Skip to main content

BRIEF RESEARCH REPORT article

Front. Sports Act. Living, 14 October 2024
Sec. Sport Psychology

A single session of coordinative motor training does not improve spatial ability performances in healthy children

  • Division of Movement and Training Sciences/Biomechanics of Sport, University of Duisburg-Essen, Essen, Germany

Background: In recent years, studies have found small-to-medium positive effects of physical activity on academic achievement. Already acute bouts of exercise appear to improve certain cognitive functions. Spatial abilities are one aspect of cognition that is encountered frequently in daily life and that is closely related to success in science, technology, engineering, and mathematics (STEM)-subjects. However, little is known about the effects of an acute exercise session on spatial abilities. The aim of this study was therefore to evaluate the effect of a single session of coordinative motor training (CMT) on spatial ability performances in healthy children.

Methods: Forty-nine children were assigned to either a single session of CMT (i.e., obstacle course with motor coordinative and spatial elements) (n = 25, 12 females, mean age: 10.7 ± 0.6 years) or a resting control group (n = 24, 12 females, mean age ± SD: 11.4 ± 0.5 years). Spatial abilities were evaluated in both groups using the Paper Folding Test (PFT), Mental Rotation Test (MRT), Water Level Task (WLT), Corsi Block Test (CBT), and Numbered Cones Run (NCR).

Results: A statistical main effect for Test was observed for the majority of outcomes (i.e., all but the MRT). Test × Group interactions did not reach the level of significance.

Conclusion: The results indicate that a single session of CMT does not improve spatial ability performances of healthy children. Future research should evaluate whether repeated longer-term interventions might be more suitable to generate significant improvements in spatial abilities.

1 Introduction

The positive effects of physical activity and exercise on physical and mental health as well as quality of life are recognized widely (13). Not only does the risk for cardiovascular disease, diabetes, obesity, and musculoskeletal disorders decrease with increased physical activity, there also appear to be benefits for cognitive functions and brain health (35).

In recent years, a variety of studies have investigated the connections between physical activity and aspects of academic achievements in school aged children and adolescents indicating a small-to-medium positive relation between the two factors (e.g., 68). Considering the deteriorating results of the recent OECD PISA-study 2022 in combination with global health problems like overweight and noncommunicable disease, this positive association could be a way to address several of today's problems at once (9, 10).

Spatial abilities are one aspect of complex cognitive function that is not only encountered frequently in daily life (e.g., wayfinding, problem solving or map reading) (11, 12) but is also closely linked to academic achievements, particularly to success in STEM-subjects (i.e., science, technology, engineering, mathematics) (1315). Spatial abilities should therefore receive particular attention in the school curriculum (16, 17).

Starting to develop from early infancy on, maturation of spatial abilities continues right into adolescence and appears to be closely linked to the ongoing development of motor abilities, sensory systems, and brain maturation (18, 19). Increasing motor independence allows infants new ways to explore and interact with their environments which aids spatial development (e.g., spatial knowledge and perception) (19, 20). The development of spatial orientation in particular has been linked to the maturation of hippocampal and parahippocampal areas. Moreover, the frontal (i.e., dorsolateral prefrontal, medial and superior frontal lobe), temporal (i.e., inferior, superior and medial temporal lobe), parietal (i.e., superior and inferior parietal lobe) and occipital (i.e., precuneus) lobe, cingulate (i.e., retrosplenial cortex) and motor (i.e., premotor cortex) cortices as well as subcortical structures (i.e., thalamus, striatum, cerebellum) appear to be relevant for spatial orientation (12, 18, 2123). While there is a large overlap in brain activation patterns of children and adults regarding spatial orientation and navigation abilities, children and adolescents show increased central nervous system (CNS) activity (e.g., in brain areas related to visuospatial functioning) during navigational activities compared to adults (18, 24, 25). This might indicate that CNS structures are still maturing and neural connectivity as well as efficient use and switch between orientation strategies (e.g., allocentric or egocentric) still need to be acquired or enhanced (18, 24).

While it has been shown that spatial abilities can be trained using paper-and-pencil and computer-based tasks (17, 26) recent research also found that coordinative motor training (CMT) approaches lasting for several weeks positively impact on children's spatial abilities (2730). Jansen and Richter (31) further detected that a single session of creative dance training (i.e., training of orientation in space) resulted in superior mental rotation (i.e., one aspect of spatial abilities) than regular PE-class in second graders (n = 64; mean age ± SD: 7.09 ± 0.73 years).

In line with these findings, a positive impact of single exercise sessions on cognitive functions and based on this also on academic achievements have been reported by several researchers (e.g., 3135). The adaptive mechanisms underlying theses positive effects, however, are not entirely understood to date and studies examining the neurophysiological backgrounds are limited and vary considerably in methodology (36). Research in children suggests that acute bouts of exercise may cause functional adaptations in the CNS like prompt neurochemical responses improving cognition (e.g., growth factors like brain derived neurotrophic factors) or different functional brain activation patterns (for reviews see 2, 37, 38). Regarding training content, Voelcker-Rehage and Niemann (39) and Meijer et al. (40) point out that there is a significant research gap when it comes to investigating the effect of CMT (acute and chronic) on CNS adaptations (functional and structural) and cognitive functions with the majority of research implementing cardiovascular interventions.

