Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Sports Act. Living, 23 May 2022
Sec. Elite Sports and Performance Enhancement

Application of the Polar Coordinate Technique in the Study of Technical-Tactical Scoring Actions in Taekwondo

  • 1Facultad de Psicología, Universidad de Málaga, Málaga, Spain
  • 2Department of Social Psychology, Social Work, Anthropology and East Asian Studies, University of Málaga, Málaga, Spain
  • 3School of Sport Sciences and Exercise, Faculty of Medicine and Surgery, University of Rome “Tor Vergata”, Rome, Italy
  • 4Department of Sport, Food and Natural Sciences, Western Norway University of Applied Sciences, Bergen, Norway

The objective of this study was to design, validate and update an observation tool to analyse the technical-tactical actions by which taekwondo players win points. An ad hoc observational tool was developed for subsequent use in HOISAN software by viewing seven finals (14 viewings) in the Rome 2019 Grand Prix, collecting data (1,382 technical/tactical actions) from both winners and losers (women, n = 3; men, n = 4). An observational methodology based on a nomothetic, follow-up and multidimensional (N/F/M) observational design was used. In the statistical analysis, to check the validity of the generalizability analysis, the Category/Observer (C/O) and Observer/Category (O/C) models were employed, and to determine reliability between observations (intra- and interobserver), the Pearson, Spearman, Kendall's tau-b and Cohen's kappa correlation coefficients were applied. One point were awarded for every penalty given against the opponent. Two points were obtained for a circular technique to the trunk protector (in winners) or by scoring a point for a punch plus a penalty against the opponent, three points in melee actions, anticipatory actions with the left side (winners), or after a punch action, and circular technique (losers). Four and five points were only obtained by winners, in a direct attack with the right leg, turning (five points) or simultaneous (four points). The results of this study provide information on the most effective technical-tactical actions in taekwondo for scoring points in a contest.

Introduction

Taekwondo has been an Olympic combat sport since the Sydney 2000 Olympics. A contest or bout consists of three 2-min rounds with a 1-min rest between rounds. If the third round finishes in a tie, a further period is added, with a maximum duration of 1 min, and the first competitor to score two or more points or whose opponent receives two penalties is declared the winner (World Taekwondo, 2020). Since becoming an Olympic sport, taekwondo has undergone several modifications in its scoring system, the latest being in October 2020 (World Taekwondo, 2020). So according to the rules, since June 2018 the valid scoring areas have been as follows: one point (1P) for a valid punch to the trunk protector, two points (2P) for a valid kick to the trunk protector, four points (4P) for a valid turning kick to the trunk protector, three points (3P) for a valid kick to the head, five points (5P) for a valid turning kick to the head, and one point (1P) for each penalty (“Gam-jeom”) given against the opponent.

Observational methodology is suitable for studying the situational factors in a taekwondo contest. Moreover, it is a valid support for undertaking a description of how taekwondo players behave in their natural setting (Anguera et al., 2001; Castellano and Hernández-Mendo, 2003). In observational methodology, interrelated behaviors are important in order for athletes and coaches to be able to find out which types of behavior are dependent or non-existent, while others do occur (Menescardi et al., 2020a). This methodology makes it possible to record and quantify performance objectively, reveal technical and tactical behaviors, and introduce patterns of behavior (Menescardi et al., 2020b). When it comes to creating and using an observation tool, one must take account of the factors that affect athletes' behavior, as well as ensuring that the tool is suited to the scoring system (Menescardi et al., 2020b). The observation tool must be based on prior studies and data to be able to produce an improved and exceptionally valid design, provide precise observations and take the most important performance variables into account (Menescardi et al., 2017). The tool must be exhaustive and mutually exclusive for each criterion; in other words, it must be precise and a behavior cannot be classified in two categories of the tool simultaneously (Anguera-Argilaga et al., 2011; Blanco-Villaseñor et al., 2014; Menescardi et al., 2017). If this requirement is not complied with, the definitions of the categories used in the tool must be reviewed, since it means that the data are not reliable, and moreover they may alter the results of the study erroneously (Anguera et al., 2007; Menescardi et al., 2017). In combat sports, the use of observational methodology is transcendental, mainly to study technical-tactical interactions in competition. In Karate, it has been used to examine technical-tactical actions and identify the regular behavioral structures of elite competitors (Ibáñez et al., 2018). In fencing to analyse the behaviors in competition that consolidate or modify performances during each round (Tarragó et al., 2017), in elite men's foil they study tactical actions and their impact on the scoreboard (Iglesias et al., 2018) and in judo they analyse the technical-tactical patterns that score in bouts at the highest level of competition (Gutiérrez-Santiago et al., 2019).

