- 1Physiologie de la Nutrition et du Comportement Alimentaire (PNCA), INRAE, Université Paris-Saclay, Paris, France
- 2The Roslin Institute, The Royal (Dick) School of Veterinary Studies, The University of Edinburgh, Edinburgh, United Kingdom
- 3Animal and Veterinary Sciences, Scotland's Rural College (SRUC), Edinburgh, United Kingdom
- 4Modélisation Systémique Appliquée aux Ruminants (MoSAR), INRAE, Université Paris-Saclay, Paris, France
- 5Universities Federation for Animal Welfare (UFAW), Wheathampstead, United Kingdom
Introduction
It has been over 20 years since the first scientific papers on rat tickling were published (1, 2). Rats were found to emit ultrasonic vocalizations in the 50-kHz range (hereafter referred to as USVs) when a human performed rapid manual stimulation on their dorsoventral region. Such tickling of rats by a human hand is trying to imitate the rough-and-tumble play seen in young rats of both sexes (3, 4). Emission of USVs by the rat indicates that it enjoys being tickled (5–7), as USVs have been linked with positive emotions (8, 9), are emitted in anticipation of, and during, social play (10, 11), and have been suggested to be homologous to human laughter (12).
To date, more than 70 scientific articles on rat tickling have been published1, and the consensus is that tickling induces positive emotions in rats. Indeed, in a systematic review, LaFollette et al. (13) found that tickling increased USVs and human hand approach behavior, and decreased measures of anxiety in rats. In this Opinion paper, we consider whether current methods of tickling overemphasize the use of pinning (Figure 1) to which there may be a wider response variation than commonly acknowledged. We do not dispute that tickling can be a positive way to handle juvenile rats, but tickling may not always be perceived as a positive interaction by the rat, and we raise the possibility that tickling methods need to be revised. In particular, we suggest incorporating more aspects of play during tickling (increased diversity) and adapting the method to individual rats' responses (increased flexibility) to achieve positive emotions and increased welfare across a wider cohort of rats.
Figure 1. (A) Pinning is a component of social play in rats, where one rat has its dorsal surface pinned to the ground, while the other rat is in a dominant posture above. (B,C) Pinning has become the main component of the methodology used when tickling rats, as it elicits 50-kHz ultrasonic vocalizations (USVs). Pinning has been used in the past as a means to quantify rough-and-tumble play between rats (14). However, rough-and-tumble play is much more than just pinning, and USVs during pinning are frequent when being tickled by a human (1), but less so when being pinned during play (10, 15). Drawings are by Tayla J. Hammond.
From Play to Tickling
Play is at the origin of tickling, and the inspiration for Jaak Panksepp to start tickling rats came from his study two decades earlier of the ontogeny of play in this species (14). Play behavior in any species is often complex and unpredictable by nature, making quantification challenging (16–19). By observing adolescent rats engaged in non-aggressive social interactions, Panksepp (14) found that the rats frequently ended up with one rat having their dorsal surface to the ground, with the other rat above holding down (pinning) the supine rat for 2–3 s. Based on these observations, he1 suggested that pinning was an easy and objective way to quantify social play in rats.
A comparison of different tickling methods was later published by Panksepp and Burgdorf (1), and the method that induced the most USVs in the 50-kHz range was a full-body tickling with repeated pinning. This was described as “vigorous whole-body playful tickling (focusing on the ribs and ventral surface), with animals being repeatedly pinned four to six times, throughout the fifteen-second interval” [p. 361, (1)]. This tickling paradigm (the Panksepp method) has been used widely and is currently employed by a number of different research groups [e.g., (20, 21)].
Panksepp and Burgdorf (1) used the expressions “playful tickling” and “heterospecific handplay” when referring to their rapid manual stimulation of rats by a human hand. This has also been called “heterospecific play” [e.g., (22)] and “playful handling” [e.g., (23)] and is generally referred to simply as “tickling” [e.g., (24)]. The frequent inclusion of the word “play” reflects the original purpose of tickling: to mimic the behavior displayed by rats during play. However, the Panksepp method involves the rat spending most of each 15-s tickling bout on its back, being pinned by the human hand while receiving vigorous finger movements on the ventral area. This contrasts with rat play, which is much more than just pinning, and includes scampering, chasing, sparring, pouncing, darting, dorsal grooming, and wrestling (10, 14, 25, 26).
We suggest that quantifying social play in rats by the occurrence of pinning has given rise to a rat tickling technique (the Panksepp method) that overemphasizes the use of pinning compared with rough-and-tumble play in rats.
