Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Sleep, 18 October 2022
Sec. Sleep, Behavior and Mental Health
This article is part of the Research Topic Understanding the Link Between Sleep and Mental Health View all 7 articles

Effects of pleasant sound on overnight sleep condition: A crossover randomized study

\nShota SaedaShota Saeda1Koichi Fujiwara
Koichi Fujiwara1*Takafumi KinoshitaTakafumi Kinoshita2Yukiyoshi SumiYukiyoshi Sumi3Masahiro MatsuoMasahiro Matsuo3Kiyoshi YamakiKiyoshi Yamaki4Takahiro KawashimaTakahiro Kawashima5Hiroshi KadotaniHiroshi Kadotani3
  • 1Department of Material Process Engineering, Nagoya University, Nagoya, Japan
  • 2Department of Systems Science, Kyoto University, Kyoto, Japan
  • 3Department of Psychiatry, Shiga University of Medical Science, Otsu, Japan
  • 4Division of Electronic Device, Yamaha Corporation, Shizuoka, Japan
  • 5Division of Audio Equipment, Yamaha Corporation, Shizuoka, Japan

It is desirable to improve sleep quality since poor sleep results in decreases in work productivity and increases in risks of lifestyle-related diseases. Sleep spindles in sleep EEG are waveforms that characterize non-REM sleep Stage 2 (Stage N2). Music therapy has been adopted as a non-pharmacological therapy for sleep quality improvement; however, few studies mention the relationship between music during sleep and spindles. We conducted a crossover randomized study to investigate music's effects on spindles and sleep parameters. Polysomnography (PSG) was performed on 12 adult males with sleep difficulties over three nights, during which they were exposed to three different acoustic environments–silent, white noise, and pleasant sounds–throughout the night, in a crossover randomized setting. Half of the participants with large WASO were defined as the sleep maintenance difficulty group. We investigated whether pleasant sounds shortened sleep onset latency (SOL) and increased the number of spindles (SN) and spindle density (SD) compared to white noise, using silent as the reference. The spindles were detected using the previously reported automatic spindle detection algorithm. After one patient was excluded due to data corruption, a total of 11 participants, including the sleep maintenance difficulty group (n = 5), were analyzed. For all participants, SOL was not significantly shorter with pleasant sound than with white noise (p = 0.683); for the sleep maintenance difficulty group, SOL tended to be shorter with pleasant sound than with white noise (p = 0.060). Compared to white noise, the SN increased in pleasant sound for 7 of 11 (4 of 5 in the sleep maintenance difficulty group), and SD increased for 5 of 11 (3 of 5 in the sleep maintenance difficulty group). The results suggest that all-night background sound exposure may affect SN and SD. Future research should investigate whether background sound exposure reduces sleep-related distress, achieves sound sleep, or improves daytime psychomotor function.

Introduction

Insomnia is a highly prevalent sleep disorder (Mai and Buysse, 2008). Lack of sleep decreases physical and mental quality of life and work productivity and is also a risk factor for future lifestyle-related diseases, which suggests that improving sleep quality is important for quality of life and prevention of lifestyle-related diseases (Tasali et al., 2009; Rangaraj and Knutson, 2016; Takami et al., 2018).

Various intervention methods, including pharmacotherapy and psychological and behavioral interventions, have been investigated for the treatment of insomnia. However, there are some concerns about the long-term use of medications, such as the risk of abuse, dependence, and adverse effects such as residual daytime sedation, cognitive impairment, and reduced motor co-ordination (Mahfoud et al., 2009). Cognitive-behavioral therapy (CBT-I) is recommended as a first-line treatment for chronic insomnia in adults of any age (Edinger et al., 2021). This treatment provides sustained improvement in sleep and is well accepted by patients (Riemann et al., 2017; Morin et al., 2021). In recent years, music therapy has also been considered a non-invasive intervention to improve sleep in a simple manner (Jespersen et al., 2015).

The influence of music on the structure of sleep is still largely unknown. However, it has been reported that pleasant music has a relaxing effect and reduces anxiety, stress, aversion, cortisol levels, and noradrenaline circulation (Gerra et al., 1998; Lai and Li, 2011; Ventura et al., 2012; Matsuo et al., 2019). It is expected that listening to pleasant sounds during sleep may contribute to improvement of sleep quality.

Several studies have reported on the relationship between music and sleep, and positive effects of music on sleep have been observed in a wide range of age groups (Field, 1999; Tan, 2004; Lai and Good, 2005; Harmat et al., 2008). Previous studies have shown that background music can help preschool children fall asleep faster during naptime (Field, 1999) and improve subjective sleep quality (Tan, 2004). Another study examining the effects of music on young people with poor sleep showed that, while music improved depressive symptoms, it did not improve sleep quality (Harmat et al., 2008). In addition, it has been reported that background music improves subjective sleep quality ratings in older adults as well (Lai and Good, 2005). It has been suggested that music preference is related to the effects of music on sleep (Lai and Good, 2005).

The relationship between music and electroencephalogram (EEG) has been investigated. It is well-known that auditory stimuli containing cyclic changes affect the coherence of EEG (Berger and Turow, 2012). Cordi et al. (2019) composed relaxing music with a frequency of 0.01–2 Hz, which increased slow-wave sleep (SWS) duration.

