- 1Department of Physical Education, Moscow Polytechnic University, Moscow, Russia
- 2Federal State Budgetary Educational Institution of Higher Education, Vyatka State University, Kirov, Russia
- 3Faculty of Physical Education and Sport, Ovidius University of Constanta, Constantia, Romania
- 4Department of Biomedical Sciences for Health (SCIBIS), Università Degli Studi di Milano, Milan, Italy
- 5Department of Teacher Education, NLA University College, Oslo, Norway
The demands of the school day, which includes multiple lessons, require sustained attention from students; this can be challenging, especially for young children. Concentration of attention is a critical cognitive function that impacts learning. This study involved 129 healthy schoolchildren aged 8–10 years (spanning grades 2 and 3) from a regular secondary school in Kirov, Russian Federation. A “physical education minute” (PEM), consisting of various physical exercises, was introduced during the middle of a regular lesson for the experimental group (EG), while the control group (CG) continued with their regular studies. Concentration and attention were assessed before and after the PEM using the Bourdon test. The Bourdon test results indicated a significant decrease in concentration during the lesson for the CG (p < 0.001), while the EG exhibited significant improvement in concentration after the PEM (p < 0.001). The effect size was large, demonstrating a substantial impact of this break for physical activity on concentration. It can be concluded that implementing a set of exercises in the form of a PEM in the middle of a lesson significantly improves concentration among students aged 8–10. This study underscores the effectiveness of integrating short breaks for physical activity into the daily classroom routine, ultimately benefiting students' attention, learning, and overall educational experience. Further research could explore additional factors affecting concentration and the long-term effects of the PEM on cognitive development.
1. Introduction
Human health constantly plays a prominent role in numerous studies. The problem of schoolchildren's health and physical development is particularly pertinent (1–3). Most authors underline the importance of a culture of physical activity in schools for the health and well-being of students (4, 5). Children in grades 2 and 3 have 5–6 lessons every day, according to the curriculum. Math, their native language, a foreign language, the world around them, and other subjects are covered; each class is 40 min long. Children must listen to the teacher, memorize the instructional material, and complete various tasks throughout the course of each class. All of this necessitates a high level of concentration. Naturally, children grow fatigued in the classroom, particularly elementary school students. At this age, it is difficult for them to maintain optimum concentration throughout the class. Concentration of attention is a feature of attention that refers to the retention of information about an item in short-term memory. Concentration is essential for task completion, especially when dealing with a novel activity. A person with high attention levels completes work more quickly and more creatively, enhancing their overall productivity. They also operate more accurately and with fewer errors. The importance of concentration of attention at school (specifically, the ability of pupils to concentrate their attention) lies in the fact that it allows pupils to better digest instructional material (6, 7). Physical education sessions are crucial, not only for the development of physical skills, but also for the so-called “discharge” of pupils' energy, which is needed when they are distracted from their regular lessons and become “charged up” with physical and psychological energy (4). However, the academic curriculum does not provide for daily instruction in physical education (8).
Physical activity is recognized as an important component of overall human culture, and this aspect of culture encompasses the components of physical education, sports, physical recreation, physical rehabilitation, and physical stabilization (9). The “physical education minute” or “physical culture pause” is one available component of physical activity culture. This brief activity usually lasts one and a half to two minutes and is held in the middle of the class period. Its primary objective is to divert pupils' attention away from the educational process and to relax their muscles, joints, spine, and eyes.
A “physical education minute” is a brief period of physical exercise implemented to prevent weariness and restore mental function (by stimulating parts of the cerebral cortex that were not participating in prior activities, while resting those that were). There have been several studies conducted in this area. They discuss the significance of this type of physical education minute for school-aged children (10, 11). However, there has been little research to prove the value of a physical education minute in the development of attentional focus in schoolchildren (12). Thus, the goal of our research was to examine the impact of a physical education minute on markers of schoolchildren's concentration levels.
