Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Child Adolesc. Psychiatry, 02 August 2024
Sec. Developmental Psychopathology and Mental Health

Understanding psychotic-like experiences in children in the context of dimensions of psychological problems

\r\nHee Jung JeongHee Jung Jeong1Benjamin B. Lahey,Benjamin B. Lahey2,3Gabrielle E. ReimannGabrielle E. Reimann1E. Leighton DurhamE. Leighton Durham1Camille ArcherCamille Archer1Tyler M. MooreTyler M. Moore4Krisha ShahKrisha Shah1Antonia N. Kaczkurkin
\r\nAntonia N. Kaczkurkin1*
  • 1Department of Psychology, Vanderbilt University, Nashville, TN, United States
  • 2Department of Health Studies, University of Chicago, Chicago, IL, United States
  • 3Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Neuroscience, University of Chicago, Chicago, IL, United States
  • 4Department of Psychiatry, Perelman School of Medicine, University of Pennsylvania, Philadelphia, PA, United States

Introduction: Although psychotic behaviors can be difficult to assess in children, early identification of children at high risk for the emergence of psychotic symptoms may facilitate the prevention of related disorders. Psychotic-like experiences (PLEs), or subthreshold thought and perceptual disturbances, could be early manifestations of psychosis that may predict a future diagnosis of a psychosis-related disorder or nonspecific correlates of a wide range of psychological problems. Additional research is needed regarding how PLEs map onto dimensions of psychopathology in children.

Methods: In the present study, we examined the association between PLEs and general and specific dimensions of psychological problems in a sample of 10,692 children from the Adolescent Brain Cognitive Development Study (ABCD Study).

Results: The results of this study showed that self-reported PLEs were associated with a general psychopathology factor and an ADHD factor, which were defined in hierarchical models of parent-rated psychological problems.

Discussion: These findings suggest that PLEs are broadly associated with a wide range of psychological problems through the general psychopathology factor even before psychotic disorders typically manifest. This study supports the need for longitudinal analyses of future waves of the ABCD Study to determine if PLEs can detect children at high risk for serious psychological problems in adulthood.

Introduction

Psychotic behaviors that meet diagnostic criteria for schizophrenia or other mental health disorders are difficult to assess in children as the assessment requires specialized skills and it may be difficult to fully capture these symptoms in children with a relatively limited ability to share their experiences. Yet there is a sharp increase in the incidence of diagnosed psychotic disorders beginning in late adolescence (1), suggesting that detection of children at high risk for the emergence of serious psychotic behavior before the manifestation of a diagnosis could facilitate early identification and prevention efforts. One possible risk factor for the onset of psychosis in the future is self-reported psychotic-like experiences (PLEs) during childhood. PLEs refer to psychotic-related symptoms reported in non-clinical samples (2), with a prevalence estimated to be 5%–8% in the general population (3). The concept of PLEs has received attention due to an increased recognition that psychotic experiences exist on a continuum from few symptoms to many symptoms, suggesting the existence of a spectrum of psychosis symptoms (4). The prevalence of PLEs is estimated to be higher in children than in the general population, with increased rates in younger children than older children (5). Specifically, some children may report hearing strange sounds or having unusual sensations, being distrustful of others, believing that they have magical powers or that their mind is trying to trick them, or other atypical perceptions that resemble psychotic-related experiences (69). The meaning of these experiences is unclear, especially as most of these symptoms disappear over time in children (10), suggesting that some PLEs are not necessarily associated with any specific risk for psychopathology. However, there are two possibilities regarding how PLEs in children may be related to psychopathology.

First, PLEs could be early manifestations of psychosis that may predict a future diagnosis of a psychotic disorder in a subset of children who experience these perceptions (3, 6, 11, 12). Second, there is emerging evidence that PLEs may be nonspecific presentations of a wide range of internalizing and externalizing psychological problems (1315). Furthermore, the broad range of dimensions of psychological problems that are associated with PLEs raises the possibility that they are associated with a general factor (or p factor) of psychological problems (1618). Several researchers have proposed hierarchical models of the structure of correlated dimensions of psychological problems (8, 16, 19, 20). Although these models differ in some important ways, including the specific statistical approaches used to describe the correlational structure, each model assumes that the shared variance across all symptoms is captured by a general factor of psychopathology.

