- 1Department of Psychology, The Ohio State University, Columbus, OH, United States
- 2Chronic Brain Injury Initiative, The Ohio State University, Columbus, OH, United States
It is important that principles of laboratory-based studies with implications for academic performance be implemented in naturalistic learning environments to gauge their feasibility. Here, an adaptation of a laboratory-based study of exercise breaks during a single video lecture was implemented during large, in-person lectures at Ohio State University for the duration of a semester. The rationale for this approach was based on findings that research participants who took exercise breaks during a video lecture were more likely to be on task towards the end of the lecture and performed significantly better on a multiple choice exam. The current project had three goals: (1) Establish the feasibility of integrating student-led exercise breaks during in-person lectures in a large university setting (2) Provide practical guidelines for implementing exercise breaks during in-person lectures (3) Provide preliminary evidence of positive effects of exercise breaks in a higher-education setting. One to two student-led exercise breaks (5 min each) were implemented during each 80 min, in-person lecture for the duration of a semester in four upper level Psychology courses with student enrollment ranging from 20 to 93 students (total enrollment = 223 students). Students reported that the exercise breaks were a strength of the courses and a positive experience, including self-reported improvement in attention to lecture content. Self-reported quantitative data indicated that exercise breaks improved attention, increased course enjoyment, and enhanced peer engagement. Compared to other classes, the students preferred exercise breaks during lectures. The current approach establishes the feasibility of integrating exercise breaks in a large, in-person university lecture environment for the duration of a semester with preliminary data indicating a positive impact on attention, engagement, and enjoyment. Practical guidelines for implementing exercise breaks during in-person lectures are provided.
1 Introduction
There is a great deal of interest in cognitive enhancement. One of the more promising methods for optimizing cognitive performance is exercise, which has demonstrated positive effects on executive functions and memory (1, 2) as well as brain structure and function (3–5). Examination of the acute effects of exercise on cognition has occurred in two broad domains: controlled laboratory settings or naturalistic classroom settings. Studies in laboratory settings have (1) tended to use cognitive tests as the outcome measure (2) tested undergraduate students (age = 18–23 years) and (3) used an aerobic exercise stimulus (cycling, treadmill running). A meta-analysis of predominately laboratory studies showed a small but positive effect of acute exercise on executive function (attention, planning, and inhibition (6) and a recent study indicated that a 10 min bout of exercise may enhance memory (7).
Classroom-based exercise studies have typically assessed children and adolescents, and incorporated a range of outcome measures, including cognition, classroom behaviors, or academic performance (8). These studies, the majority of which used aerobic exercises, have reported positive effects on cognition (9). However, one meta-analysis (8) did not report beneficial effects of exercise on cognition or academic performance, but exercise was associated with better classroom behavior. A limitation of the meta-analysis was the small number of studies (n = 8) within a given intervention type or outcome measure. Nevertheless, time on task was improved in most studies and the relationship was dose-dependent based on exercise volume.
Given the absence of studies of exercise interventions in collegiate classrooms, there is limited guidance on best practices to optimize attention and classroom performance in young adults. However, a recent laboratory study by (10), who implemented prescribed 5 min aerobic exercise breaks in undergraduates during a single videotaped lecture, provides a conceptual foundation. The authors implemented three, 5 min aerobic exercise breaks throughout a single 50 min videotaped lecture. Compared to students who played a video game or had no break (traditional lecture format), the exercise group was more likely to be on-task towards the end of the lecture. The exercise group also exhibited better memory performance on an immediate and a delayed multiple-choice exam and provided superior ratings on narrator clarity and their own understanding of the materials.
