- 1Department of Psychology, Manchester Metropolitan University, Manchester, United Kingdom
- 2Centre for Academic Mental Health, Population Health Sciences, Bristol Medical School, University of Bristol, Bristol, United Kingdom
- 3Department of Primary Care & Mental Health, University of Liverpool, Liverpool, United Kingdom
- 4Research Department, Cheshire and Wirral Partnership NHS Foundation Trust, Chester, United Kingdom
- 5Florence Nightingale Faculty of Nursing, Midwifery & Palliative Care, King’s College London, London, United Kingdom
- 6Great Ormond Street Hospital NHS Foundation Trust, London, United Kingdom
Background: Current observational methods to understand adolescent-parent interaction are limited in terms of ecological and content validity. We outline initial results and a protocol for future work from a programme of work to: (1) establish a new method for data capture of adolescent-parent interaction at home using wearable cameras and; (2) develop a new relevant and comprehensive observational micro-coding scheme. In Part 1, we report our completed preliminary work, comprised of an initial scoping review, and public engagement work. In Part 2, we present a protocol for the development of the new measure.
Methods: Part 1—We searched Pubmed for existing observational measures of adolescent-parent interaction for the scoping review. We also undertook public engagement work utilising a mobile research van, taken to multiple locations around Bristol, UK to engage with a variety of populations through interactive methods. Part 2—Our protocol describes plans for: (1) A systematic review of the psychometric properties of observational measures of adolescent-parent interaction; (2) Focussed public engagement workshops; (3) Harmonisation of information from existing coding schemes and literature with information from public engagement with adolescents and parents; (4) A pilot study to assess the acceptability and feasibility of the method; (5) Development of a coding scheme in consultation with expert and lay panels, and through real-life application to recorded videos from a pilot sample.
Results: Scoping review: we identified 21 adolescent-parent observational schemes, of which eight used micro-coding and 13 used globalcoding schemes. The majority of micro-coding schemes were not developed specifically for adolescents. Most studies used conflict or problem-solving tasks, which may not adequately capture positive adolescent-parent interactions. The mobile van event received views from 234 young people and/or parents. Families were positive about taking part in research using headcams. “Trust” and “understanding” were most frequently reported as important adolescent-parent relationship constructs.
Conclusions: This work represents the first attempt to truly co-design a method to assess parenting in adolescence. We hope to develop an observational measure using novel technological methods that can be used across a range of research and therapeutic settings.
Introduction
Adolescence constitutes a crucial developmental period in which most mental health disorders typically arise (1). It also represents a period of relational transition with growing sense of identity and autonomy or independence from parents (2–4), which is accompanied by increases in adolescent-parent conflict (5) and decreases in closeness (6). Mental health disorders are rising in young people at an alarming rate (7) and child and adolescent mental health services are unable to meet the level of demand for treatment (8). Family interventions are recommended in the treatment of adolescent mental health disorders including conduct disorder, eating disorders and depression (9–11). However, understanding of adolescent-parent interaction is limited, particularly regarding micro processes underlying relationships (12). Such processes are key to relational and behavioural interventions and may be best understood from observing and analysing behavioural interactions.
There is a large body of high-quality observational evidence from studies showing that specific parenting constructs in early childhood promote later cognitive and socio-emotional development, and these findings have been successfully translated into behavioural interventions which improve relationships (13). However, such understanding is lacking in adolescence. In addition, in child, adolescent and adult romantic relationship interaction research there is a greater focus on negative and problematic interaction patterns (14, 15). Thus, there is a need for observational measures of adolescent-parent interaction that can assess relational strengths as well as difficulties, with well-established ecological and content validity. The measures are needed to allow researchers to capture real life interactions and then to make meaningful observations from them to aid better understanding of relationships during adolescence. Secondly once established to provide tools to assess and evaluate relational change in clinical settings as well as to provide feedback to families using the tools directly in interventions. It is key to achieve this so that tools are accessible to families, researchers and professionals in educational, psychology or psychiatric settings. Such measures could have several applications: firstly, to inform the development of relational interventions for clinical populations with mental health problems; secondly, to understand family processes in the general population; and thirdly, to better understand family relationships and how that links to mental health and other outcomes through use in large international prospective cohort studies. The present paper aims to set out a program of research to develop such a measure and is structured in two parts. In Part 1, we report preliminary work we have already conducted, comprised of an initial scoping review and public engagement exercise. In Part 2, we report a protocol for the next steps we will undertake to develop the measure. However, to begin with, we present an overview of the field, highlighting some of the key methodological limitations of prior research.
Overview of observational measures of adolescent-parent interaction
Firstly, we review what observational work has been conducted with parents and adolescents to date. By “observational” we mean methods where behaviour between parents and adolescents is observed and recorded to allow objective behavioural analysis. One aspect of observational analysis concerns how the interaction is elicited and captured (i.e., where and how it is recorded and what the family is doing) and the second aspect concerns how the behaviour and relationship is then assessed or “coded” from the observation. We explore both improvements to methods of data capture and the coding of interactions here.
We begin by scoping existing measures. Researchers have utilised observational measures of adolescent-parent interaction since at least the 1980s (16), yet to our knowledge there has never been a systematic review focused on such measures. Twenty years ago, Aspland and Gardner (17) provided a helpful narrative review of observer-rated measures of parent-child interactions, highlighting issues including differences in task (structured vs. unstructured), setting (laboratory vs. home) as well as discussing issues of validity and reliability. However, this review did not aim to identify all existing schemes, and only a few measures were discussed for purposes of illustration. Locke and Prinz (18) reviewed all measures they identified as assessing discipline and nurturance developed in the preceding two decades. Only three observational measures were identified for the adolescent period, and six identified for early adolescence. The authors highlight the need for validation studies in different cultural groups, as well as the importance of research into the ethnicity of coders as a variable which may influence coding. Jewell et al. (19) conducted a systematic review of attachment measures in middle childhood and adolescence, but only identified one scheme assessing observed adolescent-parent interaction, the goal-corrected partnership adolescent coding system (GPACS) (20), with all other observer-rated schemes assessing the child on their own, typically using attachment interviews.
Marshall et al. (21) recently conducted a systematic review of measures of adolescent-parent conflict processes and their measurement, identifying 568 measures from 467 articles, of which 54 measures utilised observer ratings. The approach to inclusion was generous, with many included studies providing scant details of the observer-rated task or measure [Marshall (2023) personal communication]. Moreover, the aim of this review was to analyse the ways in which adolescent-parent conflict has been conceptualised and measured (e.g., frequency of disagreement, duration of conflict), and the psychometric properties of measures were not assessed. Finally, there have been two systematic reviews conducted on the psychometric properties of observer-rated parent-child interaction measures for children: Cañas, Ibabe & De Paúl (22) assessed measures for 0–12-year-olds at risk of child abuse and neglect, whilst Gridley et al. (23) investigated measures for 0–5-olds used in randomized controlled trials. Both reviews highlighted a lack of strong validity evidence for such measures.
In summary, it is known that observational-measures of adolescent-parent interaction have been utilised frequently in research, yet the literature has not been reviewed, and thus the full scope of the field, as well as the psychometric properties of measures, are currently unknown. Furthermore, the extent to which previous observed measures have conducted in depth micro-analysis (where all behaviours and sequences between behaviours are explored) remains unclear because few previous studies include micro-coding systems across all behavioural domains.More in depth micro-coding is important given the potential for such micro codes to guide the development of more specific and person-centred interventions.
Limitations of existing observational measures
Whilst there are advantages to observational measures, there are challenges too, with perhaps the most widely acknowledged difficulty being the length of time needed to capture and then code recorded or live interactions. Less commonly discussed are several important threats to the validity of such measures both in terms of data capture and coding of content: firstly, a lack of ecological validity due to lab-based observation; secondly, insufficient content validity evidence due to non-involvement of parents and adolescents in the development of measures; and thirdly, a lack of evidence of content validity and measurement invariance across different cultures and changing times.
Ecological validity and methods of observation (data capture)
Home or Lab settings
In terms of ecological validity, existing observational measures of adolescent-parent interaction typically involve visits to a laboratory to take part in a structured task, such as a conflict discussion [e.g., Allen et al. (24)]. Evidence suggests that behaviour under “laboratory conditions” does not reflect behaviour at home (25, 26).
Lab assessments and researcher-administered home assessments may lead to reactivity effects, whereby people change their behaviour due to the presence of the researcher or knowledge that they are being recorded. There is limited evidence examining this: Gardner (25) reviewed the literature based on audio-recorded assessments, which suggested no evidence of reactivity effects, but this did not compare researcher-present to researcher-absent conditions. When assessments from lab and home observations are compared, lower levels of negative behaviours are shown in the lab (15) which may reflect reactivity effects in the lab setting.
In addition, even though home settings improve ecological validity as compared to lab settings, in most situations a researcher is still present and has visited at a scheduled time. We have previously provided evidence for improved ecological validity of observational assessment conducted using wearable cameras with families of infants. Lee et al. (26) found that when families with infants self-recorded interactions with wearable cameras alone, as compared to the same dyads interacting at home but recorded by a researcher, more “negative” and socially undesirable maternal behaviours were observed. Footage recorded from wearable cameras also has a major advantage in that it captures a “first-person” perspective on an interaction, in contrast to the “third-person” perspective recorded from a static camera positioned to film an interaction from a side-view. Firstly, the first-person perspective allows for superior capture of faces and eye gaze; secondly, there is the potential for the development of therapeutic uses for such footage, such as video feedback techniques in which a therapist reviews footage with a parent or adolescent, allowing them to see themselves “through the eyes” of the other person. Figure 1 provides a photograph of the wearable cameras.