These two explanations for the existence of PLEs (that PLEs index an early manifestation of psychosis or that PLEs are nonspecifically associated with general psychopathology) are not necessarily mutually exclusive. PLEs could indicate early signs of a future diagnosis of psychosis and be linked to general mental health issues at the same time. This is supported by prior work suggesting that severe psychotic experiences are associated with overall mental health problems. In particular, problematic psychotic experiences considered to be symptoms of schizophrenia, schizotypal personality disorder, and mania such as the individual being “out of touch with reality” have been proposed to be strongly associated with the general factor of psychopathology (20). Likewise, Caspi and Moffitt proposed that disordered thought (illogical and distorted thinking) is one of the dysfunctional processes underlying the general factor of psychological problems (17). Thus, PLEs might indicate both early symptoms of psychosis and a component of a general psychopathology factor. While it should be noted that some PLEs reported in children are normative and may not be associated with mental health diagnoses, it is possible that PLEs can reflect both early psychosis symptoms and general mental health problems.

These hypotheses are tentatively supported by previous findings from several studies of adolescents and adults showing that symptoms of psychosis load robustly onto the general factor of psychopathology (16, 2022). Furthermore, there is growing evidence from family and molecular studies that schizophrenia shares much of its genetic risk with a broad range of other mental health disorders (2325) and with PLEs (26). This suggests that at least some of the genetic and environmental variance that gives rise to disordered thought is shared with essentially all other dimensions of psychological problems, as represented by a general psychopathology factor. However, this work has been primarily conducted in adolescents and adults; it remains unclear whether these associations are apparent in children.

Additionally, prior work suggesting that psychosis is associated with a general factor of psychopathology was limited because it only examined the association between psychotic behavior and the general factor, neglecting PLEs and specific factors such as internalizing and externalizing dimensions (16, 27). When examining associations between PLEs and the general or specific factors, different interpretations may arise depending on the hierarchical model used (28). In a bifactor model, every item loads on both the general factor and one specific factor, thereby partitioning the total explained variance in each item between the general and specific factors (2931). Thus, dimensions like conduct problems and attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) are orthogonal (uncorrelated) when defined in a bifactor model because the shared variance among all items is allocated to the general factor (3032). In contrast, in a higher-order model, each item loads on one of several correlated lower-order factors and the lower-order factors then load onto a higher-order general factor (19). In this model, the general factor is defined by the variance shared by the lower-order factors, and there is no direct relationship between items and the general factor.

Despite the growing number of studies using dimensional models to define psychopathology, there has been a dearth of work examining the relationship between PLEs and psychopathology dimensions in children. Thus, the purpose of the current study was to examine the association between PLEs and dimensions of psychopathology measured with bifactor and higher-order models in a large sample of children. Since the meaning of the association between the general and specific factors and PLEs changes based on the hierarchical modeling being used, the current study advances our understanding of PLEs in children by examining their association with the general and specific dimensions of psychological problems defined using two different models. Based on previous findings, it was hypothesized that PLEs would be associated with the general psychopathology factor. The associations between PLEs and the specific factors were exploratory.

Methods

Participants

The present analyses used data from 10,692 participants (48.02% female) from wave 1 (collected from September 2016 to June 2018) of the Adolescent Brain Cognitive DevelopmentSM Study (ABCD Study®; release 4.0). Participants with missing data on parent ratings of psychological problems, age, sex, race-ethnicity, and non-participation and post-stratification weight variables were excluded from analyses. The sample was recruited at 22 sites across the United States at 9–10 years of age as part of a planned longitudinal study. The sites do not perfectly represent the population of the United States, but the same unbiased recruitment process was used within every site (33). Furthermore, the ABCD Study provides post-stratification weights which are used to adjust the sample to better approximate population parameters in terms of demographics such as sex, race/ethnicity, and family income (34). These post-stratification weights were applied in all analyses. Most (66.42%) participants were one child of singleton birth from different families, but some had a twin or non-twin sibling in the study, so family was taken into account (see Data Analyses section). Parents classified their children as non-Hispanic White (52.5%), Hispanic (20.53%), Black (14.5%), and other racial-ethnic groups (12.48%). See Table 1 for additional details about the demographic breakdown of the sample.

Table 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Summary of the demographic characteristics of the sample (N = 10,692).