To date, the scientific literature has shown us various tools (Gutiérrez-Santiago et al., 2020; Menescardi et al., 2020b; Barrientos et al., 2021) validated for the analysis of tactical actions in taekwondo. However, rule changes (the latest was made in 2018), as well as the evolution of athletes and their technical-tactical approaches, make it necessary to update these tools constantly, in order to adapt the observations and analyses based on the resources they deploy. In this connection, Gutiérrez-Santiago et al. (2020) have recently proposed a new taxonomy consisting of 10 criteria and 51 categories, which analyses the arm technique (type of block), the technique used (out of a total of 13), as well as the movement of the attacking leg (in its initiation and landing, out of a total of 5), or the type of change of position (out of a total of 15). Barrientos et al. (2021), in turn, propose a model with five criteria, 31 categories and 96 subcategories. Among these models, we can highlight combined actions and clinches. Combined actions are those in which two or more techniques are performed before the first striking leg touches the floor; these may be combined techniques with the same leg (corrections) or a different leg (double techniques). Clinch actions, for their part, have appeared as a result of the rule change establishing that when the two opponents come into contact, the contest shall not be stopped, provided that one of the athletes is trying to score a point in a permitted area (World Taekwondo, 2010).

One of the various analytical techniques that can be found in observational methodology is polar coordinate analysis (Sackett, 1980), which offers the possibility of estimating the types of relationships that are established between focal and conditioned behaviors (Castellano and Hernández-Mendo, 2003). Polar coordinate analysis originated in the study by Sackett (Sackett, 1980) which is based on a sequential analysis of prospective and retrospective lags, subsequently optimized with the “genuine technique” (Anguera, 1997), which makes it possible to achieve a drastic reduction of the data on the various behaviors that have occurred and a vector representation of the various interrelationships established between the different categories in the taxonomic system (López-López et al., 2015). This type of technique is becoming increasingly important in the world of sport, as polar coordinate analysis along with sequential analysis are characteristic of observational methodology (Anguera and Hernández-Mendo, 2014; López-López et al., 2015).

Few studies have so far analyzed the behaviors of winning and losing taekwondo athletes (Falcó et al., 2014; González-Prado et al., 2015; Menescardi et al., 2019, 2020c). It seems that the participants in six world championships and world cups between 2000 and 2008 scored more points the more offensive actions they performed. In the university sphere, winners perform more counterattacks than losers, and among their attacks and counterattacks there are fewer indirect attacks and more anticipatory counterattacks (Falcó et al., 2014). In the same sample, and in relation to rounds, the results showed that winners perform more anticipatory counterattacks (in the second and third rounds), fewer subsequent actions (in the third round) and a lower number of simultaneous actions in the first and especially in the third round than losers (Menescardi et al., 2015). According to Menescardi et al. (2020c), winners perform more anticipatory actions (scoring one and three points), whereas losers execute a larger number of back-leg, indirect and turning actions.

A study carried out by Menescardi et al. (2019), in which they analyzed the contests of two Olympic medallists over the course of two Olympics (London 2012 and Río 2016), shows that a point was obtained with direct attacks to the trunk protector. After scoring, they continued with an action to the head. Two points were achieved with simultaneous turning actions, in which subsequent actions occurred afterwards, whereas cutting was performed before and after scoring. Three points were obtained with indirect attacks and subsequent counterattacks to the head. Cutting, feinting and linear techniques preceded obtaining three points, whereas feints, direct attacks and linear actions to the trunk protector occurred in succession after three points were scored. In a sample of 12 taekwondo athletes who participated in the 2016 Río Olympics, Menescardi et al. (2020a) found that winners obtained one point by executing a direct attack to the trunk protector and that after scoring they performed a back-leg technique or a cut. Three points were obtained with a turning kick to the head, continuing with a front-leg action. Cutting actions occurred before scoring three points, while direct attacks were executed afterwards. Therefore, taking the rule change as well as previous studies into account (Falcó et al., 2014; Menescardi et al., 2015, 2020b,c) the objective of this article is twofold: firstly to design an observation tool under the new rules (World Taekwondo, 2020), validate it, certify its reliability and update it, and secondly, using a polar coordinate analysis, to identify the technical-tactical actions by which winning and losing taekwondo competitors managed to score in the finals of the Rome 2019 Grand Prix.

Methods

Methodology and Study Design

This study was conducted using observational methodology. It involved a nomothetic, follow-up and multidimensional (N/F/M) observational design to study all the participants' movements, taking account of their behaviors and the dimensions matching the criteria of the observation instrument. An ad hoc observation tool was developed to be used subsequently in HOISAN software, in which seven bouts were viewed, collecting the data on the participants individually (14 viewings).

Participants

The study sample comprised a total of seven contests, consisting of the finals of the Roma 2019 Grand Prix (women, n = 3; men, n = 4). Women: Fly −49 Kg (Russia vs. South Korea), Feather −57 Kg (South Korea vs. Turkey), Welter −67 Kg (Croatia vs. South Korea). Men: FLY −58 Kg (South Korea vs. Spain), Feather −68 Kg (Iran vs. South Korea), Welter −80 Kg (Russia vs. Spain), Heavy +80 Kg (Russia vs. Kazakhstan).

