Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Sports Act. Living, 20 February 2023
Sec. Elite Sports and Performance Enhancement

Differences in visio-spatial intelligence between non-athletes and netball players

  • Department of Human Movement Science, University of Zululand, Kwadlangezwa, South Africa

There is conflicting evidence regarding whether athletes have better visio-spatial skills than non-athletes. This gap may result from athletes' superiority in only some visio-spatial abilities (VSS), rather than all areas of vision. The aim of this study was to determine whether there is a significant difference in the visio-spatial intelligence between female netball players (n = 40) and non-athletes (n = 40) when comparing six visual skills (accommodation facility, saccadic eye movement, speed of recognition, peripheral awareness, hand-eye coordination, and visual memory). Following an optometric evaluation, the participants were assessed in six distinct established tests, including the hart near far rock, saccadic eye movement, evasion, accumulator, ball wall toss tests, and flash memory, to evaluate the VSS components of non-athletes and premier league netball players. For five of the six tests, there was a statistically significant (p ≤ 0.05) difference between netball players and non-athletes. Conversely, there is no concrete evidence that netball players have better visual memory than non-athletes (p = 0.277). When compared to non-athletes, netball players have significantly improved accommodation facility (p < .001), saccadic eye movements (p < .001), speed of recognition (p < .001), peripheral awareness (p < .001), and hand-eye coordination (p < .001), but not visual memory (p = 0.277). The findings that netball players perform better on a certain VSS have broad ramifications for theories of sport vision, the optimal way to choose tests, and the creation of VSS testing batteries for specific sports.

1. Introduction

Top-level sports performance in stressful and challenging situations necessitates the optimal functioning of a variety of cognitive skills such as decision making, attention and working memory (1). According to Vestberg et al. (2) improved brain functioning in male and female athletes may improve performance and predict competitive success. This cognitive functioning is thought to be especially important in open sports that require constant attention, management of multiple factors, or adaptation to changing settings (3). The visual motor system makes use of three primary ocular motor skills: vergence which is the coordinated movement of the eyes' pupils during focusing, either toward or away from one another; focusing (to eliminate irrelevant background information and concentrate on crucial visual information) and tracking (a visual processing ability that takes place when the eyes fixate on a moving object in the field of vision). Wilson and Falkel (1) state that the eyes must be able to converge (or cross) as the ball comes toward the player and uncross (or diverge) when the ball moves away from the player.

In the past, classification, and comparison of athletes to non-athletes have depended on physical condition assessment and training methods (4). However, there is now growing interest in how testing and training for vision might influence sports performance and help differentiate between athletes and non-athletes (5, 6). According to Gao et al. (7), athletes may have better vision than the general population. However, because different components of vision are used in different sports and because there are various visual skills, athletes can develop their visual abilities through practice and assessment (8, 9). Visio-spatial skills (VSS) are a broad category of specialized abilities that range from recognizing light and dark, recognizing intricate intersecting angles and curves, to recognizing faces from the features of their hair, noses, lips, and eyes (10, 11). and is an essential element of coordination in everyday living and across all sporting codes.

Netball can be categorized as non-static sport due to the visual information changing and needing to be processed continuously for longer than an hour, which requires visual attention, and dynamism for the player to keep processing game-based information (12). According to Abernethy (13), most of the visual testing and training programs tend to concentrate primarily on the fundamental visual functions, failing to recognize the necessity for sport-specific testing and training to be effective and increase on-field performance.

In the past, studies on netball players have mostly concentrated on physiology, performance enhancement, injuries, and masculinity (1417). Limited research exists on netball players' visio-spatial intelligence, especially with relation to a sport specific test battery, with the majority focusing on multiple team sports and recreation (1820). However, it has been proved that VSS can improve brain function when stimulated appropriately (21), and non-athletes use their VSS daily to carry out activities, yet they are frequently inactive, which might cause a decline in several VSS (22). In this vein, it is critical to identify which VSS are amplified in netball players, if any, because there is conflicting information on VSS in athletes vs. non-athletes.

2. Aims and hypothesis

The aim of this study was to discern whether there is a significant difference in the visio-spatial intelligence between netball players and non-athletes. In this study, six VSS components including accommodation facility, saccadic eye movement, speed of recognition, peripheral awareness, hand-eye coordination, and visual memory were examined between netball players and non-athletes. The hypothesis is as follows: The visio-spatial intelligence will be significantly higher in netball players when compared to non-athletes. There is a definite and clear lack of research with regards to using sport specific test batteries when testing athletes (1820). For the purposes of this study a proven visio-spatial intelligence test battery was employed and a study using such a test battery on netball players, and then comparing the results to non-athletes seems crucial (18).