An exemption is the study by Mochizuki and Kirino (41) evaluating the neural activation patterns (fMRI) during a single session of watching or performing coordination exercises compared to control exercises. Results indicate a noticeable overlap between CNS structures addressed with these coordination exercises and those required for the conduction of spatial orientation or navigation tasks. These findings are supported by studies performing longer term CMT (e.g., 42, 43). Particularly, parietal and occipital areas, the prefrontal cortex, premotor areas, and subcortical structures seem to be involved in spatial and coordinative activities (39, 4143). When evaluating the effect of acute CMT on attention and concentration, Budde et al. (44) further suggest that CMT is associated with higher cerebellar and frontal lobe activation, structures also involved in spatial orientation.

With many children spending a majority of their time in school or childcare facilities, these environments are essential to provide cognitively stimulating surroundings and promote physical activity as well as academic success (45). However, time is limited, school curricula have little flexibility and many subjects and socioeconomic topics require consideration (46). It is therefore of great interest to pursue research on the effect of physical activity interventions (e.g., CMT) that are little time-consuming (i.e., single bouts of exercise) on specific cognitive functions like spatial abilities and academic success.

The purpose of this brief research report is to evaluate the effect of a single session of CMT (i.e., obstacle course with motor coordinative and spatial elements) on spatial abilities of healthy children. Previous research has already shown positive effects of a longer-term coordinative motor intervention on spatial abilities using the same measurement tools in the same population (30). Building on this, in a next step we planned to evaluate the effects of an acute intervention. Initial evidence by Jansen and Richter (31) has shown beneficial effects of an acute motor training intervention on mental rotation (i.e., one aspect of spatial abilities) in healthy children. Moreover, previous research finds overlapping brain activation patterns regarding spatial orientation and CMT indicating that activation of these areas during CMT could also improve spatial functions by e.g., aiding functional connectivity, increasing cerebral blood flow or neurochemical adaptations (4044, 47). Based on these findings and aiming to contribute to this still relatively limited field of research, we hypothesized that participation in a single session (i.e., 30 min) of CMT would result in superior spatial ability performance compared to a resting control group (i.e., no training at all) as measured by five established spatial ability tests. Brief active movement breaks during cognitively challenging subjects and breaks between classes that encourage movement are already used within the school context to facilitate concentration and attention, improve posture and promote general motor skills (48, 49). They are, however, not entirely established yet and can be subject to objections and fear of loss of control in educational staff (49). Structured approaches like this acute CMT might have the potential to generate additional effects on specific cognitive functions like spatial abilities and thereby contribute not only to health, wellbeing and better learning conditions but also address academic achievement and improve the currently limited base of empirical evidence on this topic.

2 Methods

2.1 Participants

Using G*Power (version 3.1.9.7) the power analysis (f = 0.29, α = 0.05, 1−β = 0.80, number of groups: n = 2, number of measurements: n = 2, correlation between testing: r = 0.40, drop-out rate per group: 10% due to reasons not attributable to treatments) was calculated from a previous study on acute effects of training on spatial ability in children (31) and revealed that a total sample size of N = 36 participants would be sufficient to detect medium-sized treatment effects (50). A total of forty-nine healthy children participated in this study and were randomly assigned to an intervention group (n = 25, 12 females, 13 males, mean age ± SD: 10.7 ± 0.6 years) or a control group (n = 24, 12 females, 12 males, mean age ± SD: 11.4 ± 0.5 years). All participants attended the fifth grade of public secondary schools in the Ruhr area of North Rhine-Westphalia, Germany. The training intervention and assessments were unknown to the participants. All participants and their legal guardians provided informed consent before entering the study. The study protocol was approved by the Human Ethics Committee of the University of Duisburg-Essen, Faculty of Educational Sciences (EA-PSY20/23/04102023).

2.2 Experimental design

The study was conducted within a school setting. Group allocation was performed by drawing lots and based on class affiliation. The class assigned to be the intervention group (INT) performed a single 30-minute CMT, while the class assigned to be the resting control group (CON) performed no training at all (Figure 1). Before and after the intervention or resting a spatial ability assessment consisting of five established spatial ability tests was conducted with the participants from both groups. The assessment procedure (i.e., pretest and posttest) was spread over three testing days and took place in the mornings. On the first day, three paper-and-pencil assessments were performed with the entire class in the classroom. A computer-based test was conducted on the second day. It was instructed in small groups of five participants and then carried out individually in a separate room. Lastly, on the third day, a motoric test was instructed and conducted in small groups of five participants in the school gym. A graduated sports scientist conducted the testing procedures. The single training session was implemented by a second graduated sport scientist.

Figure 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Schematic description of the study design.

2.3 Measures

The spatial ability assessment took place on three testing days and included three paper-and-pencil measures (i.e., Paper Folding Test [PFT], Mental Rotation Test [MRT] and Water Level Task [WLT]), one computer-based measure [i.e., Corsi Block Test (CBT)], and one motoric test [i.e., Numbered Cones Run (NCR)]. The testing procedure was identical to the one used in our previous work (30). A brief depiction of the measures applied can be found in Figure 2. All measures were proven to be reliable for the age group presented in this study [i.e., PFT: intraclass correlation coefficient (ICC) = 0.78, MRT: ICC = 0.81, WLT: ICC = 0.88, CBT span: ICC = 0.95, CBT composite score: ICC = 0.93, NCR: ICC = 0.91] (51).