Tasks to elicit interactions
Another key to ecological validity is not only where the interaction is recorded but how the interaction is evoked. Very early work observing parent-child interaction was conducted at home involving multiple visits from researchers who observed and coded naturally occurring family behaviour “live” (27). The time-consuming nature of this led to the use of “structured” tasks designed to elicit behaviours of interest. Dishion and Stormshak (14) conceptualise these tasks as assessing either relationship quality or behavioural management. Relationship quality tasks in infancy and childhood are free-play or separation and reunion tasks. From childhood to adolescence, planning discussion tasks (such as planning a fun family activity) are typically used to assess relationship quality. In childhood, “clean-up” tasks are used to assess behavioural management and in later childhood and adolescence problem solving or conflict discussion tasks are used. The vast majority of adolescent studies use problem solving or conflict discussion, and these tasks have been shown to elicit relevant negative interaction behaviours when they use real rather than hypothetical scenarios (14). There is conflicting evidence on whether these behaviours elicited in the lab generalise to behaviours observed in unstructured settings at home. Behaviours observed in home settings are more strongly linked to ratings of psychopathology than behaviours observed in a lab, with the strongest associations found for unstructured tasks in the home (25). Behavioural coding across structured and unstructured tasks within the same dyads also show only modest agreement.
Although the traditional view of adolescence as a time of high conflict with parents is not supported by evidence, there is an increase in more minor conflicts in early and middle adolescence (5, 6), meaning that conflict discussions are a relevant context for assessing adolescent-parent interaction. These discussions allow observation of negative conflict processes such as criticism, hostility and coercion, and also more positive behaviours such as validation, support, and listening which may reflect adaptive conflict resolution strategies (28). However, conflict discussions are less likely to evoke other positive behaviours such as affection or shared enjoyment (14, 15). In addition, given that engaging in a conflict discussion generates negative emotion and increased physiological arousal (29–31) this context may not be best suited to assess positive adolescent-parent interaction quality. Tasks which may evoke more positive interaction qualities are needed. Such tasks may also be more acceptable and ethical in the context of parents and adolescents completing and recording tasks at home without the presence of a researcher.
Threats to the validity of existing measures that code or assess observations
Content validity refers to the extent that the content of an instrument adequately reflects the construct to be measured (32). For adolescent-parent interaction measures, the perspectives of parents and adolescents on content validity are strikingly absent in the development of measures. Instead, measures appear to have typically been developed in a “top-down” fashion by researchers based on theoretical constructs such as attachment [e.g., Obsuth et al. (20)] or coercive family cycles (33, 34). The lack of involvement of parents and adolescents hampers not only the content validity of measures, but also represents a missed opportunity to gain valuable suggestions on the acceptability and feasibility of tasks. Involving parents and adolescents through co-design of a measure through participatory methods would help to strengthen the content validity of a measure, in terms of both the task and the scope of the construct/s to be measured. Such work can be combined in iterative fashion during the substantive phase of validation (35) and feed into other key early tasks such as mapping concepts and reviewing literature.
Moreover, insufficient attention has been paid to the role of culture in developing measures, both across and within countries. For instance, content validity cannot be assumed for minoritized groups within a culture, such as ethnic minorities. Adolescent-parent relationships will be strongly influenced by cultural context, thereby complicating attempts to define norms for concepts such as communication. This points to the need for careful validation work, which can include consultation with stakeholders, qualitative interviews, as well as formal studies of measurement invariance, when seeking to validate a measure for a specific population. In addition, the culture of researchers may influence their coding: for instance, Yasui and Dishion (36) found inter-rater reliability to vary as a function of the ethnicity of coders, as well as of the concordance between the ethnicity of the coders and the families being rated.
Finally, we must consider the neglected concept of time in relation to content validity. Many measures of adolescent-parent interaction were developed several decades ago. Notwithstanding the absence of parent/adolescent involvement in their development, we can ask: do such measures retain their validity across time, or does validity evidence require continual renewal? Before answering this question, it is worth going back to first principles and considering the meaning of validity, which refers to the degree to which evidence and theory support the interpretations of test scores for proposed uses of tests (37). Thus, validity is not a property of a measure; rather, statements about validity should be made with regard to specific uses of a measure (37). Researchers are likely to be familiar with the notions of cross-cultural validity and measurement invariance—put simply, the need to evaluate the performance of a measure in a new context, such as a translated measure used in a new country. Returning to the question of validity across time, we suggest that adolescent-parent relationships have changed so much in Western societies that they represent a new cultural context when compared to families of several decades ago. Consequently, whilst the validity evidence for all measures has the potential to degrade over time, we suggest that measures aiming to assess interactions between family members are particularly vulnerable to this due to the significant changes that have impacted adolescent-parent interaction in developed countries. Such changes include: a trend towards older age for completing transitions out of adolescence such as family formation or ending education (38); the arrival of new technology and social media, giving rise to “digital parenting” (39); and broader societal changes in parent-child relationships such as increased communication and quality-time, reduced authoritarian control and corporal punishment, and decreased parental self-efficacy (40). In summary, construct validation is an ongoing process (41) which requires evidence to justify specific uses (37); viewed in this light, existing observational measures of adolescent-parent interaction are far-from convincing in terms of content and construct validity.
Micro and global approaches to coding of observational content
Most observational coding schemes of parent-child interaction assess “global” constructs. Global ratings require the observer to form overall impressions from multiple different units of behaviour and consider frequency, intensity and duration within an entire interaction to make a numerical rating on a single scale. For example, in the global rating of maternal sensitivity, the most well-known construct in infancy, a researcher makes a numerical rating based on their global impression of how well the mother identifies, interprets and appropriately responds to her child's cues (42). This requires specific and time-consuming training to be able to assess reliably with baselines which were often developed several decades ago. Micro-coding, in contrast, focuses on individual units of behaviour and allows examination of moment-to-moment temporal sequences of behaviour (43, 44). This approach considers more subtle interactional processes and allows the identification of specific behaviours which may precede desirable or less desirable “target behaviours”, thus providing unique (and flexible) information which may be readily translated into intervention to promote positive parent-child interactions (44). Micro-coding allows tracking of frequency, sequence and duration of interactive behaviour (see Figure 2). Some micro-coding approaches focus on individual units of behaviour such as facial expression [e.g., the Facial Action Coding System; FACS; (45)]. Individual units of behaviour permit automated coding, which considerably reduce the time and costs associated with observational data. Most micro-coding approaches outside of infancy reflect behavioural groupings which integrate across multiple different behaviours (e.g., facial expression, bodily movements, speech) and require consideration of the context. In the SPAFF (Specific Affect Coding System), Coan and Gottman (46) refer to these codes as “gestalt behaviour codes” and conceptualise them as unobservable latent variables which are directly observed via specific behavioural indicators. For example, “validation” is observed with the indicators of agreement/apology, summarising, and head nodding with eye contact.
Figure 2. Example of micro coded data on the behavioural domain of facial expressions. The number and duration of each behaviour is visualised by the colour blocks in Observer XT.
Pearson and colleagues (47) developed a comprehensive micro-coding scheme for parent-infant interaction including both discrete and gestalt behaviours from a review of all existing coding schemes and have shown that reliability can be easily achieved (47, 48). The scheme is applied using the Observer XT [version 16 (49)] software which permits coding of observational data on a timeline. Figure 2 shows an example of micro coded data on the behavioural domain of facial expressions. The number and duration of each behaviour is visualised by the colour blocks using Observer XT software.
Limitations of existing micro-coding and considerations for adolescence
Micro-coding also has limitations and particular considerations regarding its suitability for adolescent behaviour. Most notably, in micro-coding in infancy the context of each specific behaviour is not usually captured. For example, consider a negative or sad face by a mother. The context of this facial expression could lead to very different meanings and thus targets for interactions. For example, it may reflect a negative emotional state in the mother and lack of warmth or may reflect emotional mirroring by the mother (which is likely to be highly sensitive). This can be lost in micro-coding even if combinations or sequences are used.
In infancy micro-coding schemes, one facial expression category that is highly predictive is a “woe” face. As first described by Beatrice Beebe (50), this is an exaggerated sad face that is specifically linked to compassion/sympathy. It does not necessarily mirror a sad face in the child but instead marks an understanding by the caregiver which signals sympathy rather than negativity. Given adolescents’ vastly more sophisticated social and cognitive development relative to infants, the ability of micro-codes to capture contextual subtleties such as humour or sarcasm may be a particular challenge for coding schemes of adolescent-parent interaction.
Summary and aims
There is a need for methods to collect observational data which are acceptable, feasible and ecologically valid, and coding schemes which adequately characterise modern adolescent-parent relationships, with strong content and construct validity. Such methods can advance our understanding of parenting in adolescence and have the potential to inform the development of new interventions, based on observed interactions. This could include therapies that directly utilise observational material within the course of treatment, such as video feedback, or the adaptation of existing approaches, such as parenting groups or family therapy, utilising evidence gleaned through observational research. New methods are required due to the limitations of available measures, including substantial threats to content and construct validity.
The programme of work described here aims to develop a new observational measure of adolescent-parent interaction by: (1) conducting a scoping review of the literature to identify existing adolescent-parent observational coding schemes and extracting all interaction codes; (2) conducting public engagement work with relevant stakeholders (adolescents, parents and professionals) to identify currently relevant constructs; (3) harmonising the “top-down” information from the existing literature with the “bottom-up” information from relevant stakeholders to generate a micro-coding scheme for adolescent-parent interactions and suitable scenarios or tasks to record behaviour; (4) carrying out a feasibility study of the wearable camera methodology and initial validation of the coding-scheme in a pilot study of adolescents and parents. We believe this is the first attempt to generate a tool to measure adolescent-parent interaction which combines both the traditional “top down” approaches taken in research and clinical practice with “bottom up” information from adolescents and parents.
Overall project aims
1) Identify existing concepts important in adolescent-parent interactions from both Public Patient Involvement (PPI) and existing literature.
2) Develop an observational system to capture adolescent-parent interaction behaviour based on the existing literature and co-creation methods.
3) Explore feasibility and acceptability of wearable cams with adolescents and their parents at home.
4) Develop a protocol (including tasks) which (a) can be conducted at home (b) allows capture of relevant footage and (c) evokes / captures relevant and authentic domains of the relationship.
