Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Child Adolesc. Psychiatry, 24 July 2023
Sec. Autism and Other Neurodevelopmental Disorders
This article is part of the Research Topic Advancements and Challenges in Autism and Other Neurodevelopmental Disorders View all 8 articles

Characteristics of Sluggish Cognitive Tempo among adults with ADHD: objective neurocognitive measures align with self-report of executive function

\r\nBeth Krone
&#x;Beth Krone1*Lenard A. Adler,&#x;Lenard A. Adler2,†Deepti AnbarasanDeepti Anbarasan2Terry LeonTerry Leon2Richard GallagherRichard Gallagher2Pooja PatelPooja Patel2Stephen V. FaraoneStephen V. Faraone3Jeffrey H. Newcorn\r\nJeffrey H. Newcorn1
  • 1Department of Psychiatry, Icahn School of Medicine at Mount Sinai (ISMMS), New York, NY, United States
  • 2Department of Psychiatry, New York University (NYU) Grossman School of Medicine, New York, NY, United States
  • 3Norton College of Medicine, SUNY Upstate Medical University, Syracuse, NY, United States

Introduction: Sluggish Cognitive Tempo (SCT) is a syndrome characterized by cognitive hypo-arousal that often appears as daytime sleepiness or drowsiness, mental fogginess, being easily confused, having difficulty with holding and manipulating information in working memory, and being forgetful. Although it frequently co-travels with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) or other conditions and confers significantly greater impairment, there are few studies examining SCT among adults with ADHD. Understanding what features SCT confers in association with ADHD, distinct from other conditions associating with ADHD, is critically important to confirm if SCT is a distinct syndrome that requires special assessment methods and special, distinct treatment efforts to reduce its impact. This study describes the clinical and neuropsychological features of SCT in a sample of adults with well-defined ADHD, and examines the relationship of SCT with other measures of ADHD, neurocognition, executive function (EF), and impairment.

Methods: A sample of n = 106 adults with ADHD, ages 18-57 years, was assessed for SCT using the Barkley SCT scale. Adults with (SCT+) and without (SCT-) SCT received a comprehensive clinical assessment battery, and neuropsychological testing. Clinical and neuropsychological variables were examined for their associations with SCT. The variables were treated with Principal Axis Factoring with Promax with Kaiser Normalization to elucidate latent constructs and determine performance profiles associated with SCT among people with ADHD.

Results: EF Deficits and emotional dyscontrol (ED) symptoms significantly differentiated adults with ADHD and SCT whether measured via self or clinician report. Additionally, significantly greater impairment via both clinician and participant report was seen in the SCT + versus SCT - cohorts. SCT was also associated with a significantly distinct profile on the neuropsychological battery, characterized by a pattern of slower latencies and cognitive strategy choices across CANTAB and WAIS subtests, that reveals difficulty with increased cognitive load, which primarily accounted for the higher level of impairment in the SCT group.

Discussion: The convergence of clinical ratings and neurocognitive measures of EF deficits is consistent with the conclusion that SCT represents a distinct subgroup of adults with ADHD.

Introduction

Sluggish Cognitive Tempo (SCT) describes a set of symptoms that includes daydreaming, trouble initiating tasks, under-motivation, under-arousal, mental confusion, and being slow-moving (1, 2). In a large population survey, Barkley posited nine core symptoms of SCT as follows: (1) prone to daydreaming instead of concentrating; (2) trouble staying alert/awake in boring situations; (3) being easily confused; (4) being easily bored; (5) feeling spacey/in a fog; (6) frequently feeling lethargic; (7) being underactive/having less energy than others; (8) being slow-moving; and (9) not processing information quickly/accurately. Barkley identified adults as having SCT if they had five or more of the nine symptoms scored as “often” or “very often” on the SCT subscale of the Barkley Adult ADHD Rating Scale-IV: Self-Report (3) (BAARS-IV). The syndrome has recently been referred to as cognitive disengagement syndrome (4), but for consistency with earlier literature, we will refer to it as SCT throughout this manuscript.

SCT is common in adults; Barkley found a population rate of 5.8% and SCT frequently co-occurs with ADHD (about half of the Barkley sample) as well as anxiety, depressive, and learning disorders (1, 2, 5). SCT can also be significantly impairing (6). Lunsford-Avery et al. found that increasing SCT severity was associated with multiple aspects of impairment (e.g., work, social relations, and community activities) as reported by self or collateral ratings, even after controlling for ADHD ratings (7).

