eggshell a*, and eggshell b*). Eggshell color was assessed using a portable spectrophotometer (CM 700d, Konica Minolta Holdings Inc., Tokyo, Japan), and the results of eggshell color were expressed according to the International Commission on Illumination (CIE) L*a*b* system color profile.

Poult morphological appraisal and phaneroptics, behavioral traits, and handling for sexing assignation methods

To carry out the sex determination procedure, each turkey poult was held by the neck during the examination, with the index and middle fingers of the sexer, keeping the poult's head down. Defecation of the animal was caused by applying pressure on the abdominal cavity. Finally, the cloaca was cleaned with a piece of tissue paper (5).

Various sexing methods based on the poult morphological appraisal and phaneroptics and behavioral traits were performed by three sexers through eight methods, and a dichotomous scale (male or female) was used to classify the animals (5, 29) (Figure 1). The examination tests were performed after hatching, considering it as days 1 and 2 since hatching, and were carried out by three different non-trained evaluators. The different methodologies employed are described as follows:

• M8 (English method). The bird is suspended for 5 s by holding the beak with two fingers, thus analyzing the acquired behavior. If the bird stands still, it is considered male, and if the bird kicks, it will be considered female.

• M9 (Tail inclination method). The turkey poults will be taken as a female when the direction of the tail feathers is toward the ground. However, it will be considered as male if the tail is straight.

• M10 (Japanese method or cloaca examination). As described in the “Introduction” section, vent sexing starts from the basis of the appreciation of morphological visual distinction of genital anatomical structures between sexes in hatched poults by trained experts. Cloaca needs to be externalized by carefully applying pressure with the fingers, and then focusing on the central and ventral parts of it. An individual can be considered a male if it shows a unique outline in the cloaca, or a female if two little bulges are observed.

• M11 (General coloring of down feathers method). This method involves observation of the color pattern displayed by the down feathers on both sides. Individuals displaying a uniform coloration will be considered females, while the poults that exhibit heterogeneous coloring of down feathers will be considered males.

• M12 (Fan-shaped wings and general wing metrics determination). It is based on the growth of primary and secondary reminge feathers of the wings. In this regard, a female is identified when all primary and secondary reminge feathers of the wing show a parallel growth and describe a uniform fan edge. In contrast, a wing that exhibits feathers at a different growth stage, describing an irregular contour, will be considered a characteristic of males.

• M13 (Body size and head morphology method). Males have been described to present proportionally smaller and more rounded heads compared to females, showing a bigger and more angular head shape. To state the limits to consider whether the head of a poult is big or small, the median sizes were computed (the sample was not normally distributed, p > 0.05), to set over and below the median categories.

• M14 [Leg length method (from femorotibial joint to the end of the medial phalange)]. Male poults are considered to have long legs when compared to female ones. To state the limits to consider whether the legs are long or short, the average measure of the sizes was computed to set over and below the median categories, since the sample was not normally distributed (p > 0.05). The complete leg was considered, and not only the shanks.

• M15 (Behavior/coping styles or slap technique method). Hands are clapped at a prudent distance of 20 cm from the animal. This technique is applied individually for each poult in an isolated place, distant from the rest of the poults. Two different reactions can be observed: freezing (male) and fleeing or attempting to escape (female).

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Sex assignment methods. (A) Egg length and width test. (B) Eggshell color. (C) English test. (D) Tail inclination. (E) Cloaca. (F) Side feathers. (G) Wing fan. (H) Legs. (I) Head size and morphology. (J) Behavior/coping style.

All the methods used in Sections External egg metrics and eggshell color and Poult morphological appraisal and phaneroptics, behavioral traits, and handling for sexing assignation methods of the present work are depicted in Figure 1.

Sex confirmation

After achieving 25 weeks of age, Andalusian turkey breed females and males exteriorized the secondary sexual characters that enabled the confirmation of the real sex of the individuals.

Statistical analysis

Binary logistic regression

Binary logistic regression was used to fit the statistical model described below. This model represents how the chance of an animal belonging to one of the two possible categories (sexes) may depend on the results for covariates or predictors (sexing methods). In this context, Y was defined as a binary outcome with two categories (sex).

Ordinary least squares (OLS) on a dichotomous dependent variable and binary logistic regression are the two alternatives that can be considered in the case of regressing binary outcomes. OLS is a type of linear least squares method that is used to estimate the unknown parameters in a linear regression model.

Particularly, OLS selects the parameters of a linear function of a set of explanatory variables (sex determination methods) by the principle of least squares, minimizing the sum of the squares of the differences between the observed dependent variable (values of the variable being observed) in the given dataset and those predicted by the linear function of the independent variable.

However, there are three assumptions that must be met prior to running the analyses. First, the error terms need to be heteroskedastic. Thus, the variance of the dependent variable and independent variables must be different, the error terms must not distribute normally, and the predicted probabilities can be > 1 or <0, which can be a problem for subsequent analysis. The “logit” model solves these problems:

ln[p/(1-p)]=a+BX

where p is the probability that Y for cases equals 1, p (Y=1), 1-p is the probability that Y for cases equals 0, 1–p(Y=1), p/(1-p)” is the odds, and ln[p/1-p] is the log odds, or “logit.”

In logistic regression, we predict Z, not p, because of Z's convenient mathematical properties. Z is a linear function of the predictors, and we can translate that prediction into a probability. The natural log of the odds is called the “logit” = “Z.” Z can be described as follows;

Z=log(p/1-p)=B0+B1·X1+B2·X2+B3·X3 

B's in logistic regression are analogous to b's in OLS, B1 is the average change in Z per one unit increase in X1, controlling for the other predictors, and so on.

The set of independent covariates and categorical predictors (B) consisted of the external egg metrics and eggshell color and sexing methods outcomes using the logistic regression procedure of the Modeling Data Package in XLSTAT Version 2014.5.03 (30). A single model was performed for each of the varieties (black, black-roan, and bronze-roan). The Hosmer–Lemeshow test was used to determine the goodness of fit of the logistic regression model. Essentially, it is chi-square goodness of fit test. When P > 0.05 (assuming α = 0.05), we conclude that the logistic regression model is a good fit.

Interpreting logistic coefficients

Once significant covariates and predictors have been identified, the sign of Bs will determine the changes in the log odds of the dependent variable, but not changes in the dependent variable (as in OLS). If B for a specific predictor is positive, a unit change in its related x will raise the odds of the event happening, after controlling for the other predictors, while if B is negative, the odds of the event decrease with a unit increase in x.