Initial completed work presented in this paper:
1) Scoping review of existing adolescent-parent observational measures (links to aim 1 and 2)
2) Scoping public engagement with adolescents and parents of methods and concepts (links to aim 2)
Next steps to be described as protocols in this paper:
1) Systematic review of measurement properties of existing coding schemes (links to aims 1 and 2).
2) Construct definition through focused public engagement workshops (links to aims 1, 2 and 4).
3) Harmonisation of information from existing coding schemes with information from public engagement with adolescents and parents (links to aims 1, 2 and 4).
4) Assess acceptability and feasibility of data capture by wearable camera technology (links to aims 3 and 4).
5) Development of a coding scheme in consultation with an expert panel and lay/PPI panel and through real-life application to recorded videos from a pilot sample (links to all aims).
Table 1 links the aims and objectives of the project to each piece of work outlined below, and summarises the output and current status of each.
Part 1—Completed ground work
Methods
Scoping review to identify existing coding schemes, relevant behaviours, and indices of relationships
To identify papers with observed adolescent-parent interaction data we conducted a search in Pubmed using the terms “parent adolescent observed interaction coding behaviour” (and all related synonyms, see Supplementary file S1 for full search terms). Abstracts were screened to identify papers which included observation of parent-child interaction with samples with a mean child age of between 10 and 19 years. The methods sections of relevant papers were then checked to identify coding schemes to be included. Coding schemes were not included if they focussed on a very specific scenario (e.g., discussions about cigarette smoking; parent-child interaction within a therapy session) or included codes only on whole-family interaction.
Public engagement/co-creation
Scoping public engagement: we conducted a PPI mobile research van event in multiple locations around Bristol, UK, in August 2022 (51) with the aim of engaging with a variety of populations including those typically considered “hard to reach”. Researchers spoke to young people and their parents to find out their views on what they considered to be the characteristics of a “good” adolescent-parent relationship and key concepts were recorded using whiteboards. Parents and young people were invited to try out the wearable cameras and give their views on taking part in research using them. They were also asked what tasks or everyday scenarios would be acceptable and would best capture their relationship interactions at home. Initial conversations were used to create a voting system where subsequent families voted on their chosen task.
Results
Scoping review of coding schemes
The search terms identified 128 papers. Of those, 44 papers included adolescent-parent interaction observational coding, reflecting 9 micro- and 13 global- coding schemes. An additional 4 micro- and 2 global- schemes were identified from the reference lists of the Marshall et al. (21), Locke & Prinze (18) and Jewell et al. (19) reviews and from reviewing reference lists of relevant papers. One global scheme was added from a preliminary search ran for the systematic review of measures (described below). Given our focus on micro-coding and space constraints, we will provide a more detailed account of micro-coding schemes in this paper. These initial summaries for this paper focus on the characteristics of the schemes rather than a description or synthesis of the specific concepts assessed in the codes.
Summary of micro-coding schemes
Table 2 displays the identified micro-coding schemes. Columns summarise characteristics including: whether they were developed for adolescents; their history; what tasks they were developed for; and the type of codes contained. Micro-coding can be “event” or “timing” based. In event-based coding, once a behaviour is coded, for example when a parent smiles, they will be coded as smiling until that changes; that is, codes are updated according to new “events”. In time-based coding, the behaviour is assessed within specific time windows. For example, in a time-based coding scheme using two second intervals, at two seconds a parent could be coded as smiling; at four seconds, if the parent is still smiling, then they will again be coded as such. We use the term “behaviour groups” to describe codes that involve combinations of individual units of behaviour. For example, “fear” in the Specific Affect Coding System [SPAFF (46)] is a combination of a range of different verbal characteristics (e.g., shifts in frequency in speech, speech disturbances), body movements (e.g., gestures or fidgeting) and facial movements (e.g., gulping and biting lip).
Scheme history: Most of the identified schemes were either directly developed within or were influenced by work in the Oregon Social Learning Centre (OSLC), using social learning theory (SLT) principles, particularly Patterson's cycle of coercion (34). As described in the table, the initial Family Interaction Coding System [FICS (27)], which has not been used with adolescents so not included in the review, has been developed and extended into several other schemes which were identified (Family Process Code (FPC), RACS, Peer Process Code (PPC) (52, 68, 72)). This work is largely based on clinical samples of children and families with behavioural problems, but also some studies with general population samples with further developments of the scales. Schemes identified in this review were also influenced by schemes developed using social learning theory principles to assess conflict resolution in adult marital relationships such as the Marital Interaction Coding System [MICS (61)] and the Couples Interaction Scoring System [CISS (59)]. The Living in Familial Environments (LIFE), Codebook for Marital and Family Interaction (COMFI), Parent-Adolescent Interaction Coding System-Revised (PAICS-R) (44, 63, 70) all identified as being developed from these marital relationship schemes. The PAICS-R also draws on family systems theory. The LIFE scheme included consideration of the SLT processes involved in maintaining depression (reciprocity). Another scheme developed for children, INTERACT (57), draws on both coercion and reciprocity models from social learning theory. Another school of schemes which focus more narrowly on affect were developed from Ekman's work on coding facial expression [SPAFF & Simple Affect Coding System (SACS)] (58, 62). One identified scheme specifically focused on conversational discourse in relation to language learning (Contingency Coding System; CCS) (72). Finally, one scheme drew on psychoanalytic theory on adolescent ego development (Constraining & Enabling Coding System; CECS) (16).
Code types: Most schemes were designed to use event-based coding; the INTERACT and SPAFF are timing based; and the Relationship Process Code [RPC (55)] was described as suitable for both event and timing-based coding. All the identified schemes described “behaviour group” codes, and none focussed on coding individual units of behaviour (such as just facial expression or visual attention). Schemes from the OSLC tend to separate verbal from non-verbal in their behavioural codes, whereas other schemes combine the two modalities (e.g., the SPAFF) and some focus only on speech (PAICS-R, CCS). For example, “validation” is assessed as affect (non-verbal behaviour) in the RACS, but from speech and non-verbal combined in the SPAFF, or just speech in the CCS. Most codes reflect one person's behaviour to another (e.g., command/direct, criticism, positive physical contact), although nearly all schemes contain concepts such as “validation” which are inherently dependant on the prior behaviour of the other. Only one scheme specifically conceptualises a dyadic behaviour [discourse change; (CECS)].
Developed for adolescents: Only three schemes were described as being developed for adolescents (the RACS, PAICS-R, CECS). The CECS is described as drawing on adolescent-specific theory, the RACS is a combination of the RPC developed for children and the SPAFF developed for adults. The PAICS-R was developed from an adult scheme but describes focusing on behaviours relevant to adolescent-parent conflict. Other schemes were either developed for children and applied to adolescents, or developed to apply to all family relationships (e.g., parent-parent, parent-child, parent-adolescent).
Tasks: Most schemes were developed for problem-solving tasks, specifically conflict resolution tasks where the parent and child identify three areas of disagreement and are asked to discuss them and attempt to reach a resolution. One scheme was developed for a similar “revealed differences” task where parent and adolescent completed a moral judgement task and then discussed the answers where they differed (CECS). The OSLC schemes are described as being suitable for a variety of structured and unstructured settings (e.g., mealtimes). This includes the structured task of planning an activity. The RACS has been used with the Family Assessment Task (69) battery of discussion tasks which includes tasks designed to elicit positive discussion on: encouragement of personal growth (parent and child pick a topic of growth, such as academic achievement, to discuss) and positive recognition of family member (parent and child have a discussion about positive features of the other). No tasks were specifically designed to elicit shared enjoyment.
Summary of global coding schemes
Table 3 displays the identified global-coding schemes, with columns summarising whether they were developed for adolescents, their history, what tasks they were developed for, and the type of codes contained. A briefer and less detailed narrative summary of the characteristics of the global schemes is provided below.
Scheme history: schemes drew on a variety of theoretical backgrounds, including social learning theory, psychoanalytic work on ego development and autonomy promotion, attachment theory, family systems theory, structural family systems theory, Baumrind's (100) parenting typologies and Olson's (101) circumplex theory. Some drew on research findings on conflict resolution as well as on parenting of children with anxiety.
Code types: as all schemes are global they combine multiple different units of behaviour (e.g., speech, facial expression, body language), although some schemes rely more on speech (e.g., The autonomy and relatedness coding system (ARCS); (24)) than others which take a more balanced view of all behaviours (e.g., System for coding interactions and family functioning (SCIFF); (111)). There is some variation across schemes in how much the codes reflect an overall impression or “feeling” regarding the interaction, with some comprising specific rules about which point on the scale is assigned [e.g., ARCS; (24)] and others purely relying on global impression (e.g., Coder impressions questionnaire) (93). Some schemes describe all codes as dyadic [e.g., the Adapted Mealtime Family Interaction Coding System; MICS (91)], but most include a mix of ratings about the parent and the adolescent separately, with a smaller number of specific dyadic codes.
Developed for adolescents: Seven schemes were specifically developed for adolescents, or adolescents and their families. These often also drew on adult schemes. The remainder were developed for children and applied to adolescents or developed for both children and adolescents.
Tasks: all schemes were developed for problem solving tasks, mainly conflict discussion but also revealed differences and puzzle tasks. Many schemes were also used with planning tasks, typically planning a family activity. A broader range of discussion tasks which were designed specifically to evoke positive interactions (such as planning “the best day ever”, celebrating a family member) were also used with four of the schemes (the Coder Impressions Questionnaire [COIMP, (93)], the Coding Interactive Behavior [CIB, (95)], the Family Interaction Macro-coding System [FIMS, (102)], the Iowa Family Interaction Rating Scales [IFIRS, (115)].
Overall summary
Less micro- compared to global coding schemes were identified, and many of the identified micro-schemes were further developments or extensions of existing schemes. Very few of the existing micro-coding schemes were developed specifically for adolescents and so there is less influence of adolescent specific theory on their development. The majority of micro-schemes were mostly based on social learning theory principles and were largely designed for and employed with problem solving or conflict discussion tasks. Global coding schemes were more commonly developed specifically for adolescents and drew on a wider range of theory. Whilst conflict and problem-solving tasks still predominate, these schemes have more often been employed with tasks designed to evoke positive interaction. This underscores the importance of drawing on both global and micro-schemes in the development of a new comprehensive and relevant adolescent coding scheme.