Figure 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Schematic depiction of the spatial ability measures.

2.4 Intervention

The intervention was conducted during regular PE-class and consisted of brief warm-up (5–10 min) followed by a main part that was conducted for approximately 20 min. During the warm-up period, students first had to find the best path through eight hoops that were distributed on a square of 10 m × 10 m. Then, paths of fellow students had to be replicated. There were four different starting points. The warm-up was based on suggestions for skill-oriented training of spatial orientation by Hirtz et al. (52) as well as the LifeKinetik® program by Lutz (53). The main CMT consisted of an obstacle course that had to be completed ten to twelve times by each participant in different running directions (i.e., running forward, backward, and sideways) (Figure 3). Participants were asked to focus on movement accuracy rather than speed. Compliance was visually monitored and participants were verbally motivated by the graduated sports scientist conducting the intervention. The obstacle course consisted of coordination exercises with spatial elements that were derived from various tests for motor abilities and coordination (e.g., Hamburg Parcours, Koko-Test, Swiss Cross, Mini-Biathlon) (52, 5456). Tasks included amongst others rolling sideways, forwards, and backwards, slalom running, climbing over, and passing under obstacles, balancing, running with changes of direction, target throwing, jumping, rotations, and single leg landing.

Figure 3
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 3. Schematic depiction of the intervention.

2.5 Statistical analysis

Data were presented as group mean value ± SD. For all analyses, assumptions of normality (Shapiro–Wilk Test) and homogeneity of variance/sphericity (Mauchly Test) were checked and met prior to conducting parametric analyses. Afterwards, performance differences between groups at the pretest were checked and detected for the WLT, CBT CS, and NCR. Consequently, a series of baseline-adjusted 2 (test: pretest, posttest) × 2 (group: INT, CON) repeated measures ANOVA were performed. Where significant Test × Group interactions were detected, post hoc analyses (i.e., paired t-test) using Bonferroni-adjusted α determined the location of any differences. The significance level was a priori set at α < 0.05. For the ANOVA, effect size was calculated as partial eta-squared (ηp2) and reported as small (0.02 ≤ ηp2 ≤ 0.12), medium (0.13 ≤ ηp2 ≤ 0.25), or large (ηp2 ≥ 0.26) (57). For the paired t-test, effect size was calculated as Cohen's d and stated as small (0 ≤ d < 0.50), medium (0.50 ≤ d < 0.80), or large (d ≥ 0.80) (57). All analyses were performed using SPSS version 28.0 (IBM Inc., Chicago, IL).

3 Results

Table 1 displays descriptive and inference statistics for all analysed variables. Baseline differences between groups were observed for the WLT, the CBT CS, and the NCR. The baseline-adjusted statistical analysis showed significant main effects of Test for all but one outcome (i.e., MRT: p = 0.095), indicating improvements from the pretest to the posttest, irrespective of group. However, the Test × Group interactions did not reach the level of significance (i.e., PFT: p = 0.492; MRT: p = 0.051; WLT: p = 0.622; CBT span: p = 0.497; CBT CS: p = 0.341; NCR: p = 0.688). This indicates that no group-specific changes occurred from the pretest to the posttest. Additionally performed gender-specific sub-analysis did not reveal any significant Test × Group interactions (data not shown).

Table 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Acute effects of a single coordinative motor training session on measures of spatial ability performance in healthy children by group.

4 Discussion

The aim of this brief research report was to investigate the effect of a single session of CMT on spatial ability performances of healthy children. It was hypothesized that participation in the CMT would have superior effects on spatial abilities than no training at all as measured by five established spatial ability tests. The results, however, do not support our hypothesis. No significant Test × Group interactions could be observed in the present study, indicating that changes in spatial ability performance from pretest to posttest cannot be attributed to either INT or CON.

Overall, many studies evaluate the effect of acute bouts of exercise on cognition in children or adolescents. However, many of those focus on executive functions with some others analyzing cognitive functions like e.g., attention, working memory or information processing (e.g., 33, 5861). To our knowledge, though, only one study investigates the effect of an acute bout of exercise (i.e., creative dance training) on spatial abilities (i.e., mental rotation) in children (31). Comparable studies in different populations are also scarce. Only one study evaluating the effect of either 45 min of physical activity (i.e., running, jumping, rope skipping, and callisthenic) or a lecture on kinematics on mental rotation performance in healthy young adults could be identified (62). Comparison of our findings to other research in the field is therefore limited. While both studies mentioned above reveal that the exercise interventions resulted in superior mental rotation performance than the control conditions, mental rotation was only one of the spatial abilities tested in the present research. Moreover, participant age, differences in testing procedures and tools used to evaluate mental rotation as well as intervention content and duration [i.e., 60 min (31) and 45 min (62) respectively vs. 30 min (present study)] need to be taken into consideration. Even though both studies can be considered an indication that acute bouts of exercise might influence mental rotation, it is impossible to predict the impact of acute bouts of exercise on other spatial abilities. More high-quality studies are needed in this research field that focus on dose-response relationships in association with participant age and the type of cognitive function measured.