The Role of Pinning
There are two main issues connected with the overuse of pinning during rat tickling. One is that pinning does not occur as frequently during social play as in the tickling method described above. During rough-and-tumble play, group housed rats perform fewer than 10 pins per pair during 5 min of play (14). The other more important issue is that pinning is also the pre-dominant form of rat-to-rat contact during serious fights between adult male rats (27) as frequency and duration of pinning is linked to dominance (14, 28–30), and role reversals are only seen in roughly 30% of play fights among juvenile rats across a variety of strains (31). Pinning during social play in rats has also been referred to as the consummatory phase of play (14).
The occurrence of pinning cannot be used to distinguish play fighting from serious fighting (32), and a supine position cannot be assumed to be submissive or aversive in the context of juvenile play. Play is a means for cooperation as well as competition (33), and most pins during rough-and-tumble play are the result of a full rotation defense tactic of the receiving (pinned) rat in response to a nape attack from the play partner (34). Thus, the adoption of a supine posture during social play is a choice; it peaks during juvenile play as the preferred defensive strategy and declines as rats mature, and rough-and-tumble play takes on a more serious nature (35–37). When pinning is used extensively during tickling, the supine position is not chosen by the rat, and pinning by a human hand is forced rather than offered. During social play, rats have been found to emit more USVs when pinning than when being pinned (15), suggesting that the pinning rat gets greater enjoyment than the pinned rat.
The experience of being pinned by another rat depends on the context of the social interaction (play or fight) and may vary with age. The supine position of the pinned rat during play is a choice, whereas for tickling, the posture of the rat during pinning is involuntary.
Individual Variability in Ultrasonic Vocalization During Tickling
Ticklishness varies in humans, but people who describe themselves as being extremely ticklish usually do not like it. Two types of tickling exist: knismesis, which is when a light touch or stroke evokes a shiver or a twitch, and gargalesis, which is a hard, rhythmic probing leading to an intense, often pleasurable sensation (38). Although gargalesis will almost always provoke laughter in humans, this is not necessarily an expression of pleasure, and many people find vigorous tickling aversive (39). Both types of tickling have been tested on rats, and when dorsal knismesis has been compared with gargalesis performed during pinning, the former elicits fewer USVs (6, 40). However, the type of dorsal knismesis used in these studies, i.e., repeated gentle touching of the rat's dorsal surface, is not comparable with the types of touch and movements experienced by rats during play.
Rats show large individual variability in their expression of 50-kHz USVs (21, 41, 42), with some rats not emitting any when tickled (1, 43). Using an affective bias test, Hinchcliffe et al. (21) demonstrated that USVs are correlated with the level of positive affect on a gradual scale, so that rats producing frequent USVs during tickling can be assumed to like it more than their less vocal conspecifics. High callers show positive cognitive bias when tested after tickling (44). Rats can be genetically selected based on their USV frequency during tickling (2, 41, 45), and rats selected for low USV emission display more pins during social play (46). This is robust evidence that tickling to induce positive emotions needs to take into account the individual response in the level of rat enjoyment from being tickled.
The enjoyment of being tickled varies among rats. The use of pinning may not always contribute positively to the tickling experience for all rats, and a tickling protocol with extensive use of pinning does not allow the rat to express the degree to which it finds the handling enjoyable or even aversive.
How Can We Simulate Play Better During Tickling?
The Panksepp method [systematized by (20) and (22)] is widely used and consists of structured and repeated pinning, which makes it difficult both to assess the affective state of the rat and adapt the tickling to its behavior. We would argue that the resemblance to rough-and-tumble play is lost when the method becomes predictable, follows a protocol, and does not take into account individual variation in the response of the rats. If pinning is aversive to some rats, the inclusion of more aspects of rat play when tickling would have a higher likelihood of being pleasurable for more rats. Schwarting et al. (43) was among the first to use different types of tactile manipulations to tickle rats. However, this study used adult rats, with tickling sessions lasting 10 min, and without adjusting the manipulations to the rat's response. In contrast, Bombail et al. (5) used various tactile manipulations resembling the elements of play listed in the section on From play to tickling, and with their type and vigor adjusted to the response of each rat, and found USVs increasing from 83 to 233 per minute, on average, from the first to the fifth playful handling session. In comparison, using the Panksepp method, Burgdorf et al. (45) recorded around 90 USVs/min during the fourth tickling session in their randomly selected line. This suggests that by making tickling more diverse and more flexible instead of focusing on pinning, we can cater for the likes (and avoid the dislikes) of more rats.