Scoping public engagement
The mobile event visited 5 unique locations in the South-West of the UK, in busy family areas during the school holidays, including a beach front, local parks in residential areas out of the city, central areas directly in the city centre (close to fairgrounds and museums) and rural “car boot” sales. The areas varied in deprivation indices and the ethnicity of the populations. The mobile research event received active responses (in the form of a written response on a white board and voting for proposed measures using tokens and slot boxes) from 234 young people and/or parents. Where votes were collected, we recorded whether the vote was from an adolescent, a young adult (18–25) or a parent.
Acceptability of wearable head cameras: We asked families about the use of wearable head cameras as well as several other wearable technologies including rings, watches, actigraphy and on-body cameras (raw data in Supplementary file S2). Families had mixed views about taking part in research using wearable cameras: some were initially sceptical, but once it was explained that the proposal would be to use them for short-term interactions, not all-day surveillance, the method was deemed acceptable. Young people especially favoured seeing the footage capture from their own camera which represented the world through their eyes.
We also heard that the advent of body worn cameras used by police and in security settings has led to a sense of being watched in young people. At the heart of people's concerns was not being recorded per se, but the imbalance; in the police/security context, cameras are only worn by those in authority, and these cameras capture one perspective only. By contrast, in our proposed measure, both perspectives are represented. Young people were positive about collecting footage that showed the events through their eyes, not just footage watching them from another person's perspective. As shown in Figure 3, when asked about the acceptability of different wearable technologies in research, 25% of adolescents were happy to wear a wearable camera. Given that the other wearable measures were far less intrusive and mainstream (watches and rings), this demonstrates a reasonable engagement with wearable cameras. For purposes of illustration, Figure 1 shows two adolescents playing a card game wearing a wearable camera.
Figure 3. Summary of adolescent, young adult and parent views on acceptability of wearable technology.
Task to adequately capture interactions at home: As shown in Figure 4 (raw data in Supplementary file S2), the most voted for task to adequately capture interactions at home was a discussion task to plan the day or week, with 40% of adolescents choosing this task, followed by 26% choosing meal-time, 18% playing a card or board game, 8% completing chores together and 8% reviewing their day. Parents voted for mealtimes, followed by planning their day or week and then playing a game. Many families described that playing a board or card game was not something that they would typically do, but that they would be happy to do it. We brought an example game called “Sussed” (130), a conversation-starter card game specifically developed for modern family interactions (further described below in the pilot section). Many parents and adolescents enjoyed trying out the task.
Figure 4. Summary of adolescent and parent report views on task to capture adolescent-parent interaction at home.
Constructs: Figure 5 presents the terms provided by parents and young people in a word cloud (raw data is presented in Supplementary file S3). “Understanding” and “trust” were described most often, followed by love, caring, communication and respect.
Part 2—Protocol for further work
Below we set out the next steps for the development of observational capture and coding of adolescent-parent interactions.
Systematic review of measurement properties of coding schemes
A systematic review of the measurement properties of existing observational measures of adolescent-parent measures is required, to better understand the strengths and weaknesses of existing measures, as well as to identify gaps. We have registered the review with PROSPERO (CRD42023397423) and our protocol is publicly available at [https://www.crd.york.ac.uk/prospero/display_record.php?ID = CRD42023397423]. In brief, we will conduct a new search, informed by the work presented earlier. We will search MEDLINE, EMBASE, APA PsycINFO, Web of Science (Core Collection), ProQuest Dissertations and Theses Global, Scopus and CINAHL, limited to English language, without date limits. In addition, we will search for unpublished studies on Google Scholar and PsyArXiv. The reference lists of eligible studies will be manually screened to identify other relevant studies.
We will search for studies investigating psychometric properties of observational measures of adolescent-parent interaction. We define such measures as any instrument in which a coder evaluates any aspect of adolescent-parent interaction on the basis of observed behaviour, utilising some form of a coding system, which can be micro or global. We will include: any empirical study reporting psychometric properties of an observer-rated measure of adolescent-parent interaction; the mean age of children in the sample must be between 10 and 19; by parent, we refer to biological and adoptive parents, as well as carers whose role is that of a primary caregiver to the child (e.g., foster carers); studies must be in the English language; we will include both published and unpublished studies that are available as completed full-text reports, including dissertations, book chapters or pre-print papers. We will exclude studies of interactions with grandparents or any non-primary caregiving role, as well as commentaries, letters, conference abstracts and review.
The aim of the review is to identify the best measures of adolescent-parent interactions based on their psychometric properties, using the COnsensus-based Standards for the selection of health Measurement INstruments (COSMIN) (131). We will utilise the COSMIN tools for appraising study quality, adequacy of measurement properties and overall strength of evidence, which are freely available at www.cosmin.nl. We will assess content validity, structural validity, internal consistency, cross-cultural validity, reliability, measurement error, criterion validity, and responsiveness. By identifying the existing measures with the best evidence of adequate psychometric properties, we will be able to give greater weight to such measures in terms of informing our own task and coding scheme.
Construct definition and measure development through focused public engagement workshops
We will conduct a series of workshops with adolescents, parents and health professionals, including an interactive live performance with creative arts organisation Made by Mortals (https://www.madebymortals.org/) to operationalise and “bring to life” the constructs identified in the scoping public engagement. The adolescent workshop will include some adolescents with lived experience of mental health problems and accessing mental health services as well as parents and professionals.
In contrast to traditional approaches to consultation through focus groups, we believe that the use of an arts-based approach will provide a more engaging and less abstract way to elicit views. A key strength is that the creative arts materials presented will be based on the story of a family who will represent numerous lived experiences, without disclosing any one story personally. Attendees at these workshops will be invited to share ideas relating to these stories, which connect to themes arising in our earlier public engagement work.
The workshops will include the use of pre-recorded audio and video clips, as well as use of drama performance and opportunities to experience the task and wearable camera technology. We will conduct workshops at various stages in the development of the interaction measure, with the following aims:
1. To inform the development of the coding system, particularly through exploring the extent to which theoretical constructs derived from the literature align with the “experience-near” perspectives of adolescents and parents.
2. To inform the choice of task for the measure.
Harmonisation of information from existing coding schemes with information from public engagement with adolescents and parents
This will follow a stepped process:
1) Extract all codes from the identified observational parent-interaction coding schemes, including both higher-order (e.g., autonomy promotion) and lower-order (e.g., positive affect) concepts. Codes from global coding schemes will be assessed for suitability of use as micro-codes or whether they require separation into multiple concepts.
2) Research team will perform initial clustering of common and related codes and concepts to identify a taxonomy of constructs. This will involve additional broad literature searching (including fields outside psychology such as philosophy and economics) on key constructs to support operationalisation.
3) Consultation with an “expert panel” of clinicians, academics and a PPI panel of adolescents and parents to appraise the content validity of the clusters and resolve any queries or lack of consensus on concepts from the previous step.
Initial pilot of the wearable camera methodology to provide proof of concept and initial validation study
Methods of data capture through use of wearable cameras
We propose that recent techniques for measuring parent-infant interaction from a first-person perspective using wearable cameras within the family home setting (47, 26) could be adapted to investigate adolescent-parent interaction. The advantages of such an approach are:
• Increased ecological validity if used in the home setting, without the need for a researcher to be present during the interaction.
• Content Validity
∘ The face-to-face view of the wearable cams captures full face view of both interactors better than mounted cameras in one position only (see Supplementary Figure S1). This allows more subtle detections of facial expressions and vocalisations.
∘ The unique perspective allowing researchers to capture interaction “through the eyes” of the young person and parent (26). Pearson and colleagues have previously established that mother-infant interaction can be reliably observed at home using this methodology (26, 132).
∘ Potential use of face-reader technology to automate some aspects of coding, thereby dramatically reducing coding time.
• Therapeutic potential
∘ This approach has significant potential for future development into video-feedback intervention.
∘ For example, the approach is currently being used to feedback to new mums in South Africa (see Cantrell et al. in this edition). Of note a goal of parents is often whether their child looks at them/knows them, the side-by-side view of mum’s perspective and child perspective allows more explicit observation of key moments that can be “re-lived”, thus reinforcement of key connecting behaviours.
Pilot study aims: (1) Assess feasibility and acceptability of the wearable camera methodology in a general population sample (2) Collect footage to apply micro-coding scheme to.
Sample
The Wirral Child Health & Development study (WCHADS) is a prospective birth cohort of mothers, fathers and their first-born children based in the Wirral, UK [see Sharp et al. (133) and https://www.liverpool.ac.uk/population-health/research/groups/first-steps/ for further details]. The study has completed 13 waves of data collection since birth, including observations of parenting in the lab or at home with a researcher present at ages 6 months, 14 months, 2.5 years 3.5 years, 7 and 9 years. At age 11–12 (N = 743) and age 12–13 (N = 724) families completed postal questionnaires only. The sample are currently aged 13–15 years.
Procedure
The whole sample will be approached via email to invite a volunteer sample of 50 families to participate in the pilot. The first 50 families who express interest will be recruited. Families will be visited by a researcher to drop off the cameras and explain the procedures, and an appointment arranged for the researcher to return to collect the cameras. Primary caregivers and adolescents will complete two tasks in their own time at home, and complete questionnaires on the acceptability of the wearable cameras and the tasks and validated questionnaires assessing relationship quality.
Measures
Observations of adolescent-parent interaction.
The head-cameras
The head-cameras (shown in Figure 1) are small circular cameras attached to a mount on an elastic adjustable headband. The camera devices are off the shelf body cameras. The headband mounts were created by an experienced user centred design company (Kinneir Dufort, KD) to ensure suitable positioning of the camera. Footage is recorded on to a micro SD card.