Measures

The Child Behavior Checklist (CBCL) (35) is a parent rating scale of child behavior consisting of 119 symptoms (113 items with some items representing multiple symptoms) describing problem behaviors and emotions on a scale of 0 = not true (as far as you know), 1 = somewhat or sometimes true, or 2 = very true or often true. Missing data on CBCL items was <0.1%. PLEs were measured using the Prodromal Questionnaire–Brief Child Version (PQ-BC), which has been shown to be a valid measure of PLEs in children (7). Children reported whether they had experienced each of 21 psychotic-like experiences and then rated the extent to which the endorsed experience bothered them on a 1–5 scale. The PQ-BC has been shown to have adequate internal consistency, exhibit measurement invariance across the sexes and racial/ethnic groups, and exhibit construct validity in a subset of the ABCD Study sample (7). Furthermore, the PQ-BC has been shown to be associated with a family history of psychotic disorder specifically, but not depression or mania (7). This measure is also associated with other known correlates of PLEs including internalizing and externalizing symptoms, neuropsychological test performance deficits, and motor and speech developmental delays (7). For the current analysis, the total number of PLEs, which is the sum of endorsed questions (0 = No; 1 = Yes; possible range = 0–21) and distress-weighted PLEs, which is the total number of endorsed questions (0 = No; 1 = Yes) weighted by level of distress (1 = Not very bothered; 2 = Slightly bothered; 3 = Moderately bothered; 4 = Very much bothered; 5 = Extremely bothered; possible range = 0–126) were used.

Data analyses

Structural equation modeling (SEM) was conducted in Mplus 8.4 using the mean- and variance-adjusted weighted least squares (WSLMV) estimator and pairwise deletion for missing data (36). All analyses included clustering based on family membership to account for siblings and multiple births in the sample, stratification by site to account for data collection across 21 sites, and weighting by post-stratification weights to make the sample more representative of the U.S. population. In our previous study (37), exploratory structural equation modeling identified three psychopathology dimensions: conduct problems, internalizing symptoms, and ADHD symptoms. We examined the hierarchical structure of the CBCL items using two models: a bifactor model and a higher-order model. In a bifactor model (Figure 1A), every CBCL item loads onto both the general factor and on one and only one orthogonal specific factor obtained from exploratory analyses. In a bifactor model, the total explained variance in each item is partitioned between the general and specific factors (2931) so that the general factor and specific factors are uncorrelated with one another. In a higher-order model (Figure 1B), each item loads onto one of several correlated lower-order factors and the lower-order factors load onto a higher-order general factor. The general and specific factors in the bifactor and the higher-order models have been shown to exhibit adequate construct reliability and estimated replicability and demonstrate robust criterion validity in a random half of the ABCD Study sample (37).

Figure 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. A diagram of the bifactor and higher-order models. (A) In a bifactor model, each CBCL item loads both onto a general factor and only one of the specific factors. All factors are orthogonal to each other. (B) In a higher-order model, each CBCL item loads onto lower-order factors, which then load onto a general factor. The lower-order factors in the higher-order model are allowed to correlate.

SEM was performed to examine the association between PLEs and the general and specific factors of psychopathology. The total number of PLEs and distress-weighted PLEs were predicted by the general and specific factors of psychopathology controlling for demographic covariates (age, sex, and race/ethnicity). Using a bifactor model, the general factor and specific factors were included in the model at the same time, which is possible because the factors are orthogonal. Due to the perfect collinearity between the general factor and the specific factors defined through higher-order modeling, separate analyses were performed with the general factor and the specific factors predicting PLEs. Sensitivity analyses were performed controlling for parental education and family income to account for socioeconomic status. To correct for multiple comparisons, the false discovery rate (q < 0.05) was used to the adjust p-values across each factor obtained through bifactor and higher-order models. The analysis code is available at https://github.com/VU-BRAINS-lab/ABCD-PLE.

Results

The general and specific factors defined in a bifactor model exhibited adequate determinacy, construct replicability, and other psychometric properties, as did the general and lower-order factors defined in a higher-order model (Table 2). Using a bifactor model that controlled for age, sex, and race/ethnicity and after correction for multiple comparisons, the results showed that PLEs were significantly associated with the general psychopathology, conduct problems, and ADHD factors (Table 3). When parental education and family income were added as covariates, the magnitudes of the associations with the general and ADHD factors decreased but were still statistically significant (Table 3). In contrast, the specific conduct problems factor was no longer significantly associated with PLEs after controlling for socioeconomic variables. The same pattern of significant findings was found when children's self-reported PLEs were weighted by ratings of how much the experiences upset the child—the general and ADHD factors remained significant but the specific conduct problems was not associated with distress-weighted PLEs (Table 3).