USVs during rough-and-tumble play in rats differ between different aspects of play, e.g., Kisko et al. (10) found significantly more USVs emitted during wrestling and chasing than during pinning. It is also yet unknown if USVs can be “forced” when using pinning during tickling in the same way as when humans cannot prevent laughing when being tickled forcefully, even if they do not like it. Several studies indicate that these vocalizations serve a function for the nature of play. Rats that cannot vocalize play less and with role reversals more than halved (47), and wrestling is reduced in play fighting between deaf rats (48). Burke et al. (49) found that some calls were associated with a particular play behavior. Certain calls affect the likelihood of different aspects of play occurring, indicating that some calls communicate specific information to the play partner (50). However, most studies of tickling do not report the type of vocalizations, only total USV counts. The USVs emitted during tickling are not in the range of human hearing, so without special equipment, the evaluation of the rat's enjoyment is based purely on the rat's behavioral response.
We suggest that more components observed during social play in rats are included during playful handling while reducing the use of pinning. This would be a more inclusive tickling method that is more playlike and likely to be a pleasant experience for more rats, including individuals that do not enjoy being pinned.
Conclusions and Perspectives
There is a clear evidence that the majority of rats enjoy playful handling by a human. The findings that (1) pinning frequency can be used to quantify play, (2) 50-kHz USVs are associated with enjoyment, and (3) using pinning when tickling provokes USVs may have led to tickling protocols focused on the use of pinning and the assumption that these are always enjoyable. We question this notion.
We suggest that the expression “playful handling” should be used when the interaction between the rat and the human hand resembles the full repertoire of rough-and-tumble play between two rats, i.e., manipulations involving some or all of dorsal and ventral tickling; chasing, sparring, and wrestling by both hand and rat, and which may include some pinning. It should be flexible and aim to be somewhat unpredictable (38), as the human participant gauge what the rat would find the most enjoyable. “Tickling” could then be used to describe the Panksepp method (20, 22), with repeated pinning and vigorous finger movements on the ventral area of the rat. This method would be a standardized treatment of a group of rats, similar to giving the same dosage of a drug to all subjects. However, such tickling does not have the same effect on all animals, and appropriate controls would therefore be difficult to generate.
Our description of playful handling may come across as subjective, but playful handling can be used scientifically, as demonstrated in a number of trials (5, 51, 52). Just like we adjust drug dosage to body weight, or train rats for different durations depending on their learning skills, maximizing enjoyment of playful handling requires adaptation to the individual rat's behavioral responses to interactions with the human hand. More diversity and flexibility in the tickle paradigm is called for, allowing the experimenter to respond sensitively to the rat's behavior with the aim to achieve equitable affective experiences for all rats. Taking individual variation into account is increasingly being promoted in the scientific literature, and by being more inclusive of individual differences, playful handling can improve the welfare of all rats in our care by actively promoting positive affective states (53–55).
Author Contributions
VB, BN, and AL initiated the discussion of the subject and the writing of this paper. SB, TH, SM, and ET contributed significantly to the discussion of the subject, and the development, writing, and final version of this paper. All authors contributed to the article and approved the submitted version.
Funding
SB, SM, and AL acknowledge the Roslin Institute strategic grant funding from the U.K. Biotechnology and Biological Sciences Research Council (BB/P013759/1). AL also acknowledges the funding support from the Scottish Government's Rural and Environment Science and Analytical Services Division (RESAS). VB acknowledges the funding support from the Institut National de Recherche pour l'Agriculture, l'Alimentation et l'Environnement (INRAE). ET is supported by the Principal's Career Development Scholarship from the University of Edinburgh.
Conflict of Interest
The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.
Acknowledgments
We wish to recognize the critical contribution of our colleague Aurélien Raynaud, who is involved in animal care, for his insights into the subjective experience of tickled rat. The authors are grateful to Sally Till, Peter Kind, and Raven Hickson for their useful comments on earlier versions of this article.
Footnotes
1. ^Web of Science search with [(rat OR rats) AND tickl*] in title, abstract, or keywords omitting meeting abstracts.
References
1. Panksepp J, Burgdorf J. Laughing rats? Playful tickling arouses high-frequency ultrasonic chirping in young rodents. In: Hameroff S, Kaszniak A, editors. Toward a Science of Consciousness (III). Cambridge D, MA: MIT Press (1999). p. 231–44.