Sleep spindles in the sleep EEG are waveforms that characterize non-REM sleep Stage 2 (StageN2) (Fernandez and Lüthi, 2020), which play a role in sleep maintenance (De Gennaro and Ferrara, 2003). Sleep spindles have been suggested to be involved in the inhibition of sensory processing during sleep (Steriade et al., 1993; Cote et al., 2000). Cote et al. (2000) investigated changes in event-related potentials (ERP) during sound stimulation of sleeping participants. The amplitude of EPR becomes highest shortly after onset of sound stimulus, which suggests that the spindles may be a consequence of prior thalamic inhibition due to information processing, particularly when being confronted with loud, intrusive, external stimuli (Cote et al., 2000). In addition, sleep spindles are thought to be involved in the maintenance of non-REM sleep continuity (Steriade and Llinás, 1988). In summary, sleep spindles are related to important mechanisms of sleep maintenance.

Sleep spindles have also been reported to be associated with music. Antony and Paller (2017) reported an increase in slow spindles (11–13.5 Hz) when napping participants were exposed to sounds at 11–13.5 Hz, and an increase in fast spindles (13.5–16 Hz) when they were exposed to sounds at 13.5–16 Hz.

Previous studies on the relationship between music and sleep have mainly focused on naps of 45–90 min (Antony and Paller, 2017), while largely ignoring the relationship between music and nighttime sleep. This is because it is burdensome even for a skilled polysomnographic technician to score the EEG for the entire night, particularly to detect spindles (Kinoshita et al., 2020). Thus, in previous studies, the analysis of all-night sleep was limited by the heavy burden on specialists. In this study, we analyzed all-night sleep by using a machine learning algorithm to perform EEG analysis on behalf of specialists.

In this study, we use music similar to the sound of ocean waves (Morishima et al., 2016) as pleasant sounds, which have been confirmed to improve sleep quality. We adopt the automatic spindle detection algorithm developed by Kinoshita et al. (2020) to detect spindles from EEG data, which allows realizing an objective assessment of sleep quality from the viewpoint of spindle occurrence.

The objectives of this study were (1) to investigate whether pleasant sleep sounds shorten sleep onset latency (SOL) and (2) to investigate whether pleasant sleep sounds affect sleep parameters, number of spindles (SN), and spindle density (SD).

Method

Trial design

Participants

The study was conducted in Shiga Prefecture, Japan. Japanese participants who were aware of difficulties related to sleep in their daily lives were recruited for the experiment. Participants were recruited between June 24, 2015 and August 28, 2015. Figure 1 shows the flow chart of this study.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Flow diagram of the participants. Two participants were allocated to each of the three sound environment combinations (six combinations). Each of the three sound environments was played overnight. Each participant took part in the experiment for three nights. Each experiment was conducted at intervals of every 2–3nights. S, Silent environment; N, White noise; P, Pleasant sound.

Inclusion criteria were as follows: (a) Athens Insomnia Scale (AIS) (Okajima et al., 2020) score ≥ 6, (b) adherence to a controlled sleep schedule, in which participants go to bed at 0:00 a.m. and wake up at 7:00 a.m. for 1 week before the experiment, and (c) not being an employee or a family member of YAMAHA corporation. AIS is a validated 8-item self-report questionnaire that assesses the severity of insomnia with a possible score ranging from 0 to 24 points (Soldatos, 1995). An AIS score of 6 is the optimal cutoff score for insomnia based on a balance of sensitivity and specificity (Soldatos et al., 2003). Cronbach's alpha at pre-assessment and post-assessment was 0.843 and 0.824, respectively.

Exclusion criteria were as follows: (a) those already diagnosed with sleep disorders, psychiatric disorders, or hearing impairment, and (b) no data in questionnaires or polysomnography (PSG). Participants were asked to complete the Japanese version of the Pittsburgh Sleep Quality Index [PSQI (Buysse et al., 1989)], the PSQI-J (Doi et al., 2000), in a pre-test, and participants with a PSQI-J score of 5 or higher were defined as having perceived sleep difficulties. The cutoff score of 5 is the value at which subjective sleep quality is evaluated to be good (Chiu et al., 2016). In a Japanese epidemiological study (Kadotani et al., 2017), about half of the population had PSQI-J < 5. We excluded participants with PSQI-J < 5, because we considered that participants who were unaware of their sleep difficulties would not show any change in their sleep condition even if pleasant sleep sounds were used. The PSQI is an effective self-report questionnaire used to measure sleep quality and patterns in older adults (Buysse et al., 1989). The PSQI distinguishes between “poor” and “good” sleep by measuring seven domains: subjective sleep quality, sleep latency, sleep duration, habitual sleep efficiency, sleep disturbances, use of sleep medication, and daytime dysfunction over the previous month (Buysse et al., 1989). Subjects responded to each item on a Likert Scale of 0 to 3, with 3 corresponding to the negative extreme of the Likert Scale. The PSQI had a Cronbach's alpha of 0.83 for the seven domains.

Next, multiple psychiatrists confirmed that the participants had no psychiatric comorbidities using the Mini-International Neuropsychiatric Interview (M.I.N.I.) (Sheehan et al., 1998), in addition, they confirmed that participants had no diagnosed or currently-treated sleep disorders. Participants were confirmed to have no hearing impairment using an audiometer, AA-77A (RION Co.,Ltd., Tokyo, Japan). No participant satisfied the exclusion criteria before participation. After all experiments were completed, we confirmed that no adverse events had occurred. In this study, follow-up survey was not conducted.