3. Methods

3.1. Participants

The study sampled (N = 80) 18- to 27-year-old females. The participants were divided into non-athletes (n = 40) who have not participated in any form of sport, and female premier league netball players (n = 40) (Table 1). One requires a lot of participants to get the richness of data or the significant difference necessary for qualitative analysis (at least 30 to 60) (23). It was decided to test 40 participants since that's the number of netball players that were available. Since research indicates that there is a VSS difference between males and females, it was decided to only compare females to one another so that sex differences could not influence the results (24). After institutional ethics approval and informed consent. Non-athletes were recruited from the Zululand region of South Africa's KwaZulu-Natal province and were not required to participate in any structured sporting/physical activity program (25). A non-probability sampling strategy was used to choose netball players from all premier league netball teams in the same geographic area as the non-athletes. Players in the Premier League are required to have participated in at least one competitive match per week for the previous three months and have completed 30 h of netball training. Age matching was done among participants in each group to maintain balance. Institutional ethics approval date and number (UZREC 171110-030 PGM 2022/55).

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Comparing descriptive information of netball players and non-athletes.

3.2. Optometric assessment

The Spectrum Eyecare program (Version 6.0.0, Digital Optometry, Republic of South Africa) was used to conduct a general optometric screening on each participant to ascertain whether any visual impairments prevented them from participating in the study. All participants claimed to have normal or corrected-to-normal 20/20 vision, only 2 netball players wore glasses normally for corrective vision, but used contact lenses during the testing to ensure playing conditions (as they use contact lenses in matches and practice sessions) were simulated. None of the participants had ever taken a VSS test before.

3.3. Visio-spatial skill test battery

To reduce the impact of any food or supplement influences, testing was performed in the post-absorptive state after a 9 to 12-hour fast on weekday mornings between 07:00 and 12:00. Participants were only evaluated 48 h after participating in any physical activity to minimize any physical and psychological consequences. All participants were verbally questioned to ensure they had followed the dietary requirements. The same VSS test battery was administered to both groups, and tests were run in the order listed below to prevent the influence of earlier tests on subsequent test: Accommodation facility, saccadic eye movements, speed of recognition, hand-eye coordination, peripheral awareness, and visual memory. To ensure appropriate recovery, a five-minute rest interval was observed in between each session.

3.4. Strengths and limitations

The study had several strengths. It gave an in-depth look at the visual abilities needed for netball and the visual differences between athletes and non-athletes, the study described in detail the design of the testing protocol and how it was performed, for replication purposes. For the purposes of the study, participants were picked in accordance with the research design. The study appropriately conveys the data's statistical significance that was found between the two groups, which is novel in nature specifically with relation to netball players. Sample size is a possible limitation, as there was only a limited amount of netball players in the region which was available and willing to be part of the study. There are multiple factors affecting vision, and even though this was taken into account when testing and creating the protocol, there is always the possibility of factors outside the parameters of control having an influence on the tests.

3.4.1. Accommodation facility

A Hart chart was used to assess an eye's capacity to locate and identify an item at a certain distance, as well as its position, orientation, and refractive errors (26, 27). The test has a reliability of 0.7, making it a legitimate and reliable test to use (18, 28). On a wall three meters away, the Hart Chart was positioned at each participant's head height. A second chart had to be held by the participants at arm's length using their dominant hand, the paper was held perpendicular to the body at shoulder height in the frontal plane and their hands had to be stationary. The participants were then requested to read aloud the first letter of the first line of the chart from a three meters distance, and they were then told to read aloud the first letter of the chart from an arm's length away (28, 29). After 30 s, the participants were instructed to continue in this manner, at which point their errors and final score were recorded (i.e., how many letters were read correctly). The participant's number of errors was deducted from the final score to arrive at the final score. To verify that letters cannot be memorized, various charts were utilized (26, 28). A five-minute break was taken between each session, and the highest score from the two trials was recorded and applied in the analysis. This test was time based the participants had to score as high as possible in the allocated time.

3.4.2. Saccadic eye movements

A saccadic eye movement chart was used to measure how quickly the participant's eyes travel when moving the fovea to a new spot in the visual surroundings (1, 30, 31). This test has a reliability of 0.703 (28). On both sides of the page, this chart included movable letters that ran vertically downward. To prevent letters from being memorized, various charts were utilized (28). The subject stood 3 m away from two saccadic eye movement charts that were mounted on a wall and spaced apart by 1 m. The subject was then required to quickly turn to the right chart and read aloud the first letter on the lateral side before doing the same on the left chart's lateral side. The participants were told to just move their eyes and to keep their heads steady. The duration of the test was 30 s, and both the final score and the number of faults were recorded (i.e., how many letters were read). By deducting the number of mistakes from the total number of letters read, the final score was determined. The highest score from the two trials—each with a five-minute rest period between them was recorded and used in the analysis (28). This test was time based the participants had to score as high as possible in the allocated time.