The tasks
Parents and adolescents will be asked to record themselves completing two tasks at home. The choice of task for this initial pilot was informed by the scoping PPI work, review of the literature, and consideration of the potential issues with using conflict discussion tasks at home and to evoke positive interaction. In the first task, parents and adolescents are asked to play the commercially available card game “Sussed” which was developed by researchers and funded by the UKRI (130). “Sussed” is a conversation starter game designed to improve social health by players answering multiple choice questions about each other, allowing players to find out how others see them, talk about how others see them and understand why people think or feel a certain way. Example question: Who do I most like to work with? (A) People who are the same as me, (B) People who are different to me, (C) I prefer to work on my own. Parents and adolescents will be provided with 10 cards which were identified from the standard yellow Sussed set (suitable for age 6+) and the green emotional health Sussed set (suitable for age 8+) by A level and first year undergraduate students. Parents and adolescents will be asked to play the game and record for 10 min. In the second task, the parent and adolescent are asked to share a drink or a snack and discuss their plans for their week, and record for 5 min.
Questionnaires
Self-developed questionnaires to assess acceptability of the methodology: Questions ask about the acceptability of using the wearable cameras, the tasks they were asked to complete, and their thoughts on collecting footage from the first-person perspective of both mother and adolescents. Parent and adolescent report.
Adolescent-parent relationship quality: the Network of Relationships (NRI) “seeks safe haven” subscale (support seeking from parent, parent and adolescent report) (134), the brief version of the Parental Feelings Questionnaire (positive and negative feelings about child, parent report and adapted for adolescent) (135) and the trust subscale of the Inventory of Parent and Peer Attachment (IPPA, adolescent report) (136).
Planned output
Feasibility indices: this will include proportion of families invited to use the wearable cameras that agree to take part, of those who agree how many provide footage, we will also record reasons for not managing to obtain recorded footage, challenges in both the initial recruitment and the recording of interactions. For example, a similar protocol was developed for parents and infants in the ALSPAC study (Using a wearable camera at home—Children of the Children of the 90s—YouTube). Such information can be used to inform protocols for use in cohort studies, time needed for researcher time and likely uptake. Potential cohorts include ALSPAC-G2 (137).
Proof of concept: recorded footage will be used to develop the adolescent-parent coding scheme by testing application of identified “behaviour groupings”/parent-child interaction codes to the videos in order to refine definitions.
1. Initial reliability and validation study: inter-rater reliability of the newly identified coding scheme will be assessed. Self-reported relationship quality will be used to provide an initial exploration of construct validity of the observed adolescent-parent relationship constructs. Given the sample size of 40 power will be too low to provide strong statistical evidence for associations between self-report and observations, which are expected to be around effect sizes of 0.3 based on previous studies linking observed and self-report of parenting (25).
Co-design of a comprehensive and relevant coding scheme in consultation with an expert panel and lay/PPI panel
Agreed behavioural domains will be operationally defined and incorporated into an Observer XT coding structure. We will apply the coding structure to the WCHADS videos and consult with experts and young people as “coders” to determine face validity regarding whether the defined behaviours can be applied to live behaviour examples. The aim is that multiple, diverse coders can use the coding scheme to accurately capture all behaviours and dyadic behaviours observed in real life interactions, and that multiple coders give the same code for the same observation (inter-rater reliability). The coding scheme will be refined during this process and may include updated codes to capture behaviours that emerge and redefinition of behaviours that do not reach inter-rater-reliability. Once finalised we will publish an open access, co-developed, fully illustrated manual of behavioural codes and observer syntax in the same way that we have for micro-coding in the early years (47).
Conclusion
The initial work presented in this paper suggests that there is a need to co-design both new methods of data capture, and new coding schemes, to assess adolescent-parent interaction. In our scoping review, we identified that numerous observational measures have been developed over the years, but fewer coding schemes were developed specifically with adolescents in mind this was particularly the case in existing micro-coding schemes. None of the coding schemes were co-designed with adolescents. This is important since adolescence constitutes a unique period in which parent-child relationships are in transition in a variety of ways, such as becoming more symmetrical as adolescents develop more independence and autonomy (4), therefore requiring adolescent-specific theory to guide their development.
Our initial review of the evidence indicates that negative and conflictual parent-child interaction processes are well characterised, largely by the application of social learning theory principles such as Patterson's coercive cycle (34). In the programme of work described here we place emphasis on characterising the more positive aspects of adolescent-parent interaction. Positive adolescent-parent relationships are associated with greater adolescent well-being (38), and the most promising relational interventions are strengths based (138, 139). Consistent with a focus on problematic interactions, we note that a large number of data capture methods utilise problem-solving or conflict-related tasks. Whilst conflict tasks are used to code positive adolescent-parent interaction qualities, they are less likely to evoke positive interaction qualities (14, 15), and the negative affect and arousal which accompanies conflict (29–31) may mean they are not best suited to assess positive interactions. In addition, measures eliciting conflict may be less suitable for use in the home context, in the absence of researchers.
Planning activities were also quite commonly used, particularly in studies employing global scales. Planning discussions involves some problem solving and negotiation but are not conflict based. We also identified some structured discussion tasks designed to elicit positive interactions, including celebrating a family member and parent encouragement of an area of personal growth in the child or planning a fun family activity (69). In our public engagement work we asked families what the best contexts at home would be to observe their most natural adolescent-parent interaction at home. Planning their day or week was the most voted for task. Given the focus in infancy observation on tasks which elicit shared enjoyment, and our aim to observe positive adolescent-parent interaction, we also suggested a card game task which most families found acceptable. There has been very little consideration in the literature on the content and ecological validity of different observational tasks, particularly in relation to characterising positive interactions, in adolescence. This is essential for developing a method to adequately characterise adolescent-parent relationships.
In our public engagement work, we found that parents and adolescents were open to the possibility of utilising technology such as wearable cameras or heart-rate monitors and the possibility of automated technology coding behaviours. In terms of content validity, we have found that the most relevant concepts expressed in PPI work such as Trust and Understanding are not explicitly covered in existing coding or assessment tools. These higher-order concepts are likely represented by multiple individual codes contained in existing schemes. Careful conceptual and practical mapping of top down and bottom-up content will be required to ensure new coding schemes capture what is seen as most relevant.
Our work thus far has some limitations. Firstly, since the literature on observational adolescent-parent measures appears to use a wide range of terms, it is probable that our scoping review may have missed some existing measures. In addition, as most observational coding schemes are not published and can be difficult to locate, our review is likely not complete. Secondly, whilst our public engagement work will have reached individuals not usually approached by researchers and contained a diversity of class and ethnicity, it was exclusively conducted in the South-West of England. Future public engagement work should be conducted in other regions of the United Kingdom prior to developing the coding scheme or undertaking formal validation studies. Furthermore, we are currently evaluating the feasibility and acceptability of the measure in a community sample of adolescents, which will shed light on the potential use of the measure for research purposes in cohort studies. As discussed earlier in this paper, the use of the measure for different purposes and different populations requires additional evidence to support each specific use. In particular, the use of the measure in clinical samples, either for research or therapeutic purposes, will require separate evaluation in research studies. The suitability of the measure for individuals with learning disabilities or an autism spectrum condition also requires evaluation.
In summary, the work presented and planned here represents the first attempt to truly co-create a method to assess parenting in adolescence, combining bottom-up processes to harness insights from adolescents and parents, with top-down approaches to utilise and build on existing theory and research. By using creative approaches to public engagement, as well as rigorous plans for psychometric investigation, we hope to develop an observational measure that captures salient aspects of adolescent-parent interaction using novel technological methods, which can be used across a range of research and therapeutic settings.
Data availability statement
The original contributions presented in the study are included in the article/Supplementary Material, further inquiries can be directed to the corresponding author.
Ethics statement
An ethics checklist for the scoping public engagement van tour was submitted to the University of Bristol School of Psychologys IRB and met criteria for automatic approval (no reference number required). Ethical approval for the pilot study in the WCHADS was granted by the Cheshire North and West Research Ethics Committee on the 22 November 2022 (ref: 14/NW/1484_22_01). Written informed consent was obtained from the individual(s), and minor(s) legal guardian/next of kin, for the publication of any potentially identifiable images or data included in this article.
Author contributions
RP, NW and TJ designed all elements of the protocol. NW, DC and AB conducted and reported the scoping review. RP, TJ and NW conducted the PPI work. NW, TJ and RP wrote the manuscript. DC and AB reviewed and revised the manuscript. All authors contributed to the article and approved the submitted version.
Funding
This work is part of a project that has received funding from the European Research Council (ERC) under the European Union’s Horizon 2020 research and innovation programme (Grant agreement No. 758813; MHINT). TJ was supported by a NIHR Development and Skills Enhancement Award (NIHR302102).
Acknowledgments
We would like to acknowledge the Principal Investigators of the Wirral Child Health & Development Study (WCHADS) Professor Helen Sharp, Professor Jonathan Hill, and Professor Andrew Pickles, for granting permission to conduct the initial pilot study. We are very grateful to all the families who took part. The views expressed are those of the author(s) and not necessarily those of the NHS, the NIHR or the Department of Health and Social Care.
Conflict of interest
The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.
The authors NW and RP declared that they were editorial board members of Frontiers at the time of submission. This had no impact on the peer review process and the final decision.
Publisher's note
All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.
Supplementary material
The Supplementary Material for this article can be found online at: https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/frcha.2023.1214890/full#supplementary-material
References
1. Solmi M, Radua J, Olivola M, Croce E, Soardo L, Salazar de Pablo G, et al. Age at onset of mental disorders worldwide: large-scale meta-analysis of 192 epidemiological studies. Mol Psychiatry. (2022) 27:281–95. doi: 10.1038/s41380-021-01161-7
2. Allen JP, Hauser ST, Bell KL, O'Connor TG. Longitudinal assessment of autonomy and relatedness in adolescent-family interactions as predictors of adolescent ego development and self-esteem. Child Dev. (1994) 65:179–94. doi: 10.2307/1131374
3. McElhaney KB, Allen JP, Stephenson JC, Hare AL. Attachment and autonomy during adolescence. In: Lerner RM, Steinberg L, editors. Handbook of adolescent psychology: Individual bases of adolescent development. New Jersey, United States: John Wiley & Sons, Inc (2009). p. 358–403.