Table 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Psychometric indices for each factor in the bifactor and the higher-order models.

Table 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. Primary and sensitivity analyses testing associations of general and specific factors of psychological problems defined in either bifactor or higher-order models with two measures of psychotic-like experiences, adjusting for demographic covariates.

When PLEs were associated with the general factor defined in the higher-order model while accounting for age, sex, and race/ethnicity, both measures of PLEs (counts and distress-weighted) were significantly associated with the general factor (Table 3). When PLEs were related to the three lower-order factors defined in the higher-order model while accounting for the same covariates, both measures of PLEs (counts and distress-weighted) were significantly associated with the ADHD factor. The associations between measures of PLEs and the general factor and ADHD factor remained significant after controlling for parental education and family income (Table 3).

Discussion

Using two different statistical models and controlling for socioeconomic-related covariates, the results of the current study showed that both the number of PLEs endorsed and the amount of distress associated with these PLEs were significantly associated with the general factor of psychological problems. This is consistent with previous observations that PLEs are associated with a broad range of mental health problems and provides support for the hypothesis that disordered thought may be associated with the general factor of psychological problems (16, 20). PLEs were also found to be associated with specific ADHD problems in both statistical models. An association with conduct problems was found, but this did not survive after accounting for socioeconomic status. Finally, no associations between PLEs and internalizing symptoms were found.

The relationship between PLEs the general factor of psychopathology suggests that the presence of PLEs in children could be an indicator of risk for overall psychopathology, regardless of whether psychosis is present or not. This is consistent with previous studies that found high rates of mood and anxiety disorders in a community sample who reported psychotic experiences (38, 39). In this regard, van Os and Reininghause (40) suggested that PLEs represent two constructs: (1) a specific phenotype of attenuated psychotic phenomena and (2) a trans-phenotype related to different domains of psychopathology such as positive, negative, affective, and disorganized domains. The current findings support the second construct defined by van Os and Reininghause by demonstrating an association between PLEs and the general factor of psychopathology in children. The future waves of the longitudinal ABCD Study will be helpful in identifying whether the PLEs continue to predict transdiagnostic psychopathological phenomena or whether a specific association with psychotic symptoms becomes apparent as these symptoms emerge in adolescence and young adulthood.

The current study also showed a consistent association between PLEs and ADHD symptoms. The association between PLEs and ADHD is interesting in light of previous evidence that ADHD in childhood is associated with an increased risk for later diagnoses of psychotic disorders (4143). Furthermore, previous studies have found shared genetic (44) and environmental risk (45) between childhood ADHD and adult schizophrenia. To explain the link between childhood ADHD and adulthood psychosis, it has been suggested that motor, perceptual, and attentional difficulties that resemble ADHD symptoms in children are early manifestations of behavioral alterations related to schizophrenia (46). While the current study builds upon previous work by using a large sample of children and supports the specific association between ADHD and PLEs, PLEs are not the same as psychosis—additional evidence is needed to confirm the association between ADHD and the risk of psychosis using future time points in this sample.

The present findings are also notable because the significant associations between PLEs and dimensions of psychological problems were found across informants (i.e., parent-rated CBCL problems and youth-reported PLEs). This is important because it rules out common method variance as an explanation for the observed associations. Since parent and youth ratings of psychological problems only correlate moderately (47), perhaps because of differences in maturity, perspectives, and opportunities to observe the child's behavior, it will be important to examine these associations in later waves of the ABCD Study when measures of youth-reported psychological problems are available.

Limitations

The current study has limitations that should be taken into consideration. First, although the measure used in the current study has been shown to be a valid measure of PLEs in children (7), it was originally developed to measure prodromal states (48) which are distinctive from PLEs. Thus, it would be helpful to replicate these findings using a dedicated measure of PLEs. Secondly, although PLEs represent a heterogeneous construct, we used a single summary score of PLEs which limits our ability to draw inferences regarding whether specific types of PLEs are related to a greater extent to each psychopathology dimension. Rather than relying on a total score, future work that delineates distinct dimensions of PLEs utilizing factor analysis may be useful for examining more nuanced associations with psychopathology in children.