Exp(B) means “e to the power B” or eB. It is called the “odds ratio” (Gr. symbol: Ψ), e is a, mathematical constant used as the “base” for natural logarithms. In logistic regression, eB is the factor by which the odds change when X increases by one unit.

New odds/Old odds=eB=odds ratio

Odds ratios > 1 indicate a positive relationship between IV and DV (event likely to occur)

Odds ratios <1 indicate a negative relationship between IV and DV (event less likely to occur)

The significance of logistic coefficients is determined by a Wald test. Wald is χ2 with 1 df and equals a two-tailed t2 with a p-value exactly the same.

The knowledge of the distribution of sex yielded the likelihood of the sample. To estimate the B parameters of the model (the coefficients of the linear function), the likelihood function was maximized. As opposed to linear regression, an exact analytical solution does not exist; hence, an iterative algorithm had to be applied.

Maximization of the likelihood function was performed using the Newton–Raphson algorithm with 100 iterations and a convergence level of 0.000001, which are given as default by XLSTAT Version 2014.5.03 (30).

Specificity and sensitivity

Sensitivity, true positive rate, or the recall measured the proportion of individuals correctly attributed to sex, and specificity (also called the true negative rate) measured the proportion of individuals incorrectly attributed to sex. These two parameters were computed using the logistic regression procedure of the Modeling Data Package in XLSTAT Version 2014.5.03 (30).

Results

Table 1 displays the existing correlations across external egg traits. In this case, as the probability of these variables modeling for real sex determination was lower than 0.001 (Tables 1, 2), the variables chosen were concluded to statistically significantly condition and model for real sex determination. Table 3 determined whether the set of variables evaluated in this study may have significantly conditioned (i.e., have been responsible for) real sex determination by comparing the model as it was defined with a simpler model with only one intercept.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Correlation matrix for external egg characteristics across Andalusian turkey variety pairs.

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Goodness of fit statistics for each Andalusian turkey variety.

TABLE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. Test of the null hypothesis (Black: Y = 0.5476, Roan–black: Y = 0.4643, and Roan–bronze: Y = 0.5714).

Table 2 provides several indicators of the quality of the model (or goodness of fit). These results were equivalent to R2 and the analysis of the variance table in linear regression and ANOVA. The most important value was the probability of the chi-square test on the log ratio. This is equivalent to Fisher's F test, and it is used to evaluate whether the variables bring significant information by comparing the model when it is defined with a simpler model with only one constant. In this case, as the probability was lower than 0.0001 (Table 1), we could conclude that data could be significantly modeled by the set of variables chosen.

Parameter analysis

Table 4 provides details on the model and presents a measure of the effect of the variables considered on the categories of the response variable. There is one intercept for each category of the response variable and one set of coefficients, since the parallel curves hypothesis is supposed to be met.

TABLE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Table 4. Summary of the results for strength of association between the plumage varieties of the Andalusian turkey breed and the ability to succeed or fail when assigning sex for the different methods using the chi–square independence test.

When the regression coefficient for a specific category within a variable was equal to 0.000, this indicated that the said category was taken as the reference to measure the higher or lower repercussions of the subsequent categories in the same variable. The standardized regression coefficient measured the number of times that a certain level or category had a higher (positive standardized coefficient) or lower (negative standardized coefficient) repercussion.

The interpretation of parameters was not immediate. Based on the results in Table 3, it was concluded that the model equation for each variety was as follows:

Black variety

Pred(REAL SEX) = 1/[1 + exp(-(-9.01007+0.00635*Egg Weight+0.09983*Egg Length+0.01051*Egg Width+0.03087*Shape index-0.00983*L*-0.02763*a*-0.04581*b*-0.19432*English method-1+0.65821*General coloring of down feathers method-2+0.72086* Behavior/coping styles or slap technique method))]

Black-roan variety

Pred(REAL SEX) = 1/[1 + exp(-(-166.45357+0.54722*Egg Weight+1.49114*Egg length-3.68572*Egg Width+2.43531*Shape index+0.22455*L*+0.04886*a*+0.36373*b*-0.15040*English method-1-1.09496*General coloring of down feathers method-1-0.19976* Behavior/coping styles or slap technique method))]

Bronze-roan variety

Pred(REAL SEX) = 1 / [1 + exp(-(-42494-233.76746*Egg Weight+337.60584*Egg length o+822.15956*Egg Width+6.06954*L*-20.84484*a*-13.93728*b*+0.21860*English method-1-0.85095*General coloring of down feathers method-1+0.68119* Behavior/coping styles or slap technique method))]

Specificity and sensitivity

Specificity values were 49.12, 93.33, and 100%, while sensitivity values were 74.64, 91.03, and 100%, which translated into the accuracy of 63.10, 92.26, and 100% in black, black–roan, and bronze-roan poults, respectively. A detailed report of the classification table for the estimation sample used to compute the aforementioned parameters is presented in Table 5.

TABLE 5
www.frontiersin.org

Table 5. Classification table for the estimated sexes according to different plumage varieties.

Discussion

High variability in the ability of the different methods used to predict sex across the different plumage varieties was found. However, the combination of external characteristics of egg, the coloring of down feathers, and behavioral techniques (“English method” and slap technique) reported the best sexing performance with 63.10, 92.26, and 100 % of individuals being correctly classified as black, black-roan, and bronze-roan varieties, respectively (Table 5).

Our results suggest that larger turkey eggs, and hence heavier turkey eggs, are more likely to develop into black and bronze-roan female poults. Literature references have reported a significant relationship between egg metrics and poult sex determination in hens (5, 31). In line with these results, (32) suggested that male turkey poults display higher weights at hatching, as a result of the smaller difference existing between male poult weight and egg preincubation weight than in females. This finding has also been reported for the eggs of other species, such as those of the white-crowned sparrow. In this particular case, the male-containing eggs were larger than the eggs containing females (17) would ascribe this early live sexual dimorphism finding to an adaptive mechanism background in the species (17). This would also be supported by the findings in our study that although male-containing eggs were slightly lighter than the ones containing females in the previously named plumage varieties, no influence of egg size on the adult weight of bird has been reported (33).

Although black Andalusian eggs were larger than those laid by the rest of the plumage varieties, greater difficulties were encountered during the sexing of black Andalusian turkey poults based on the external characteristics of egg. This translates into a disadvantage at the time of sexing, considering the external appearance. Such difficulties may rely on the lower existing variability across eggs of this variety. Indeed, a low genetic variability was reported by (34) in the black plumage variety of the Spanish turkey population. These authors described low values for the number of nucleotides and haplotypes estimated in this population, which is indicative of populations originating from a small number of founders (35). In contrast, high variability in the products of roan varieties may evidence potential traces of hybridization with other nearby Spanish breeds, such as the Oscense and the Minorcan Gall D'Indis turkey breeds, with which Andalusian roans share a similar plumage pattern (7, 36).