4. Steinberg L. We know some things: parent–adolescent relationships in retrospect and prospect. Journal of Research on Adolescence. (2001) 11:1–19. doi: 10.1111/1532-7795.00001
5. Meeus W. Constantly connected: the role of parental mediation styles and self-regulation in pre- and early adolescents’ problematic mobile device use. Hum Commun Res. (2019) 45:119–47. doi: 10.1093/hcr/hqy015
6. Meeus W. Adolescent psychosocial development: a review of longitudinal models and research. Dev Psychol. (2016) 52:1969–93. doi: 10.1037/dev0000243
7. Analytical Services, Population Health, Clinical Audit & Specialist Care Team. Mental health of children and young people in England 2022 - wave 3 follow up to the 2017 survey. UK: NHS Digital (2022) Available at: https://digital.nhs.uk/data-and-information/publications/statistical/mental-health-of-children-and-young-people-in-england/2022-follow-up-to-the-2017-survey/part-1—mental-health (Cited April 21, 2023).
8. Shim R, Szilagyi M, Perrin JM. Epidemic rates of child and adolescent mental health disorders require an urgent response. Pediatr. (2022) 149:e2022056611. doi: 10.1542/peds.2022-056611
9. National Institute for Health and Care Excellence (NICE). Antisocial behaviour and conduct disorders in children and young people: recognition and management: Clinical guideline [CG158]. (2017). Available at: https://www.nice.org.uk/guidance/cg158 (Accessed April 4, 2023).
10. National Institute for Health and Care Excellence (NICE). Eating disorders: recognition and treatment: NICE guideline [NG69] Available at: https://www.nice.org.uk/guidance/ng69 (Accessed April 4, 2023).
11. National Institute for Health and Care Excellence (NICE). Depression in children and young people: identification and management: NICE guideline [NG134]. Available at: https://www.nice.org.uk/guidance/ng134 (Accessed April 4, 2023).
12. Bülow A, van Roekel E, Boele S, Denissen JJ, Keijsers L. Parent–adolescent interaction quality and adolescent affect—an experience sampling study on effect heterogeneity. Child Dev. (2022) 93:e315–31. doi: 10.1111/cdev.13733
13. van IJzendoorn MH, Schuengel C, Wang Q, Bakersmans-Kranenburg MJ. Improving parenting, child attachment, and externalizing behaviors: meta-analysis of the first 25 randomized controlled trials on the effects of video-feedback intervention to promote positive parenting and sensitive discipline. Dev Psychopathol. (2022) 35:241–56. doi: 10.1017/S0954579421001462
14. Dishion TJ, Stormshak EA. Family and peer social interaction. In: Dishion TJ, Stormshak EA, editors. Intervening in children’s lives: an ecological, family-centered approach to mental health care. United States: American Psychological Association (2007). p. 31–45.
15. Heyman RE. Observation of couple conflicts: clinical assessment applications, stubborn truths, and shaky foundations. Psychol Assess. (2001) 13:5–35. doi: 10.1037/1040-3590.13.1.5
16. Hauser ST, Powers SI, Noam GG, Jacobson AM, Weiss B, Follansbee DJ. Familial contexts of adolescent ego development. Child Dev. (1984) 55:195–213. doi: 10.2307/1129845
17. Aspland H, Gardner F. Observational measures of parent-child interaction: an introductory review. Child Adolesc Ment Health. (2003) 8:136–43. doi: 10.1111/1475-3588.00061
18. Locke LM, Prinz RJ. Measurement of parental discipline and nurturance. Clin Psychol Rev. (2002) 22:895–929. doi: 10.1016/s0272-7358(02)00133-2
19. Jewell T, Gardner T, Susi K, Watchorn K, Coopey E, Simic M, et al. Attachment measures in middle childhood and adolescence: a systematic review of measurement properties. Clin Psychol Rev. (2019) 68:71–82. doi: 10.1016/j.cpr.2018.12.004
20. Obsuth I, Hennighausen K, Brumariu LE, Lyons-Ruth K. Disorganized behavior in adolescent-parent interaction: relations to attachment state of mind, partner abuse, and psychopathology. Child Dev. (2014) 25:370–87. doi: 10.1111/cdev.12113
21. Marshall SK, Tilton-Weaver L, Shankar S, Johnston A. Parent-adolescent conflict processes and their measurement: a systematic review. J Fam Theory Rev. (2022) 15:118–32. doi: 10.1111/jftr.12479
22. Cañas M, Ibabe I, De Paúl J. Promising observational instruments of parent-child (0–12 years) interaction within the child protection system: a systematic review. Child Abuse Negl. (2020) 109:104713. doi: 10.1016/j.chiabu.2020.104713
23. Gridley N, Blower S, Dunn A, Bywater T, Whittaker K, Bryant M. Psychometric properties of parent–child (0–5 years) interaction outcome measures as used in randomized controlled trials of parent programs: a systematic review. Clin Child Fam Psychol Rev. (2019) 22:253–71. doi: 10.1007/s10567-019-00275-3
24. Allen JP, Hauser ST, Bell KL, McElhaney KB, Tate DC. The autonomy and relatedness coding system. Unpublished manuscript. Charlottesville, VA: University of Virginia (1998).
25. Gardner F. Methodological issues in the direct observation of parent–child interaction: do observational findings reflect the natural behavior of participants? Clin Child Fam Psychol Rev. (2000) 3:185–98. doi: 10.1023/a:1009503409699
26. Lee R, Skinner A, Bornstein MH, Radford AN, Campbell A, Graham K, et al. Through babies’ eyes: practical and theoretical considerations of using wearable technology to measure parent-infant behaviour from the mothers’ and infants’ view points. Infant Behav Dev. (2017) 47:62–71. doi: 10.1016/j.infbeh.2017.02.006
27. Reid JA. Social learning approach, volume 2: observation in home settings. Oregon: Castalia Publishing Company (1978).
28. Laursen B, Hafen C. Future directions in the study of close relationships: conflict is bad (except when it’s not). Soc Dev. (2010) 19:858–72. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9507.2009.00546.x
29. Buerki S, Adler RH. Negative affect states and cardiovascular disorders: a review and the proposal of a unifying biopsychosocial concept. Gen Hosp Psychiatry. (2005) 27:180–88. doi: 10.1016/j.genhosppsych.2004.12.003
30. Spies LA, Margolin G, Susman EJ, Gordis EB. Adolescents’ cortisol reactivity and subjective distress in response to family conflict: the moderating role of internalizing symptoms. J Adolesc Health. (2011) 49:386–92. doi: 10.1016/j.jadohealth.2011.01.014
31. Beijersbergen MD, Bakermans-Kranenburg MJ, Van IJzendoorn MH, Juffer F. Stress regulation in adolescents: physiological reactivity during the adult attachment interview and conflict interaction. Child Dev. (2008) 79:1707–20. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2008.01220.x
32. Mokkink LB, Terwee CB, Patrick DL, Alonso J, Stratford PW, Knol DL, et al. The COSMIN study reached international consensus on taxonomy, terminology, and definitions of measurement properties for health-related patient-reported outcomes. J Clin Epidemiol. (2010) 63:737–45. doi: 10.1016/j.jclinepi.2010.02.006
33. Patterson GR. The aggressive child: victim and architect of a coercive system. In: Mash EJ, Hamerlynck LA, Handy LC, editors. Behavior modification and families. New York, NY: Brunner/Mazel (1976). p. 267–316.
34. Patterson GR. A social learning approach, volume 3: coercive family process. Oregon: Castalia Publishing Company (1982).
35. Loevinger J. Objective tests as instruments of psychological theory. Psychol Rep. (1957) 3:635–94. doi: 10.2466/pr0.1957.3.3.635
36. Yasui M, Dishion TJ. Direct observation of family management: validity and reliability as a function of coder ethnicity and training. Behav Ther. (2008) 39:336–47. doi: 10.1016/j.beth.2007.10.001
37. American Educational Research Association, American Psychological Association, National Council on Measurement in Education. Standards for educational and psychological testing. Washington, DC, US: American Psychological Association (2014).
38. Smetana JG, Rote WM. Adolescent-parent relationships: progress, processes, and prospects. Annu Rev Dev Psychol. (2019) 1:41–68. doi: 10.1146/annurev-devpsych-121318-084903
39. Modecki KL, Goldberg RE, Wisniewski P, Orben A. What is digital parenting? A systematic review of past measurement and blueprint for the future. Perspect Psychol Sci. (2022) 17:1673–91. doi: 10.1177/17456916211072
40. Glatz T, Buchanan CM. Exploring how adolescent boys’ and girls’ internalizing and externalizing behviors impact parental self-efficacy: a vignette study. Fam Relat. (2023) 72:347–60. doi: 10.1111/fare.12696
41. Flake JK, Pek J, Hehman E. Construct validation in social and personality research: current practice and recommendations. Soc Psychol Personal Sci. (2017) 8:370–78. doi: 10.1177/19485506176930
42. Ainsworth MD, Bell SM, Stayton DJ. Individual differences in the development of some attachment behaviors. Merrill Palmer Q Behav Dev. (1972) 18:123–43.
43. Beebe B, Steele M. How does microanalysis of mother–infant communication inform maternal sensitivity and infant attachment? Attach Hum Dev. (2013) 15:583–602. doi: 10.1080/14616734.2013.841050
44. Hops H, Davis B, Longoria N. Methodological issues in direct observation: illustrations with the living in familial environments (LIFE) coding system. J Clin Child Psychol. (1995) 24:193–203. doi: 10.1207/s15374424jccp2402_7
45. Ekman P, Friesen WV. Facial action coding system (FACS): a technique for the measurement of facial movement. California: Consulting Psychologists Press (1978).
46. Coan JA, Gottman JM. The specific affect coding system (SPAFF). In: Coan JA, Allen JJ, editors. Handbook of emotion elicitation and assessment. London, UK: Oxford University Press (2007). p. 267–85.