A total of 1,382 technical-tactical actions were compiled (women, n = 687; men, n = 695). One women's final did not take place because one of the participants was injured. The number of viewings was estimated on the basis of a generalizability analysis (Blanco-Villaseñor et al., 2014). According to the rules laid down by the Ethics Committee and by the Belmont report (National Commission for the Protection of Human Subjects of Biomedical Behavioural Research, 1978) the competitors' informed consent was not necessary, since they were public videos viewed on a website (YouTube) and did not involve any kind of experimentation (López-López et al., 2015).

Instruments

A mixture of field format and category systems was used as an observation instrument, following previous studies (Falcó et al., 2014; Menescardi et al., 2015, 2020b). Each category was exhaustive and mutually exclusive for each criterion. This instrument (Table 1) is composed of five criteria (Tactical action, Type of technique, Strike area and Sidedness, Scoring) and 30 categories. HOISAN software was used for coding the athletes' actions (Hernández-Mendo et al., 2012a, 2014). It allows coding, analysis, and calculation and vectorial representation of polar coordinates (López-López et al., 2015).

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Observation tool and coding system.

Procedure

The process involved three observers with the support of an expert observer. The observer training and coaching phase (Anguera, 2003) determined the observation design, sampling plan, instrument development, coding system and recording. A consensus agreement (Anguera, 1990, 2003) was carried out to reach an agreement prior to recording.

Subsequently, two observers recorded the data of the Fly −58 kg bouts (South Korea vs. Spain) in the men's category and Welter −67 kg (Croatia vs. South Korea) in the women's category. After 15 days, the same bouts were recorded again, obtaining the results of intra-observer agreement. Agreements were established with the other two observers to observe and record the same bouts, obtaining the results of the interobserver concordance. The values obtained in the correlation coefficients (Table 2) and Cohen's kappa indexes (Table 3) were used to verify the reliability of the observational data.

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Interobserver and intraobserver correlation coefficient.

TABLE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. Cohen's kappa indexes for agreement between observers.

Having passed this step, a Generalizability analysis (Cronbach et al., 1972; Cardinet et al., 1976, 1981) was performed following the study by Blanco-Villaseñor et al. (2014). The computer software SAGT v.1.0 (Hernández-Mendo et al., 2012b) was used to verify the interobserver reliability, the homogeneity of the categories and to estimate the minimum number of sessions needed to generalize accurately.

Finally, five focal behaviors (1P, 2P, 3P, 4P, 5P) were selected for the sequential and polar coordinate analyses with HOISAN software (Hernández-Mendo et al., 2012a, 2014) and the graphic representations of the vectors were optimized with the R program (Rodríguez-Medina et al., 2019). Relationships with a vector length of 1.96 or more, based on the characterization of each quadrant proposed by Hernández-Mendo and Anguera (1999), were considered significant (p < 0.05): in quadrant I, behaviors with a mutually excitatory relationship between the focal and conditioned behaviors in a prospective perspective and a retrospective perspective; in quadrant II, inhibitory relationships in a prospective perspective and excitatory relationships in a retrospective perspective; in quadrant III, inhibitory relationships in prospective and retrospective perspectives; and finally, in quadrant IV, inhibition relationships in a retrospective perspective and activation relationships in a prospective perspective.

Statistical Analysis

The generalizability analysis was performed with two facets: categories (C) and observers (O); the Category/Observer (C/O) model was used to test reliability and the Observer/Category model for validity (Table 4), using SAGT v1.0 software (Hernández-Mendo et al., 2012b). It was determined that with two observations a Relative and Absolute Generalizability Coefficient of 0.92 is obtained, and the observation of five bouts guarantees a 0.98. To certify the reliability of the observations, Pearson's, Spearman's and Kendall's tau-b correlation coefficients and Cohen's kappa agreement index were used. In this way, the validity and accuracy of both the tool and the observers were confirmed and accepted; the values were adequate, as they approached or exceeded 0.70 (Blanco et al., 2000).

TABLE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Table 4. Relative and absolute generalizability coefficients.

Results

The following section shows the results of the polar coordinate analysis of the selected one-point (1P), two-point (2P), three-point (3P), four-point (4P), and five-point (5P) focal behaviors and the behavioral maps obtained for winners and losers.

The results for the one-point (1P) focal behavior in winners showed significant relationships in three quadrants (see Table 5). In quadrant I, mutually excitatory relationships were obtained with the correction (CORR) and direct attack (DA) actions, with a high intensity of 4.95. The other associations were with the circular (CIR), single (SING), and double (DOU) techniques and with strikes to the head area (HEA). Quadrant III presented mutual inhibition effects with the indirect attack (IA), block (BLO), clash (CLA), and simultaneous counterattack (SIMCA) conditioned behaviors and with the linear technique (LIN). In losers, the results obtained for the one-point (1P) focal behavior showed relationships with subsequent counterattack (SUBCA), right sidedness (RT), and circular technique (CIR) behaviors in quadrant I; the last of these was also found in winners. In quadrant IV, a significant association of inhibition was found in the retrospective perspective and of activation in the prospective perspective with the simultaneous counterattack (SIMCA) behavior.