The CMT performed in the present study might further not have been specific or challenging enough to generate significant improvements in spatial abilities. Generally, cognitively more challenging types of exercise or open skill exercises (OSE) appear to yield superior results to closed skill exercises (CSE), however, research regarding the role of exercise type on cognitive abilities is still inconclusive (2, 58, 63). OSE are characterized by unpredictable or dynamic environments and have higher cognitive demands as they require extrinsic abilities, decision making, and reaction to present situations, whereas CSE can be identified as motor tasks performed in stable and predictable environments that are cognitively less demanding (64, 65). Environmental requirements in the present study were rather predictable for the participants, particularly since they completed the course ten to twelve times each. It can therefore be considered a CSE. OSE focusing on coordination might thus have been more efficient in improving spatial abilities. At the same time O’Brien et al. (60) reported that a single session of OSE resulted in improved verbal working memory and a single session of CSE improved motor working memory. However, neither benefitted visuospatial working memory as measured by a computerized CBT, which is one of the measurement tools also used in the present study.

Even though CMT parameters might be more complex to regulate than other motor skills (e.g., strength, endurance, flexibility), it is indispensable to establish a monitoring system to control intervention parameters. In the present study, participants’ performance on the CMT was controlled in a quantitative as well as qualitative way. Quantitative control was based on the following parameters: training duration (i.e., 20 min), frequency (i.e., 10–12 rounds/participant), duration per round (i.e., 90–120 s), intensity (i.e., moderate speed), and rest between rounds (i.e., 0–30 s). Qualitative control was executed by the graduated sports scientist conducting the CMT. Participants’ compliance was visually monitored and maintenance of performance was encouraged verbally.

Still, exercise intensity might be a determining factor for spatial ability outcomes. Even though, participants were asked to complete the obstacle course rather accurate than fast, no precise statement can be made about exercise intensity, as we did not control participants’ heart rate (HR). Neither Jansen and Richter (31) nor Jansen and Pietsch (62) provide detailed information on exercise intensity for their creative dance and their physical activity interventions respectively. Research evaluating the effect of acute bouts of exercise on other cognitive functions are not conclusive in terms of exercise intensity and outcomes (4). Hillman et al. (33) found positive effects on cognitive control and attention after 20 min treadmill walking at an estimated 60% of the maximum HR in healthy children. Similarly, positive effects on some executive functions were found by Chen et al. (34) after either jogging for 30 min at moderate intensity (60%–70% of maximum HR) or reading. Budde et al. (44) discuss superior effects on a concentration and attention task when comparing coordinative exercises with a normal sports lesson in adolescents performed at approximately 60% of the maximum HR. van den Berg et al. (66) on the other hand reported that adolescents (mean age ± SD: 11.7 ± 0.7 years) performing either 12 min of aerobic, coordination or strength exercises at comparable or slightly lower intensities did not improve information processing and selective attention. Also Bedard et al. (67) and Best (68) were not able to determine superior outcomes of exergaming interventions at 75% and 70%–80% of the maximal HR respectively over control conditions on executive functions in children (mean age ± SD: 7.04 ± 1.37 years and 8.1 ± 1.3 years respectively). More research is needed to evaluate the optimal dose-response relationship with regard to the participants’ age. Moreover future research needs to take a closer look at different training intensities and the particular cognitive functions targeted at theses intensities (69).

The results further showed a main effect of Test for the majority of outcomes measured (i.e., all but the MRT), indicating that performance on those spatial ability measures improved significantly from pretest to posttest for children in the INT and in the CON. As there appears to be no superiority of the intervention, findings might be suggestive of retest effects. Retest effects are common in cognitive testing and stand for improvements in test results when repeatedly performing the same test (70, 71). While retest effects can be reduced by longer test-retest intervals, they cannot be prevented completely and therefore need to be taken into account when interpreting study results as otherwise findings are easily overestimated (70). Previous studies have already reported retest effects on some of the measures used in this study (i.e., PFT and CBT) (72, 73). Even though retest effects also occurred in one of our previous studies, we were still able to demonstrate that all spatial ability measures applied in the present study are reliable and can be used to detect intervention changes in the represented age- and population group (51). Based on the results, one can therefore assume that the acute CMT intervention did not or only marginally impact on the participant's spatial ability performance.

4.1 Limitations and directions for future research

Several limitations of the present research need to be taken into consideration for future research. Only healthy children participated in this study. Results can therefore neither be generalized to other age groups or populations nor to other types of exercise or assessments.

Although the present study was adequately powered (i.e., 80%) to investigate its hypothesis, the relatively small sample does limit the generalisability of the findings. Studies that replicate and extend our findings could increase the statistical power and thus the probability of detecting an effect by increasing the sample size.

To date there is no standardized way to evaluate the effect of acute bouts of exercise on cognitive and particularly spatial abilities and studies vary widely in methodology. Controlling parameters like HR, ratings of perceived exertion or step counts could help to gain deeper insights into exercise intensities. Other moderating variables like dosage, timing of the testing procedure and the type of cognitive demand tested also need to be taken into consideration when conducting future research (32, 69, 74, 75).

Even though the suitability of the applied spatial ability measures for this age group was established previously (51), tests might not have been specific enough to capture improvements in spatial abilities resulting from the CMT. For future research, more appropriate standardized, established and accessible measures might be determined that allow reproducible evaluation of spatial abilities after acute motor training interventions.