Regarding eggshell color, being the descendants of reptiles, ancestral birds were thought to have laid white eggs at first (37). Eggshell pigmentation may have appeared as a mechanism to hide the nest from antipredators, prevent parasitic infestations, or protect the embryo from light-filtering harmful irradiations (38). Furthermore, some species have developed different eggshell pigmentations as a response to other features, such as reinforcement mechanisms for weak shell structure (39), as cooling mechanisms, due to protoporphyrin's ability to reflect infrared light (40), or by the hen to act as a sexual decoy for mating (41).

While eggs containing male poults displayed significantly intenser pigmented eggshells, the color of female-containing eggs was less intense and brighter in the Andalusian turkey breed due to lower pigment depositions. These results are supported by (42), who reported a strong significant association between shell pigmentation intensity and increased male hatching numbers in the barn swallow species. These authors suggested that visual cues about brood sex ratio before egg hatching may let parents prepare for provisioning a highly energy-demanding male-biased brood. Indeed, (43) suggested that turkey hens could adjust the water vapor conductance of eggshells by manipulating carcass porosity, and proposed that secretor cells could adjust the carcass pore number to match embryo metabolism. This maternal ability to influence the egg functional characteristics of turkey, probably mediated by thyroid or iodine metabolism, could affect shell pigment deposition as well, as a response to early sex dimorphism properties in the egg.

The possibility of a hormonal basis in the correlation between sex ratio and egg color could be presumed. However, the mechanism of eggshell color deposition mediated by eggshell glands remains unclear. Contextually, although no influence of blood estrogen or ovulation mechanisms has been determined (44), higher progesterone blood levels prior to ovulation have been proved to influence the accumulation of colored substances in the shell gland, thus in the shell that will eventually be deposited (45), via their implication in the activation of the δ-aminolevulinic acid synthetase (46). However, references that either contrast (47) or support our results can be found in the literature, which may provide pieces of evidence of a multifactorial nature for the aforementioned correlation.

According to the Trivers–Willard hypothesis, hens in good metabolic conditions could bias the sex ratio of their progeny toward males, while hens exhibiting poor conditions tend to bias toward females (4850). In line with this finding, females in good body condition maintain eggshell color to limit visible changes and conceal their eggs in anti-predator behavior. Nevertheless, food-restricted females in lower body conditions modify biliverdin and protoporphyrin concentrations in the eggshell (51). This reinforces the results obtained in the present study, since the ratio of different sexes correlates with the shell color. After the turkey poults are hatched, the present research not only reports acceptable results for the down feather's color method, especially for the black and black-roan patterns, but also allows for a rather efficient early identification of females (better fit). Although this method had been successfully used before for sex determination in hybrid and local fowl strains (13, 52, 53), our study constitutes the first to report its application in the early sex determination of turkeys.

Feather color is strongly influenced by the endocrine system, with thyroid hormone activity being considered one of the most highly conditioning elements of the system (54). Parallelly, pituitary hormones like the α-melanocyte-stimulating hormone (a-MSH), follicle-stimulating hormone (FSH), and luteinizing hormone (LH) are also involved in the plumage coloring process (55), but are thought to be less related to sexual dimorphism and chick feather pigmentation. Sex hormones also influence plumage pigmentation, particularly in terms of sexual dimorphic color pattern, probably acting at the level of melanoblast differentiation (56). Contextually, (54) described that although the expression of feather color is mostly influenced by genetics, estrogen or testosterone levels produce alterations in the plumage pattern of the embryo. In this regard, estrogen has been described to have a high impact on feather color in Brown Leghorn's birds (5). This finding was supported by (52) who was able to differentiate male and castrated female chicks from the New Hampshire x Light Sussex cross with a high success rate, suggesting that early endocrine sexual dimorphism may determine down feather color differences across sexes.

The bronze-roan variety did not report satisfactory results when the down feather's color technique was used. In this way, differences in sexual dimorphism patterns across different genotypes of a single breed population were suggested (5). The most extended feather varieties of Andalusian turkey are black and black-roan, which are originally presented in the ancestral domesticated turkey in Mexico (57). The presence of bronze-roan plumage in the Andalusian breed population may derive from the hypothetical hybridization of individuals belonging to this breed with other similar breeds, which may have translated into the interferences impeding the efficiency of the method.

Although less frequently approached, behavior-based sexing methods, which have barely been included in scientific reports, have reported scarce but interesting results. This gap of knowledge is even larger in turkeys, a species for which worldwide animal production integration is relatively recent. The behavior of this species is comparable to that of other birds that had been domesticated earlier in history (58). The first reference to the scientific application of the “English method” (or “inversion test”) dates back to the past century in Argentina and reported nearly 70% accuracy in the sex determination of hen chicks (29, 59). These results and those in the present article are in line with those reported by (5), who confirmed the significant accuracy of the method for chick sexing in the Utrerana hen breed, a local breed from Spain.

Despite the fact that differences in the reaction to acoustic stimuli between the sexes have been thoroughly studied in chicks (6067), the behavior/coping style or “slap” technique has scarcely been reported as a sex discriminant technique. It was only (5) who evaluated its applicability in a chick sex-determining study. In line with these results, the “slap technique” reports significant results supporting its feasible applicability for sex determination in domesticated Meleagris gallopavo which had never been described in turkey species prior to this study.

The influence of sex on chick behavior has been a widely studied topic during the second half of the 20th century (62, 63, 67, 68). Chick fear was known to inhibit general activity, and scared animals were described to perform both low activity and peeps (62). When 7-day-old chicks were submitted to new stimuli in an ‘open field' test, males were less active and displayed rather fearful responses, displaying freezing, sitting, lying, and eye-enclosure patterns more frequently than females (68).

Chick behavior studies have also focused on isolated animals. In this case, chicks react to loud noises, such as the ring of a bell, and females were much more reactive than males, displaying higher walking and peeping activities and lower freezing, sitting, and eye-enclosure responses (64). Thus, the “English method” can be analogous to this open-field reaction test research made on chicks. The results of the present study showed a lack of activity in males and a higher response when exposed to a new environment, like the hand of the observer, in females. In the “slap technique,” similar results were obtained. Males showed a significant decrease in walking and peeping activity, and thus a significantly decreased reactivity when compared to females. This finding is in contrast to the outcomes of previous research, as males tended to experience increased physiological fear responses, which were reduced after medical tranquilization to the same fear levels displayed by females (64).