2. Panksepp J, Burgdorf J. 50-kHz chirping (laughter?) in response to conditioned and unconditioned tickle-induced reward in rats: effects of social housing and genetic variables. Behav Brain Res. (2000) 115:25–38. doi: 10.1016/S0166-4328(00)00238-2
3. Cloutier S, Baker C, Wahl K, Panksepp J, Newberry RC. Playful handling as social enrichment for individually- and group-housed laboratory rats. Appl Anim Behav Sci. (2013) 143:85–95. doi: 10.1016/j.applanim.2012.10.006
4. Panksepp J, Burgdorf J. Laughing rats and the evolutionary antecedents of human joy? Physiol Behav. (2003) 79:533-47. doi: 10.1016/S0031-9384(03)00159-8
5. Bombail V, Jerôme N, Lam H, Muszlak S, Meddle SL, Lawrence AB, et al. Odour conditioning of positive affective states: rats can learn to associate an odour with being tickled. PLoS ONE. (2019) 14:e0212829. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0212829
6. Burgdorf J, Panksepp J. Tickling induces reward in adolescent rats. Physiol Behav. (2001) 72:167–73. doi: 10.1016/S0031-9384(00)00411-X
7. Knutson B, Burgdorf J, Panksepp J. Ultrasonic vocalizations as indices of affective states in rats. Psychol Bull. (2002) 128:961–77. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.128.6.961
8. Barker DJ. Ultrasonic vocalizations as an index of positive emotional state. In: Brudzynski S, editor. Handbook of Ultrasonic Vocalization: A Window into the Emotional Brain. Academic Press, Elsevier (2018). p. 253-60.
9. Burgdorf J, Kroes RA, Moskal JR, Pfaus JG, Brudzynski SM, Panksepp J. Ultrasonic vocalizations of rats (Rattus norvegicus) during mating, play, and aggression: behavioral concomitants, relationship to reward, and self-administration of playback. J Comp Psychol. (2008) 122:357–67. doi: 10.1037/a0012889
10. Kisko TM, Braun MD, Michels S, Witt SH, Rietschel M, Culmsee C, et al. Cacna1c haploinsufficiency leads to pro-social 50-kHz ultrasonic communication deficits in rats. Dis Model Mech. (2018) 11:dmm034116. doi: 10.1242/dmm.034116
11. Knutson B, Burgdorf J, Panksepp J. Anticipation of play elicits high-frequency ultrasonic vocalizations in young rats. J Comp Psychol. (1998) 112:65–73. doi: 10.1037/0735-7036.112.1.65
12. Panksepp J. The riddle of laughter: neural and psychoevolutionary underpinnings of joy. Curr Dir Psychol Sci. (2000) 9:183–6. doi: 10.1111/1467-8721.00090
13. LaFollette MR, O'Haire ME, Cloutier S, Blankenberger WB, Gaskill BN. Rat tickling: a systematic review of applications, outcomes, and moderators. PLoS ONE. (2017) 12:e0175320. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0175320
14. Panksepp J. The ontogeny of play in rats. Develop Psychobiol. (1981) 14:327–32. doi: 10.1002/dev.420140405
15. Kisko TM, Euston DR, Pellis SM. Are 50-kHz calls used as play signals in the playful interactions of rats? III. The effects of devocalization on play with unfamiliar partners as juveniles and as adults. Behav Process. (2015) 113:113–21. doi: 10.1016/j.beproc.2015.01.016
16. Ahloy-Dallaire J, Espinosa J, Mason G. Play and optimal welfare: does play indicate the presence of positive affective states? Behav Process. (2018) 156:3–15. doi: 10.1016/j.beproc.2017.11.011
17. Boissy A, Manteuffel G, Jensen MB, Moe R, Spruijt B, Keeling LJ, et al. Assessment of positive emotions in animals to improve their welfare. Physiol Behav. (2007) 92:375–97. doi: 10.1016/j.physbeh.2007.02.003
18. Newberry RC, Wood-Gush DGM, Hall JW. Playful behaviour of piglets. Behav Process. (1988) 17:205–16. doi: 10.1016/0376-6357(88)90004-6
19. Oliveira AFS, Rossi AO, Silva LFR, Lau MC, Barreto RE. Play behaviour in non-human animals and the animal welfare issue. J Ethol. (2010) 28:1–5. doi: 10.1007/s10164-009-0167-7