Conclusion

In conclusion, our findings on the association of PLEs with dimensions of psychopathology in children are consistent with the hypothesis that psychotic-like experiences share some variance with general psychological problems and ADHD symptoms. However, given that many children outgrown their reported PLEs by adolescence and adulthood, future work is needed to determine whether PLEs observed in childhood are associated with future psychotic diagnoses. The additional waves of the longitudinal ABCD Study will provide a unique opportunity to examine whether PLEs represent risk factors for psychotic behavior prospectively from childhood through young adulthood.

Data availability statement

Publicly available datasets were analyzed in this study. This data can be found here: https://nda.nih.gov/abcd.

Ethics statement

Use of this de-identified publicly available dataset was approved by the Vanderbilt University Institutional Review Board. The ABCD study was conducted in accordance with the local legislation and institutional requirements. Written informed consent for participation was obtained from the participants or the participants’ legal guardians/next of kin in accordance with the national legislation and institutional requirements.

Author contributions

HJ: Writing – review & editing, Writing – original draft. BL: Writing – review & editing, Writing – original draft, Methodology, Funding acquisition, Conceptualization. GR: Writing – review & editing. ED: Writing – review & editing. CA: Writing – review & editing. TM: Writing – review & editing. KS: Writing – review & editing. AK: Writing – review & editing, Writing – original draft, Funding acquisition.

Funding

The author(s) declare financial support was received for the research, authorship, and/or publication of this article.

Data used in the preparation of this article were obtained from the Adolescent Brain Cognitive DevelopmentSM (ABCD) Study (https://abcdstudy.org), held in the NIMH Data Archive (NDA). This is a multisite, longitudinal study designed to recruit more than 10,000 children age 9–10 and follow them over 10 years into early adulthood. The ABCD Study® is supported by the National Institutes of Health and additional federal partners under award numbers U01DA041048, U01DA050989, U01DA051016, U01DA041022, U01DA051018, U01DA051037, U01DA050987, U01DA041174, U01DA041106, U01DA041117, U01DA041028, U01DA041134, U01DA050988, U01DA051039, U01DA041156, U01DA041025, U01DA041120, U01DA051038, U01DA041148, U01DA041093, U01DA041089, U24DA041123, U24DA041147. A full list of supporters is available at https://abcdstudy.org/federal-partners.html. A listing of participating sites and a complete listing of the study investigators can be found at https://abcdstudy.org/consortium_members/. ABCD consortium investigators designed and implemented the study and/or provided data but did not necessarily participate in the analysis or writing of this report. This manuscript reflects the views of the authors and may not reflect the opinions or views of the NIH or ABCD consortium investigators. The ABCD data repository grows and changes over time. The ABCD data used in this report came from RRID: SCR_015769, DOI 10.15154/1523041 (data release 4.0) and NDA study DOI 10.15154/1527878. DOIs can be found at https://nda.nih.gov/abcd/study-information. This research was supported by grants R01MH098098 (BL), R01MH117014 (TM), R00MH117274 (AK), and T32-MH18921 (ED is a trainee on this grant) from the National Institute of Mental Health, the NARSAD Young Investigator Grant from the Brain & Behavior Research Foundation (AK), the Sloan Research Fellowship (AK), the David H. and Beverly A. Barlow Grant from the American Psychological Foundation (AK), and the Lifespan Brain Institute of the University of Pennsylvania and the Children’s Hospital of Philadelphia (TM). This material is based upon work supported by the National Science Foundation Graduate Research Fellowship Program under Grant No. 1937963 (GR). Any opinions, findings, and conclusions or recommendations expressed in this material are those of the authors and do not necessarily reflect the views of the National Science Foundation.

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher's note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

References

1. Dalsgaard S, Thorsteinsson E, Trabjerg BB, Schullehner J, Plana-Ripoll O, Brikell I, et al. Incidence rates and cumulative incidences of the full spectrum of diagnosed mental disorders in childhood and adolescence. JAMA Psychiatry. (2020) 77(2):155–64. doi: 10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2019.3523

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

2. Kelleher I, Cannon M. Psychotic-like experiences in the general population: characterizing a high-risk group for psychosis. Psychol Med. (2011) 41(1):1–6. doi: 10.1017/S0033291710001005

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

3. Linscott RJ, Van Os J. An updated and conservative systematic review and meta-analysis of epidemiological evidence on psychotic experiences in children and adults: on the pathway from proneness to persistence to dimensional expression across mental disorders. Psychol Med. (2013) 43(6):1133–49. doi: 10.1017/S0033291712001626