2. Materials and methods
2.1. Participants
This research involved children aged 8–10 years who were studying at ordinary secondary school number 60 in the city of Kirov, the Russian Federation. An a priori power analysis was conducted using the G*Power software package (Version 3.1.9.4, University of Kiel, Kiel, Germany) to determine the required sample size, with α set to 0.05 and power (1−β) set to 0.80. The analysis revealed that a total sample size of 86 subjects would be sufficient to identify significant differences with an actual power of 0.82. A total of 129 schoolchildren took part in the study (14 [13%] of whom were left-handed, based on a handedness questionnaire (13)). The participants were boys and girls from grades 2 (65 students) and 3 (64 students). The inclusion criteria specified that all the children must be healthy and able to exercise. However, children who had health limitations and did not take part in regular physical education lessons also took part in this research, since participation in a physical education minute does not require special physical training or a high level of development of physical abilities. Half of the children from grades 2 and 3 were assigned to the control group (CG) and the remaining children were assigned to the experimental group (EG). All procedures met the ethical standards of the 1964 Declaration of Helsinki and were approved by the Research Ethics Committee of Vyatka State University (January 17, 2022 №1). Informed consent was obtained from all the parents of the adolescents included in the study.
2.2. Research procedure
The study was conducted at a regular school in October 2022. The duration of lessons in grades 2 and 3, according to the general education program at the school, was 40 min.
We consider the most logical timing for the minute of exercise to be the middle of the lesson (12). The children have sufficient strength and energy for the first part of the lesson; conversely, it makes no sense to spend a minute on physical activity at the end of the lesson, because the children will change to a new activity (namely, some active rest) in a few minutes' time. Thus, the physical education minute was implemented in the middle of a regular lesson and on a day when the children did not have a physical education lesson according to the school timetable.
To engage in a physical education minute, schoolchildren left their classroom for an available adjacent area and performed physical exercises (stretching, bending, up-and-down arm movements, squatting, jumping, walking on the spot, and other exercises). An example of a physical education minute is one consisting of an activity named “Three bears,” with the following supporting song:
Three bears were walking home (children waddle on the spot),
Dad was big and big (they raise their hands above their head and pull up),
Mom was smaller than him (arms at chest level),
And the little son even smaller (sit down),
He was very small (sitting down and swinging from side to side),
I walked with rattles (children stand up with hands clenched into fists in front of their chest),
And ding-ding, ding-ding (children imitate a game with rattles).
There are a number of examples of physical education minutes. At the same time, any set of exercises can be supplemented, expanded, and modified. The main objective is to distract students from the educational process, to “awaken” them, to activate them, and to attract their attention.
In the present study, 20 min after the beginning of the lesson, all the children completed a control Bourdon test together in their classroom for 1 min; this test assesses their level of development in terms of concentration and attention. Next, the children in the e.g., performed a “physical education minute” set of exercises for 1.5–2 min in an adjacent area, whereas the children from the CG continued to study the educational material. Immediately after this period of time, all the students together completed the Bourdon test again in their classroom for 1 min. Thus, all children completed the Bourdon test twice during a single lesson, such that this test showed the effect of a physical education minute on concentration and attention among children in the e.g., whereas in children from the CG it could indicate so-called adaptation or habituation to this test.
The (Benjamin) Bourdon test (with numbers) is a well-known test that has been repeatedly and positively evaluated over time in terms of its validity and reliability (14, 15). In this test, a set of numbers from 0 to 9 is presented. Its features are (14, 15):
1. There must be exactly 15 numbers per row (the number of rows is unlimited),
2. The numbers are arranged in any order, and
3. There should be 3 numbers in each row that the test-taker could cross out.
A partial example of a Bourdon test grid is presented in Table 1.
The essence of the test is: within 1 min and at maximum speed, students must cross out any number that the teacher calls, for example, the number 5. At the end of 1 min, the student receives a score.
The results are processed according to the formula C = R/E, where
C = The concentration and attention score,
R = The number of rows attempted, and
E = The number of errors, including omissions and incorrectly crossed out numbers.