TABLE 5
www.frontiersin.org

Table 5. Results of the polar coordinate analysis for the 1P focal category in winners and losers.

Graphic representations or polar coordinate maps for the 1P focal behavior for both losers and winners are shown in Figure 1.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Representation of the behavioral map for the 1P focal behavior in winners (left) and losers (right).

The two-point (2P) focal behavior showed a lower number of significant relationships with the conditioned categories (see Table 6). In winners, the only mutually excitatory association in the first quadrant was with the circular technique (CIR). In the second quadrant, two associations were found, with the punch technique (PUN) and strike to the trunk area (TRU) conditioned behaviors. In losers, only one significant relationship, corresponding to the first quadrant, was found with the punch technique (PUN), with a high vector length of 3.48.

TABLE 6
www.frontiersin.org

Table 6. Results of the polar coordinate analysis for the 2P focal category in winners and losers.

Graphic representations of polar coordinate maps for the 2P focal behavior for both losers and winners are shown in Figure 2.

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Representation of the behavioral map for the 2P focal behavior in winners (left) and losers (right).

The results obtained for the three-point (3P) focal behavior (see Table 7) show three significant relationships for winners in quadrant I, with the melee attack (MEL), the punch technique (PUN) and strikes to the head (HEA). In quadrant II, the focal behavior was significantly associated with left sidedness (LF) and anticipatory counterattack (ANTCA); the latter reached an intensity of 3.86 in the relationship. Quadrant IV, on the other hand, showed a relationship with right sidedness (RT). In losers, the results revealed an association with circular techniques in quadrant I, with strikes to the head in quadrant II and with the linear technique in quadrant III.

TABLE 7
www.frontiersin.org

Table 7. Results of the polar coordinate analysis for the 3P focal category in winners and losers.

Graphic representations of the polar coordinates for the 3P focal category for both losers and winners are shown in Figure 3.

FIGURE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 3. Representation of the behavioral map for the 3P focal behavior in winners (left) and losers (right).

The data obtained for the four-point (4P) focal behavior showed significant relationships with conditioned behaviors only in winners (see Table 8). In quadrant I, there was a mutually excitatory association between the 4P behavior and the direct attack (DA) tactical action and right sidedness (RT) in prospective and retrospective perspectives. In quadrant II, a highly significant relationship of inhibition in a prospective perspective and excitation in a retrospective perspective was obtained with simultaneous counterattack (SIMCA), with a vectorization of 4.26. There was mutual inhibition with left sidedness (LF) in quadrant III and with the single (SING) technique in quadrant IV.

TABLE 8
www.frontiersin.org

Table 8. Results of the polar coordinate analysis for the 4P focal category in winners.

A graphic representation of the polar coordinates for the 4P focal category for winners is shown in Figure 4.

FIGURE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 4. Representation of the behavioral map for the 4P focal behavior in winners.

Similarly, the results obtained for the five-point (5P) focal behavior did not produce any significant associations in losers. In winners (see Table 9), the first quadrant was significantly associated with the circular technique (CIR), generating a radius of 4.64, and also with right sidedness (RT) and direct attacks (DA). The second quadrant showed a relationship with the turning (TUR) technique, the third quadrant with left sidedness (LF), the linear technique (LIN) and the clash (CLA) tactical action, and the fourth quadrant revealed a single significant relationship with the punch technique (PUN), which reached an intensity of 4.08.

TABLE 9
www.frontiersin.org

Table 9. Results of the polar coordinate analysis for the 5P focal category in winners.

A graphic representation of the polar coordinates for the 5P focal category for winners is shown in Figure 5.

FIGURE 5
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 5. Representation of the behavioral map for the 5P focal behavior in winners.

Discussion

The objective of this study was to design, validate and update an observation tool and use it to analyse the technical-tactical actions by which taekwondo players win points, comparing winners and losers of the finals of the Rome 2019 Grand Prix in this respect. In pursuit of this objective, a generalizability analysis was first performed using the C/O and O/C models (Table 4) for validity and the Pearson, Spearman, Kendall's tau-b, and Cohen's kappa coefficients for reliability; the values were appropriate, as they were higher than or close to 0.70 (Blanco et al., 2000). This was followed by a polar coordinate analysis, taking as focal behaviors those related to obtaining points (1P, 2P, 3P, 4P and 5P) and as conditioned behaviors those related to technical-tactical actions: direct attack (DA), indirect attack (IA), correction (CORR), melee (MEL), subsequent counterattack (SUBCA), simultaneous counterattack (SIMCA), anticipatory counterattack (ANTCA), clash (CLA), block (BLO), dodge (DOD), linear technique (LIN), circular technique (CIR), turning technique (TUR), punch (PUN), single corrective action (SING), double corrective action (DOU), strike to the helmet (HEA), to the trunk protector (TRU), with the right leg (RT) or the left leg (LF) or in a neutral position when in a melee (NEU).