3.4.3. Speed of recognition

A Batak Pro (Quotronics, Surrey, United Kingdom) was used to measure the participant's speed of recognition (32), which has a reliability of 0.946 and requires the subject to react to a mobile stimulus (28). The Batak pro equipment is distinguished by its ability to emit light with a lifespan of one second, which is utilized to trigger a neurological speed of response in test subjects in sports such as tennis, basketball, rugby, and netball as well as non-athletes (18). In addition to the “scoring” LED screen displaying each successful strike, the Batak's “time” LED panel counts down the targets from 100 to zero, this software allowed 100 timed targets to glow at random. The targets were kept lit for a possible strike window of one second. The entire procedure accelerated if the incorrect target was hit, or if it was hit “out of time.” The subject was instructed to avoid hitting any flashing targets while performing this exercise. The athlete received a verbal “foul” from the apparatus and lost five points for hitting the target. The infrared beam was released, and the participant had to dodge it instantly to avoid losing five points if all the center targets lit up simultaneously. The device kept track of the mistakes made, and after automatically subtracting them from the final score, it calculated the score (32). The highest score from the two trials was recorded and used in the analysis after a five-minute break between each session. The maximum score for this test was 100 points.

3.4.4. Peripheral awareness

The Accumulator Program of the Batak Pro (32) was used to measure participants' capacity to see things and motion that are not directly in front of them. This test has a reliability of 0.885 (18). As part of the Accumulator Program, random targets on the Batak Pro remained lighted for 60 s before the participant touched them with their hands (32). Two trials were conducted with a five-minute break in between, and the system recorded the targets that were successfully touched in 60 s. The top score was then noted and used to the analysis's final stage. This test was time based the participants had to score as high as possible in the allocated time.

3.4.5. Hand-eye coordination

The current study used the tennis ball wall throw to evaluate hand-eye coordination (33). This test has a reliability of 0.708 (28). Each participant had to toss the ball at the wall from a point that was indicated 2 meters away and catch it with their alternate hands, they started tossing the ball using their non-dominant hand first. This was done by participants for 30 s (28). Scores were tallied for each ball that the participant successfully caught. Within the allotted 30 s, the participants had to complete as many catches as they could. The highest score from the two trials were recorded and used in the analysis after a five-minute break between each session. This test was time based the participants had to score as high as possible in the allocated time.

3.4.6. Visual memory

To assess the memory from which the visual system received the stored information, six randomly chosen targets were flashed for 0.5 s using the Batak Pro Flash Program (28). The reliability of this test is 0.735 (28). Participants had to correctly recall the individual targets and the order in which they were triggered after hearing the “double beep” instruction to strike the lighted targets (which lit up in a specific order). There was a maximum number of fifty-four targets the subject could strike, and with every incorrect target struck, this score would decrease. The “score b” LED panel showed the maximum score, and the “score a” LED screen indicated the points scored for each successful strike (32). The number of successful strikes was then recorded. In the final data analysis, the highest score from the two trials was recorded and used. Between trials, there was a five-minute rest period. A possible highest score for this test is 54 points.

3.5. Data analysis

The study made use of pre-existing VSS evaluations and quantitative research methods. Using the Statistical Package for Social Sciences (SPSS) version 22 for Windows (IBM Corporation, Armonk, NY, United States) the descriptive statistics for the data collected, including standard deviations, means, percentage difference and ranges, were assessed for this study. The Mann-Whitney U test was used to assess differences between the two independent groups because the dependent variable was continuous and not regularly distributed. To confirm the Mann–Whitney U test findings and determine which netball players had the more superior visio-spatial skills, a rank-ordered analysis was conducted. This was done in order to establish the significance of differences that exist between the results of the specific tests, to determine which visual skill and test showed the highest significant difference. Statistical significance was set at p ≤ 0.05.

4. Results

The findings showed that there was a significant difference (p ≤ .05) between netball players and non-athletes in five of the six tests (Table 2). In terms of accommodation facility (p < .001), saccadic eye movement (p < .001), speed of recognition (p < .001), peripheral awareness (p < .001) and hand-eye coordination (p < .001) netball players significantly outperformed non-players, but not in terms of visual memory (p = .277).

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Visio-motor expertise comparison in netball players and non-athletes.

Netball players performed 55% faster at speed of recognition than non-athletes, according to a rank-ordered analysis, followed by 26% for saccadic eye movement, 19% for accommodation facility, 16% for hand-eye coordination, and 11% for peripheral awareness (Table 1).

5. Discussion

The aim of this study was to discern whether the visio-spatial intelligence is higher in netball players in comparison to non-athletes. In this study, six VSS components including accommodation facility, saccadic eye movement, speed of recognition, peripheral awareness, hand-eye coordination, and visual memory were particularly examined between netball players and non-athletes.