Alternative theories propose that higher rates of activity in females (ambulation and distress calls), when compared to males, might not be ascribed to lower fear reactions but to a stronger need to reinstate social contact with conspecifics (69). In addition, male nestlings have shown higher exploration of unfamiliar objects than females when it is required to separate them from their partners, reinforcing less social attachment behavior among males (70). These different responses across chick sexes reflect adult sexual behavior and social organization (71). This way, newly hatched Japanese quail females displayed fear reactions less frequently when a male chick was present (72). This behavior relates to the adult social organization of a certain avian species, where a single male guards a small female group. In this regard, Andalusian turkey females show high social attachment due to their flock idiosyncracies, while males may display lower social needs owing to their solitary nature (73).

The aforementioned sex-related chick responses might be the consequence of early endocrine modulation of post-hatching sexual dimorphism behavior. Contextually, the presence of first steroid hormones in the egg has a maternal origin and plays an important role in the offspring's sex establishment, since they influence post-hatched chick behavior (74). Although the endocrine system is not matured yet, the hormonal synthesis in embryos begins during the egg developmental stage (75, 76). Indeed, the establishment of the hypothalamic–hypophyseal–adrenal axis is known to take place during days 17 and 18 of early development in turkeys, and hormonal activity increases during hatching (77). Hence, differences in the embryonal hormone profiles between sexes could suggest that endocrine sexual dimorphism might influence gallinaceous chicks right from the egg stage.

The aforementioned fact particularly concerned sexual hormones. On the one hand, testosterone has been reported to be the first hormone that is present equally in the plasma of the embryos of both sexes until day 7 of incubation, although a significant increase in the testosterone levels in males is observed (78). On the other hand, (79) described that the embryo ovary produces higher levels of estrogen than the testes during egg development. This produces a higher estrogen/androgen ratio in females, which was suggested to determine the reproductive behavior of adult Japanese quails. Additionally, steroids have proved their influence on both hormone receptors' tissular density and hormone-secreting cell distribution during early development and have direct implications in showing distinct sensitivity to hormones in adulthood (80).

Apart from its influence on physiological fear responses, the influence of hormones on anatomical neural development explains a female's greater reactions and activity (5) explained that a chick's sexual dimorphism behavior may be a direct consequence of the impact of different steroids on the development of visual vias' lateralization. Neural and behavioral lateralization has been reported to play a fundamental role in brain organization, where androgens, especially testosterone, are involved (81). In this sense, several functions are lateralized in avian species (82), and two vias are described. First, the ‘right-eye system' is specialized to see large distant objects, whereas the ‘left-eye system' is skilled to analyze the changes in special relations and positions (70) suggested that the specialization of the left-eye system is lower in female chicks than in male ones. This particularly lower space sight could explain the female's particular closeness to the hen and its relatives, and therefore their higher partnership needs described above. When considering the early feeding rates of female chicks after hatching and their higher willingness to eat novel colored food (71), greater development in the right-eye system of female chicks is suggested (83). This phenomenon could support the major reactiveness of females to fear-generating stimuli, which can be attributed to the right-eye system's implication in fixing large and distant objects, similar to the human observer (70).

In the present research, the ‘slap test' not only performed efficiently in black-feathered poults, but this method was also seven times more significant than the other techniques for this plumage. The effects of plumage color on behavior have been extensively studied with contradictory results. Indeed, while in hens, white-feathered individuals have been suggested to be more aggressive than black and gray ones (84, 85). In an indigenous turkey breed, more aggressive behavior has been reported in black- and lavender-feathered individuals when compared to white individuals (86). In line with these results, as aggression can frequently be initiated by a fear-producing stimulus, (87) described white-feathered turkeys may be less fearful than bronze birds.

The basis for this observation may stem from the fact that a significant relationship has been reported between skin pigmentation and certain conduct patterns in many species, such as Norway rats, lions, and wild foxes (8890). The link between different physiological mechanisms and dark feather patterns has been described to be the result of broad pleiotropic effects of the gene network encoding melanin synthesis or its transport and deposition (42, 91). Genes controlling the deposition of plumage pigments, such as the agouti signaling protein gene, melanocortin-1 gene, or the tyrosine gene, have been proposed to also affect the hormonal status and influence sexual behavior or aggressiveness (92). Thus, darker animals may tend to be more aggressive, possibly due to a higher release of self-stimulating pheromones and a greater secretion of exocrine glands that melanocortins promote (86).

Due to plumage color selection during domestication and its well-studied relationship with behavior (91), higher primitive behavioral gene preservation in black-feathered turkeys should be considered. At this concern, behavior has been closely related to animal domestication, being directly and indirectly modified by humans, since individuals that show better tolerance to human presence also showed the highest production (25, 93, 94). This can be attributed to a lower hypothalamic–hypophyseal–adrenal axis reactivity, a consequence of genetic modifications that selection for docility achieved during domestication (25). For instance, in an open-field test, black-feathered poults displayed greater reactiveness to fear (86). It has been reported that black plumage performed fear-avoidance behaviors (escaping, jumping, and flight) more frequently than lighter-colored individuals.

The effect of early selection practices along with the domestication process of turkeys may be more relevant in the Andalusian turkey breed. The Andalusian turkey breed is a very rudimentary population that has barely been submitted to selection or improvement. This population has often been described as a living representation of the first turkeys that arrived in Europe in the 16th century, and before that, these birds had only accomplished 1,300 years of domestication (10), while comparatively, the chicken species may have probably been domesticated for 4,700 years by that time (95, 96). Therefore, black-feathered Andalusian turkeys could present a greater degree of relatedness to wild ancestors than the individuals presenting one of the remaining plumage patterns.

Bertin and Richard-Yris (97) reported that despite thousands of years of domestication, the free-range-reared domestic animals showed behaviors that still closely resembled those of their wild ancestors. Contextually, the increased frequencies of sex-biased fear responses to strong human stimuli may be the reason why statistically significant reliability in the “slap test” was only reached in black-feathered poults. This hypothesis is supported by previous research in which a non-selected fowl genotype shared more behavioral patterns with a wild ancestor than a highly selected strain (98).