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

4. Hinterbuchinger B, Mossaheb N. Psychotic-Like experiences: a challenge in definition and assessment. Front Psychiatry. (2021) 12. Frontiers Media S.A. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2021.582392

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

5. Kelleher I, Connor D, Clarke MC, Devlin N, Harley M, Cannon M. Prevalence of psychotic symptoms in childhood and adolescence: a systematic review and meta-analysis of population-based studies. Psychol Med. (2012) 42(9):1857–63. doi: 10.1017/S0033291711002960

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

6. Horwood J, Salvi G, Thomas K, Duffy L, Gunnell D, Hollis C, et al. IQ And non-clinical psychotic symptoms in 12-year-olds: results from the ALSPAC birth cohort. Br J Psychiatry. (2008) 193(3):185–91. doi: 10.1192/bjp.bp.108.051904

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

7. Karcher NR, Barch DM, Avenevoli S, Savill M, Huber RS, Simon TJ, et al. Assessment of the prodromal questionnaire-brief child version for measurement of self-reported psychoticlike experiences in childhood. JAMA Psychiatry. (2018) 75(8):853–61. doi: 10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2018.1334

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

8. Karcher NR, Perino MT, Barch DM. An item response theory analysis of the prodromal questionnaire-brief child version: developing a screening form that informs understanding of self-reported psychotic-like experiences in childhood. J Abnorm Psychol. (2020) 129(3):293–304. doi: 10.1037/abn0000502

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

9. Tang W, Xu D, Yang Y, Xu J. Psychotic-like experiences in Chinese children and adolescents: the effect of earthquake exposure, maltreatment and negative life events. Early Interv Psychiatry. (2021) 15(3):536–46. doi: 10.1111/eip.12976

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

10. Remberk B. Znaczenie kliniczne doznań podobnych do psychotycznych u dzieci i młodziezy. Psychiatr Pol. (2017) 51(2):271–82. doi: 10.12740/PP/63894

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

11. Guloksuz S, Van Os J. The slow death of the concept of schizophrenia and the painful birth of the psychosis spectrum. Psychol Med. (2018) 48(2):229–44. doi: 10.1017/S0033291717001775

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

12. O’Brien KJ, Barch DM, Kandala S, Karcher NR. Examining specificity of neural correlates of childhood psychotic-like experiences during an emotional n-back task. Biol Psychiatry Cogn Neurosci Neuroimaging. (2020) 5(6):580–90. doi: 10.1016/j.bpsc.2020.02.012

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

13. Bourgin J, Tebeka S, Mallet J, Mazer N, Dubertret C, Le Strat Y. Prevalence and correlates of psychotic-like experiences in the general population. Schizophr Res. (2020) 215:371–7. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2019.08.024

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

14. Isaksson J, Vadlin S, Olofsdotter S, Åslund C, Nilsson KW. Psychotic-like experiences during early adolescence predict symptoms of depression, anxiety, and conduct problems three years later: a community-based study. Schizophr Res. (2020) 215:190–6. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2019.10.033

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

15. Laurens KR, Tzoumakis S, Dean K, Harris F, Carr VJ, Green MJ. Population profiles of child-reported psychotic-like experiences and their differential association with other psychopathologies. Br J Clin Psychol. (2020) 59(1):22–38. doi: 10.1111/bjc.12230

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

16. Caspi A, Houts RM, Belsky DW, Goldman-Mellor SJ, Harrington H, Israel S, et al. The p factor: one general psychopathology factor in the structure of psychiatric disorders? Clin Psychol Sci. (2014) 2(2):119–37. doi: 10.1177/2167702613497473

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

17. Caspi A, Moffitt TE. All for one and one for all: mental disorders in one dimension. Am J Psychiatry. (2018) 175(9):831–44. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2018.17121383

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

18. Lahey BB, Applegate B, Hakes JK, Zald DH, Hariri AR, Rathouz PJ. Is there a general factor of prevalent psychopathology during adulthood? J Abnorm Psychol. (2012) 121(4):971–7. doi: 10.1037/a0028355

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

19. Kotov R, Waszczuk MA, Krueger RF, Forbes MK, Watson D, Clark LA, et al. The hierarchical taxonomy of psychopathology (HiTOP): a dimensional alternative to traditional nosologies. J Abnorm Psychol. (2017) 126(4):454–77. doi: 10.1037/abn0000258

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

20. Lahey BB, Krueger RF, Rathouz PJ, Waldman ID, Zald DH. A hierarchical causal taxonomy of psychopathology across the life span. Psychol Bull. (2017) 143(2):142–86. doi: 10.1037/bul0000069