The current study discovered that the accommodation facility is higher in netball players when compared to non-athletes. This is comparable to studies by Jafarzadehpur et al. (34) and Gao et al. (7) where the female volleyball players showed better facility of accommodation than non-athletes. However, Ripoll & Latiri (35) found that accommodative facility did not differ significantly in this regard. For ball interceptions, control, passing, and throwing as well as analyzing the positions of teammates and opponents, among other things, near/far visual alterations are constantly occurring (36). These actions encourage the vergence/accommodative system's continual involvement, which may have effects like those of visual therapy exercises (37). Some athletes were concluded to have a lower amplitude of accommodation than non-athletes, suggesting that they are only superior to non-athletes when they focus on multiple targets rather than single targets which may explain why no increase in accommodation facility was found in the studies of Zwierko et al. (38). Accommodation facility allows athletes to quickly shift their attention to follow the ball while sustaining visual clarity.

This study agrees with the study by Zwierko et al. (39) where soccer players showed better saccadic eye movements than non-athletes. The study by Babu, Mellers and Irving (40) showed that the athletes did not respond better in terms of accuracy of saccades compared to non-athletes. Athletes need to suppress prosaccades strongly and prepare for voluntary events more quickly to do this (41). Training shortens the antisaccade production phase, according to Vera et al. (42). Abernethy (13), on the other hand asserts that visual training is unable to enhance either motor or fundamental visual function. However, Maman, Gaurang and Shadu (43) revealed that visual training improves visual functions, including saccadic eye movements. Yilmaz and Polat (44), hypothesized that training and having a greater visual attention level may be crucial factors in an athlete's ability to suppress reflexive prosaccades. For this reason, experienced athletes show more effective gaze strategies than novice athletes (45). The study by Babu, Mellers and Irving (40) showed that the athletes did not respond better in terms of accuracy of saccades compared to non-athletes which was possibly due to poor spatial organization skills caused by a lack of controllability in monitoring factors that effects visual skills like caffeine intake. A smooth tracking movement or a quick hop may be required when the athlete quickly scans from one sight to the next.

The finding of speed of recognition in netball players and non-athletes in this study is consistent with that of Millard et al. (18), where they demonstrated that rugby players were superior to non-athletes, and Zwierko (46) who found better speed of recognition skills when comparing handball players to non-athletes. The current study discovered that the largest difference between netball players and non-athletes (in that netball was visio-spatial scores were higher for netball players when compared to non-athletes) was found in speed of recognition, with netball players scoring 55% higher than non-athletes. According to Kaya (47), decision-making in the world of sport exhibits three features. First off, since decision-making is naturalistic, athletes naturally confront it in a sport context with some level of task familiarity. Additionally, if the test battery is sport-specific, athletes will do better than non-athletes because they have improved decision-making abilities when presented with a visio-spatial decision that frequently arises in their sport (47). Lastly, most sports decisions are dynamic, thus they usually become clearer with time. The dynamic feature of decision-making has a double impact, which means that information is not instantaneously gathered and processed, but rather a decision-maker must accumulate knowledge over time, and subsequent processing of this information takes additional time. Sports-related decisions, however, include external dynamics, which means that the circumstance itself can change over time (47). In addition, most decisions made by athletes are made while they are moving, which makes them more difficult to make. Speed of recognition may be the most important VSS associated with netball success since decision making in sport is realistic, dynamic, and executed in motion (which raises the difficulty level). To make play-related choices like whether to pass the ball, where to position oneself next, and other things, players need to be able to process a lot of information with just a fast sweep of the court.

Several studies have shown that athletes have better peripheral awareness and the ability to track a moving target, as well as a different strategy for processing visual information than non-athletes (4850). Ciućmański and Wątroba (51) found that 12-year-old football players performed better than their non-athletic classmates in tests of depth perception, peripheral vision, and the capacity to visually track a moving object. It was found that visual perception training raised the levels of these skills and, as a result, improved the effectiveness of an athlete's perception. However, football players aged 9 to 17 with varying degrees of experience and non-athletes had their peripheral vision tested by Ward and Williams (52). Although they noticed an increase in visual function with aging, expert soccer players did not perform noticeably better than players with less athletic background. Furthermore, none of the groups under comparison demonstrated visual abilities that were above average compared to the overall population. In situations like those that demand watching a ball and numerous players in order to process essential information, peripheral vision will likely be used extensively, and attention will be distributed proportionally to various locations.

The findings of this study can be compared with those of Venter and Ferreira (53), who evaluated rugby players of various ages on their visual skills and had better hand-eye coordination compared to non-athletes. The authors anticipated that age and associated motor development could have an impact on visual perception abilities. Moreso, Kioumourtzoglou et al. (54) found a comparable result in members of a Greek national team, arguing different perceptual strategies from experts to novices in relevant and irrelevant cues. The fact that people with inherent neurological advantages like eye-hand reaction time and visuospatial intelligence can easily participate in sports and the fact that exercise is good for eye-hand reaction time and visio-spatial intelligence are two explanations for the athlete's advantages in both areas (21). To sprint, jump, catch, change directions, push, shoot, and block, players must master high-level of eye body coordination.