All participants gave written informed consent in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki. The Ethics Committee of Shiga University of Medical Science approved the study protocol (#26-227). The study was registered at UMIN-CTR (UMIN000019045, Cross-over test for music and sound on sleep, registered on September 16, 2015).

The complete protocol for this study is available upon reasonable request.

Details of experiment

PSG was performed in the sleep laboratory at Shiga University of Medical Science. Each participant participated in the experiment every two to three nights. The objective dataset consisted of PSG data (Alice 5, Respironics Inc., Murrysville, PA, USA), for three nights recorded from people during sleep in three types of acoustic environments: silent, white noise, and pleasant sound. Participants arrived at the sleep laboratory at 9:00 p.m., the lights were turned off at 10:00 p.m., and the participants woke up around 6:00 a.m. When exposed to the pleasant sound, they were asked to answer a questionnaire on pleasant sound after arousal (Table 1). The questionnaire on the pleasant sound sleep was answered on a 4-point Likert scale.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Questionnaire on pleasant sounds.

The PSG recordings were from around 10:00 p.m. to 6:00 a.m. the following day. EEG was acquired from Fp1, Fp2, C3, C4, O1, and O2 channels in accordance with the international 10-20 method and recorded by means of referential recording using the earlobe A1 and A2 as reference electrodes.

The details of the order of sound exposure are described below. Two participants were assigned to each of the six possible orders of sound exposure (see Table 2). The sleep laboratory at Shiga University of Medical Science has two PSG rooms, and two different sound conditions were set in advance for each room. The participants were assigned to one of the two rooms by means of the envelope method. The participants were not told which sound would be played on the day of the experiment until the start of the experiment. Due to the characteristics of the experiment, the type of sound that was played during exposure was not concealed from the participants.

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Participant information.

An experienced PSG technologist, who was blind to which background sound was selected in each setting, manually scored sleep stages, and confirmed that none of the participants had abnormal EEG waveforms, such as epileptic discharges.

Presented sounds

Three types of acoustic environments were used to investigate the effects of sound on sleep quality: white noise, pleasant sounds, and silent, the last one being used as the control. The experimental order of the different acoustic environments was randomly determined for each participant. Pleasant sound, named Kaiminon by Yamaha Corporation (Tokyo, Japan), is background sound that is adjusted to oscillate like an ocean wave at a tempo slightly slower than the breathing rate of the person listening to the background sound (Morishima et al., 2016). A previous study has shown that the pleasant sounds used in this study reduce SOL and improve sleep quality in patients with insomnia (Morishima et al., 2016).

The sound was played through speakers in the sleep laboratory overnight. The volume was determined using a sound level meter placed at the position of the participant's head. The silent environment was <30 dB, which is about the sound level of a whisper. The white noise and the pleasant sounds were played at the same volume with an average of 40 dB, which is the same sound level as that of a typical library. The WHO recommends that noise during sleep should be 40 dB or less to prevent negative effects on sleep (World Health Organization., 2018). The volume was determined according to the WHO recommendations in order to avoid negative effects due to sound volume.

Automatic spindle detection algorithm

Although we investigated effects of sound during sleep on spindles overnight, it is difficult for even skilled polysomnographic technicians to visually label spindles with high accuracy from long-term EEG data. In order to automatically and accurately detect spindles in long-term EEG data, we employed a machine learning-based spindle detection algorithm developed by Kinoshita et al. (2020).

Figure 2 shows the flow of Kinoshita's algorithm. In this algorithm, a synchro-squeezed wavelet transform (SST) is applied to the EEG to estimate the frequency of the EEG, and the result is used to create a feature matrix. The small amount of spindle data is then classified using an algorithm called RUSBoost, which is useful for classifying datasets with unbalanced correct labels, such as large amounts of non-spindle data, and spindles are detected. Kinoshita et al. (2020) validated the proposed algorithm using the Montreal archives of sleep studies cohort 1 (MASS-C1), a well-known open PSG dataset. Despite variations in performance when training and validation data were interchanged, Kinoshita et al. (2020) reported an average performance index of 76.9% sensitivity, 61.2% positive predictive value, and F-measure of 0.70, showing higher performance than other spindle detection algorithms.

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Framework of Kinoshita's algorithm. EEG data are segmented by a moving window of 0.5 s with an overlap of 0.25 s, and SST is applied to the segmented EEG to extract features. The pre-trained classifier detects spindles from the extracted features. SST, wavelet synchrosqueezed transform; RUSBoost, random under-sampling boosting; EEG, Electroencephalography.

In this analysis, as in Kinoshita's, spindle detection was performed by analyzing the EEG acquired by PSG with a machine learning model trained using MASS-C1.

Analysis procedure

We originally planned to evaluate SOL as the primary outcome, and Stage N3 time, WASO frequency, and ECG LF and HF power as secondary outcomes. The spindle detection algorithm, which was developed after the experiment was conducted, enabled us to evaluate spindles as well, so we added SN and SD as outcomes. Other general sleep parameters were also explored as outcomes.