The results are interpreted as follows: the higher the final figure, the higher the student's concentration and attention score. This test has no set numerical values. If the student has switched to a new line, but the time is up, this line is not fully completed and is not counted in the final result.
To ensure the reliability of the results of the indicators for children in these grades, the protocol was administered twice during October 2022. For this study, days when there was no physical education for the grades included were selected, namely Monday, Wednesday, and Friday. On Tuesday and Thursday, physical education was timetabled for grades 2 and 3.
2.3. Statistical analysis
The results are presented in the form of means and standard deviations. Statistical analyses were conducted using SPSS 20.0 (IBM, Armonk, USA). The normality of the data was assessed by means of the Kolmogorov–Smirnov test (with a threshold of p < 0.05). The Kolmogorov–Smirnov test indicated a non-normal distribution for Bourdon test scores (p < 0.0001, D = 0.24033 for both groups). The reliability of the measurements (16) was assessed in the form of the intra-class correlation coefficient (ICC). The Student's t-test was used to test for significance differences between groups at baseline. Between-group differences in changes in Bourdon test score over time were analyzed using the Mann–Whitney test; differences between post-test and baseline scores for each group were assessed using the Wilcoxon test. The magnitude of differences was interpreted using the standardized effect size [Cohen's d (17)]: < 0.1 was taken to indicate no effect, 0.20–0.40 a small effect, 0.50–0.70 an effect of intermediate size, and 0.80–1.0 a large effect. The statistical significance threshold was set at p < 0.05.
3. Results
ICC was 0.887 (95% confidence interval: 0.849–0.917, p < 0.001). The Mann–Whitney U statistic was 128.500, with z-score −9.299, p < 0.001, and Cohen's d = 2.756 (large effect). At baseline, a t-test showed no significance difference (p = 0.071) between the two groups. A Wilcoxon test showed a significant change for both groups (p < 0.001) in the post-intervention Bourdon test compared with the baseline score. Furthermore, there was a difference between the two group in the changes that occurred. In the CG, there was a decrease in Bourdon test score (−4.0%), whereas in the e.g., test scores increased (+13.5%) when comparing post-test (9.86 ± 0.64 a.u. and 11.17 ± 0.75 a.u. for the CG and e.g., respectively) and baseline scores (10.88 ± 1.07 a.u. and 10.42 ± 1.00 a.u. for the CG and e.g., respectively).
4. Discussion
The effectiveness of “physical education minutes” during ordinary lessons at school has been proven by some experts. Researchers have noted that a physical education minute improves blood circulation, relieves fatigue in the muscles and nervous system, activates children's thinking, creates positive emotions, and increases their interest in classes. The optimal timing for a physical education minute is in the middle of a lesson, when children's attention is decreasing and fatigue setting in (10, 11).
Here, the influence of a physical education minute on concentration of attention in 8- to 10-year-olds was investigated for the first time. According to the results of this research, this influence is positive in second- and third-graders considered together.
Data from in children in the CG indicate that children's concentration decreases during a lesson. This is proven by the results in the CG in all three trials conducted from the beginning to the end of the experiment. The overall group scores decreased on Monday, Wednesday, and Friday. This may also indicate that children did not show adaptation (habituation) to the test between their first and second attempts to complete it, since their scores would have increased in this case. In our case, in the CG, the scores decreased between attempts in all trials.
In the e.g., scores improved between the pre-test (before the start of the minute of exercise) and the post-test (after its completion) in all trials. This indicates that the introduction of a physical education minute in the educational process is an effective means of improving concentration of attention in schoolchildren aged 8–10 years.
It should be noted that a number of studies have reported on the beneficial influence of physical activity on indicators of attention, thinking, and cognitive abilities. However, as a rule, these studies have involved either fully-fledged physical education lessons at school or sports research (18, 19). In our case, the influence of a physical education minute on concentration of attention in 8- to 10-year-olds was examined. According to the results obtained by applying the Benjamin Bourdon test, this effect turned out to be positive.