The only VSS out of the six assessed that was shown to be comparable between netball players and non-athletes was visual memory. A study by Tomczyk et al. (55) and this study's results are comparable. Another study compared the visual memory capacity of non-athlete college students to that of team sport athletes and found no significant differences (56) but researchers also examined exercise's potential to enhance visual memory, the capacity to actively monitor, alter, and interpret fresh information. These studies have validated the idea that more engagement in sports is linked to improved visual memory abilities, much like exercise (57, 58). Our contradictory result may have several explanations, but one of them is that while individual differences in naturalistic exercise habits do not correspond to noticeable between group differences in visual memory, improvements in visual memory have been shown in previous studies to occur relative to each participant's baseline skills in response to the experiment's increased levels of physical activity (56). Players will be able to make rapid and accurate decisions about what to do next in a game by employing visual memory with the aid of the visuals they record.

The findings of this research demonstrated that netball players surpassed non-athletes in VSS performance. There is evidence that during visio-spatial demands, athletes develop unusual mechanisms of occipital brain synchronization, demonstrating superior visio-motor performance to non-athletes (59). It is crucial to recognize the significance of a fortuitous genetic tendency towards an increased VSS in sportsmen. Components of athletic performance like power, endurance, flexibility strength, neuromuscular coordination, psychological qualities, and other phenotypes are greatly influenced by genetic variables. Athlete status is therefore a heritable trait, with additive genetic variables accounting for an average of 66% (depending on the athletic discipline) of the variance in athlete status. Non-shared environmental factors are responsible for the remaining variation (60). Sports vision training methods and consistent practice help to enhance specific visual skills.

The current study's hypothesized that netball players would have better VSS than non-athletes, based on previous research. This was supported by the fact that this study found that netball players had 55% higher speed of recognition skills than non-athletes, followed by 26% for saccadic eye movement, 19% for accommodation facility, 16% for hand-eye coordination, and 11% for peripheral awareness. These results imply that these VSS may be required in varying degrees of importance for netball performance. To ascertain whether the level of play or some other variable may be associated to VSS in athletes and/or for a certain sport, it is also important to record and correlate the level of expertise of the netball players (i.e., the number of years of practice).

It is crucial to comprehend how the visual system works during sport performance in order to decide whether, when, and how sportsmen and sportswomen might be supported to improve their skill performance through vision-based intervention programs (61). Analog “eye fitness” activities were once employed as the first methods of sport visual training (62). These challenging drills forced students to rapidly switch from convergence, accommodation, saccadic, and/or pursuit eye movements to visual targets, placing great demands on the oculomotor system. Using the same set of analog vision training activities over a five-week period, it was shown that individuals who got vision training exhibited faster and more accurate netball passing compared to the control group (62).

6. Conclusion and practical implications

The finding that only a few VSS are superior to others has far-reaching implications for vision theories such as theories of visual perception (direct, indirect, and computational), the best way to select tests, and the development of sport specific VSS testing batteries. While this study supports the idea that athletes, particularly netball players, have better VSS than non-athletes, this study also found that this is not the case with all VSS, as with visual memory in this study. Similarly, to how anthropometric and physiological tests are used in athlete selection and recruitment, research should be conducted to determine which VSS tests are more capable of distinguishing athletes from non-athletes. Beginners and other netball players who could require greater visual performance may be considered to need visual training. Future research should examine how much visual perceptual learning and physical fitness influence the findings now seen in elite athletes.

Data availability statement

The raw data supporting the conclusions of this article will be made available by the authors, without undue reservation.

Ethics statement

The studies involving human participants were reviewed and approved by University of Zululand Ethical Review Board. The patients/participants provided their written informed consent to participate in this study.

Author contributions

NM, LM, GJB and MM, all had equal input, in the design of the study, article dissection, content of the study, writing as well as the formulation and direction of the article. All authors contributed to the article and approved the submitted version.

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher's note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

References

1. Wilson TA, Falkel JE. Sports vision: Training for better performance. Champaign: Human Kinetics (2004).

2. Vestberg T, Reinebo G, Maurex L, Ingvar M, Petrovic P. Core executive functions are associated with success in young elite soccer players. PLoS One. (2017) 12(2):e0170845. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.017084528178738

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

3. Verburgh L, Scherder EJA, van Lange PAM, Oosterlaan J. Executive functioning in highly talented soccer players. PLoS One. (2014) 9(3):e91254. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.009125424632735

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

4. Du Toit PJ, Krüger PE, Fowler KF, Govender C, Clark J. Influence of sports vision techniques on adult male rugby players. African J Physic Health Edu Rec Dance. (2010) 16(3):487–94. doi: 10.4314/ajpherd.v16i3.60931