Sleep stages of the collected PSG data were scored from the EEG data in accordance with the AAMS manual, version 2.0 (Berry et al., 2012) by a polysomnographic technician certified by the Japanese Society of Sleep Research. He / she did not know which acoustic environment of the PSG data was being analyzed. In order to evaluate time in bed [min] (TIB), total sleep time [min] (TST), SOL [min], wake after sleep onset [min] (WASO), sleep efficiency [%] (SE), Stage R(%TST), Stage N1(%TST), Stage N2(%TST), Stage N3(%TST) were obtained for each acoustic environment from each participant based on the sleep stage scoring results.

In addition, EEG data measured from the C3 channel during StageN2 were extracted. Sleep spindles were automatically detected using Kinoshita's algorithm. Based on the spindle detection results, StageN2 duration [min] and SD [min−1], which is defined as SN [min−1], were calculated. Because changes in the training data may affect the spindle detection performance, the mean and standard deviation of the number of spindles were calculated while switching the training data of the spindle detection model ten times. This performance evaluation procedure followed Kinoshita et al. (2020). Since we confirmed that there was no left-right difference in the appearance of spindles in the C3 and C4 channels, only spindles in the C3 channel were used for comparison. Details are described in the Results section.

To investigate the effects of acoustic environment on sleep, sleep parameters (TIB, TST, SOL, WASO, SE, Stage R(%TST), Stage N1(%TST), Stage N2(%TST), Stage N3(%TST), SN, and SD) were compared between two acoustic environment groups (white noise and pleasant sound). The ratios of the sleep parameters between those for environmental noise and silent were compared with the ratios of the parameters between those for pleasant sounds and silent.

Several sleep parameters, including the number of spindles and spindle density, widely vary among individuals. Thus, to investigate changes in sleep parameters in white noise and pleasant sound, we investigated changes in the parameters within individuals compared to silence.

Subgroup analysis

In a previous study, pleasant sounds shortened SOL in patients with insomnia (Morishima et al., 2016). In view of this result, we focused on healthy people with a tendency of insomnia. WASO of each participant in silent was evaluated, and the five participants with the largest WASO (PARTs 1, 2, 5, 7, and 11) were defined as the sleep maintenance difficulty group. The same parameters were compared among participants in the sleep maintenance difficulty group.

Statistical analysis

We used the paired-samples t-test with a significance level p < 0.05 to compare sleep parameters between white noise and pleasant sounds. Effect size (Cohen, 2013) was calculated for comparison of background sound environment on sleep parameters. All computations in this study were performed in MATLAB 2020b (Mathworks Inc.).

We planned a study of a continuous response variable from matched pairs of study participants. Prior data indicate that pleasant sound reduced 40% of SOL. If the difference in the mean response of matched pairs is 600 s with a standard deviation of 500 s, we estimated that we needed a study with 9 pairs of participants to be able to reject the null hypothesis that this response difference is zero with probability (power) 0.8 (PS Power and Sample Size Calculations, ver 3.0, Vanderbilt School of Medicine). The Type I error probability associated with this test of this null hypothesis is 0.05.

Results

We conducted the experiment on twelve participants who satisfied the participation eligibility criteria. One patient (PART 9) was excluded due to PSG corruption, so that 11 participants were ultimately included in the analysis (21.1 ± 0.83 yrs). The range of PSQI-J global scores was 5–9 points. Table 2 shows the gender, age, PSQI-J global score, and order of exposure of the participants to the sound environment.

Sleep parameters for each participant in the silent environment, white noise, and pleasant sounds are shown in Tables 35. Significant differences were not observed between the white noise and the pleasant sounds when comparing the ratios of the changes in the sleep parameters to the silent environment (control) for all participants (n = 11). The results of the comparison for all participants are shown in Table 6.

TABLE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. Sleep parameters with silent environment.

TABLE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Table 4. Sleep parameters with white noise.

TABLE 5
www.frontiersin.org

Table 5. Sleep parameters with pleasant sound.

TABLE 6
www.frontiersin.org

Table 6. Effects of white noise and pleasant sounds on sleep parameters compared to silent environment (All participants n = 11).

Table 7 shows the sleep efficiency for each participant in the silent environment. Five participants with large WASO were selected as the sleep maintenance difficulty group. These five subjects also displayed poor sleep efficiency. The same sleep parameters were compared only among the participants in the sleep maintenance difficulty group. The changes in sleep parameters from the silent environment in the sleep maintenance difficulty group are illustrated in Table 8. As a result of the comparisons, significant differences were not found in any of the sleep parameters, even for participants who had difficulty staying asleep. With regard to SOL, pleasant sounds tended to decrease SOL with large effect sizes (p = 0.060, Cohen's d = −1.15).

TABLE 7
www.frontiersin.org

Table 7. Sleep efficiency for each participant in silent environment.

TABLE 8
www.frontiersin.org

Table 8. Effects of white noise and pleasant sounds on sleep parameters compared to silent environment (Sleep maintenance difficulty group n = 5).

Figure 3 shows a boxplot of SOL for all participants and a boxplot for the sleep maintenance difficulty group. SN and SD were not significantly different for the five participants in the sleep maintenance difficulty group overall (Table 8).