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

21. Pettersson E, Larsson H, Lichtenstein P. Common psychiatric disorders share the same genetic origin: a multivariate sibling study of the Swedish population. Mol Psychiatry. (2016) 21(5):717–21. doi: 10.1038/mp.2015.116

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

22. Pettersson E, Larsson H, D’Onofrio B, Almqvist C, Lichtenstein P. Association of fetal growth with general and specific mental health conditions. JAMA Psychiatry. (2019) 76(5):536–43. doi: 10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2018.4342

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

23. Pettersson E, Lichtenstein P, Larsson H, Song J, Agrawal A, Borglum AD, et al. Genetic influences on eight psychiatric disorders based on family data of 4 408 646 full and half-siblings, and genetic data of 333 748 cases and controls. Psychol Med. (2019) 49(7):1166–73. doi: 10.1017/S0033291718002039

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

24. Smoller JW, Andreassen OA, Edenberg HJ, Faraone SV, Glatt SJ, Kendler KS. Psychiatric genetics and the structure of psychopathology. Mol Psychiatry. (2019) 24(3):409–20. doi: 10.1038/s41380-017-0010-4

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

25. Smoller JW, Kendler K, Craddock N, Lee PH, Neale BM, Nurnberger JN, et al. Identification of risk loci with shared effects on five major psychiatric disorders: a genome-wide analysis. Lancet. (2013) 381(9875):1371–9. doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736(12)62129-1

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

26. Pain O, Dudbridge F, Cardno AG, Freeman D, Lu Y, Lundstrom S, et al. Genome-wide analysis of adolescent psychotic-like experiences shows genetic overlap with psychiatric disorders. Am J Med Genet B Neuropsychiatr Genet. (2018) 177(4):416–25. doi: 10.1002/ajmg.b.32630

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

27. Lahey BB, Zald DH, Perkins SF, Villalta-Gil V, Werts KB, Van Hulle CA, et al. Measuring the hierarchical general factor model of psychopathology in young adults. Int J Methods Psychiatr Res. (2018) 27(1):1–9. doi: 10.1002/mpr.1593

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

28. Lahey BB, Moore TM, Kaczkurkin AN, Zald DH. Hierarchical models of psychopathology: empirical support, implications, and remaining issues. World Psychiatry. (2020) 20(1):57–63. doi: 10.1002/wps.20824

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

29. Holzinger KJ, Swineford F. The bi-factor method. Psychometrika. (1937) 2(1):41–54. doi: 10.1007/BF02287965

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

30. Mansolf M, Reise SP. When and why the second-order and bifactor models are distinguishable. Intelligence. (2017) 61:120–9. doi: 10.1016/j.intell.2017.01.012

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

31. Reise SP. The rediscovery of bifactor measurement models. Multivariate Behav Res. (2012) 47(5):667–96. doi: 10.1080/00273171.2012.715555

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

32. Kaczkurkin AN, Moore TM, Sotiras A, Xia CH, Shinohara RT, Satterthwaite TD. Approaches to defining common and dissociable neurobiological deficits associated with psychopathology in youth. Biol Psychiatry. (2020) 88(1):51–62. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2019.12.015

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

33. Garavan H, Bartsch H, Conway K, Decastro A, Goldstein RZ, Heeringa S, et al. Recruiting the ABCD sample: design considerations and procedures. Dev Cogn Neurosci. (2018) 32:16–22. doi: 10.1016/j.dcn.2018.04.004

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

34. Heeringa SG, Berglund PA. A guide for population-based analysis of the adolescent brain cognitive development (ABCD) study baseline data. BioRxiv. (2020):2020.02.10.942011. doi: 10.1101/2020.02.10.942011

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

35. Achenbach TM. The Achenbach System of Empirically Based Assessment (ASEBA): Development, Findings, Theory, and Applications. Burlington, VT: University of Vermont Research Center for Children, Youth, and Families (2009).