During in-person university lectures, students are highly sedentary, typically sitting for the duration of the class (55–80 min). Furthermore, as time on task increases, such as listening to a lecture, mind wandering increases and memory for lecture content decreases (11, 12). These findings, coupled with those reported by Fenesi et al. (10), highlight the need to establish the feasibility of implementing an exercise protocol during live, in-person lectures in a university classroom. To address this issue, exercise breaks were implemented during four in-person courses at The Ohio State University (Columbus campus: undergraduate enrollment ∼46,000; graduate and professional student enrollment ∼6,300). Course lectures were 80 min each, twice per week, for the entire 15-week semester. In addition to potential cognitive benefits of improved attention and memory, inserting bouts of exercise during the lecture provided an opportunity to improve mood as well as model healthy behavior to students, i.e., avoid being sedentary, and challenge narrow definitions of physical activity, e.g., “I need to dedicate one hour to exercise at the gym 3–5 times per week.” This activity also provided an opportunity for students to interact in smaller groups. There were three main goals: (1) Demonstrate the feasibility of implementing exercise breaks in a large, in-person lecture setting (2) Describe practical guidelines for implementing exercise breaks during in-person lectures (3) Provide preliminary evidence of positive effects of exercise breaks in a higher-education setting.
2 Pedagogical framework and methods
Students earned class points for designing the exercise routine, serving as group leaders, and for participating in exercise breaks led by other student groups. Below is a brief description of the approach (Table 1).
(1) Students from the larger classes (70 or more students) were randomly assigned to groups of six or seven students. For the smaller classes (less than 40 students), group size was two to three students. The number and size of the groups were selected so that each group led an exercise routine for at least one class during the semester. To determine group size, one can divide the number of students in the class (e.g., 100) by the number of classes in the semester (e.g., 20; do not count classes on which there will be an exam). This example would result in a group size of 5 students, with each student group leading an exercise break once during the semester. For smaller classes, one can select a group size of two to four students, and they can lead exercise breaks multiple times during the semester.
(2) The Fenesi et al. (10) article was assigned as a required reading for the first class, which provided the scientific rationale (improved attention, better exam performance, and improved narrator quality) for in-class exercise breaks. During the first class, the study was discussed in detail. Primary points of emphasis included discussion of the experimental group assignment (exercise group vs. video game playing group vs. lecture as usual), tasks completed during the break (e.g., specifics of the exercise intervention, video game played), and main study findings (students in exercise group were more likely to be on task, gave higher narrator quality ratings, and had higher multiple choice exam scores compared to the video game playing group and the lecture as usual group). Reading and discussion of the article occurred to provide context and understanding to the students; in other words, the answers to why we would be taking exercise breaks during the class. It was the author's opinion that this was important to get “buy-in” from the students, all of whom had taken dozens of courses at the university level and had never taken an exercise break during a lecture.
(3) During the first class, the Physical Activity Readiness Questionnaire (PAR-Q & You) was presented and discussed. The PAR-Q is a brief preparticipation health screening form recommended by the American College of Sports Medicine and was to assess whether someone may safely initiate becoming more physically active. The PAR-Q was administered to confirm safety of student participation in the exercise breaks. The PAR-Q was recently updated (PAR-Q+) and it is recommended that the latest available version be used for assessment (13).
(4) After the first class, the PAR-Q was posted on-line for students to complete before the third class (when the exercise breaks would start). A small number of students (roughly 5%) responded “yes” to at least one of the questions. In these cases, the instructor spoke or emailed with them individually about the exercise breaks. The students were instructed to avoid any exercises that could exacerbate their condition. They were also given permission not to participate or stay in their seats, to stand, stretch, modify the prescribed exercise, choose a different exercise, or leave the room for a brief break. All students elected to participate in the exercises and modified or chose alternative exercises as needed.
(5) For the second class, students met with their groups. The students were assigned multiple tasks:
a. Generate a group name [e.g., Freudian Dipz, Forever Jung, The Control Group, Scott's Tots (“The Office” reference)]. This was implemented as an ice-breaker and a low-stakes, creative group activity for the students.
b. Learn how to calculate exercise intensity based on their heart rate (HR). This entailed teaching students how to calculate their age-predicted heart rate max (here we used the simple equation, heart rate max = 220-age), take their own pulse (carotid or wrist or use their smartwatch or activity tracker) and then calculate % HR max by dividing HR current/HR max. Data from Fenesi et al. (10) demonstrated that participants reached about 70% of their HR max during their prescribed routine. The class was instructed to target 50%–70% HR max, which would fall within the light to moderate intensity range (14). Rationale for this range was based on the following: (1) no definitive study showing whether various exercise intensity levels confer differential cognitive benefits in an academic setting (2) practical aspect of avoiding excessive perspiration by exercising at a vigorous intensity for the duration of the break.