FIGURE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 3. Box-and-whisker plot for sleep onset latency. Sleep onset latency for all participants (A) and for the sleep maintenance difficulty group (B). Vertical axis shows the ratio of SOL in the silent environment to 100%. A paired-samples t-test (significance level p < 0.05, two-sided test) for white noise (left) and pleasant sound (right) showed no significant difference in either case, but there was a significant trend (p = 0.060) in the sleep maintenance difficulty group with a large effect size (Cohen's d = −1.15). +, outside values.

Since classifier training and spindle detection were repeated ten times using Kinoshita's algorithm in this study, a t-test was conducted based on ten spindle detection results. The number of spindles and the spindle density were compared between each participant (white noise vs. pleasant sounds), which are summarized in Tables 9, 10. A significant increase in spindles was found in seven out of eleven participants (PARTs 1, 2, 4, 6, 7, 8, and 11). A significant increase in the spindle density was found in five out of eleven participants (PARTs 1, 2, 4, 6, and 11).

TABLE 9
www.frontiersin.org

Table 9. Changes in numbers of spindles with white noise and pleasant sounds.

TABLE 10
www.frontiersin.org

Table 10. Changes in spindle density with white noise and pleasant sounds.

On the other hand, none of the questions in the questionnaire was significant. In other words, pleasant sounds would not improve subjective sleep quality assessment, even if objective sleep quality assessment improved.

The reliability of this study largely depends on the performance of Kinoshita's algorithm. Figure 4 shows an example of Kinoshita's algorithm applied to EEG data. The colored bands in Figure 4 are the spindles detected by the algorithm, clearly showing that the detection result is consistent with the visual detection of spindles by the polysomnographic technician. Thus, we confirmed that Kinoshita's algorithm has adequate performance for spindle detection.

FIGURE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 4. Example of a spindle detection result with Kinoshita's algorithm. Electroencephalography (EEG) data were clipped from the C3 channel for 30 s during StageN2: (A) Participant 3 under the pleasant acoustic environment and (B) Participant 12 under white noise. The colored bands are the detected spindles. These figures clearly show that Kinoshita's algorithm appropriately detected spindles.

Tables 35 show that PART 11 had a notably smaller number of spindles in comparison with the other participants. Figure 5 shows his EEG data at the C3-A2 channel in sleep StageN2. The EEG and video of PART 11 showed body motion. These results suggest that PART 11's EEG data contains many body movement artifacts, which may have contributed to the spindle detection. Table 7 shows that PART 11 also had a notably low sleep efficiency, which may imply the presence of significant body motions during the participant's sleep. We should be careful when applying Kinoshita's algorithm when the objective EEG data contain significant artifacts.

FIGURE 5
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 5. Typical EEG data of the C3 channel in Participant 11. Kinoshita's algorithm could not correctly detect the spindles, because this participant's electroencephalography (EEG) data had significant artifacts. He had extremely poor sleep efficiency in comparison with other participants, suggesting that the artifacts were generated by body movement.

Spindles were detected from the C3 channels in this study. The number of spindles is considered to be almost the same between the left and right channels (Ebersole and Pedley, 2003). We applied Kinoshita's algorithm to the C4 channel in order to check the laterality of the spindles detected by Kinoshita's algorithm. We confirmed that there was no laterality in the detected number of spindles by means of a paired-samples t-test (C3:601 ± 339 vs. C4:517 ± 289, p = 0.0510). Thus, it was appropriate to perform analysis only on the C3 channel.

Discussion

In this study, we investigated the effects of three types of acoustic environments on overnight sleep. The results showed that pleasant sleep sounds did not have a statistically significant effect on sleep status in any of the participants, even when limited to those who had sleep maintenance difficulty. Pleasant sounds tended to shorten SOL with large effect sizes compared to white noise (p = 0.060, Cohen's d = −1.15). In a previous study, a significant reduction in SOL was observed in patients with insomnia exposed to pleasant sounds (Morishima et al., 2016), and although the results of the present study were not consistent with this finding, a trend toward a reduction in SOL was observed. In this study, the number of participants recruited was determined based on the expectation that pleasant sounds would shorten SOL by 40%. However, pleasant sounds had no expected effect on shortening SOL, and the small sample size may be another reason why no significant differences in sleep parameters were observed.

We confirmed that the pleasant sounds did not improve subjective sleep quality. This is not consistent with results of previous studies that investigated the relationship between sound and subjective sleep quality (Field, 1999; Harmat et al., 2008; Jespersen et al., 2015). According to a result of the questionnaire about pleasant sounds shown in Table 1, five out of eleven participants gave a score of third in the four-point scale for the pleasant sounds in item No. 3 “Your impression on the pleasant sounds.” The other six participants gave a score of second. This suggests that the participants may not have had a good impression of the pleasant sounds. It has been reported that the effect of music on emotions is also related to the individual's music preferences (Field, 1999; Tan, 2004; Lai and Good, 2005; Harmat et al., 2008). PARTs 1, 2, 7, and 11 belonged to the sleep maintenance difficulty group. This suggests that the pleasant sounds affect sleep, at least in terms of spindles, especially in participants who have difficulty maintaining sleep. Despite the fact that the results of the questionnaire showed that the pleasant sound was not a favorite, some participants showed a change in the SN and SD, suggesting that background sound environment may influence brain activity during sleep, regardless of the preference for background sound.