Google Scholar

36. Muthén LK, Muthén BO. Mplus User’s Guide. 8th ed Los Angeles, CA: Muthén & Muthén (2017). doi: 10.1111/j.1600-0447.2011.01711.x

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

37. Moore TM, Kaczkurkin AN, Durham EL, Jeong HJ, McDowell MG, Dupont RM, et al. Criterion validity and relationships between alternative hierarchical dimensional models of general and specific psychopathology. J Abnorm Psychol. (2020) 129:677–88. doi: 10.1037/abn0000601

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

38. Varghese D, Scott J, Welham J, Bor W, Najman J, O’Callaghan M, et al. Psychotic-like experiences in major depression and anxiety disorders: a population-based survey in young adults. Schizophr Bull. (2011) 37(2):389–93. doi: 10.1093/schbul/sbp083

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

39. Wigman JTW, Van Nierop M, Vollebergh WAM, Lieb R, Beesdo-Baum K, Wittchen HU, et al. Evidence that psychotic symptoms are prevalent in disorders of anxiety and depression, impacting on illness onset, risk, and severity—implications for diagnosis and ultra-high risk research. Schizophr Bull. (2012) 38(2):247–57. doi: 10.1093/schbul/sbr196

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

40. Van Os J, Reininghaus U. Psychosis as a transdiagnostic and extended phenotype in the general population. World Psychiatry. (2016) 15(2):118–24. doi: 10.1002/wps.20310

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

41. Björkenstam E, Pierce M, Björkenstam C, Dalman C, Kosidou K. Attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder and risk for non-affective psychotic disorder: the role of ADHD medication and comorbidity, and sibling comparison. Schizophr Res. (2020) 218:124–30. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2020.01.021

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

42. Dalsgaard S, Mortensen PB, Frydenberg M, Maibing CM, Nordentoft M, Thomsen PH. Association between attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder in childhood and schizophrenia later in adulthood. Eur Psychiatry. (2014) 29(4):259–63. doi: 10.1016/j.eurpsy.2013.06.004

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

43. Hennig T, Jaya ES, Koglin U, Lincoln TM. Associations of attention-deficit/hyperactivity and other childhood disorders with psychotic experiences and disorders in adolescence. Eur Child Adolesc Psychiatry. (2017) 26(4):421–31. doi: 10.1007/s00787-016-0904-8

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

44. Hamshere ML, Stergiakouli E, Langley K, Martin J, Holmans P, Kent L, et al. Shared polygenic contribution between childhood attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder and adult schizophrenia. Br J Psychiatry. (2013) 203(2):107–11. doi: 10.1192/bjp.bp.112.117432

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

45. Peralta V, de Jalón EG, Campos MS, Zandio M, Sanchez-Torres A, Cuesta MJ. The meaning of childhood attention-deficit hyperactivity symptoms in patients with a first-episode of schizophrenia-spectrum psychosis. Schizophr Res. (2011) 126(1–3):28–35. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2010.09.010

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

46. Keshavan MS, Sujata M, Mehra A, Montrose DM, Sweeney JA. Psychosis proneness and ADHD in young relatives of schizophrenia patients. Schizophr Res. (2003) 59(1):85–92. doi: 10.1016/s0920-9964(01)00400-5

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

47. Achenbach TM, McConaughy SH, Howell CT. Child/adolescent behavioral and emotional problems: implications of cross-informant correlations for situational specificity. Psychol Bull. (1987) 101(2):213–32. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.101.2.213

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

48. Loewy RL, Pearson R, Vinogradov S, Bearden CE, Cannon TD. Psychosis risk screening with the prodromal questionnaire—brief version (PQ-B). Schizophr Res. (2011) 129(1):42–6. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2011.03.029

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: psychotic-like experience, general psychopathology, ADHD, conduct problems, children

Citation: Jeong HJ, Lahey BB, Reimann GE, Durham EL, Archer C, Moore TM, Shah K and Kaczkurkin AN (2024) Understanding psychotic-like experiences in children in the context of dimensions of psychological problems. Front. Child Adolesc. Psychiatry 3:1410804. doi: 10.3389/frcha.2024.1410804

Received: 1 April 2024; Accepted: 18 July 2024;
Published: 2 August 2024.

Edited by:

Tiziana Pisano, Azienda Ospedaliera Universitaria Meyer IRCCS—Firenze, Italy

Reviewed by:

Barbara Remberk, Institute of Psychiatry and Neurology (IPiN), Poland
Yi Nam Suen, The University of Hong Kong, Hong Kong SAR, China

© 2024 Jeong, Lahey, Reimann, Durham, Archer, Moore, Shah and Kaczkurkin. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Antonia N. Kaczkurkin, YW50b25pYS5rYWN6a3Vya2luQHZhbmRlcmJpbHQuZWR1

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.