Moreover, little is known about the sustainability of acute exercise effects on cognitive functions and specifically spatial abilities. Most studies evaluate the effects immediately after the intervention and only few researchers conduct a follow-up to establish how long potential immediate effects last (76). Future research should take follow-up assessments into consideration to identify the efficiency and durability of acute exercise effects on cognitive functions.

5 Conclusion

In conclusion, the hypothesis that a single session of CMT improves spatial ability performances of healthy children could not be confirmed. To date, little is known about the optimal intervention content dosage, timing of testing, cognitive demands tested, measurement tools or sustainability in relation to single bouts of exercise and cognitive functions. Future research should focus on these topics as findings may be of great value to tackle sedentary behavior and at the same time foster complex cognitive functions like spatial abilities. The rejection of our hypothesis suggests that repeated longer-term interventions might be more suitable to generate significant improvements in spatial abilities, which implies an additional field of research.

Data availability statement

The raw data supporting the conclusions of this article will be made available by the authors, without undue reservation.

Ethics statement

The studies involving humans were approved by Human Ethics Committee of the University of Duisburg-Essen, Faculty of Educational Sciences. The studies were conducted in accordance with the local legislation and institutional requirements. Written informed consent for participation in this study was provided by the participants’ legal guardians/next of kin.

Author contributions

CM: Writing – review & editing, Writing – original draft, Methodology, Data curation, Conceptualization. AW: Writing – review & editing, Data curation. TK: Writing – review & editing, Data curation. TM: Writing – review & editing, Methodology, Formal Analysis, Data curation, Conceptualization.

Funding

The author(s) declare financial support was received for the research, authorship, and/or publication of this article. We acknowledge support by the Open Access Publication Fund of the University of Duisburg-Essen. The funding body is independent of the design of the study and collection, analysis, and interpretation of data and in writing the manuscript. Open Access funding enabled and organized by Project DEAL.

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher's note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

Abbreviations

CBT, Corsi Block Tests; CMT, coordinative motor training; CNS, central nervous system; CON, control group; CS, composite sore; CSE, closed skill exercise; ERP, effect related potential; fMRI, functional magnetic resonance imaging; HR, heart rate; ICC, intraclass correlation coefficient; INT, intervention group; MRT, Mental Rotation Test; NCR, Numbered Cones Run; OSE, open skill exercise; PE, physical education; PFT, Paper Folding Tests; SD, standard deviation; WLT, Water Level Task.

References

1. Penedo FJ, Dahn JR. Exercise and well-being: a review of mental and physical health benefits associated with physical activity. Curr Opin Psychiatry. (2005) 18(2):189–93. doi: 10.1097/00001504-200503000-00013

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

2. Best JR. Effects of physical activity on children’s executive function: contributions of experimental research on aerobic exercise. Dev Rev. (2010) 30(4):331–51. doi: 10.1016/j.dr.2010.08.001

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

3. World Health Organisation. Global Status Report on Physical Activity 2022. Geneva: World Health Organization (2022).

Google Scholar

4. Donnelly JE, Hillman CH, Castelli D, Etnier JL, Lee S, Tomporowski P, et al. Physical activity, fitness, cognitive function, and academic achievement in children: a systematic review. Med Sci Sports Exerc. (2016) 48(6):1197–222. doi: 10.1249/MSS.0000000000000901

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

5. Bidzan-Bluma I, Lipowska M. Physical activity and cognitive functioning of children: a systematic review. Int J Environ Res Public Health. (2018) 15:800. doi: 10.3390/ijerph15040800

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

6. Alvarez-Bueno C, Hillman C, Cavero-Redondo I, Sánchez-López M, Pozuelo-Carrascosa D, Martinez-Vizcaino V. Aerobic fitness and academic achievement: a systematic review and meta-analysis. J Sports Sci. (2020) 38:1–8. doi: 10.1080/02640414.2020.1720496

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

7. Barbosa A, Whiting S, Simmonds P, Scotini Moreno R, Mendes R, Breda J. Physical activity and academic achievement: an umbrella review. Int J Environ Res Public Health. (2020) 17(16):5972. doi: 10.3390/ijerph17165972

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

8. James J, Pringle A, Mourton S, Roscoe CMP. The effects of physical activity on academic performance in school-aged children: a systematic review. Children. (2023) 10(6):1019. doi: 10.3390/children10061019

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

9. OECD. PISA 2022 Ergebnisse (Band I): Lernstände und Bildungsgerechtigkeit [PISA 2022 results (Part I): learning status and educational equity]. Bielefeld: wbv Media (2023).