When considering the application of music intervention to sleep, the lack of a subjective sleep quality assessment may be disadvantageous in terms of motivating treatment. In order to consider individual preferences, music used for therapy should be selected from various musical categories.

This study was conducted based on a method that was developed after the experiment was conducted, so there is a limitation in the interpretation of the results shown. Limitations of this study include the collected data, such as all the participants being Japanese and healthy young males; that is, we could not consider racial, sex, or age differences in this study. We did not record AIS to confirm the inclusion criteria. Instead, we used the PSQI-J to confirm the subjective sleep quality of the participants. We used a cutoff score of 5 on the PSQI-J to select participants with sleep problems (as described in the methods section). However, it is a limitation that we did not use a score of 5.5, which is the usual cutoff for PSQI-J (Doi et al., 2000). The trend toward shorter SOL suggests that pleasant sounds may have some effect on improving sleep quality in a non-invasive manner in people who are aware of their insomnia. We need to collect additional data from a variety of people to confirm our results. In addition, it is noteworthy that our analysis results cannot be directly applied to insomnia patients because they may be using sleeping pills, which could affect sleep quality and spindles. Another limitation was that only one type of pleasant sound was used in the experiment. This limitation did not allow us to determine the influence of the sleep index on subjective music preference.

The protocol and data that support the findings of this study are available from the corresponding author upon reasonable request.

We confirmed a significant increase in spindles under the pleasant acoustic environment in seven out of eleven participants as compared with white noise. In addition, there was a trend toward a decrease in SOL in the sleep maintenance difficulty group. The results suggest that pleasant sounds may have some effect on sleep for participants with large WASO. Further research is needed to determine whether background sound exposure reduces sleep-related distress, produces a sense of sound sleep, or improves psychomotor function.

Conclusion

In this study, we conducted PSG with eleven healthy young male participants under three types of acoustic environments: silent, white noise, and pleasant sounds. We detected spindles from the EEG data recorded by PSG with an automatic spindle detection algorithm and evaluated the spindles and other sleep parameters.

While this study was conducted on healthy young participants, future studies will be conducted on patients with insomnia. In addition, we will test an intervention method for insomnia that combines music therapy with pharmacotherapy or CBT-I. In this study, the effect of pleasant sound on hyperarousal, which often occurs in patients with insomnia, is unknown, and thus is worth further investigation.

Data availability statement

The raw data supporting the conclusions of this article will be made available by the authors, without undue reservation.

Ethics statement

The studies involving human participants were reviewed and approved by the Ethics Committee of Shiga University of Medical Science. The patients/participants provided their written informed consent to participate in this study.

Author contributions

YS, KF, MM, and HK conceived the idea. YS, MM, and HK performed the experiments, interpreted the data, and revised the manuscript. YS and KF analyzed data. TK contributed to the materials and the music used in this study. SS and KF wrote the draft. All authors contributed to the article and approved the submitted version.

Funding

This study received funding from Yamaha Corporation. The funder was involved partially in the study design, but the funder was not involved in the collection, analysis, and interpretation of data, the writing of this article, or the decision to submit it for publication.

Conflict of interest

The data in this study were obtained in a joint research project between Shiga University of Medical Science and Yamaha Corporation. This study received funding from Yamaha Corporation. The funder was involved partially in the study design, but the funder was not involved in the collection, analysis, and interpretation of data, the writing of this article, or the decision to submit it for publication. KF is employed by Quadlytics Inc. HK received grants from the Investigator-Initiated Studies Program of Merck Sharp and Dohme LLC/MSD K.K., Eisai Co., Ltd. HK reports consulting fees from Takeda Pharmaceutical Co. Ltd. HK was supported by donations from FukudaLifetech Co., Ltd. and Fukuda Life Tech Keiji Co., Ltd. KY and TKa were employed by Yamaha Corporation. Dohme LLC/MSD K.K., Eisai Co. Ltd., Fukuda Life Tech Co. Ltd., and Fukuda Life Tech Keiji Co. Ltd. had no role in the design of the study; in the collection, analyses, or interpretation of data; in the writing of the manuscript, or in the decision to publish the results.

The remaining authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher's note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

Supplementary material

The Supplementary Material for this article can be found online at: https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/frsle.2022.986333/full#supplementary-material

Abbreviations

CBT-I, Cognitive behavioral therapy; EEG, electroencephalogram; ECG, electrocardiogram; SWS, slow-wave sleep; PSG, polysomnography; WHO, World Health Organization; SST, synchro-squeezed wavelet transforms; RUSBoost, random under-sampling boosting; MASS-C1, Montreal archives of sleep studies cohort 1; PART, Participant; TIB, Time in bed; TST, Total sleep time; SOL, Sleep onset latency; WASO, Wake after sleep onset; SE, Sleep efficiency; Stage R, REM sleep; Stage N1, non-REM sleep stage 1; Stage N2, non-REM sleep stage 2; Stage N3, non-REM sleep stage 3; SN, Number of spindles; SD, Spindle density.