With regard to the 1P focal behavior (obtaining one point with a valid punch to the trunk protector or by a penalty given against the opponent), the results show different patterns in winners and losers. In winners, obtaining a point was preceded and followed by direct attack and correction behaviors, with a single or double technique, using a circular kick to the head (quadrant I). In losers, obtaining a point was preceded and followed by a subsequent counterattack, with a circular kick executed with the right leg. It is important to emphasize that one point is scored by a punch or by a penalty given against the opponent. In this regard, it is reasonable to think that as a consequence of the actions performed, a penalty is awarded against the opponent, adding a point to the score. The results of our study are in line with previous studies (Kazemi et al., 2006; González-Prado et al., 2015) which show that losers incur more penalties than winners.

It is also interesting to highlight the occurrence of single or double correction actions, which, even if they do not succeed in scoring, do give rise to an action by the opponent that incurs a penalty. Furthermore, the occurrence of these behaviors reveals the tactical evolution of the sport. This is the first study that includes these variables (single and double correction, as well as clash), and therefore more studies showing the effectiveness of such technical-tactical actions are needed. It is worth emphasizing that the fact that winners provoke a penalty through direct attack and correction behaviors, whereas losers obtain it through a subsequent counterattack, is in line with previous studies (González-Prado et al., 2015) showing that the more offensive actions are executed, the more points are scored, and contrary to other studies showing that winners carry out more counterattacks than losers (Falcó et al., 2014; Menescardi et al., 2015) or perform more anticipatory actions (obtaining one point), while losers execute a higher number of back-leg, indirect and turning actions (Menescardi et al., 2020c). Regarding inhibited (neutralized) actions in winners, these are indirect attack, simultaneous counterattack, block, clash, and linear technique (quadrant III), whereas inhibited behaviors are not observed in losers, which presumably reveals the active attacking style in winners.

The 2P focal behavior (obtaining two points by means of a valid kick to the trunk protector, or scoring one point for a punch plus a penalty given against the opponent) shows a similar behavioral pattern in winners and losers, since both groups use the punch to obtain a point, while the opponent receives a penalty at the same time (and therefore they obtain two points in the action). It is an uncommon score both in winners and in losers. According to studies that refer to it (González-Prado et al., 2015) it is a difficult action to score, but is executed in this case in order to continue adding after obtaining one point and not as a counterattacking or defensive action to evade the opponent, while in winners the circular behavior is found. Previous studies that use a different scoring system, but that have similarly analyzed obtaining points by executing a linear or circular kick to the trunk protector, have shown that effective actions are performed with direct strikes to the trunk and that after scoring, players continue with an action to the helmet (Menescardi et al., 2019) or execute a kick with the back leg or a cutting action (Menescardi et al., 2020a). Other studies show that winners perform more anticipatory actions (managing to score), whereas losers execute more rear-leg indirect actions (Menescardi et al., 2020c). The lack of two-point scores could be due to better blocking by taekwondo athletes, on the one hand, and to the difficulty of scoring on the trunk protector, owing to their power threshold levels (Daedo, 2020).

For the 3P focal behavior (obtaining three points by a valid kick to the helmet), winners show the melee, punch and head strike conditioned behaviors in quadrant I, while losers perform circular actions. Prior to scoring three points (quadrant III), winners execute anticipatory actions with the left leg. These results are in line with previous studies (Menescardi et al., 2020c) which show the use of anticipatory actions by winners to obtain three points, because of their effectiveness (Falcó et al., 2014). The range of possibilities that winners display to obtain three points is interesting, as they do so either with a melee action, a punch action followed by another melee, an anticipatory action with the left leg followed by a melee action, a punch or another technique with the right leg (quadrant IV). Once again we can see how tactical action has evolved, since previous studies have shown that to obtain three points winners execute more anticipatory actions (Menescardi et al., 2020c), indirect attacks or subsequent counterattacks to the helmet (Menescardi et al., 2019) cutting actions occur before scoring (Menescardi et al., 2019, 2020b).

Finally, in relation to the 4P behavior (obtaining four points for a valid turning kick to the trunk protector) and the 5P behavior (obtaining five points for a valid kick to the helmet preceded by a turn), it should be noted that they were only performed by winners. According to some studies (Falcó et al., 2014; Menescardi et al., 2019) very specific actions must be executed to achieve these scores: not being averse to failing, and taking account of the time needed to carry it out. To score, both are performed in a direct attack with the right leg (the left is inhibited), with turning (in the case of scoring five points) or simultaneous (in the case of four points). In previous studies, when two points were obtained (instead of four) with actions preceded by turning, they were achieved with simultaneous turning actions, followed by subsequent counterattacks, whereas cutting actions occurred before and after scoring (Menescardi et al., 2019). Three points (instead of five) were obtained with a turning kick to the head, continuing with an action with the front leg (Menescardi et al., 2020b). As has been suggested (Menescardi et al., 2020b), it seems that winners are capable of using more complex actions, and by doing so they achieve higher scores which enable them to win the contest.