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

5. Du Toit PJ, Krüger PE, Chamane NZ, Campher J, Crafford D. Sport vision assessment in soccer players. Afr J Phys Health Edu Rec Dance. (2009) 15:594–604. doi: 10.4314/ajpherd.v15i4.49546

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

6. Millard L, Shaw I, Breukelman GJ, Shaw BS. Factors affecting vision and visio-spatial intelligence (VSI) in sport: a review of the literature. Asian J Sports Med. (2020) 11:e101670. doi: 10.5812/asjsm.101670

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

7. Gao Y, Chen L, Yang S, Wang H, Yao J, Dai Q, et al. Contributions of visuo-oculomotor abilities to interceptive skills in sports. Optom Vis Sci. (2015) 92:679–89. doi: 10.1097/OPX.000000000000059925930979

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

8. Barrett BT, Flavell JC, Bennett SJ, Cruickshank AG, Mankowska A, Harris JM, et al. Vision, and visual history in elite/near-elite-level cricketers and rugby-league players. Sports Med Open. (2017) 3:39. doi: 10.1186/s40798-017-0106-z29127516

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

9. Millard L, Shaw I, Breukelman GJ, Shaw BS. Visio-spatial skills in athletes: comparison of rugby players and non-athletes. Sport Sci Health. (2020) 17:137–43. doi: 10.1007/s11332-020-00663-1

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

10. Wolbers T, Hegarty M. What determines our navigational abilities? Trends Cogn Sci. (2010) 14:138–46. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2010.01.00120138795

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

11. Sasikumar N, Roja MP, Fathima MP. Visuospatial skills and teaching learning process. Indian Streams Res J. (2014) 4:1–4.

Google Scholar

12. Coker CA. Motor learning and control for practitioners. 4th ed. New York: Routledge (2017). doi: 10.4324/9781315185613

13. Abernethy B. Training the visual-perceptual skills of athletes: insights from the study of motor expertise. Am J Sports Med. (1996) 24:S89–92. doi: 10.1177/036354659602406S24

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

14. Shynkaruk O, Shutova S, Serebriakov O, Nagorna V, Skorohod O. Competitive performance of elite athletes in modern ice hockey. J Phys Educ Sport. (2020) 20:511–6. doi: 10.7752/jpes.2020.s1076

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

15. Barboza SD, Joseph C, Nauta J, van Mechelen W, Verhagen E. Injuries in field hockey players: a systematic review. Sports Med. (2018) 48:849–66. doi: 10.1007/s40279-017-0839-329299879

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

16. Omar R, Kuan YM, Zuhairi NA, Manan FA, Knight VF. Visual efficiency among teenaged athletes and non-athletes. Int J Ophthalmol. (2017) 10:1460–4. doi: 10.18240/ijo.2017.09.2028944208

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

17. Goodman D, Gaetz M, Meichenbaum D. Concussions in hockey: there is cause for concern. Med Sci Sports Exerc. (2001) 33:2004–9. doi: 10.1097/00005768-200112000-0000511740291

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

18. Millard L, Shaw I, Breukelman GJ, Shaw BS. Differences in visio-spatial expertise between 1st division rugby players and non-athletes. Heliyon. (2021) 7:e06290. doi: 10.1016/j.heliyon.2021.e0629033665445

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

19. Heppe H, Kohler A, Fleddermann M, Zentgraf K. The relationship between expertise in sports, visuospatial, and basic cognitive skills. Front Psychol. (2016) 7:904. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2016.0090427378994

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

20. Kumar A, Vishal . Visual response time & visuospatial intelligence scores of athletes & nonathletes. J Exerc Sci Physiother. (2013) 9:125–8. doi: 10.18376//2013/v9i2/67565

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

21. Akarsu S, Çalişkan E, Dane Ş. Athletes have faster eye-hand visual reaction times and higher scores on visuospatial intelligence than nonathletes. Turk J Med Sci. (2009) 39:871–4. doi: 10.3906/sag-0809-44

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

22. Khanal S. Impact of visual skills training on sports performance: current and future perspectives. Adv Ophthalmol Vis Syst. (2015) 2:26–8. doi: 10.15406/aovs.2015.02.00032

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

23. Morse JM. Determining sample size. Qual Health Res. (2000) 10:3–5. doi: 10.1177/104973200129118183

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

24. Popowczak M, Domaradzki J, Rokita A, Zwierko M, Zwierko T. Predicting visual-motor performance in a reactive agility task from selected demographic, training, anthropometric, and functional variables in adolescents. Int J Environ Res Public Health. (2020) 17:5322. doi: 10.3390/ijerph1715532232722036

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

25. Stern BS, Kysilka ML. Contemporary readings in curriculum. California: SAGE Publications, Inc (2008).

26. Vera J, Redondo B, Molina R, Koulieris G, Jiménez R. Validation of an objective method for the qualitative and quantitative assessment of binocular accommodative facility. Curr Eye Res. (2020) 45:636–44. doi: 10.1080/02713683.2019.168883731675903

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

27. Radomski MV, Trombly Latham CA. Occupational therapy for physical dysfunction. 6th ed. Philadelphia: Lippincott Williams and Wilkins (2008).