20. Cloutier S, LaFollette MR, Gaskill BN, Panksepp J, Newberry RC. Tickling, a technique for inducing positive affect when handling rats. J Vis Exp. (2018) 135:e57190. doi: 10.3791/57190
21. Hinchcliffe JK, Mendl M, Robinson ESJ. Rat 50 kHz calls reflect graded tickling-induced positive emotion. Curr Biol. (2020) 30:R1034–5. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2020.08.038
22. LaFollette MR, O'Haire ME, Cloutier S, Gaskill BN. Practical rat tickling: determining an efficient and effective dosage of heterospecific play. Appl Anim Behav Sci. (2018) 208:82-91. doi: 10.1016/j.applanim.2018.08.005
23. Cloutier S, Wahl KL, Panksepp J, Newberry RC. Playful handling of laboratory rats is more beneficial when applied before than after routine injections. Appl Anim Behav Sci. (2015) 164:81–90. doi: 10.1016/j.applanim.2014.12.012
24. Ishiyama S, Kaufmann LV, Brecht M. Behavioral and cortical correlates of self-suppression, anticipation, and ambivalence in rat tickling. Curr Biol. (2019) 29:3153–64. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2019.07.085
25. Panksepp J, Siviy S, Normansell L. The psychobiology of play: theoretical and methodological perspectives. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. (1984) 8:465–92. doi: 10.1016/0149-7634(84)90005-8
26. Pellis SM, Pellis VC. Rough-and-tumble play and the development of the social brain. Curr Dir Psychol Sci. (2007) 16:95–8. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8721.2007.00483.x
27. Boice R, Adams N. Degrees of captivity and aggressive-behavior in domestic Norway rats. Bull Psychonom Soc. (1983) 21:149-52. doi: 10.3758/BF03329980
28. Löfgren M, Johansson M, Strömberg J, Meyerson B, Bäckström T. The influence of social subordinate housing on the withdrawal effects from progesterone and estradiol in male rats. Gen Comp Endocr. (2012) 177:62–9. doi: 10.1016/j.ygcen.2012.02.011
29. Panksepp J, Jalowiec J, DeEskinazi FG, Bishop P. Opiates and play dominance in juvenile rats. Behav Neurosci. (1985) 99:441–53. doi: 10.1037/0735-7044.99.3.441
30. Parent CI, Del Corpo A, Cameron NM, Meaney MJ. Maternal care associates with play dominance rank among adult female rats. Dev Psychobiol. (2013) 55:745-56. doi: 10.1002/dev.21070
31. Himmler SM, Himmler BT, Pellis VC, Pellis SM. Play, variation in play and the development of socially competent rats. Behaviour. (2016) 153:1103–37. doi: 10.1163/1568539X-00003307
32. Pellis SM, Pellis VC. Play fighting differs from serious fighting in both target of attack and tactics of fighting in the laboratory rat Rattus norvegicus. Aggress Behav. (1987) 13:227–42. doi: 10.1002/1098-2337(1987)13
33. Bauer EB, Smuts BB. Cooperation and competition during dyadic play in domestic dogs, Canis familiaris. Anim Behav. (2007) 73:489–99. doi: 10.1016/j.anbehav.2006.09.006
34. Himmler BT, Pellis VC, Pellis SM. Peering into the dynamics of social interactions: measuring play fighting in rats. J Vis Exp. (2013) 71:e4288. doi: 10.3791/4288
35. Otterbein S, Borta A, Schwarting RKW. Monitoring of rough-and-tumble play and its relationship to ultrasound vocalization. J Psychophysiol. (2005) 19:135–6. doi: 10.1027/0269-8803.19.2.106
36. Pellis SM. Sex differences in play fighting revisited: traditional and nontraditional mechanisms of sexual differentiation in rats. Arch Sex Behav. (2002) 31:17–26. doi: 10.1023/a:1014070916047
37. Pellis SM, Pellis VC. Differential rates of attack, defence, and counterattack during the developmental decrease in play fighting by male and female rats. Dev Psychobiol. (1990) 23:215–31. doi: 10.1002/dev.420230303
38. Leavens DA, Bard KA. Quick guide: tickling. Curr Biol. (2016) 26:R91–3. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2015.06.014
39. Harris CR. Tickling. In: Ramachandran VS, editor. Encyclopedia of Human Behavior, 2nd ed. Academic Press, Elsevier (2012). pp. 611-615.