Google Scholar

10. Phelps NH, Singleton RK, Zhou B, Heap RA, Mishra A, Bennett JE, et al. Worldwide trends in underweight and obesity from 1990 to 2022: a pooled analysis of 3663 population-representative studies with 222 million children, adolescents, and adults. Lancet. (2024) 403(10431):1027–50. doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736(23)02750-2

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

11. Tzuriel D, Egozi G. Gender differences in spatial ability of young children: the effects of training and processing strategies. Child Dev. (2010) 81:1417–30. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2010.01482.x

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

12. Fernandez-Baizan C, Arias J, Méndez M. Spatial orientation assessment in preschool children: egocentric and allocentric frameworks. Appl Neuropsychol Child. (2019) 10(2):171–93. doi: 10.1080/21622965.2019.1630278

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

13. National Research Council. Learning to Think Spatially. Washington, DC: The National Academies Press (2006).

Google Scholar

14. Uttal DH, Meadow NG, Tipton E, Hand LL, Alden AR, Warren C, et al. The malleability of spatial skills: a meta-analysis of training studies. Psychol Bull. (2013) 139(2):352–402. doi: 10.1037/a0028446

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

15. Ishikawa T, Newcombe NS. Why spatial is special in education, learning, and everyday activities. Cogn Res Princ Implic. (2021) 6(1):20. doi: 10.1186/s41235-021-00274-5

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

16. Wai J, Lubinski D, Benbow CP. Spatial ability for STEM domains: aligning over 50 years of cumulative psychological knowledge solidifies its importance. J Educ Psychol. (2009) 101(4):817–35. doi: 10.1037/a0016127

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

17. Lowrie T, Logan T, Harris D, Hegarty M. The impact of an intervention program on students’ spatial reasoning: student engagement through mathematics-enhanced learning activities. Cogn Res Princ Implic. (2018) 3(1):50. doi: 10.1186/s41235-018-0147-y

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

18. Murias K, Slone E, Tariq S, Iaria G. Development of spatial orientation skills: an fMRI study. Brain Imaging Behav. (2019) 13(6):1590–601. doi: 10.1007/s11682-018-0028-5

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

19. Newcombe NS. Navigation and the developing brain. J Exp Biol. (2019) 222:jeb186460. doi: 10.1242/jeb.186460

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

20. Farran EK, Bowler A, Karmiloff-Smith A, D’Souza H, Mayall L, Hill EL. Cross-domain associations between motor ability, independent exploration, and large-scale spatial navigation; attention deficit hyperactivity disorder, Williams syndrome, and typical development. Front Hum Neurosci. (2019) 13:225. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2019.00225

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

21. Iaria G, Chen J-K, Guariglia C, Ptito A, Petrides M. Retrosplenial and hippocampal brain regions in human navigation: complementary functional contributions to the formation and use of cognitive maps. Eur J Neurosci. (2007) 25(3):890–9. doi: 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2007.05371.x

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

22. Chen Y, Monaco S, Byrne P, Yan X, Henriques DYP, Crawford JD. Allocentric versus egocentric representation of remembered reach targets in human cortex. J Neurosci. (2014) 34(37):12515. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1445-14.2014

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

23. Saluja RS, Chen J-K, Gagnon IJ, Keightley M, Ptito A. Navigational memory functional magnetic resonance imaging: a test for concussion in children. J Neurotrauma. (2014) 32(10):712–22. doi: 10.1089/neu.2014.3470

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

24. Pine DS, Grun J, Maguire EA, Burgess N, Zarahn E, Koda V, et al. Neurodevelopmental aspects of spatial navigation: a virtual reality fMRI study. NeuroImage. (2002) 15(2):396–406. doi: 10.1006/nimg.2001.0988

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

25. Zaehle T, Jordan K, Wüstenberg T, Baudewig J, Dechent P, Mast FW. The neural basis of the egocentric and allocentric spatial frame of reference. Brain Res. (2007) 1137:92–103. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2006.12.044

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

26. Hawes Z, Moss J, Caswell B, Poliszczuk D. Effects of mental rotation training on children’s spatial and mathematics performance: a randomized controlled study. Trends Neurosci Educ. (2015) 4:60–8. doi: 10.1016/j.tine.2015.05.001

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

27. Blüchel M, Lehmann J, Kellner J, Jansen P. The improvement in mental rotation performance in primary school-aged children after a two-week motor-training. Educ Psychol. (2013) 33(1):75–86. doi: 10.1080/01443410.2012.707612

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

28. Pietsch S, Böttcher C, Jansen P. Cognitive motor coordination training improves mental rotation performance in primary school-aged children. Mind Brain Educ. (2017) 11(4):176–80. doi: 10.1111/mbe.12154

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

29. Latino F, Cataldi S, Fischetti F. Effects of a coordinative ability training program on adolescents’ cognitive functioning. Front Psychol. (2021) 12:620440. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2021.620440

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

30. Morawietz C, Wissmann AM, Muehlbauer T. A 6-week coordinative motor training program improves spatial ability performances in healthy children. Front Cogn. (2024) 3:1396399. doi: 10.3389/fcogn.2024.1396399

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

31. Jansen P, Richter S. Effects of a one-hour creative dance training on mental rotation performance in primary school aged children. Int J Learn Teach Educ Res. (2015) 13:49–57.

Google Scholar

32. Tomporowski P. Cognitive and behavioral responses to acute exercise in youths: a review. Pediatr Exerc Sci. (2003) 15(4):348–59. doi: 10.1123/pes.15.4.348

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

33. Hillman CH, Pontifex MB, Raine LB, Castelli DM, Hall EE, Kramer AF. The effect of acute treadmill walking on cognitive control and academic achievement in preadolescent children. Neurosci. (2009) 159(3):1044–54. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2009.01.057

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

34. Chen A-G, Yan J, Yin H-C, Pan C-Y, Chang Y-K. Effects of acute aerobic exercise on multiple aspects of executive function in preadolescent children. Psychol Sport Exerc. (2014) 15(6):627–36. doi: 10.1016/j.psychsport.2014.06.004

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

35. Chen A, Zhu L, Xiong X, Li Y. Acute aerobic exercise alters executive control network in preadolescent children. Rev Psicol Deporte. (2017) 26:132–7.