28. Millard L. Impact of a visual skills training program on the visual ability of elite rugby players. [dissertation]. Nelson Mandela University (2016).

29. Dhote SA. Eye exercises- an eyesight rejuvenation therapy. World J Pharm Res. (2015) 4:1552–63.

Google Scholar

30. Crawford TJ, Higham S, Renvoize T, Patel J, Dale M, Suriya A, et al. Inhibitory control of saccadic eye movements and cognitive impairment in Alzheimer's disease. Biol Psychiatry. (2005) 57:1052–60. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2005.01.01715860346

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

31. Crawford TJ, Smith ES, Berry DM. Eye gaze and aging: selective and combined effects of working memory and inhibitory control. Front Hum Neurosci. (2017) 11:563. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2017.0056329230169

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

32. BATAK Pro Hardware Operation. Users manual. Horley: Quotronics Limited (2011).

33. Faber IR, Oosterveld FGJ, Nijhuis-Van der Sanden MWG. Does an eye-hand coordination test have added value as part of talent identification in table tennis? A validity and reproducibility study. PLoS One. (2014) 9:e85657. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.008565724465638

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

34. Jafarzadehpur E, Aazami N, Bolouri B. Comparison of saccadic eye movements and facility of ocular accommodation in female volleyball players and non-players. Scand J Med Sci Sports. (2007) 17:186–90. doi: 10.1111/j.1600-0838.2005.00535.x17394481

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

35. Ripoll H, Latiri I. Effect of expertise on coincident-timing accuracy in a fast ball game. J Sports Sci. (1997) 15:573–80. doi: 10.1080/0264041973670019486434

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

36. Mangine GT, Hoffman JR, Wells AJ, Gonzalez AM, Rogowski JP, Townsend JR, et al. Visual tracking speed is related to basketball-specific measures of performance in NBA players. J Strength Cond Res. (2014) 28:2406–14. doi: 10.1519/JSC.000000000000055024875429

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

37. Ciuffreda KJ. The scientific basis for and efficacy of optometric vision therapy in nonstrabismic accommodative and vergence disorders. Optometry. (2002) 73:735–62. PMID: 1249856112498561

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

38. Zwierko T, Lesiakowski P, Redondo B, Vera J. Examining the ability to track multiple moving targets as a function of postural stability: a comparison between team sports players and sedentary individuals. PeerJ. (2022) 10:e13964. doi: 10.7717/peerj.1396436071825

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

39. Zwierko T, Jedziniak W, Florkiewicz B, Stępiński M, Buryta R, Kostrzewa-Nowak D, et al. Oculomotor dynamics in skilled soccer players: the effects of sport expertise and strenuous physical effort. Eur J Sport Sci. (2019) 19(5):612–20. doi: 10.1080/17461391.2018.153839130378462

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

40. Babu RJ, Meyers JP, Irving EL. Athletes’ vs. Non–athletes: an investigation comparing the latency, accuracy, peak velocity and adaptation of saccades. Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci. (2004) 45:2511.

Google Scholar

41. Munoz DP, Everling S. Look away: the anti-saccade task and the voluntary control of eye movement. Nat Rev Neurosci. (2004) 5:218–28. doi: 10.1038/nrn134514976521

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

42. Vera J, Jiménez R, Cárdenas D, Redondo B, García JA. Visual function, performance, and processing of basketball players vs. Sedentary individuals. J Sport Health Sci. (2020) 9:587–94. doi: 10.1016/j.jshs.2017.05.00133308808

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

43. Maman P, Gaurang S, Sandhu JS. The effect of vision training on performance in tennis players. Serb J Sports Sci. (2011) 5:11–6.

Google Scholar

44. Yilmaz A, Polat M. Prosaccadic and antisaccadic performance of the athletes in different types of sports. Biomed Res. (2018) 29:539–43. doi: 10.4066/biomedicalresearch.29-17-3224

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

45. Piras A, Lobietti R, Squatrito S. A study of saccadic eye movement dynamics in volleyball: comparison between athletes and non-athletes. J Sports Med Phys Fitness. (2010) 50:99–108. PMID: 2030898020308980

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

46. Zwierko T. Differences in peripheral perception between athletes and nonathletes. J Hum Kinet. (2007) 19:53–62. doi: 10.2478/v10078-008-0004-z