Conclusion

Conclusively, the combination of egg external characteristics, down feather coloring, and two behavioral techniques (“English method” and slap technique) allows effective sexing in newly hatched poults belonging to the Andalusian turkey breed, chiefly for the two roan varieties (black-roan and bronze-roan). Sexual dimorphism is not very evident in egg size, since egg dimensions do not influence adult weight in turkeys. Early sexual dimorphism is significant when eggshell color is considered, since female-containing eggs were less intensely colored and brighter due to a lower pigment deposition. Color differences of the bronze-roan variety with the predominant black-based colors render this method significantly invalid for sex determination in this plumage pattern. Behavioral techniques like the “English method” and the “slap test” presented high discriminatory power. In any case, the development of this battery of tests, their high predictive potential, and the ease of implementation in non-industrialized farms allows a reliable determination of sexual relationship in a population due to their low economic cost of implementation and the relative improvement in efficiency at data collection, which deems this tool a time and resource-economic alternative to other methods.

Data availability statement

The original contributions presented in the study are included in the article/Supplementary material, further inquiries can be directed to the corresponding authors.

Ethics statement

Ethical review and approval was not required for the animal study because Bird management was directed under the European Union Direction regulations (2010/63/EU) as transposed to Spanish Royal Decree-Law 53/2013. This study did not need to be subjected to evaluation by the Ethics Committee of Animal of the University of Córdoba, since it is not part of the legislation for the protection of animals used for scientific purposes.

Author contributions

JS, FN, and AG: conceptualization and original draft preparation. FN, AG, and AA: methodology and data curation. FN and AA: software and visualization. JL, JD, and MC: validation and formal analysis. JS, FN, AG, and AA: investigation. JD and MC: resources and funding acquisition. MC: project administration. FN and JL: reviewing and editing. FN: supervision. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.

Funding

This research was funded by FEDER project P20_00893.

Acknowledgments

This work would not have been possible without the assistance of ACPA (Asociación de Criadores del Pavo Andaluz), IFAPA (Instituto de Investigación y Formación Agraria y Pesquera), Diputación de Córdoba, and PAIDI AGR 218 research group.

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher's note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

Supplementary material

The Supplementary Material for this article can be found online at: https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fvets.2022.948502/full#supplementary-material

References

1. Clark D, Nestor K, Velleman S. Continual selection for increased 16 wk body weight on Turkey growth and meat quality: 50 generation update. J Appl Poult Res. (2019) 28:658–68. doi: 10.3382/japr/pfz017

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

2. Jahan B, Ashraf A, Rahman M, Molla M, Chowdhury S, Megwalu F, et al. Rearing of high yielding turkey poults: problems and future prospects in Bangladesh: a review. J Biotechnol Biomed Eng. (2018) 1:1008.

Google Scholar

3. Aviagen. Management Guidelines for Breeding Turkeys. Tattenhall: Aviagen Turkeys Limited (2019). Available online at: http://www.aviagenturkeys.com/uploads/2019/04/10/BR28_V2.1_Management%20Guidelines%20for%20Breeding%20Turkeys_UK.pdf

Google Scholar

4. Steiner G, Bartels T, Krautwald-Junghanns ME, Boos A, Koch E. Sexing of turkey poults by Fourier transform infrared spectroscopy. Anal Bioanal Chem. (2010) 396:465–70. doi: 10.1007/s00216-009-3273-z

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

5. Iglesias Pastrana C, Navas González FJ, Marín Navas C, Arando Arbulu A, González Ariza A, León Jurado JM, et al. Sexual dimorphism for coping styles complements traditional methods for sex determination in a multivariety endangered hen breed. Animals. (2019) 9:1165. doi: 10.3390/ani9121165

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

6. Gutiérrez-Reinoso MA, Aponte PM, Cabezas J, Rodriguez-Alvarez L, Garcia-Herreros M. Genomic evaluation of primiparous high-producing dairy cows: Inbreeding effects on genotypic and phenotypic production–reproductive traits. Animals. (2020) 10:1704. doi: 10.3390/ani10091704

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

7. Fernández M, Gómez M, Delgado J. Adán S, Jiménez. M. Guía de campo de las razas autóctonas españolas. SERGA: Madrid, Spain. (2009). p. 683–4.

Google Scholar

8. González Ariza A, Navas González FJ, Arando Arbulu A, León Jurado JM, Barba Capote CJ, Camacho Vallejo ME, et al. Non-parametrical canonical analysis of quality-related characteristics of eggs of different varieties of native hens compared to laying lineage. Animals. (2019) 9:153. doi: 10.3390/ani9040153

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

9. Akter S, Das S, Apu A, Ahmed T, Lahiry A, Afrin A, et al. Early sex determination of Turkey by observation of differences in body weight between male and female. Progress Agric. (2020) 31:218–26.

Google Scholar

10. Arando A, González-Ariza A, Lupi T, Nogales S, León J, Navas-González F, et al. Comparison of non-linear models to describe the growth in the Andalusian turkey breed. Ital J Anim Sci. (2021) 20:1156–67. doi: 10.1080/1828051X.2021.1950054

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

11. Ralls K, Mesnick S. “Sexual dimorphism” in Encyclopedia of marine mammals. Amsterdam, Netherlands: Elsevier (2009). p. 1005–11.

Google Scholar

12. Punzalan D, Hosken DJ. Sexual dimorphism: why the sexes are (and are not) different. Curr Biol. (2010) 20:972–3. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2010.09.067

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

13. Göhler D, Fischer B, Meissner S. In-ovo sexing of 14-day-old chicken embryos by pattern analysis in hyperspectral images (VIS/NIR spectra): a non-destructive method for layer lines with gender-specific down feather color. Poult Sci. (2017) 96:1–4. doi: 10.3382/ps/pew282

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

14. Vučićević M, Stevanović J, Vučićević I, Pantelić A, 0elić N, Resanović R, et al. Sex determination in game birds management. In Proceedings of the International symposium, on hunting,≫ Modern aspects of sustainable management of game population. Zemun-Belgrade Serbia. (2012) p. 91–4).