Conclusion and Pratical Applications

Four conclusions and practical applications can be drawn from this study in relation to how to score in a taekwondo bout:

1. With regard to obtaining one point (for a valid punch to the trunk protector or for a penalty given against the opponent), both winners and losers do so by the opponent being penalized. It should note that in winners this action is preceded and followed by a direct attack or correction, a single or double technique or a circular kick to the head. In losers, it is preceded and followed by a subsequent counterattack, with a circular kick executed with the right leg.

2. Two points are obtained for a valid circular kick to the trunk protector (in winners), or by scoring a point for a punch plus a penalty against the opponent (in both winners and losers).

3. Three points (obtained with a valid kick to the helmet) are observed in winners in melee actions or after a punch, whereas in losers they occur in circular actions. Beforehand, winners execute either anticipatory actions with the left leg, a melee action, an action consisting of a punch followed by another melee, an anticipatory action with the left leg followed by a melee or punch action, or another kick with the right leg.

4. Behaviors obtaining four points (by a valid turning kick to the trunk protector) and five points (by a valid kick, preceded by a turn, to the helmet) are only performed by winners. To do so, they execute a direct attack with the right leg (the left is inhibited), with turning (in the case of scoring five points) or simultaneous (in the case of obtaining four points).

Limitations and Future Lines of Research

As regards the limitations of this study, the results obtained are significant and provide athletes and coaches with the necessary information. Additional studies should consider increasing the number of observations to provide a larger number of patterns. In this study, the planned sample was the eight final bouts of the Grand Prix of Rome 2019 (four female and four male bouts). The female HEAVY +67 kg final was not held due to the injury of one of the participants in that category, limiting the observation and recording of that bout. The data obtained did not affect the results of the study, since the generalizability analysis determined that the observation of five bouts guaranteed a Relative and Absolute Generalizability Coefficient of.98. Therefore, the significant relationships obtained in the polar coordinates analysis between the selected focal categories and the rest of the categories of the tool are considered generalizable in the study. As a future line of research it would be interesting to track two of the winners throughout the whole championship.

Data Availability Statement

The original contributions presented in the study are included in the article/supplementary material, further inquiries can be directed to the corresponding author.

Author Contributions

GA, AH-M, VM-S, and CF conceived and designed the study. JG-C and YQ-R collected the data and analyzed them. All authors contributed to the interpretation of the results, reviewed and provided feedback to the manuscript, and the final version of the manuscript and approved it for publication.

Conflict of Interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher's Note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

References

Anguera, M. T.. (1990). “Metodología observacional,” in J. Arnau, M. T. Anguera, J. Gómez, editors, Metodología de la investigación en Ciencias del Comportamiento (Murcia: Secretariado de Publicaciones de la Universidad de Murcia), 125–236.

Google Scholar

Anguera, M. T.. (1997). From Prospective Patterns in Behaviour to Joint Analysis With a Retrospective Perspective. In Colloque sur invitation Méthodologie d'analyse des interactions sociales. Paris: Université de la Sorbonne.

Google Scholar

Anguera, M. T.. (2003). “La observación,” in Sanz, T., editor, Evaluación psicológica Concepto, proceso y aplicación en las áreas del desarrollo y de la inteligencia (Madrid: Moreno Rosset), 271–308.

Google Scholar

Anguera M. T. Blanco-Villaseñor A. Losada J. (2001). Diseños observacionales, cuestión clave en el proceso de la metodología observacional. Metodol. Cienc. Comport. 3, 103–109.

Google Scholar

Anguera, M. T., and Hernández-Mendo, A. (2014). Metodología observacional y psicología del deporte. Psicol Deporte. 23, 103–109.

Google Scholar

Anguera, M. T., Magnussson, M. S., and Jonsson, G. K. (2007). Non-standard instruments: approach, development and possibilities. Avances en Medición. 5, 63–82.

Google Scholar

Anguera-Argilaga M. T. Blanco-Villaseñor A. Hernández-Mendo A. Losada-López J. L. (2011). Diseños observacionales: ajuste y aplicación en psicología del deporte. Cuad Psicol Deporte. 11, 63–76.

Google Scholar

Barrientos, M., Saavedra-García, M. A., Arriaza-Loureda, R., Menescardi, C., and Fernández-Romero, J. J. (2021). An updated technical–tactical categorisation in taekwondo: from general tactical objectives to combat situations. Sustainability 13:1910493. doi: 10.3390/su131910493

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Blanco, A., Castellanos, J., and Hernández-Mendo, A. (2000). Generalizabilidad de las observaciones de la acción del juego en el fútbol. Psicothema 12, 81–86. Available online at: http://www.redalyc.org/articulo.oa?id=72797022

Google Scholar

Blanco-Villaseñor A. Castellano J. Hernández-Mendo A. Sánchez-López C. R. Usabiaga O. (2014). Aplicación de la TG en el deporte para el estudio de la fiabilidad, validez y estimación de la muestra. Rev. Psicol. Deporte 23, 131–137.