Within the existing literature, there are no publications replicating our research exactly. However, Geji et al. (20) showed that short bouts of aerobic exercise improved subsequent cognitive performance in adolescents by influencing their inhibitory control. Furthermore, Siersbaek et al. (21) found that participation in sport during leisure time improved working memory, the ability to begin an activity, and the ability to generate ideas, responses, or problem-solving strategies in first-graders.
A limitation of this study was the lack of assessment of the development quotient of participants (22). Furthermore, only a correlation was identified between PEM administration and the concentration test, prompting further research on additional co-determinants of concentration.
Of course, the design of this study could be expanded and supplemented by studying the effect of a physical education minute on memory, thinking, and other mental processes by applying a number of other mental tests. However, this study and its results scientifically prove the effectiveness of the use of a physical education minute in the educational process for schoolchildren aged 8–10 years.
5. Conclusions
When students aged 8–10 years participated in a set of exercises forming a “physical education minute” in the middle of a lesson, their concentration indicators improved significantly. The present study has demonstrated the effectiveness of the physical education minute in regular lessons at school. It is recommended that a small set of exercises be performed for one and a half or two minutes in the middle of the lesson. Following this, children's concentration of attention will improve, which is important for further learning and processing of educational material.
Data availability statement
The raw data supporting the conclusions of this article will be made available by the authors, without undue reservation.
Ethics statement
The studies involving humans were approved by Research Ethics Committee, Vyatka State University. The studies were conducted in accordance with the local legislation and institutional requirements. Written informed consent for participation in this study was provided by the participants’ legal guardians/next of kin.
Author contributions
GP: Conceptualization, Data curation, Formal Analysis, Investigation, Methodology, Project administration, Resources, Software, Supervision, Validation, Visualization, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing. FC: Conceptualization, Data curation, Formal Analysis, Investigation, Methodology, Project administration, Resources, Software, Supervision, Validation, Visualization, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing. JP: Conceptualization, Data curation, Formal Analysis, Investigation, Methodology, Project administration, Resources, Software, Supervision, Validation, Visualization, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing. LPA: Conceptualization, Data curation, Formal Analysis, Investigation, Methodology, Project administration, Resources, Software, Supervision, Validation, Visualization, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing.
Funding
The author(s) declare that no financial support was received for the research, authorship, and/or publication of this article.
Acknowledgments
The authors wish to thank the local secondary school for assisting with program recruitment.
Conflict of interest
The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.
The author(s) declared that they were an editorial board member of Frontiers, at the time of submission. This had no impact on the peer review process and the final decision.
Publisher's note
All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.
References
1. Noor S, Haseen F, Tomal LA, Noor N. Assessment of electronic gadgets use and its’ effects on daily life and health of primary school children. J Prev Soc Med. (2020) 39:78–87. doi: 10.3329/jopsom.v39i1.51866
2. Radwan N, Kabbash IA, Abd-Ellatif E, Abu-Elenin M. Bullying and associated common health manifestations among primary school children in Tanta city, Egypt. Egyp Fam Med J. (2021) 5:4–19. doi: 10.21608/EFMJ.2021.41645.1044
3. Verma N, Bajpai PK. Health status of primary school children: study in urban slums of lucknow. Int J Community Med Public Health. (2021) 8:2228–32. doi: 10.18203/2394-6040.ijcmph20211449
4. Miheeva AI. The importance of physical education in modern schools. Theor Appl Sci. (2022) 110:224–32. doi: 10.15863/TAS.2022.06.110.37
5. Singh LS. A global perspective of physical education in school. Int J Pharmamedix India. (2019) 7:38–41.
6. Caamaño-Navarrete F, Latorre-Román PÁ, Párraga-Montilla J, Jerez-Mayorga D, Delgado-Floody P. Selective atten-tion and concentration are related to lifestyle in Chilean schoolchildren. Children (Basel). (2021) 8:856. doi: 10.3390/children8100856
7. Gelabert J, De la Vega R, Palou PP, Muntaner-Mas A. The effects of a moderate-to-high intensity interval exercise training programme on selective and sustained attention in schoolchildren aged 11 and 12. J Hum Sport Exerc. (2022) 18:1–15. doi: 10.14198/jhse.2023.181.17
8. Kainov AN, Kuryerova GI. Working Programs. Physical Culture. Grades 1–11. Comprehensive Program of Physical Educa-tion of School Children. Teacher: Moskow, Russia. (2019).