Google Scholar

36. Hillman CH, Kamijo K, Scudder M. A review of chronic and acute physical activity participation on neuroelectric measures of brain health and cognition during childhood. Prev Med. (2011) 52:S21–8. doi: 10.1016/j.ypmed.2011.01.024

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

37. Piepmeier AT, Etnier JL. Brain-derived neurotrophic factor (BDNF) as a potential mechanism of the effects of acute exercise on cognitive performance. J Sport Health Sci. (2015) 4(1):14–23. doi: 10.1016/j.jshs.2014.11.001

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

38. Paschen L, Lehmann T, Kehne M, Baumeister J. Effects of acute physical exercise with low and high cognitive demands on executive functions in children: a systematic review. Pediatr Exerc Sci. (2019) 31:267–81. doi: 10.1123/pes.2018-0215

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

39. Voelcker-Rehage C, Niemann C. Structural and functional brain changes related to different types of physical activity across the life span. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. (2013) 37(9, Part B):2268–95. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2013.01.028

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

40. Meijer A, Königs M, Vermeulen GT, Visscher C, Bosker RJ, Hartman E, et al. The effects of physical activity on brain structure and neurophysiological functioning in children: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Dev Cogn Neurosci. (2020) 45:100828. doi: 10.1016/j.dcn.2020.100828

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

41. Mochizuki A, Kirino E. Effects of coordination exercises on brain activation: a functional MRI study. Int J Sport Health Sci. (2008) 6:98–104. doi: 10.5432/ijshs.6.98

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

42. Debaere F, Wenderoth N, Sunaert S, Van Hecke P, Swinnen SP. Changes in brain activation during the acquisition of a new bimanual coordination task. Neuropsychologia. (2004) 42(7):855–67. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2003.12.010

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

43. Demirakca T, Cardinale V, Dehn S, Ruf M, Ende G. The exercising brain: changes in functional connectivity induced by an integrated multimodal cognitive and whole-body coordination training. Neural Plast. (2015) 2016:8240894. doi: 10.1155/2016/8240894

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

44. Budde H, Voelcker-Rehage C, Pietrabyk-Kendziorra S, Ribeiro P, Tidow G. Acute coordinative exercise improves attentional performance in adolescents. Neurosci Lett. (2008) 441(2):219–23. doi: 10.1016/j.neulet.2008.06.024

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

45. World Health Organization. Promoting Physical Activity Through Schools: A Toolkit. Geneva: World Health Organization (2021).

Google Scholar

46. Hardman K, Routen A, Tones S. UNESCO-NWCPEA: world-wide survey of school physical education; Final Report. (2014).

Google Scholar

47. Hillman CH, Snook EM, Jerome GJ. Acute cardiovascular exercise and executive control function. Int J Psychophysiol. (2003) 48(3):307–14. doi: 10.1016/S0167-8760(03)00080-1

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

48. Andrä C, Macedonia M. Bewegtes Lernen: Handbuch für Forschung und Praxis [Learning in Motion: Handbook for Research and Practice]. Berlin: Lehmanns Media (2020).

Google Scholar

49. Andrä C. Bewegtes lernen [learning in motion]. Pädagogik. (2023) 2:48. doi: 10.3262/PAED2302048

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

50. Faul F, Erdfelder E, Lang A-G, Buchner A. G*power 3: a flexible statistical power analysis program for the social, behavioral, and biomedical sciences. Behav Res Methods. (2007) 39(2):175–91. doi: 10.3758/BF03193146

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

51. Morawietz C, Dumalski N, Wissmann AM, Wecking J, Muehlbauer T. Consistency of spatial ability performance in children, adolescents, and young adults. Front Psychol. (2024) 15:1365941. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2024.1365941

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

52. Hirtz P, Hotz A, Ludwig G. Orientierung [Orientation]. Hofmann-Verlag: Schorndorf (2010).

Google Scholar

53. Lutz H. Life Kinetik®: Gehirntraining Durch Bewegung [Brain Training Through Movement]. Munich: BLV, ein Imprint von GRÄFE UND UNZER Verlag GmbH (2010).

Google Scholar

54. Hamburger Sportbund e.V. Hamburger Parcours (2012). Available online at: https://www.hamburger-sportbund.de/service/download-arena/26-leistungssport/34-unterlagen-hamburger-parcours (Accessed March 14 2024).

Google Scholar

55. Bundesamt für Sport BASPO. Schweizerkreuz-plus [Swisscross-Plus] (2013). Available online at: https://www.mobilesport.ch/assets/lbwp-cdn/mobilesport/files/2013/08/l_B2_12SJ-Gym_ABC_T2.pdf (Accessed March 14 2024).

Google Scholar

56. Bundesamt für Sport BASPO. Konditions-Koordinationstest (Koko-Test) [Condition-coordination Test] (2015). Available online at: https://www.sportunterricht.ch/test/download/Kokotest-print.pdf (accessed March 14 2024).