47. Costantini I, Cordero M, Campbell A, Burgess R, Glen K, Moraitopoulou G, et al. Mental health intergenerational transmission (MHINT) process manual. (2021) doi: 10.31219/osf.io/s6n4h
48. Burgess R, Costantini I, Bornstein MH, Campbell A, Cordero Vega MA, Culpin I, et al. A quantitative evaluation of thin slice sampling for parent–infant interactions. J Nonverbal Behav. (2023) 47(2):117–210. doi: 10.1007/s10919-022-00420-7
49. Behavioral coding - Event logging software | The Observer XT. Available at: http://www.noldus.com/human-behavior-research/products/the-observer-xt (Accessed March 23, 2023).
50. Beebe B, Messinger D, Bahrick LE, Margolis A, Buck K, Chen H. A systems view of mother-infant face-to-face communication. Dev Psychol. (2016) 52:556–71. doi: 10.1037/a0040085
51. Pearson R. You don’t get me: public responses to mood and behaviour monitoring wearable technology. Open Access Government. (2022). p.186–7. Available at: https://www.openaccessgovernment.org/article/you-dont-get-me-public-responses-behaviour-monitoring-wearable-technology/146014/ (Accessed November 30, 2023).36056392
52. Dishion T, Gardner K, Patterson G, Reid J, Spyrou S, Thibodeaux S. The family process code: a multidimensional system for observing family interactions. Unpublished coding manual. Eugene, oR: Oregon Social Learning Center (1983). 64.
53. Moore D, Forgatch M, Mukai L, Toobert D. Interaction coding system. Unpublished coding manual. Oregon Social Learning Center (1978).
54. Toobert D, Patterson GR, Moore D, Halper V. Multi-dimensional description of family interactions (MOSAIC). Unpublished coding manual. Eugene, oR: Oregon Social Learning Center (1982).
55. Dishion T, Rivera EK, Verberkmoes S, Jones S, Patras JM. Relationship process code: a system for coding relationship interactions. Unpublished coding manual. Eugene, oR: Child & Family Center, University of Oregon (2002).
56. Rusby JC, Estes A, Dishion T. The interpersonal process code (IPC). Unpublished coding manual. Eugene, oR: Oregon Social Learning Center (1991). 49.
57. Dumas JE. INTERACT – A computer-based coding and data management system to assess family interactions. In: Prinz RJ, editors. Advances in behavioral assessment of children and families. Greenwich, CT: JAI Press (1987). p. 177–202.
58. Gottman JM, Krokoff LJ. Marital interaction and satisfaction: a longitudinal view. J Consult Clin Psychol. (1989) 57:47–5. doi: 10.1037/0022-006X.57.1.47
59. Gottman JM. The couples interaction scoring system. In: Gottman JM, editors. Marital interaction: experimental investigations. New York, NY: Academic Press (1979). p. 77–92.
60. Stierlin H. Separating parents and adolescents: a perspective on running away, schizophrenia, and waywardness. New York: Quadrangle/New York Times Book Company (1974). 204.
61. Hops H, Wills TA, Weiss RL, Patterson GR. Marital interaction coding system. Unpublished coding manual. Eugene, oR: University of Oregon and Oregon Research Institute (1972).
62. Jabson J, Venkatraman S, Dishion T. Simple affect coding system. Unpublished coding manual. Eugene, oR: Child & Family Center, University of Oregon (2003).
63. Notarius CI, Pellegrini DS, Martin LA. Codebook of marital and family interaction (COMFI), version 5.1. Washington, DC: Department of Psychology, Catholic University of America (1991). 51.
64. Aiken CS, Wagner BM, Hinnant JB. Observed interactions in families of adolescent suicide attempters. Suicide Life Threat Behav. (2017) 49:104–19. doi: 10.1111/sltb.12423
65. Doane JA, West KL, Goldstein MJ, Rodnick EH, Jones JE. Parental communication deviance and affective style: predictors of subsequent schizophrenic spectrum disorders in vulnerable adolescents. Arch Gen Psychiatry. (1981) 38:679–85. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.1981.01780310079008
66. Weiss RL, Summers KJ. Marital interaction coding system. In: Filsinger EE, editors. A source book of marriage and family assessment. Beverley Hills, CA: Sage (1983). p. 85–115.
67. Hahlweg K, Revenstorf D, Schindler L. Effects of behavioral marital therapy on couple’s communication and problem-solving skills. J Consult Clin Psychol. (1984) 52:553–66. doi: 10.1037/0022-006X.52.4.553
68. Peterson J, Winter C, Jabson JM, Dishion TJ. Relationship affect coding system (adolescents). Unpublished coding manual. Eugene, oR: Child & Family Center, University of Oregon (2008). 20.
69. Dishion T, Kavanagh K. FAST: family assessment task. Unpublished coding manual. Eugene, oR: Oregon Social Learning Center (1997).
70. Robin AL, Foster SL. Negotiating parent-adolescent conflict: a behavioral-family systems approach. New York: Guilford Press (1989). 338.
71. Robin AL. Parent-adolescent conflict: a skill training approach. In: Rathjen DP, Foreyt J, editors. Social competence: interventions for children and adults. Elinsford, NY: Pergamon Press (1980). p. 147–211.
72. Rodriguez EM, Zuckerman T, Saylor M, Schuele CM, Compas BE. Contingency coding system coder manual. Unpublished coding manual. Nashville, TN: Department of Psychology and Human Development, Vanderbilt University (2011). 38.
73. Fey ME, Krulik TE, Loeb DF, Proctor-Williams K. Sentence recast use by parents of children with typical language and specific language impairment. Am J Speech Lang Pathol. (1999) 8:273–86. doi: 10.1044/1058-0360.0803.273
74. Lasky EZ, Klopp K. Parent-child interactions in normal and language-disordered children. J Speech Hear Disord. (1982) 47:7–18. doi: 10.1044/jshd.4701.07
75. Saxton M, Backley P, Gallaway C. Negative input for grammatical errors: effects after a lag of 12 weeks. J Child Lang. (2005) 32:643–72. doi: 10.1017/s0305000905006999
76. Stubbs J, Crosby L, Forgatch MS, Capaldi D. Family and peer process code training manual. Unpublished coding manual. Eugene, oR: Oregon Social Learning Center (1998). 91.
77. Dishion T, Crosby L, Rusby JC, Shane D, Patterson G, Baker J. Peer process code: multidimensional system for observing adolescent peer interaction. Unpublished coding manual. Eugene, oR: Oregon Social Learning Center (1989). 49.
79. Bowlby J. Attachment and loss: vol. 2. Separation, anxiety and anger. New York: Basic Books (1973).
80. Bowlby J. Attachment and loss: vol. 3. Loss, sadness and depression. New York: Basic Books (1980).
81. Allen JP, Land DL. Attachment in adolescence. In: Cassidy J, Shaver PR, editors. Handbook of attachment: theory, research, and clinical applications. New York, NY: Guilford Press (1999). p. 319–35.
82. Allen JP. The attachment system in adolescence. In: Cassidy J, Shaver PR, editors. Handbook of attachment: theory, research, and clinical applications. New York, NY: Guilford Press (2008). p. 419–35.
83. Cummings EM, Davies P. Emotional security as a regulatory process in normal development and the development of psychopathology. Dev Psychopathol. (1996) 8:123–39. doi: 10.1017/S0954579400007008
84. Barrett PM, Shortt AL, Healy L, Hartman P. Macrocoding schedule for parent and child behaviour. Unpublished coding manual. Griffith University (2000).
85. Henin A, Kendall PC. Obsessive-compulsive disorder in childhood and adolescence. Adv Clin Child Psychol. (1997) 19:75–131. doi: 10.1007/978-1-4757-9035-1_3
86. Merkel WT, Pollard CA, Wiener RL, Staebler CR. Perceived parental characteristics of patients with obsessive-compulsive disorder, depression and panic disorder. Child Psychiatry Hum Dev. (1993) 24:49–57. doi: 10.1007/BF02353718
87. Ehiobuche I. Obsessive-compulsive neurosis in relation to parental child-rearing patterns amongst Greek, Italian, and Anglo-Australian subjects. Acta Psychiatr Scand. (1988) 78:115–20. doi: 10.1111/j.1600-0447.1988.tb09009.x
88. Hoover CF, Insel TR. Families of origin in obsessive-compulsive disorder. J Nerv Ment Dis. (1984) 172:207–15. doi: 10.1097/00005053-198404000-00004
89. Hollingsworth CE, Tanguay PE, Grossman L, Pabst P. Long-term outcome of obsessive compulsive disorder in childhood. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. (1980) 19:134–44. doi: 10.1016/S0002-7138(09)60658-0
90. Dickstein S, Hayden LC, Schiller M, Seifer R, San Antonio W. Providence Family study. Family mealtime interaction coding system. Unpublished coding manual. Providence, RI: School of Medicine, Brown University (1994).
91. Hayden LC, Schiller M, Dickstein S, Seifer R, Sameroff S, Miller I, et al. Levels of family assessment: i. family, marital, and parent–child interaction. J Fam Psychol. (1998) 12:7–22. doi: 10.1037/0893-3200.12.1.7
92. Epstein NB, Baldwin LM, Bishop DS. Mcmaster clinical rating scale. Unpublished coding manual. Providence, RI: Brown/Butler Family Research Program (1982).
93. Dishion TJ, Hogansen J, Winter C, Jabson JM. The coder impressions inventory. Unpublished coding manual. Eugene, oR: Oregon Social Learning Center (2004).
95. Feldman R. Coding interactive behavior (CIB). Unpublished coding manual. Ramat-Gan, Israel: Department of Psychology, Bar-Ilan University (1998).
96. Feldman R. Parenting behavior as the environment where children grow. In: Mayes LC, Lewis M, editors. A developmental environment measurement handbook. New York, NY: Cambridge University Press (2012). p. 535–67.