40. Hori M, Shimoju R, Tokunaga R, Ohkubo M, Miyabe S, Ohnishi J. Tickling increases dopamine release in the nucleus accumbens and 50 kHz ultrasonic vocalizations in adolescent rats. NeuroReport. (2013) 24:241–5. doi: 10.1097/WNR.0b013e32835edbfa
41. Burgdorf J, Panksepp J, Brudzynski SM, Beinfeld MC, Cromwell HC, Kroes RA, et al. The effects of selective breeding for differential rates of 50-kHz ultrasonic vocalizations on emotional behavior in rats. Develop Psychobiol. (2009) 51:34–46. doi: 10.1002/dev.20343
42. Sangarapillai N, Ellenberger M, Wöhr M, Schwarting RKW. Ultrasonic vocalizations and individual differences in rats performing a Pavlovian conditioned approach task. Behav Brain Res. (2021) 398:112926. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2020.112926
43. Schwarting RKW, Jegan N, Wöhr M. Situational factors, conditions and individual variables which can determine ultrasonic vocalizations in male adult Wistar rats. Behav Brain Res. (2007) 182:208–22. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2007.01.029
44. Rygula R, Pluta H, Popik P. Laughing rats are optimistic. PLoS ONE. (2012) 7:e51959. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0051959
45. Burgdorf J, Panksepp J, Brudzynski SM, Kroes R, Moskal JR. Breeding for 50-kHz positive affective vocalization in rats. Behav Gen. (2005) 35:67–72. doi: 10.1007/s10519-004-0856-5
46. Webber ES, Harmon KM, Beckwith TJ, Peña S., et al. Selective breeding for 50 kHz ultrasonic vocalization emission produces alterations in the ontogeny and regulation of rough-and-tumble play. Behav Brain Res. (2012) 229:138–44. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2012.01.012
47. Kisko TM, Himmler BT, Himmler SM, Euston DR, Pellis SM. Are 50-kHz calls used as play signals in the playful interactions of rats? II. Evidence from the effects of devocalization. Behav Process. (2015) 111:25–33. doi: 10.1016/j.beproc.2014.11.011
48. Siviy SM, Panksepp J. Sensory modulation of juvenile play in rats. Dev Psychobiol. (1987) 20:39–55. doi: 10.1002/dev.420200108
49. Burke CJ, Euston DR, Pellis SM. What do you hear, what do you say? Ultrasonic calls as signals during play fighting in rats. Int J Play. (2020) 9:92–107. doi: 10.1080/21594937.2020.1720126
50. Kisko TM, Wöhr M, Pellis VC, Pellis SM. From play to aggression: High-frequency 50 kHz vocalizations as play and appeasement signals in rats. Curr Top Behav Neurosci. (2017) 30:91–108. doi: 10.1007/7854_2015_432
51. Hammond T, Bombail V, Nielsen BL, Meddle SL, Lawrence AB, Brown SM. Relationships between play and responses to tickling in male juvenile rats. Appl Anim Behav Sci. (2019) 221:104879. doi: 10.1016/j.applanim.2019.104879
52. Tivey E, Brown S, Lawrence AB, Meddle SL. Sex differences in the behavioural response to tickling in juvenile Wistar rats. In: Proceedings of the 53rd Congress of the ISAE. Bergen (2019). p. 158.
53. Lawrence AB, Newberry RC, Špinka M. Positive welfare: what does it add to the debate over pig welfare? In: Špinka M, editor. Advances in Pig Welfare. Cambridge, MA: Woodhead Publishing (2018). p. 415-44.
54. Lawrence AB, Vigors B, Sandøe P. What is so positive about positive animal welfare? – A critical review of the literature. Animals. (2019) 9:783. doi: 10.3390/ani9100783
Keywords: pinning, playful handling, rough-and-tumble play, social play, rats, tickling, USV
Citation: Bombail V, Brown SM, Hammond TJ, Meddle SL, Nielsen BL, Tivey EKL and Lawrence AB (2021) Crying With Laughter: Adapting the Tickling Protocol to Address Individual Differences Among Rats in Their Response to Playful Handling. Front. Vet. Sci. 8:677872. doi: 10.3389/fvets.2021.677872
Received: 08 March 2021; Accepted: 14 May 2021;
Published: 24 June 2021.
Edited by:
Janice Siegford, Michigan State University, United StatesReviewed by:
Sara Hintze, University of Natural Resources and Life Sciences Vienna, AustriaTheresa Kisko, University of Marburg, Germany
Copyright © 2021 Bombail, Brown, Hammond, Meddle, Nielsen, Tivey and Lawrence. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.
*Correspondence: Birte L. Nielsen, nielsen@ufaw.org.uk