References

Antony, J. W., Paller, K. A. (2017). Using oscillating sounds to manipulate sleep spindles. Sleep. 40, zsw068. doi: 10.1093/sleep/zsw068

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Berger, J., Turow, G. (2012). Music, Science, and the Rhythmic Brain: Cultural and Clinical Implications. 1st ed. New York, NY: Routledge. doi: 10.4324/9780203805299

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Berry, R. B., Brooks, R., Gamaldo, C. E., Harding, S. M., Marcus, C., Vaughn, B. V., et al. (2012). The AASM Manual for the Scoring of Sleep and Associated Events. Rules, Terminology and Technical Specifications. Darien, IL: American Academy of Sleep Medicine, 176.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Buysse, D. J., Reynolds, C. F., Monk, T. H., Berman, S. R., Kupfer, D. J. (1989). The pittsburgh sleep quality index: a new instrument for psychiatric practice and research. Psychiatry. Res. 28, 193–213. doi: 10.1016/0165-1781(89)90047-4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chiu, H. Y., Chang, L. Y., Hsieh, Y. J., Tsai, P. S. (2016). A meta-analysis of diagnostic accuracy of three screening tools for insomnia. J. Psychosom. Res. 87, 85–92. doi: 10.1016/j.jpsychores.2016.06.010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cohen, J. (2013). Statistical Power Analysis for the Behavioral Sciences. New York, NY: Routledge.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Cordi, M. J., Ackermann, S., Rasch, B. (2019). Effects of relaxing music on healthy sleep. Sci. Rep. 9, 9079. doi: 10.1038/s41598-019-45608-y

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cote, K. A., Epps, T. M., Campbell, K. B. (2000). The role of the spindle in human information processing of high-intensity stimuli during sleep. J. Sleep Res. 9, 19–26. doi: 10.1046/j.1365-2869.2000.00188.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

De Gennaro, L., Ferrara, M. (2003). Sleep spindles: an overview. Sleep Med. Rev. 7, 423–440. doi: 10.1053/smrv.2002.0252

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Doi, Y., Minowa, M., Uchiyama, M., Okawa, M., Kim, K., Shibui, K., et al. (2000). Psychometric assessment of subjective sleep quality using the Japanese version of the pittsburgh sleep quality index (PSQI-J) in psychiatric disordered and control subjects. Psychiatry. Res. 97, 165–172. doi: 10.1016/S0165-1781(00)00232-8

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ebersole, J. S., Pedley, T. A. E. (2003). Current Practice of Clinical Electroencephalography. Lippincott Williams and Wilkins

Google Scholar

Edinger, J. D., Arnedt, J. T., Bertisch, S. M., Carney, C. E., Harrington, J. H., Lichstein, K. L., et al. (2021). Behavioral and psychological treatments for chronic insomnia disorder in adults: an American academy of sleep medicine clinical practice guideline. J. Clin. Sleep Med. 17, 255–262. doi: 10.5664/jcsm.8986

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fernandez, L. M. J., Lüthi, A. (2020). Sleep spindles: mechanisms and functions. Physiol. Rev. 100, 805–868. doi: 10.1152/physrev.00042.2018

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Field, T. (1999). Music enhances sleep in preschool children. Early Child Dev. Care. 150, 65–68. doi: 10.1080/0300443991500106

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gerra, G., Zaimovic, A., Franchini, D., Palladino, M., Giucastro, G., Reali, N., et al. (1998). Neuroendocrine responses of healthy volunteers to 'techno-music': relationships with personality traits and emotional state. Int. J. Psychophysiol. 28, 99–111. doi: 10.1016/S0167-8760(97)00071-8

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Harmat, L., Takács, J., Bódizs, R. (2008). Music improves sleep quality in students. J. Adv. Nurs. 62, 327–335. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2648.2008.04602.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jespersen, K. V., Koenig, J., Jennum, P., Vuust, P. (2015). Music for insomnia in adults. Cochrane Database Syst. Rev. 8, CD010459. doi: 10.1002/14651858.CD010459.pub2

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kadotani, T., Kadotani, H., Arai, H., et al. (2017). Comparison of self-reported scales and structured interviews for the assessment of depression in an urban male working population in Japan: a cross-sectional survey. Sleep Sci. Pract. 1, 1–7. doi: 10.1186/s41606-017-0010-y

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kinoshita, T., Fujiwara, K., Kano, M., Ogawa, K., Sumi, Y., Matsuo, M., et al. (2020). Sleep spindle detection using rusboost and synchrosqueezed wavelet transform. IEEE Trans. Neural. Syst. Rehabil. Eng. 28, 390–398. doi: 10.1109/TNSRE.2020.2964597

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lai, H. L., Good, M. (2005). Music improves sleep quality in older adults. J. Adv. Nurs. 49, 234–244. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2648.2004.03281.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lai, H. L., Li, Y. M. (2011). The effect of music on biochemical markers and self-perceived stress among first-line nurses: a randomized controlled crossover trial. J. Adv. Nurs. 67, 2414–2424. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2648.2011.05670.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mahfoud, Y., Talih, F., Streem, D., Budur, K. (2009). Sleep disorders in substance abusers: how common are they? Psychiatry. 6, 38–42.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Mai, E., Buysse, D. J. (2008). Insomnia: prevalence, impact, pathogenesis, differential diagnosis, and evaluation. Sleep Med. Clin. 3, 167–174. doi: 10.1016/j.jsmc.2008.02.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Matsuo, M., Masuda, F., Sumi, Y., Takahashi, M., Yoshimura, A., Yamada, N., et al. (2019). Background music dependent reduction of aversive perception and its relation to P3 amplitude reduction and increased heart rate. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 13, 184. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2019.00184