Google Scholar

Cardinet, J., Tourneur, Y., and Allal, L. (1976). The simmetry of generalizability theory: applications to educational measurement. J. Educ. Measur. 13, 119–135. doi: 10.1111/j.1745-3984.1976.tb00003.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cardinet, J., Tourneur, Y., and Allal, L. (1981). Extension of generalizability theory and its applications in educational measurement. J. Educ. Measur. 18, 183–204. doi: 10.1111/j.1745-3984.1981.tb00852.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Castellano, J., and Hernández-Mendo, A. (2003). El análisis de coordenadas polares para la estimación de relaciones en la interacción motriz en fútbol. Psicothema 15, 569–574. Available online at: http://www.redalyc.org/articulo.oa?id=72715410

Google Scholar

Cronbach, L. J., Gleser, G. C., Nanda, H., and Rajaratman, N. (1972). The Dependability of Behavioral Measures: Theory of Generalizability for Scores and Profiles. New York, NY: Wiley.

Google Scholar

Daedo (2020). An Electronic Protector for Authentic Taekwondo. Available online at: http://www.daedopss.com/product/e-protector-system (accessed April 14, 2021).

Google Scholar

Falcó, C., Estevan, I., Álvarez, O., Morales-Sánchez, V., and Hernández-Mendo, A. (2014). Tactical analysis of the winners' and non-winners' performances in a Taekwondo University Championship. Int. J. Sports Sci. Coach 9, 1407–1416. doi: 10.1260/1747-9541.9.6.1407

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

González-Prado, C., Iglesias, X., and Anguera, M. D. (2015). Detección de regularidades en taekwondo de alto nivel. Cuad. Psicol. Deporte. 15, 99–110. doi: 10.4321/S1578-84232015000100010

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gutiérrez-Santiago, A., Gentico-Merino, L. A., and Prieto-Lage, I. (2019). Detection of the technical-tactical pattern of the scoring actions in judo in the men's category of −73 kg. Int. J. Perform. Anal. Sport. 19, 778–793. doi: 10.1080/24748668.2019.1655934

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gutiérrez-Santiago, A., Pereira-Rodríguez, R., and Prieto-Lage, I. (2020). Detection of the technical and tactical motion of the scorable movements in taekwondo. Physiol. Behav. 217:112913. doi: 10.1016/j.physbeh.2020.112813

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hernández-Mendo, A., and Anguera, M. T. A. (1999). Aportaciones de análisis de coordenadas polares a los deportes de equipo. La Psicología del Deporte en España al final del milenio (Las Palmas: Universidad de Las Palmas de Gran Canaria), 169–175.

Google Scholar

Hernández-Mendo A. Castellano J. Camerino O. Jonsson G. Blanco-Villaseñor A. Lopes A. . (2014). Programas informáticos de registro, control de calidad del dato, y análisis de datos. Psicol. Deporte. 23, 111–121. Available online at: http://hdl.handle.net/2445/105709

Google Scholar

Hernández-Mendo, A., López-López, J. A., Castellano, J., Morales-Sánchez, V., and Pastrana, J. L. (2012a). Hoisan 1.2: programa informático para uso en metodología observacional. Cuad Psicol. Deporte. 12, 55–78. doi: 10.4321/S1578-84232012000100006

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hernández-Mendo, A., Ramos-Pérez, F., and Pastrana, J. L. S. A. G. T. (2012b). Programa informático para análisis de Teoría de la Generalizabilidad. Las Palmas de Gran Canaria, Spain SAFE CREATIVE Código.

Google Scholar

Ibáñez, R., Lapresa, D., Arana, J., Camerino Foguet, O., and Anguera, M. T. (2018). Observational analysis of the technical-tactical performance of elite karate contestants. Cult. Cienc. y Deporte. 13, 61–70. doi: 10.12800/ccd.v13i37.1039

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Iglesias, X., Tarragó, R., and Ruiz Sanchis, L. (2018). Analysis of fencing actions turing the men's foil world championship. Revista Internacional de Deportes Colectivos. 35, 31–33.

Google Scholar

Kazemi, M., Waalen, J., Morgan, C., and White, A. R. (2006). A profile of Olympic Taekwondo competitors. J. Sports Sci. Med. 5, 114–121.

Google Scholar

López-López, J. A., Menescardi, C., Estevan, I., Falcó, C., and Hernández-Mendo, A. (2015). Análisis técnico-táctico en Taekwondo con coordenadas polares a través del software HOISAN. Cuad Psicol. Deporte. 15, 131–142. doi: 10.4321/S1578-84232015000100013

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Menescardi, C., Estevan, I., Falcó, C., and Hernández-Mendo, A. (2017). Generalizability theory applied to olympic taekwondo combats. Eur. J. Hum. Mov. 39, 65–81.