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

47. Kaya A. Decision making by coaches and athletes in sport. Procedia Soc Behav Sci. (2014) 152:333–8. doi: 10.1016/j.sbspro.2014.09.205

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

48. Quevedo-Junyent L, Aznar-Casanova JA, Merindano-Encina D, Cardona G, Solé-Fortó J. Comparison of dynamic visual acuity between water polo players and sedentary students. Res Q Exerc Sport. (2011) 82:644–51. doi: 10.1080/02701367.2011.1059980122276406

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

49. Uchida Y, Kudoh D, Murakami A, Honda M, Kitazawa S. Origins of superior dynamic visual acuity in baseball players: superior eye movements or superior image processing. PLoS One. (2012) 7:e31530. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.003153022384033

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

50. Alves M, Spaniol F, Erichsen O. Visual skills of elite Brazilian football players. Eur J Sport Exerc Sci. (2015) 4:8–13.

Google Scholar

51. Ciućmański B, Wątroba J. Training selected visual perception abilities and the efficiency footballers. In: Red Żak S, Spieszny M, Klocek T, editors. Gry zespołowe w wychowaniu fzycznym i sporcie. Studia i Monografe nr 33 AWF Kraków (2005). p. 298–303 (in Polish).

52. Ward P, Williams AM. Perceptual and cognitive skill development in soccer: the multidimensional nature of expert performance. J Sport Exerc Psychol. (2003) 25:93–111. doi: 10.1123/jsep.25.1.93

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

53. Venter SC, Ferreira JT. A comparison of visual skills of high school rugby players from two different age groups. S Afr Optom. (2004) 63:19–29.

Google Scholar

54. Kioumourtzoglou E, Kourtessis T, Michalopoulou M, Derri V. Differences in several perceptual abilities between experts and novices in basketball, volleyball and water-polo. Percept Mot Skills. (1998) 86:899–912. doi: 10.2466/pms.1998.86.3.8999656285

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

55. Tomczyk CP, Mormile M, Wittenberg MS, Langdon JL, Hunt TN. An examination of adolescent athletes and nonathletes on baseline neuropsychological test scores. J Athl Train. (2018) 53:404–9. doi: 10.4085/1062-6050-84-1729543036

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

56. Mayers LB, Redick TS, Chiffriller SH, Simone AN, Terraforte KR. Working memory capacity among collegiate student athletes: effects of sport-related head contacts, concussions, and working memory demands. J Clin Exp Neuropsychol. (2011) 33:532–7. doi: 10.1080/13803395.2010.53550621218317

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

57. Bijleveld E, Veling H. Separating chokers from nonchokers: predicting real-life tennis performance under pressure from behavioral tasks that tap into working memory functioning. J Sport Exerc Psychol. (2014) 36:347–56. doi: 10.1123/jsep.2013-005125226603

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

58. Furley PA, Memmert D. Working memory capacity as controlled attention in tactical decision making. J Sport Exerc Psychol. (2012) 34:322–44. doi: 10.1123/jsep.34.3.32222691397

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

59. Qiu F, Pi Y, Liu K, Zhu H, Li X, Zhang J, et al. Neural efficiency in basketball players is associated with bidirectional reductions in cortical activation and deactivation during multiple-object tracking task performance. Biol Psychol. (2019) 144:28–36. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2019.03.0030902565

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

60. Ahmetov II, Fedotovskaya ON. Current progress in sports genomics. Adv Clin Chem. (2015) 70:247–314. doi: 10.1016/bs.acc.2015.03.00326231489

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

61. Tate B, Paul M, Jaspal S. The impact of visual akills training training program on batting performance cricketers. Serb J Sports Sci. (2008) 2:17–23.

Google Scholar

62. Bressan ES. Effects of visual skills training, vision coaching and sports vision dynamics on the performance of a sport skill. Afr J Phys Health Edu Recreat. (2003) 9:20–31. doi: 10.4314/ajpherd.v9i1.24590

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: vision in sport, visio-spatial intelligence (VSI), visual skills, netball vision, sport vision

Citation: Mathe N, Millard L, Breukelman GJ and Mathunjwa M (2023) Differences in visio-spatial intelligence between non-athletes and netball players. Front. Sports Act. Living 5:1109967. doi: 10.3389/fspor.2023.1109967

Received: 28 November 2022; Accepted: 3 February 2023;
Published: 20 February 2023.

Edited by:

Kirsten Spencer, Auckland University of Technology, New Zealand

Reviewed by:

Danladi Ibrahim Musa, Kogi State University, Nigeria
Gregory Brown, University of Nebraska at Kearney, United States
Teresa Zwierko, University of Szczecin, Poland

© 2023 Mathe, Millard, Breukelman and Mathunjwa. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Lourens Millard millardl@unizulu.ac.za

Specialty Section: This article was submitted to Elite Sports and Performance Enhancement, a section of the journal Frontiers in Sports and Active Living

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.