9. Gurev SV. Theory and methodology of physical education with practical training (preschool and primary school age children). Moscow, Russia. (2022).
10. Livak P, Viatokha V, Melnyk Y. Meaningful content of physical education minutes in primary school lessons in the context of some research. Hum Sci Curr Issues. (2021) 2:225–32. doi: 10.24919/2308-4863/40-2-37
11. Prysyazhniuk S. The use of small forms of active rest to improve the mental capacity of second or third students elementary school classes. Sci J Natl Pedagogical Dragomanov Univ. (2020) 3:119–25. doi: 10.31392/NPU-nc.series15.2020.3(123).23
12. Masini A, Marini S, Gori D, Leoni E, Rochira A, Dallolio L. Evaluation of school-based interventions of active breaks in primary schools: a systematic review and meta-analysis. J Sci Med Sport. (2020) 23:377–84. doi: 10.1016/j.jsams.2019.10.008
13. Oldfield RC. The assessment and analysis of handedness: the Edinburgh inventory. Neuropsychologia. (1971) 9:97–113. doi: 10.1016/0028-3932(71)90067-4
15. Zakharova LM, Zakharova V. Influence of motor activity on the development of voluntary attention in children aged 6-7 years in rural and urban micro-community. Int Educ Stud. (2018) 11:126–31. doi: 10.5539/ies.v11n4p126
16. Hopkins W. Measures of reliability in sports medicine and science. Sport Med. (2000) 30:1–15. doi: 10.2165/00007256-200030010-00001
17. Cohen J. Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences, 2nd ed. Hillsdale, USA: Erlbaum (1988).
18. Dyrla-Mularczyk K, Giemza-Urbanowicz W. The influence of physical activity on the functioning of the nervous system and cognitive processes–research review. Neuropsychiatr I Neuropsychol. (2019) 14:84–91. doi: 10.5114/nan.2019.89792
19. López-Vicente M, Forns J, Esnaola M, Suades-González E, Álvarez-Pedrerol M, Robinson O, et al. Physical activity and cognitive trajectories in schoolchildren. Pediatr Exerc Sci. (2016) 28:431–8. doi: 10.1123/pes.2015-0157
20. Gejl AK, Bugge A, Ernst MT, Tarp J, Hillman CH, Have M, et al. The acute effects of short bouts of exercise on inhibitory control in adolescents. Ment Health Phys Act. (2018) 15:34–9. doi: 10.1016/j.mhpa.2018.06.003
21. Siersbaek GM, Have M, Wedderkopp N. The effect of leisure time sport on executive functions in danish 1st grade children. Children (Basel). (2022) 9:1458. doi: 10.3390/children9101458
Keywords: schoolchildren, lesson, physical education, health, concentration
Citation: Polevoy G, Cazan F, Padulo J and Ardigò LP (2023) The effectiveness of one “physical education minute” during lessons to develop concentration in 8- to 10-year-old schoolchildren. Front. Sports Act. Living 5:1283296. doi: 10.3389/fspor.2023.1283296
Received: 4 September 2023; Accepted: 10 October 2023;
Published: 10 November 2023.
Edited by:
Pedro Forte, Higher Institute of Educational Sciences of the Douro, PortugalReviewed by:
Alexandra Malheiro, Higher Institute of Educational Sciences of the Douro, PortugalSoukaina Hattabi, University of Jendouba, Tunisia
José Eduardo Teixeira, Polytechnic Institute of Bragança (IPB), Portugal
© 2023 Polevoy, Cazan, Padulo and Ardigò. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.
*Correspondence: Luca Paolo Ardigò luca.ardigo@nla.no
†These authors have contributed equally to this work and share last authorship