c. Create an exercise routine: 5 exercise bouts, 50 sec each, with a 10 sec break between each bout. The number of bouts and timing was modelled after (10). For the exercise routine, students had the freedom to implement a mix of exercises (aerobic, strength, flexibility, balance, yoga poses, or deep breathing) with the caveat that at least three needed to be aerobic. For 50 sec bouts that were targeted as flexibility or stretching, students were instructed to generate two to three different stretches per bout. If at least three of the five exercises were aerobic, this would be sufficient to elevate heart rate for the majority of the 5 min exercise period (which the students anecdotally confirmed in class when heart rate was self-assessed during the first session). A variety of exercises was preferred to maintain student engagement throughout the semester (Table 2).
d. Confirm that their exercises were feasible to implement in the classroom.
e. Post the exercise routine to online discussion group. This provides the instructor the opportunity to review the exercise routine and provide feedback. For instance, if a group had chosen five lower body aerobic exercises, suggestions were made to vary the routine.
(6) On the third class and thereafter, the exercise breaks were implemented for each lecture. At the beginning of class, the instructor announced the student group leading exercises. At around the 25 min mark of class, the group was invited to the front of the auditorium. Each group member introduced themselves. The group was responsible for demonstrating the exercises to the class, timing the duration of each exercise, and timing the 10 sec breaks between exercises. Student groups also had the option to play music, and most groups elected to play music from a smartphone which was broadcast to the class via the lecturer's microphone. After the exercise break, the group leaders and the students returned to their seats. The lecture continued with another exercise break around the 55 min mark of the class. For classes in which one exercise break was implemented, the break occurred at the 40 min mark of the 80 min class.
(7) The instructor participated and completed the exercise bouts with the students. Occasionally, the instructor intentionally deviated from the prescribed exercises, further reinforcing that exercises can be modified as needed.
2.1 Qualitative data collection
All four courses provided an opportunity for students to provide on-line anonymous comments about the course at the end of the semester. These student evaluation of instruction forms are automatically offered for all students taking courses at Ohio State University. Exercise breaks are not specifically probed in these standard evaluations, but there is an open field for students to comment (positively or negatively) about the class. The evaluations were examined for both positive and negative comments about the exercise breaks. Two classes (SP 2020 Health Psychology and SP 2020 Cognitive Aging) also had a PhD-level teaching consultant (outside of the instructor's home department and separate from the instructor peer-review process) attend a class to collect student impressions of the course (strengths and weaknesses) and provide anonymous feedback to the instructor. Pre-tenure faculty are also observed annually by tenured peers within the department. The peer review letters were available for two courses and were also used as a source for qualitative assessment of exercise breaks.
2.2 Quantitative data collection
At the end of the spring 2023 Cognitive Aging course, 27 of the 37 enrolled students completed a voluntary and anonymous on-line 7-item survey with a 5-point Likert scale plus an additional open-ended item that probed for general comments about the exercise breaks (“Please provide any other feedback/comments related to your impression of the exercise breaks.). One-sample t-tests were implemented as an item analysis to assess whether subjective ratings were significantly different from 3, which corresponded to a rating of “neutral” on the Likert scale.
3 Learning environment
This protocol was designed to be implemented in a higher education setting with adults. The exercise breaks described above were implemented in large lecture halls with movie theater style seating, as well as active learning classrooms with students seated in smaller groups of four to six students around a table. In the current study, the exercise breaks were implemented for the semester in four upper-level Psychology courses: Spring 2019 Health Psychology (enrollment = 93 undergraduates); Spring 2020 Health Psychology (enrollment = 73 undergraduates); Spring 2020 Cognitive Aging (enrollment: 13 undergraduates; 7 graduate students); Spring 2023 Cognitive Aging (enrollment: 32 undergraduates, 5 graduate students). The gap in implementation from 2020 to 2023 was due to asynchronous, on-line course offerings due to COVID-19. Each course met twice per week for 80 min for the duration of the semester, although one health psychology course and one cognitive aging course met for 10-weeks (in-person classes were cancelled in the latter part of the 2020 semester due to COVID-19). At least one student-led exercise break was implemented for every lecture.