Google Scholar

15. Magothe TM, Muhuyi WB. Influence of major genes for crested-head, frizzle-feather and naked-neck on body weights and growth patterns of indigenous chickens reared intensively in Kenya. Trop Anim Health Prod. (2010) 42:173–83. doi: 10.1007/s11250-009-9403-y

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

16. Jones P, Shearer S, Gates R. Edge extraction algorithm for feather sexing poultry chicks. Trans ASAE. (1991) 34:635–0640. doi: 10.13031/2013.31712

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

17. Mead PS, Morton ML, Fish BE. Sexual dimorphism in egg size and implications regarding facultative manipulation of sex in mountain white-crowned sparrows. Condor. (1987) 89:798–803. doi: 10.2307/1368527

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

18. Cordero PJ, Griffith SC, Aparicio JM, Parkin DT. Sexual dimorphism in house sparrow eggs. Behav Ecol Sociobiol. (2000) 48:353–7. doi: 10.1007/s002650000252

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

19. Alin K, Fujitani S, Kashimori A, Suzuki T, Ogawa Y, Kondo N, et al. Non-invasive broiler chick embryo sexing based on opacity value of incubated eggs. Comput Electron Agric. (2019) 158:30–5. doi: 10.1016/j.compag.2019.01.029

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

20. Cuervo JJ, Møller AP. Phenotypic variation and fluctuating asymmetry in sexually dimorphic feather ornaments in relation to sex and mating system. Biol J Linn Soc. (1999) 68:505–529. doi: 10.1111/j.1095-8312.1999.tb01186.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

21. Hutt F. Sex dimorphism and variability in the appendicular skeleton of the Leghorn fowl. Poult Sci. (1929) 8:202–18. doi: 10.3382/ps.0080202

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

22. Blanckenhorn WU. Behavioral causes and consequences of sexual size dimorphism. Ethology. (2005) 111:977–1016. doi: 10.1111/j.1439-0310.2005.01147.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

23. Verdiglione R, Rizzi C. A morphometrical study on the skull of Padovana chicken. Ital J Anim. Sci. (2018) 2810. doi: 10.1080/1828051X.2017.1412810

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

24. Gurusinghe CJ, Ehrlich D. Sex-dependent structural asymmetry of the medial habenular nucleus of the chicken brain. Cell Tissue Res. (1985) 240:149–52. doi: 10.1007/BF00217568

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

25. Fallahsharoudi A, Kock D, Johnsson N, Ubhayasekera M, Bergquist SKA, et al. Domestication effects on stress induced steroid secretion and adrenal gene expression in chickens. Sci Rep. (2015) 5:1–10. doi: 10.1038/srep15345

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

26. Homma K, Siopes TD, Wilson WO, Mcfarland LZ. Identification of sex of day-old quail (Coturnix coturnix japonica) by cloacal examination. Poult Sci. (1966) 45:469–72. doi: 10.3382/ps.0450469

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

27. González Ariza A, Arando Arbulu A, Navas González FJ, Delgado Bermejo JV, Camacho Vallejo ME. Discriminant canonical analysis as a validation tool for multivariety native breed egg commercial quality classification. Foods. (2021) 10:632. doi: 10.3390/foods10030632

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

28. Kaleta E, Redmann T. Approaches to determine the sex prior to and after incubation of chicken eggs and of day-old chicks. Worlds Poult Sci J. (2008) 64:391–9. doi: 10.1017/S0043933908000111

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

29. Llanos Company M. Sexaje de pollitos. Hojas Divulgadoras Ministerio de Agricultura Dir Gen Capacit. Agrar. (1963) 1:14.

Google Scholar

30. Addinsoft. XLSTAT Version 5, 03. Paris, France: Addinsoft Corp (2014).

Google Scholar

31. Yilmaz-Dikmen B, Dikmen S. A morphometric method of sexing white layer eggs. Braz J Poult Sci. (2013) 15:203–10. doi: 10.1590/S1516-635X2013000300006

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

32. Burke W. Sex differences in weight of turkey embryos. Poult Sci. (1994) 73:749–53. doi: 10.3382/ps.0730749

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

33. Scott H, Phillips R. Egg size in relation to growth of Narragansett turkeys. Poult Sci. (1936) 15:435–8. doi: 10.3382/ps.0150435

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

34. Guan X, Silva P, Gyenai K, Xu J, Geng T, Smith E, et al. Mitochondrial DNA-based analyses of relatedness among Turkeys, Meleagris gallopavo. Biochem Genet. (2015) 53:29–41. doi: 10.1007/s10528-015-9668-y

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

35. Canales Vergara AM, Landi V, Delgado Bermejo JV, Martínez A, Cervantes Acosta P, Pons Barro Á, et al. Tracing worldwide turkey genetic diversity using D-loop sequence mitochondrial DNA analysis. Animals. (2019) 9:897. doi: 10.3390/ani9110897

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

36. Monteagudo L, Avellanet R, Tejedor M, Azón R. Genetic studies in the Oscense Turkey by means of microsatellites. XIV Jornadas sobre Producción Animal, Zaragoza, España. (2011) 485–87.

Google Scholar

37. Kilner R. The evolution of egg colour and patterning in birds. Biol Rev. (2006) 81:383–406. doi: 10.1017/S1464793106007044

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

38. Maurer G, Portugal SJ, Cassey P. An embryo's eye view of avian eggshell pigmentation. J Avian Biol. (2011) 42:494–504. doi: 10.1111/j.1600-048X.2011.05368.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

39. Sparks NH. Eggshell pigments–from formation to deposition. Avian Biol Res. (2011) 4:162–7. doi: 10.3184/175815511X13228269481875

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

40. Gosler AG, Higham JP, James Reynolds S. Why are birds' eggs speckled? Ecol. Lett. (2005) 8:1105–13. doi: 10.1111/j.1461-0248.2005.00816.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

41. Moreno J, Osorno JL. Avian egg colour and sexual selection: does eggshell pigmentation reflect female condition and genetic quality? Ecol. Lett. (2003) 6:803–6. doi: 10.1046/j.1461-0248.2003.00505.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

42. Corti M, Romano A, Costanzo A, Bentz AB, Navara KJ, Parolini M, et al. Protoporphyrin-based eggshell pigmentation predicts hatching success and offspring sex ratio in the barn swallow. J Avian Biol. (2018) 49:012405. doi: 10.1111/jav.01642

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

43. Christensen V, Ort J. Influence of diet-mediated maternal thyroid alterations on functional properties of turkey eggs. Poult Sci. (1990) 69:1576–81. doi: 10.3382/ps.0691576

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

44. Soh T, Koga O. The effects of sex steroid hormones on the pigment accumulation in the shell gland of Japanese quail. Poult Sci. (1994) 73:179–85. doi: 10.3382/ps.0730179

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

45. Hargitai R, Herényi M, Török J. Eggshell coloration in relation to male ornamentation, female condition and egg quality in the collared flycatcher Ficedula albicollis. J Avian Biol. (2008) 39, 413–422 doi: 10.1111/j.0908-8857.2008.04337.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