Google Scholar

57. Cohen J. Statistical Power Analysis for the Behavioral Sciences. Hillsdale: NJ Erlbaum (1988).

Google Scholar

58. Gu Q, Zou L, Loprinzi PD, Quan M, Huang T. Effects of open versus closed skill exercise on cognitive function: a systematic review. Front Psychol. (2019) 10:1707. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2019.01707

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

59. Shijie L, Yu Q, Li Z, Cunha P, Zhang Y, Kong Z, et al. Effects of acute and chronic exercises on executive function in children and adolescents: a systemic review and meta-analysis. Front Psychol. (2020) 17(11):554915. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2020.554915

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

60. O’Brien J, Ottoboni G, Tessari A, Setti A. Multisensory perception, verbal, visuo-spatial and motor working memory modulation after a single open- or closed-skill exercise session in children. J Cogn Enhanc. (2021) 5(2):141–54. doi: 10.1007/s41465-020-00189-x

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

61. Anzeneder S, Zehnder C, Martin-Niedecken AL, Schmidt M, Benzing V. Acute exercise and children’s cognitive functioning: what is the optimal dose of cognitive challenge? Psychol Sport Exerc. (2023) 66:102404. doi: 10.1016/j.psychsport.2023.102404

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

62. Jansen P, Pietsch S. Physical activity improves mental rotation performance. Creat Educ. (2010) 1(1):58–61. doi: 10.4236/ce.2010.11009

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

63. Feng X, Zhang Z, Jin T, Shi P. Effects of open and closed skill exercise interventions on executive function in typical children: a meta-analysis. BMC Psychol. (2023) 11(1):420. doi: 10.1186/s40359-023-01317-w

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

64. Heilmann F, Weinberg H, Wollny R. The impact of practicing open- vs. closed-skill sports on executive functions-a meta-analytic and systematic review with a focus on characteristics of sports. Brain Sci. (2022) 12(8):1071. doi: 10.3390/brainsci12081071

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

65. Geisen M, Raab M, Jansen P, Klatt S. Embodied mental rotation ability in open- and closed-skill sports: pilot study with a new virtual paradigm. Exp Brain Res. (2024) 242(3):653–64. doi: 10.1007/s00221-023-06753-z

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

66. van den Berg V, Saliasi E, de Groot RHM, Jolles J, Chinapaw MJM, Singh AS. Physical activity in the school setting: cognitive performance is not affected by three different types of acute exercise. Front Psychol. (2016) 7:723. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2016.00723

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

67. Bedard C, Bremer E, Graham JD, Chirico D, Cairney J. Examining the effects of acute cognitively engaging physical activity on cognition in children. Front Psychol. (2021) 12:653133. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2021.653133

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

68. Best J. Exergaming immediately enhances children’s executive function. Dev Psychol. (2011) 48(5):1501–10. doi: 10.1037/a0026648

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

69. Basso JC, Suzuki WA. The effects of acute exercise on mood, cognition, neurophysiology, and neurochemical pathways: a review. Brain Plast. (2017) 2:127–52. doi: 10.3233/BPL-160040

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

70. Scharfen J, Peters JM, Holling H. Retest effects in cognitive ability tests: a meta-analysis. Intelligence. (2018) 67:44–66. doi: 10.1016/j.intell.2018.01.003

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

71. Fehringer BCOF. Different perspectives on retest effects in the context of spatial thinking: interplay of behavioral performance, cognitive processing, and cognitive workload. J Intell. (2023) 11(4):66. doi: 10.3390/jintelligence11040066

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

72. Lohman DF, Nichols PD. Training spatial abilities: effects of practice on rotation and synthesis tasks. Learn Individ Differ. (1990) 2(1):67–93. doi: 10.1016/1041-6080(90)90017-B

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

73. Vandierendonck A, Kemps E, Fastame MC, Szmalec A. Working memory components of the Corsi blocks task. Br J Psychol. (2004) 95(1):57–79. doi: 10.1348/000712604322779460

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

74. Moreau D, Chou E. The acute effect of high-intensity exercise on executive function: a meta-analysis. Perspect Psychol Sci. (2019) 14(5):734–64. doi: 10.1177/1745691619850568

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

75. Engeroff T, Banzer W, Niederer D. The impact of regular activity and exercise intensity on the acute effects of resistance exercise on cognitive function. Scand J Med Sci Sports. (2022) 32(1):94–105. doi: 10.1111/sms.14050

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

76. Wilke J, Giesche F, Klier K, Vogt L, Herrmann E, Banzer W. Acute effects of resistance exercise on cognitive function in healthy adults: a systematic review with multilevel meta-analysis. Sports Med. (2019) 49(6):905–16. doi: 10.1007/s40279-019-01085-x

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: treatment, acute effects, visual-spatial abilities, youth, movement coordination

Citation: Morawietz C, Wissmann AM, Kuehne T and Muehlbauer T (2024) A single session of coordinative motor training does not improve spatial ability performances in healthy children. Front. Sports Act. Living 6:1411127. doi: 10.3389/fspor.2024.1411127

Received: 2 April 2024; Accepted: 27 September 2024;
Published: 14 October 2024.

Edited by:

Karl Schweizer, Goethe University Frankfurt, Germany

Reviewed by:

Martin Musalek, Charles University, Czechia
Marinella Coco, University of Catania, Italy

Copyright: © 2024 Morawietz, Wissmann, Kuehne and Muehlbauer. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Christina Morawietz, christina.morawietz@uni-due.de

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.