4 Results
4.1 Qualitative data
The overall impression from the qualitative data was that the students self-reported benefits from the exercise breaks, including improved motivation, attention, and engagement with peers and the instructor. For instance, although exercise breaks were not specifically probed, students spontaneously provided anonymous comments with their end-of-semester student evaluation of instruction (SEIs). Comments included the following:
“I enjoyed the exercise breaks in class and really felt like they motivated me to focus more.”
“I love that the instructor puts research into practice in his classroom with the exercise breaks! I found these really fun… They also helped a lot with my focus during a long lecture.”
“Something I really enjoyed about this class were the exercise breaks given in class. They really did help break up some of the material and made staying engaged in class easier.”
There were a handful of negative comments about the exercise breaks on the end-of-semester student evaluations of instruction. One student described dreading the exercise breaks, and another suggested that one exercise break, rather than two, would be sufficient.
The positive comments were consistent with those reported in the end of semester survey for the spring 2023 cognitive aging course. Of the 27 students who completed the survey, 16 elected to provide general feedback about the exercise break. The unedited student feedback from all students is reported in Table 3. Most student responses described a positive impact of exercise breaks on their lecture experience. Many of the comments reported positive effects on attention and class enjoyment.
Additional qualitative evidence of the positive impact of exercise breaks on student's subjective lecture experience were confirmed with another reporting modality for collecting anonymous student feedback: a PhD-level teaching consultant attended two classes and solicited anonymous feedback from students (without the instructor present). During the session, the consultant posed the following question: “What are the strengths of the course and instructor that assist you in learning?” For the spring 2020 health psychology course, eight of the 12 student groups spontaneously listed the exercise breaks as one of the strengths (note that they were not specifically asked about the breaks but listed it as a strength nonetheless). For the Cognitive Aging course, although only two of the five groups spontaneously listed exercise breaks as a strength, the independent teaching consultant documented it as a general strength because “most students” liked the exercise breaks. Student group comments included “We like the exercise breaks. It helps us stay focused” and “Exercise breaks are refreshing.”
Finally, at Ohio State University, professors are peer-reviewed while teaching. In the formal course peer evaluation, a tenured colleague noted “Something quite remarkable occurred approximately halfway through the lecture…the instructor announced “Exercise break!” and—to my amazement—the students stood up and started doing jumping jacks, squats, and other physical exercises…For the record, so was I and I can attest from personal experience that the exercises had an extremely beneficial effect on my ability to listen attentively when the lecture resumed.” Two colleagues within the Psychology Department have reported that they have subsequently initiated exercise breaks in their courses.
4.2 Quantitative data
All students reported that they had never taken a class that implemented an exercise break during a lecture. The results of the self-reported quantitative data were consistent with the qualitative data (Figure 1). One-sample t-tests (test sample value = 3, corresponding to a “neutral rating”) revealed that students rated the exercise breaks as improving attention, enjoyable, and improving peer engagement, all t's (26) > 6.30, all p's < .001 (Table 4). The students reported that compared to other classes, they preferred exercise breaks during the lecture and that they would prefer that more classes offered exercise breaks t's (26) > 7.82, all p's < .001. Students reported that they preferred listening to music during the exercise break. Students were not concerned about injury (Figure 1; Table 4). All results remained significant after Bonferroni multiple comparisons correction (p = 0.05/7 = .007).
Figure 1. Percentage of students selecting response options for survey items in spring 2023 cognitive aging course.
Table 4. Mean, standard deviation (sd), and results of one-sample t-tests for each item [t-test value = 3 (neutral), two-tailed significant test].
5 Discussion
The current report establishes the feasibility of implementing student-lead exercise breaks during in-person lectures with undergraduate and graduate students in a university setting. The exercise breaks were implemented across four courses in active-learning classrooms with a class size as small as 20 students as well as larger lecture halls with movie theater style seating with 93 students. Qualitative and quantitative self-reported data were consistent: students reported positive impacts of exercise breaks on attention and motivation, engagement with their peers (which is often difficult to establish in large classes), and course enjoyment. Finally, the current report provides a protocol for implementing exercise breaks in a university setting.