46. Miller LK, Kappas A. The effect of progesterone on activities of δ-aminolevulinic acid synthetase and δ-aminolevulinic acid dehydratase in estrogen-primed avian oviduct. Gen Comp Endocrinol. (1974) 22:238–44. doi: 10.1016/0016-6480(74)90114-2

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

47. Correa SM, Adkins-Regan E, Johnson PA. High progesterone during avian meiosis biases sex ratios toward females. Biol Lett. (2005) 1:215–8. doi: 10.1098/rsbl.2004.0283

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

48. Trivers RL, Willard DE. Natural selection of parental ability to vary the sex ratio of offspring. Science. (1973) 179:90–2. doi: 10.1126/science.179.4068.90

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

49. Pike TW, Petrie M. Maternal body condition and plasma hormones affect offspring sex ratio in peafowl. Anim Behav. (2005) 70:745–51. doi: 10.1016/j.anbehav.2004.12.020

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

50. Geffroy B, Douhard M. The adaptive sex in stressful environments. Trends Ecol. (2019) E34 628–40. doi: 10.1016/j.tree.2019.02.012

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

51. Duval C, Cassey P, Mikšík I, Reynolds SJ, Spencer KA. Condition-dependent strategies of eggshell pigmentation: an experimental study of Japanese quail (Coturnix coturnix japonica). J Exp Biol. (2013) 216:700–8. doi: 10.1242/jeb.077370

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

52. Groenendijk-Huijbers MM. Sex-linked feathering of female hybrid chick embryos hormonally conditioned. Experientia. (1966) 22:302–3. doi: 10.1007/BF01900462

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

53. Abdellatif M. Sexing on down and feather colour in the Dandarawi Egyptian breed. Br Poult Sci. (2001) 42:327–32. doi: 10.1080/00071660120055287

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

54. Leeson S, Walsh T. Feathering in commercial poultry I. Feather growth and composition. Worlds Poult Sci J. (2004) 60:42–51. doi: 10.1079/WPS20033

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

55. Höhn E, Braun C. Hormonal induction of feather pigmentation in ptarmigan. Auk. (1980) 97:601–7. doi: 10.1093/auk/97.3.601

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

56. Inaba M, Chuong CM. Avian pigment pattern formation: developmental control of macro-(across the body) and micro-(within a feather) level of pigment patterns. Front Cell Dev Biol. (2020) 8:620 doi: 10.3389/fcell.2020.00620

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

57. Crawford R. Introduction to Europe and diffusion of domesticated turkeys from the America. Arch Zootec. (1992) 41:2.

Google Scholar

58. Huff G, Huff W, Rath N, Donoghue A, Anthony N, Nestor K, et al. Differential effects of sex and genetics on behavior and stress response of turkeys. Poult Sci. (2007) 86:1294–303. doi: 10.1093/ps/86.7.1294

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

59. Lunn JH. Chick sexing. Am Sci. (1948) 36:280–7.

Google Scholar

60. Nash RF, Gallup GG, Mcclure MK. The immobility reaction in leopard frogs (Rana pipiens) as a function of noise-induced fear. Psychon Sci. (1970) 21:155–6. doi: 10.3758/BF03331860

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

61. Archer J. Effects of testosterone on immobility responses in the young male chick. Behav Biol. (1973) 8:551–6. doi: 10.1016/S0091-6773(73)80047-1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

62. Faure J, Morozeau F. Etude des liaisons entre comportement en open-field et émotivité chez le jeune poussin. Ann Genet Sel Anim. (1975) 7:197–204. doi: 10.1186/1297-9686-7-2-197

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

63. Archer J. Testosterone and fear behavior in male chicks. Physiol Behav. (1976) 17:561–4. doi: 10.1016/0031-9384(76)90151-7

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

64. Jones R. Responses of male and female domestic chicks to a startling stimulus and the effects of a tranquilliser. Behav Process. (1980) 5:161–72. doi: 10.1016/0376-6357(80)90063-7

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

65. Suarez SD, Gallup GG. Predatory overtones of open-field testing in chickens. Learn Behav. (1981) 9:153–63. doi: 10.3758/BF03197812

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

66. Jones RB, Mills AD. Estimation of fear in two lines of the domestic chick: correlations between various methods. Behav Process. (1983) 8:243–53. doi: 10.1016/0376-6357(83)90015-3

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

67. Jones RB. The tonic immobility reaction of the domestic fowl: a review. Worlds Poult Sci J. (1986) 42:82–96. doi: 10.1079/WPS19860008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

68. Jones R. Sex and strain differences in the open-field responses of the domestic chick. Appl Anim Ethol. (1977) 3:255–61. doi: 10.1016/0304-3762(77)90006-2

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

69. Vallorttgara G, Zanforlin M. Open-field behavior of young chicks (Gallus gallus): antipredatory responses, social reinstatement motivation, and gender effects. Anim Learn Behav. (1988) 16:359–62. doi: 10.3758/BF03209088

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

70. Workman L, Andrew RJ. Simultaneous changes in behaviour and in lateralization during the development of male and female domestic chicks. Anim Behav. (1989) 38:596–605. doi: 10.1016/S0003-3472(89)80004-1

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

71. Vallortigara G, Cailotto M, Zanforlin M. Sex differences in social reinstatement motivation of the domestic chick (Gallus gallus) revealed by runway tests with social and nonsocial reinforcement. J Comp Psychol. (1990) 104:361. doi: 10.1037/0735-7036.104.4.361

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

72. Pittet F, Houdelier C, Lumineau S. Precocial bird mothers shape sex differences in the behavior of their chicks. J Exp Zool A: Ecol Integr Physiol. (2014) 321:265–75. doi: 10.1002/jez.1858

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

73. Marchewka J, Watanabe T, Ferrante V, Estevez I. Review of the social and environmental factors affecting the behavior and welfare of turkeys (Meleagris gallopavo). Poult Sci. (2013) 92:1467–73. doi: 10.3382/ps.2012-02943

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

74. Eising CM, Müller W, Dijkstra C, Groothuis TG. Maternal androgens in egg yolks: relation with sex, incubation time and embryonic growth. Gen Comp Endocrinol. (2003) 132:241–7. doi: 10.1016/S0016-6480(03)00090-X

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

75. Woods JE, Podczaski ES. Androgen synthesis in the gonads of the chick embryo. Gen Comp Endocrinol. (1974) 24:413–23. doi: 10.1016/0016-6480(74)90155-5

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

76. Ottinger MA, Vom Saal FS. Impact of environmental endocrine disruptors on sexual differentiation in birds and mammals. Horm Behav. (2002) 325:32. doi: 10.1016/B978-012532104-4/50070-6