Google Scholar

Menescardi, C., Falco, C., Hernandez-Mendo, A., and Morales-Sanchez, V. (2020a). Talent and creativity of Taekwondoists Winners of the 2016 Summer Olympics. Sustainability 12:104185. doi: 10.3390/su12104185

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Menescardi, C., Falco, C., Hernández-Mendo, A., and Morales-Sánchez, V. (2020b). Design, validation, and testing of an observational tool for technical and tactical analysis in the taekwondo competition at the 2016 Olympic games. Physiol. Behav. 224:112980. doi: 10.1016/j.physbeh.2020.112980

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Menescardi, C., Falco, C., Ros, C., Morales-Sánchez, V., and Hernández-Mendo, A. (2019). Technical-tactical actions used to score in taekwondo: an analysis of two medalists in two Olympic Championships. Front. Psychol. 10:2708. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2019.02708

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Menescardi, C., Liébana, E., and Falco, C. (2020c). Why do female and male taekwondo athletes win the bout? An analysis based on the Olympic weight category and the result of the bout. Revista de Artes Marciales Asiáticas. 14, 67–82. doi: 10.18002/rama.v14i2.6051

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Menescardi, C., Lopez-Lopez, J. A., Falco, C., Hernandez-Mendo, A., and Estevan, I. (2015). Tactical aspects of a National University Taekwondo Championship in relation to round and match outcome. J. Strength Cond. Res. 29, 466–471. doi: 10.1519/JSC.0000000000000645

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

National Commission for the Protection of Human Subjects of Biomedical Behavioural Research (1978). The Belmont Report: Ethical Principles and Guidelines for the Protection of Human Subjects of Research. Washington, DC: Department of Health, Education and Welfare (DHEW Publication No. OS 78-0012). Available online at: https://www.hhs.gov/ohrp/regulations-and-policy/belmont-report/index.html (accessed February 18, 2020).

Google Scholar

Rodríguez-Medina, J., Arias, V., Arias, B., Hernández-Mendo, A., and Anguera, M. T. (2019). Polar Coordinate Analysis, from HOISAN to R: A Tutorial Paper. Available online at: https://jairodmed.shinyapps.io/HOISAN_to_R/ (accessed December 25, 2021).

Google Scholar

Sackett, G. P.. (1980). “Lag sequential analysis as a data reduction technique in social interaction research,” in Exceptional Infant, eds R. C. Hawkins, J. Sawin, editors, Penticu. Phychosocial Risks in Infant Environment Transactions, 300–340. Available online at: http://www.worldtaekwondo.org/rules-wt/rules.html (accessed December 16, 2010).

Google Scholar

Tarragó, R., Iglesias, X., Lapresa, D., Anguera, M. T., Ruiz-Sanchis, L., and Arana, J. (2017). Analysis of diachronic relationships in successful and unsuccessful behaviors by world fencing champions using three complementary techniques. An. de Psicol. 33, 71–485. doi: 10.6018/analesps.33.3.271041

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

World Taekwondo (2010). Competition Rules and Interpretation. Available online at: http://www.worldtaekwondo.org/rules-wt/rules.html (accessed December 16, 2010).

Google Scholar

World Taekwondo (2020). Competition Rules and Interpretation. Available online at: http://www.worldtaekwondo.org/rules-wt/rules.html (accessed December 16, 2010).

Google Scholar

Keywords: polar coordinates, observational methodology, taekwondo, observation tool, technical-tactical actions, bout analysis

Citation: Gamero-Castillero JA, Quiñones-Rodríguez Y, Apollaro G, Hernández-Mendo A, Morales-Sánchez V and Falcó C (2022) Application of the Polar Coordinate Technique in the Study of Technical-Tactical Scoring Actions in Taekwondo. Front. Sports Act. Living 4:877502. doi: 10.3389/fspor.2022.877502

Received: 16 February 2022; Accepted: 29 April 2022;
Published: 23 May 2022.

Edited by:

Ciro José Brito, Juiz de Fora Federal University, Brazil

Reviewed by:

Mario Amatria, Pontifical University of Salamanca, Spain
Esteban Ariel Aedo-Muñoz, University of Santiago, Chile

Copyright © 2022 Gamero-Castillero, Quiñones-Rodríguez, Apollaro, Hernández-Mendo, Morales-Sánchez and Falcó. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Coral Falcó, coral.falco@hvl.no

ORCID: Yarisel Quiñones-Rodríguez orcid.org/0000-0001-7357-373X
Gennaro Apollaro orcid.org/0000-0002-0427-037X
Antonio Hernández-Mendo orcid.org/0000-0002-0002-1325
Verónica Morales-Sánchez orcid.org/0000-0001-6943-9896
Coral Falcó orcid.org/0000-0001-6565-1654

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.