Acute bouts of exercise exert multiple influences on various domains, including emotion, cognition, and physiology that are likely contributing to the students’ reported positive impacts of exercise breaks during university lectures. For instance, acute bouts of exercise have a positive impact on depression and hostility (15), anxiety (16), and affect and energy (17). Further, in addition to the Fenesi et al. (10) study, other laboratory studies have demonstrated a positive impact of acute bouts of exercise on cognition, including executive function and memory (6, 18–22). These positive benefits of exercise on mood and cognition are likely underpinned by the cascade of physiological alterations observed subsequent to an acute bout of exercise, including elevated heart rate, alterations in levels of cortisol, endorphins, neurotransmitters, and neurotrophins, and alterations in neuronal firing and cerebral blood flow (for review, see (18).
Anecdotally, throughout the semester, multiple students openly commented about how much they liked the exercise breaks, and it appeared that all students in any given class participated. One student noted it had motivated them to initiate exercise routines outside of the classroom. At a broader level, implementing exercise breaks during lectures provides an opportunity for instructors to model positive health behaviors, as multiple lab-based studies have reported positive associations between physical fitness, physical activity, cognition, and the brain (5, 23, 24).
It is important to recognize that the exercise bouts should be implemented thoughtfully and in an organized manner at the start of the semester. One should consult with knowledgeable professionals (colleagues in physical education, exercise science, or physical rehabilitation) if unsure about the practicality of an exercise or concerned about potential physical limitations of students. It is important to emphasize that both the instructor and students use common sense when thinking about the exercise routine and an individual’s exercise abilities and err on the side of caution. Most undergraduate and graduate students are younger adults, and the risk for an adverse event is extremely low. Fewer than 5% of students reported an issue on the PAR-Q, and these were predominately acute injuries (e.g., sprained ankle). Moreover, the exercises should fall within the intensity range of normal activities of daily living, further mitigating risk of adverse events. Nevertheless, some students have limitations, and the burden of inclusion in this activity falls upon the instructor and appropriate collaboration with the students.
The anecdotal nature and potential biases of these data sources are acknowledged; nevertheless, these data provide insight into the tolerance and feasibility of implementation of exercise breaks in a higher education setting and indicated multiple positive effects of exercise among students. Future studies could implement more detailed assessment or assess tolerance in other undergraduate samples or courses (beyond psychology and neuroscience). More rigorous academic assessment can be challenging to implement in a typical university class setting for the duration of the semester. One could implement an ABA design (no exercise, exercise, no exercise) during the semester, although one would need to control for course content if attempting to assess academic performance, comprehension, or memory for the materials. Similarly, one could attempt to compare exam performance for different offerings of the course with and without exercise breaks, and attempt to address the challenges of cohort effects, time of day effects, and instructor effects.
For the instructor, there were some self-reported beneficial effects from the exercise breaks. The bouts of exercise provided a brief respite from speaking, naturally introduced variability in the lecture, and reinvigorated the instructor for the next bout of lecture. It also provided a unique opportunity to get to know the students and served as a collaborative group activity for students to get to know their peers, which is often difficult to achieve in a course with many students.
Finally, these data warrant consideration for implementation of exercise breaks in more courses at the University level. Given that students typically sit for the duration of the lecture, exercise breaks reduce sedentary behavior, which is associated with negative health outcomes and may negatively impact cognition and brain health (25, 26). Moreover, these data indicate that implementation of exercise breaks throughout the semester is feasible. The exercise breaks were acceptable and well-tolerated by the students. Students perceived positive impacts on attention, peer engagement, enjoyment of the exercise breaks, and preferred the exercise breaks during the lecture. As a practical, albeit ambitious example, it is worth noting that a student taking a 12-credit semester course load (4 courses × 2 classes per week × 5 min of exercise per class = 40 min of moderate exercise per week) would reach 26% of the weekly recommended goal of 150 min of moderate physical activity per week (14) by participating in the exercise breaks during their classes. Thus, larger scale adoption of these healthy behaviors, such as exercise breaks during lectures, have the potential to contribute to weekly physical activity recommendations and associated positive mental and physical health outcomes in students attending university.
Data availability statement
The original contributions presented in the study are included in the article/Supplementary Material, further inquiries can be directed to the corresponding author.