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

77. Wentworth B, Hussein M. Serum corticosterone levels in embryos, newly hatched, and young turkey poults. Poult Sci. (1985) 64:2195–201. doi: 10.3382/ps.0642195

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

78. Woods JE, Simpson RM, Moore PL. Plasma testosterone levels in the chick embryo. Gen Comp Endocrinol. (1975) 27:543–47 doi: 10.1016/0016-6480(75)90076-3

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

79. Adkins-Regan E. Hormones and sexual differentiation of avian social behavior. Dev Neurosci. (2009) 31:342–50. doi: 10.1159/000216545

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

80. Badyaev AV. Growing apart: an ontogenetic perspective on the evolution of sexual size dimorphism. Trends Ecol. (2002) E17:369–78. doi: 10.1016/S0169-5347(02)02569-7

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

81. Pfannkuche KA, Bouma A, Groothuis TG. Does testosterone affect lateralization of brain and behaviour? A meta-analysis in humans and other animal species. Trans R Soc Lond, B, Biol. (2009) 364:929–42. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2008.0282

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

82. Güntürkün O, Hoferichter H-H. Neglect after section of a left telencephalotectal tract in pigeons. Behav Brain Res. (1985) 18:1–9 doi: 10.1016/0166-4328(85)90164-0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

83. Mench J, Andrew R. Lateralization of a food search task in the domestic chick. Behav Neural Biol. (1986) 46:107–14. doi: 10.1016/S0163-1047(86)90570-4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

84. Bright A. Plumage colour and feather pecking in laying hens, a chicken perspective? Br. Poult Sci. (2007) 48:253–63. doi: 10.1080/00071660701370483

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

85. Nie C, Ban L, Ning Z, Qu L. Feather colour affects the aggressive behaviour of chickens with the same genotype on the dominant white (I) locus. PLoS ONE. (2019) 14:e0215921. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0215921

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

86. Durosaro SO, Iyasere OS, Oguntade DO, Ilori BM, Odubola TA, Adewunmi AP, et al. Associations between plumage colour and fear behaviour in young Nigerian indigenous turkeys (Meleagris gallopavo). Appl Anim Behav Sci. (2021) 244:105483. doi: 10.1016/j.applanim.2021.105483

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

87. Taskin A, Karadavut U, Çayan H. Behavioural responses of white and bronze turkeys (Meleagris gallopavo) to tonic immobility, gait score and open field tests in free-range system. J Appl Anim Res. (2018) 46:1253–9. doi: 10.1080/09712119.2018.1495642

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

88. Cottle CA, Price EO. Effects of the nonagouti pelage-color allele on the behavior of captive wild Norway rats (Rattus norvegicus). J Comp Psychol. (1987) 101:390. doi: 10.1037/0735-7036.101.4.390

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

89. Trut LN. Early canid domestication: the farm-fox experiment. Scientifur. (2000) 24:124–124.

Google Scholar

90. West PM, Packer C. Sexual selection, temperature, and the lion's mane. Science. (2002) 297:1339–43. doi: 10.1126/science.1073257

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

91. Karlsson AC, Kerje S, Andersson L, Jensen P. Genotype at the PMEL17 locus affects social and explorative behaviour in chickens. Br Poult Sci. (2010) 51:170–77. doi: 10.1080/00071661003745802

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

92. Ducrest AL, Keller L, Roulin A. Pleiotropy in the melanocortin system, coloration and behavioural syndromes. Trends Ecol. (2008) E23:502–10. doi: 10.1016/j.tree.2008.06.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

93. Schütz KE, Kerje S, Jacobsson L, Forkman B, Carlborg Ö, Andersson L, et al. Major growth QTLs in fowl are related to fearful behavior: possible genetic links between fear responses and production traits in a red junglefowl × White Leghorn intercross. Behav Genet. (2004) 34:121–30. doi: 10.1023/B:BEGE.0000009481.98336.fc

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

94. Jensen P. Behavior genetics and the domestication of animals. Annu Rev Anim Biosci. (2014) 2:85–104. doi: 10.1146/annurev-animal-022513-114135

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

95. Kanginakudru S, Metta M, Jakati R, Nagaraju J. Genetic evidence from Indian red jungle fowl corroborates multiple domestication of modern day chicken. BMC Evol Biol. (2008) 8:1–14. doi: 10.1186/1471-2148-8-174

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

96. Thornton EK, Emery KF. The uncertain origins of Mesoamerican turkey domestication. J Archaeol Method Theory. (2017) 24:328–51. doi: 10.1007/s10816-015-9269-4

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

97. Bertin A, Richard-Yris MA. Mothers' fear of human affects the emotional reactivity of young in domestic Japanese quail. Appl Anim Behav Sci. (2004) 89:215–31. doi: 10.1016/j.applanim.2004.06.004

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

98. Schuètz KE, Jensen P. Effects of resource allocation on behavioural strategies: a comparison of red junglefowl (Gallus gallus) and two domesticated breeds of poultry. Ethology. (2001) 107:753–65. doi: 10.1046/j.1439-0310.2001.00703.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: external egg quality, poult morphological, sex determination, preincubation, post-hatching, native breed, behavior

Citation: Salgado Pardo JI, Navas González FJ, González Ariza A, Arando Arbulu A, León Jurado JM, Delgado Bermejo JV and Camacho Vallejo ME (2022) Traditional sexing methods and external egg characteristics combination allow highly accurate early sex determination in an endangered native turkey breed. Front. Vet. Sci. 9:948502. doi: 10.3389/fvets.2022.948502

Received: 19 May 2022; Accepted: 21 July 2022;
Published: 15 August 2022.

Edited by:

Abdel-Moneim Eid Abdel-Moneim, Egyptian Atomic Energy Authority, Egypt

Reviewed by:

Abdelrazeq M. Shehata, Al-Azhar University, Egypt
Mahmoud M. Alagawany, Zagazig University, Egypt
Shaaban Saad Elnesr, Fayoum University, Egypt

Copyright © 2022 Salgado Pardo, Navas González, González Ariza, Arando Arbulu, León Jurado, Delgado Bermejo and Camacho Vallejo. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Francisco Javier Navas González, fjng87@hotmail.com; Antonio González Ariza, angoarvet@outlook.es

Present Address: A. Arando Arbulu, NEIKER–Basque Institute of Agricultural Research and Development, Basque Research and Technology Alliance (BRTA), Arkaute, Spain

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.