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Morin, C. M., Inoue, Y., Kushida, C., Poyares, D., Winkelman, J., Guidelines Committee Members, et al. (2021). Endorsement of European guideline for the diagnosis and treatment of insomnia by the world sleep society. Sleep Med. 81, 124–126. doi: 10.1016/j.sleep.2021.01.023

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Morishima, M., Sugino, Y., Ueya, Y., Kadotani, H., Takadama, K. (2016). “Effects on sleep by “cradle sound” adjusted to heartbeat and respiration,” in Proceedings From the AAAI 2016 Spring Symposia (Palo Alto, CA).

Google Scholar

Okajima, I., Miyamoto, T., Ubara, A., Omichi, C., Matsuda, A., Sumi, Y., et al. (2020). Evaluation of severity levels of the athens insomnia scale based on the criterion of insomnia severity index. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health. 17, 8789. doi: 10.3390/ijerph17238789

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rangaraj, V. R., Knutson, K. L. (2016). Association between sleep deficiency and cardiometabolic disease: implications for health disparities. Sleep Med. 18, 19–35. doi: 10.1016/j.sleep.2015.02.535

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Riemann, D., Baglioni, C., Bassetti, C., Bjørn Bjorvatn 3, Leja Dolenc Groselj 4, Jason, G, Ellis, J. G., et al. (2017). European guideline for the diagnosis and treatment of insomnia. J. Sleep Res. 26, 675–700. doi: 10.1111/jsr.12594

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sheehan, D. V., Lecrubier, Y., Sheehan, K. H., Amorim, P., Janavs, J., Weiller, E., et al. (1998). The mini-international neuropsychiatric interview (M.I.N.I.): the development and validation of a structured diagnostic psychiatric interview for DSM-IV and ICD-10. J. Clin. Psychiatry. 59(Suppl 20), 22–33.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Soldatos, C. R. (1995). The Assessment of Insomnia: Rationale for a New Scale Based on ICD-10 Principles. Sleep Physiology and pathology Warszawa: Elma Books. 119–131.

Google Scholar

Soldatos, C. R., Dikeos, D. G., Paparrigopoulos, T. J. (2003). The diagnostic validity of the athens insomnia scale. J. Psychosom. Res. 55, 263–267. doi: 10.1016/S0022-3999(02)00604-9

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Steriade, M., Llinás, R. R. (1988). The functional states of the thalamus and the associated neuronal interplay. Physiol. Rev. 68, 649–742. doi: 10.1152/physrev.1988.68.3.649

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Steriade, M., McCormick, D. A., Sejnowski, T. J. (1993). Thalamocortical oscillations in the sleeping and aroused brain. Science. 262, 679–685. doi: 10.1126/science.8235588

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Takami, M., Kadotani, H., Nishikawa, K., Sumi, Y., Nakabayashi, T., Fujii, Y., et al. (2018). Quality of life, depression, and productivity of city government employees in Japan: a comparison study using the Athens insomnia scale and insomnia severity index. Sleep Sci. Pract. 2, 1–8. doi: 10.1186/s41606-018-0024-0

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tan, L. P. (2004). The effects of background music on quality of sleep in elementary school children. J. Music. Ther. 41, 128–150. doi: 10.1093/jmt/41.2.128

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tasali, E., Leproult, R., Spiegel, K. (2009). Reduced sleep duration or quality: relationships with insulin resistance and type 2 diabetes. Prog. Cardiovasc. Dis. 51, 381–391. doi: 10.1016/j.pcad.2008.10.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ventura, T., Gomes, M. C., Carreira, T. (2012). Cortisol and anxiety response to a relaxing intervention on pregnant women awaiting amniocentesis. Psychoneuroendocrinology. 37, 148–156. doi: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2011.05.016

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

World Health Organization. (2018). Regional Office for Environmental noise guidelines for the European Region. Copenhagen: World Health Organization. Regional Office for Europe.

Google Scholar

Keywords: pleasant sound, polysomnography, sleep spindle, EEG analysis, automatic spindle detection algorithm

Citation: Saeda S, Fujiwara K, Kinoshita T, Sumi Y, Matsuo M, Yamaki K, Kawashima T and Kadotani H (2022) Effects of pleasant sound on overnight sleep condition: A crossover randomized study. Front. Sleep 1:986333. doi: 10.3389/frsle.2022.986333

Received: 05 July 2022; Accepted: 26 September 2022;
Published: 18 October 2022.

Edited by:

Henning Johannes Drews, University of Copenhagen, Denmark

Reviewed by:

Miguel Meira E. Cruz, Centro Cardiovascular da Faculdade de Medicina da Universidade de Lisboa, Portugal
Akiyoshi Shimura, Tokyo Medical University, Japan

Copyright © 2022 Saeda, Fujiwara, Kinoshita, Sumi, Matsuo, Yamaki, Kawashima and Kadotani. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Koichi Fujiwara, ZnVqaXdhcmEua29pY2hpJiN4MDAwNDA7aHBzLm1hdGVyaWFsLm5hZ295YS11LmFjLmpw

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.