Ethics statement
Ethical approval was not required for the study involving humans in accordance with the local legislation and institutional requirements. Written informed consent to participate in this study was not required from the participants or the participants’ legal guardians/next of kin in accordance with the national legislation and the institutional requirements.
Author contributions
SMH: Conceptualization, Formal Analysis, Funding acquisition, Investigation, Methodology, Resources, Visualization, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing.
Funding
The author declares that financial support was received for the research, authorship, and/or publication of this article.
This research was supported by the National Institute on Aging (NIA) of the National Institutes of Health (NIH) R01AG068882 and R21AG056921 (awarded to SMH) and The Ohio State University Discovery Themes Chronic Brain Injury Initiative. The content is solely the responsibility of the author and does not necessarily represent the official views of the National Institutes of Health.
Acknowledgments
The author would like to acknowledge Drs. Barbara Fenesi and Jennifer Heisz's original work, which motivated this approach, as well as their enthusiastic support of this article.
Conflict of interest
The author declares that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.
Publisher's note
All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.
References
1. Colcombe S, Kramer AF. Fitness effects on the cognitive function of older adults: a meta-analytic study. Psychol Sci. (2003) 14(2):125–30. doi: 10.1111/1467-9280.t01-1-01430
2. Kramer AF, Colcombe S. Fitness effects on the cognitive function of older adults: a meta-analytic study-revisited. Perspect Psychol Sci. (2018) 13(2):213–7. doi: 10.1177/1745691617707316
3. Esteban-Cornejo I, Rodriguez-Ayllon M, Verdejo-Roman J, Cadenas-Sanchez C, Mora-Gonzalez J, Chaddock-Heyman L, et al. Physical fitness, white matter volume and academic performance in children: findings from the ActiveBrains and FITKids2 projects. Front Psychol. (2019) 10:208. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2019.00208
4. Hayes SM, Hayes JP, Cadden M, Verfaellie M. A review of cardiorespiratory fitness-related neuroplasticity in the aging brain. Front Aging Neurosci. (2013) 5:31. doi: 10.3389/fnagi.2013.00031
5. Prakash RS, Voss MW, Erickson KI, Kramer AF. Physical activity and cognitive vitality. Annu Rev Psychol. (2015) 66:769–97. doi: 10.1146/annurev-psych-010814-015249
6. Chang YK, Labban JD, Gapin JI, Etnier JL. The effects of acute exercise on cognitive performance: a meta-analysis. Brain Res. (2012) 1453:87–101. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2012.02.068
7. Suwabe K, Hyodo K, Byun K, Ochi G, Yassa MA, Soya H. Acute moderate exercise improves mnemonic discrimination in young adults. Hippocampus. (2017) 27(3):229–34. doi: 10.1002/hipo.22695
8. Daly-Smith AJ, Zwolinsky S, McKenna J, Tomporowski PD, Defeyter MA, Manley A. Systematic review of acute physically active learning and classroom movement breaks on children’s physical activity, cognition, academic performance and classroom behaviour: understanding critical design features. BMJ Open Sport Exerc Med. (2018) 4(1):e000341. doi: 10.1136/bmjsem-2018-000341
9. Kubesch S, Walk L, Spitzer M, Kammer T, Lainburg A, Heim R, et al. A 30-minute physical education program improves students’ executive attention. Mind Brain Educ. (2009) 3(4):235–42. doi: 10.1111/j.1751-228X.2009.01076.x
10. Fenesi B, Lucibello K, Kim JA, Heisz JJ. Sweat so you don’t forget: exercise breaks during a university lecture increase on-task attention and learning. J Appl Res Mem Cogn. (2018) 7:261–9. doi: 10.1016/j.jarmac.2018.01.012
11. Farley J, Risko EF, Kingstone A. Everyday attention and lecture retention: the effects of time, fidgeting, and mind wandering. Front Psychol. (2013) 4:619. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00619
12. Risko EF, Anderson N, Sarwal A, Engelhardt M, Kingstone A. Everyday attention: variation in mind wandering and memory in a lecture. Appl Cogn Psychol. (2012) 26(2):234–42. doi: 10.1002/acp.1814
13. Warburton D, Jamnik V, Bredin S, Shephard R, Gledhill N. The 2021 physical activity readiness questionnaire for everyone (PAR-Q+) and electronic physical activity readiness medical examination (ePARmed-X+): 2021 PAR-Q+. Health Fit J Can. (2021) 14(1):83–7. doi: 10.14288/hfjc.v14i1.351
14. American College of Sports Medicine. ACSM’s Guidelines for Exercise Testing and Prescription. 11th Ed Philadelphia: Lippincott Williams & Wilkins (2021).