97. Hagstrøm J, Spang KS, Christiansen BM, Maigaard K, Vangkilde S, Esbjørn BH, et al. The puzzle of emotion regulation: development and evaluation of the tangram emotion coding manual for children. Front Psychiatry. (2019) 10:723. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2019.00723
98. Gross JJ. Emotion regulation: conceptual and empirical foundations. In: Gross JJ, editor. Handbook of emotional regulation. 2nd ed. New York, NY: Guilford Press (2014). p. 3–20
99. Hetherington EM, Clingempeel WG. Coping with marital transitions: a family systems perspective. Monogr Soc Res Child Dev. (1992) 57:1–238. doi: 10.2307/1166050
100. Baumrind D. Child care practices anteceding three patterns of preschool behavior. Genet Psychol Monogr. (1967) 75:43–88. doi: 10.1037/h0024919
101. Olson DH, Portner J, Bell R. Family adaptability and cohesion evaluation scales. In: Olson DH, McCubbin HI, Barnes HL, Larsen A, Muxen M, Wilson M, editors. Family inventories. St. Paul, MN: University of Minnesota Press (1982). p. 5–24.
102. Holmbeck GN, Zebracki K, Johnson SZ, Belvedere M, Hommeyer JS. Parent-child interaction coding manual. Unpublished coding manual. Chicago, IL: Department of Psychology, Loyola University Chicago (2013).
103. Holmbeck GN, Belvedere M, Gorey-Ferguson L, Schneider J. Manual for family macro-coding. Unpublished coding manual. Chicago, IL: Department of Psychology, Loyola University Chicago (1995).
104. Johnson SZ, Holmbeck GN. Parental overprotectiveness coding manual. Unpublished coding manual. Chicago, IL: Department of Psychology, Loyola University Chicago (1999).
105. Smetana JG, Yau J, Restrepo A, Braeges JL. Adolescent-parent conflict in married and divorced families. Dev Psychol. (1991) 27:1000–10. doi: 10.1037/0012-1649.27.6.1000
106. Allen JP, Porter MR, McFarland FC. Autonomy-relatedness coding manual for adolescent peer dyads. Unpublished coding manual. Charlottesville, VA: University of Virginia (2002).
107. Buhrmester D, Camparo L, Christensen A, Gonzalez LS, Hinshaw SP. Mothers and fathers interacting in dyads and triads with normal and hyperactive sons. Dev Psychol. (1992) 28:500–50. doi: 10.1037/0012-1649.28.3.500
108. Julien D, Markman HJ, Lindahl KM, Johnson HM, Van Widenfelt B. Interactional dimensions coding system. Unpublished coding manual. Denver, CO: Denver Center for Marital and Family Studies, University of Denver (1987).
110. Paikoff RL. Child-centered problem-solving and scaffolding behavior coding scheme. Unpublished coding manual. Chicago, IL: Institute for Juvenile Research, Department of Psychiatry, University of Illinois (1992).
111. Lindahl KM, Malik NM. System for coding interactions and family functioning (SCIFF). A coding system for family problem discussions. Miami: University of Miami (2000). 34.
112. Boscolo L, Cecchin G, Hoffman L, Penn P. Milan systemic family therapy: conversations in theory and practice. New York: Basic Books, Inc (1987).
114. Lyons-Ruth K, Hennighausen K, Holmes B. Goal-corrected partnership adolescent coding system (GPACS). Unpublished coding manual. Cambridge, MA: Depart of Psychiatry, Harvard Medical School (2021). 37.
115. Melby J, Conger R, Book R, Rueter M, Lucy L, Repinski D, et al. The IOWA family interaction rating scales. 5th ed. Iowa: Institute for Social and Behavioural Research, Iowa State University (1998).
116. Conger RD. The social context of substance abuse: a developmental perspective. In: Robertson EB, Sloboda Z, Boyd GM, Beatty L, Kozel NJ, editors. Rural substance abuse: state of knowledge and issues. Rockville, MD: National Institute on Drug Abuse (1997). p. 6–36.
117. Conger RD, Rueter MA, Elder GH. Economic and resilient couples. J Personality Soc Psychol. (1999) 76:54–71. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.76.1.54
118. Conger RD, Simons RL. Life-course contingencies in the development of adolescent antisocial behaviour: a matching law approach. In: Thornberry TP, editors. Advances in criminological theory vol 7. Piscataway, NJ: Transaction (1997). p. 55–99.
120. Owen MT, Klausli K, Aultman C, Brown G, Little I, Milling L. Age 15 parent-adolescent interaction coding system. Texas: The University of Texas at Dallas (2007). 15.
121. Ryan RM, Deci EL, Grolnick WS, La Guardia JG. The significance of autonomy and autonomy support in psychological development and psychopathology. In: Cicchetti D, Cohen DJ, editors. Developmental psychopathology: volume one: theory and method. 2nd ed. New Jersey, United States: John Wiley & Sons, Inc (2015). p. 795–849. Available at: https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/book/10.1002/9780470939383
122. Pelham WE Jr, Meichenbaum DL, Smith BH, Sibley MH, Gnagy EM, Bukstein O. Acute effects of MPH on the parent–teen interactions of adolescents with ADHD. J Atten Disord. (2017) 21:158–67. doi: 10.1177/1087054713480833
123. Snyder J. Macro-Level family interaction coding system (MFICS): technical report. Unpublished coding manual. Wichita, KS: Department of Psychology, Wichita State University (2013).
124. Vanwoerden S. The development and validation of an observational coding system for real-time parent-adolescent mentalizing [Dissertation]. Houston, TX: University of Houston (2010).
125. Luyten P, Campbell C, Allison E, Fonagy P. The mentalizing approach to psychopathology: state of the art and future directions. Annu Rev Clin Psychol. (2020) 16:297–325. doi: 10.1146/annurev-clinpsy-071919-015355
126. Bateman A, Fonagy P. Borderline personality disorder and mood disorders: mentalizing as a framework for integrated treatment. J Clin Psychol. (2015) 71:792–804. doi: 10.1002/jclp.22206
127. Ziv Y, Cassidy J, Ramos-Marcuse F. The conflict task coding system. Unpublished coding manual. College Park, MD: University of Maryland (2002).
128. Kobak RR, Cole HE, Ferenz-Gillies R, Fleming WS, Gamble W. Attachment and emotion regulation during mother-teen problem solving: a control theory analysis. Child Dev. (1993) 64:231–45. doi: 10.2307/1131448
129. Crowell JA, Treboux D, Gao Y, Fyffe C, Pan H, Waters E. Assessing secure base behavior in adulthood: development of a measure, links to adult attachment representations, and relations to couples’ communication and reports of relationships. Dev Psychol. (2002) 38:679–93. doi: 10.1037/0012-1649.38.5.679
130. Matis C. Why your social health matters. Hertfordshire, UK: Hertfordshire Business School, University of Hertfordshire (2021). Available at: https://cdn.shopify.com/s/files/1/0488/2336/9892/files/UEZ-Games_to_Get_Social_Health_Research_Report_4778a975-b11e-4ef5-94d8-983bb61b9957.pdf?v=16567607122 (Cited April 23, 2023).
131. Prinsen CA, Mokkink LB, Bouter LM, Alonso J, Patrick DL, De Vet HC, et al. COSMIN guideline for systematic reviews of patient-reported outcome measures. Qual Life Res. (2018) 27:1147–57. doi: 10.1007/s11136-018-1798-3
132. Wrottesley SV, Pearson R, Prioreschi A. The headcam mother-infant interaction assessment tool: testing the feasibility and acceptability in Soweto, South Africa, using participatory engagement. Pilot Feasibility Stud. (2021) 7:140. doi: 10.1186/s40814-021-00875-3
133. Sharp H, Pickles A, Meaney M, Marshall K, Tibu F, Hill J. Frequency of infant stroking reported by mothers moderates the effect of prenatal depression on infant behavioural and physiological outcomes. PLoS One. (2012) 7:e45446. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0045446
134. Furman W, Buhrmester D. The network of relationships inventory: behavioral systems version. Int J Behav Dev. (2010) 33:47a0–78. doi: 10.1177/0165025409342634
135. Deater-Deckard K. Parent feelings questionnaire (PFQ). APA PsycTests. (1996). doi: 10.1037/t20071-000
136. Armsden GC, Greenberg MT. The inventory of parent and peer attachment: individual differences and their relationship to psychological well-being in adolescence. J Youth Adolesc. (1987) 16:427–54. doi: 10.1007/BF02202939
137. Lawlor DA, Lewcock M, Rena-Jones L, Rollings C, Yip V, Smith D, et al. The second generation of the avon longitudinal study of parents and children (ALSPAC-G2): a cohort profile [version 2; peer review: 2 approved]. Wellcome Open Res. (2019) 4:36. doi: 10.12688/wellcomeopenres.15087.2
138. Juffer F, Struis E, Werner C, Bakermans-Kranenburg MJ. Effective preventive interventions to support parents of young children: illustrations from the video-feedback intervention to promote positive parenting and sensitive discipline (VIPP-SD). J Prev Interv Community. (2017) 45:202–14. doi: 10.1080/10852352.2016.1198128
Keywords: parenting, measurement, family interaction, parent-adolescent, observation, adolecence, adolescent-parent
Citation: Wright N, Pearson RM, Crook D, Bond A and Jewell T (2024) Through each other's eyes: initial results and protocol for the co-design of an observational measure of adolescent-parent interaction using first-person perspective. Front. Child Adolesc. Psychiatry 2:1214890. doi: 10.3389/frcha.2023.1214890
Received: 30 April 2023; Accepted: 9 November 2023;
Published: 4 March 2024.
Edited by:
Caroline Temcheff, McGill University, CanadaReviewed by:
Sophie Hauschild, Heidelberg University Hospital, GermanyBarbara Remberk, Institute of Psychiatry and Neurology (IPiN), Poland
© 2024 Wright, Pearson, Crook, Bond and Jewell. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.
*Correspondence: Nicky Wright bmlja3kud3JpZ2h0QG1tdS5hYy51aw==