15. Crush EA, Frith E, Loprinzi PD. Experimental effects of acute exercise duration and exercise recovery on mood state. J Affect Disord. (2018) 229:282–7. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2017.12.092
16. Lucibello KM, Parker J, Heisz JJ. Examining a training effect on the state anxiety response to an acute bout of exercise in low and high anxious individuals. J Affect Disord. (2019) 247:29–35. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2018.12.063
17. Liao Y, Shonkoff ET, Dunton GF. The acute relationships between affect, physical feeling states, and physical activity in daily life: a review of current evidence. Front Psychol. (2015) 6:1975. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2015.01975
18. Basso JC, Suzuki WA. The effects of acute exercise on mood, cognition, neurophysiology, and neurochemical pathways: a review. Brain Plast. (2017) 2(2):127–52. doi: 10.3233/BPL-160040
19. Chang YK, Tsai CL, Hung TM, So EC, Chen FT, Etnier JL. Effects of acute exercise on executive function: a study with a tower of London task. J Sport Exerc Psychol. (2011) 33(6):847–65. doi: 10.1123/jsep.33.6.847
20. Loprinzi PD, Roig M, Tomporowski PD, Javadi AH, Kelemen WL. Effects of acute exercise on memory: considerations of exercise intensity, post-exercise recovery period and aerobic endurance. Mem Cognit. (2023) 51(4):1011–26. doi: 10.3758/s13421-022-01373-4
21. Waters A, Zou L, Jung M, Yu Q, Lin J, Liu S, et al. Acute exercise and sustained attention on memory function. Am J Health Behav. (2020) 44(3):326–32. doi: 10.5993/AJHB.44.3.5
22. Weng TB, Pierce GL, Darling WG, Voss MW. Differential effects of acute exercise on distinct aspects of executive function. Med Sci Sports Exerc. (2015) 47(7):1460–9. doi: 10.1249/MSS.0000000000000542
23. Hayes SM, Alosco ML, Hayes JP, Cadden M, Peterson KM, Allsup K, et al. Physical activity is positively associated with episodic memory in aging. J Int Neuropsychol Soc. (2015) 21(10):780–90. doi: 10.1017/S1355617715000910
24. Stauder M, Hiersche KJ, Hayes SM. Examining cross-sectional and longitudinal relationships between multidomain physical fitness metrics, education, and cognition in black older adults. Neuropsychol Dev Cogn B Aging Neuropsychol Cogn. (2023):1–15. doi: 10.1080/13825585.2023.2225848
25. Vaynman S, Gomez-Pinilla F. Revenge of the “sit”: how lifestyle impacts neuronal and cognitive health through molecular systems that interface energy metabolism with neuronal plasticity. J Neurosci Res. (2006) 84(4):699–715. doi: 10.1002/jnr.20979
Keywords: acute exercise, exercise, attention, mind wandering, memory, teaching, cognition, academic performance
Citation: Hayes SM (2024) Establishing the feasibility of exercise breaks during university lectures. Front. Sports Act. Living 6:1358564. doi: 10.3389/fspor.2024.1358564
Received: 20 December 2023; Accepted: 19 March 2024;
Published: 4 April 2024.
Edited by:
Thomas Quarmby, Leeds Beckett University, United KingdomReviewed by:
Ann L. Smiley, Iowa State University, United StatesGeir K. Resaland, Western Norway University of Applied Sciences, Norway
Mathias Brekke Mandelid Western, Norway University of Applied Sciences, Norway, in collaboration with reviewer [GKR]
© 2024 Hayes. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.
*Correspondence: Scott M. Hayes aGF5ZXMuMTA3NEBvc3UuZWR1