Both ADHD and SCT have been referred to as disorders of executive function (EF) [e.g., studies by Sergeant (8) and Barkley (5)]. Decades of research using objective neuropsychological assessment indicate that measurable aspects of EF often disrupted in ADHD include impulse control (measured through response inhibition tasks), planning and strategy (measured through tower/stocking and maze tasks), cognitive flexibility and control (measured through set-shifting and maintenance tasks), focus (measured through interference control), and working memory (measurable through a variety of tasks, such as the n-back). Several rating scales have been developed to quantify these EF deficits by capturing reports of the frequency and intensity of the behaviors through which they are thought to manifest. Difficulty withholding a response on a neuropsychological go/no-go task and reports of “frequently acting before thinking” are related (though not perfectly associated) constructs of impulse control. With this in mind, the clinical descriptors of SCT, which include being easily confused, having difficulty with maintaining attentional arousal, and processing information quickly and accurately, suggest SCT describes EF difficulties distinct from those in ADHD. The research suggests this is the case among children with ADHD + SCT, but there is very little research examining EF in relation to ADHD + SCT among adults. In the few prior studies of adults with ADHD + SCT, SCT was associated with EF deficits in problem-solving and organization (9, 10) and in global EF (1, 2, 913). Even when accounting for both ADHD severity and co-morbid anxiety, SCT remained independently associated with EF deficits and emotional self-control (2, 911). Stimulant treatment attenuated these effects, though it was uncertain whether the attenuation represented indirect effects from treating ADHD or from some unspecified direct treatment effect on SCT (11). Another study examining the multimodal treatment of adults with ADHD + SCT also showed these adults benefit from currently approved ADHD medications and psychotherapies tailored to EF deficits (14). In our interim analysis of the current study, our group reported SCT associated with more severe ADHD inattention, as well as with greater deficits in self-report but not clinician-rated EF (15), making an objective neurocognitive assessment of EF all the more salient for evaluating the association of SCT with EF in the context of ADHD.

As there are currently few randomized clinical trials that evaluate the response of SCT symptoms to pharmacotherapy in adults, we undertook a two-site study [NYU Grossman School of Medicine (NYU) and Icahn School of Medicine at Mount Sinai (ISMMS)] in adults with ADHD to examine the phenomenology of SCT (contrasting ADHD with and without SCT) and the responsiveness of SCT symptoms to the stimulant lisdexamfetamine vs. placebo in the SCT-positive (SCT+) adults. We previously reported the results of the treatment trial in adults with SCT and ADHD (16) and an interim analysis of the baseline ADHD sample, comparing clinical measures in the SCT+ versus SCT-negative (SCT−) cohorts in 87 participants in the NYU sample (15). The interim analysis found that adults with ADHD and SCT had greater symptoms of inattention and ratings of impairment than those with ADHD but without SCT. The cohort with both disorders also had higher ratings of EF deficits on the BRIEF-A, but not on clinical ratings assessed via the expanded ADHD symptom rating scale. However, the interim analysis did not have enough power in the SCT+ cohort to address these inconsistencies between the BRIEF-A and expanded ADHD symptom scales, and also control for potential effects of ADHD symptoms on differentiating the presence of SCT. In this manuscript, we report the rating scale findings in a larger sample, with an expanded SCT+ cohort, while also investigating neuropsychological test patterns [Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale-IV (WAIS) and The Cambridge Neuropsychological Test Automated Battery (CANTAB)]. This allows a fuller understanding of the clinical presentation and neuropsychological profile of adults with ADHD and SCT.

Methods

In total, 151 adults with ADHD were consented (NYU: n = 120; ISMMS: n = 31), with 106 adults with ADHD completing this baseline investigation (see Figure 1 for participant disposition) (NYU: n = 87, 48 SCT+, 39 SCT−; ISMMS: n = 19, 15 SCT+, 4 SCT−), of which 63 were SCT+ and 43 were SCT−.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Patient disposition.

Assessments

Participants were administered the expanded Adult ADHD Clinical Diagnostic Scale (ACDS) v1.2 to ascertain the presence of DSM-5 adult ADHD, the Adult ADHD Investigator Symptom Rating Scale (AISRS) expanded version to assess ADHD symptoms, the Mini International Neuropsychiatric Interview 7.0 (MINI) to establish DSM-5 disorders, the Behavior Rating Inventory of Executive Function-Adult Version (BRIEF-A) to measure executive functioning, the Barkley Functional Impairment (BFIS) and Clinical Global Impression (CGI) scales to measure impairment, and the Barkley SCT Scale (3) to measure the severity of SCT. Suicidality was assessed using the Columbia-Suicide Severity Rating Scale (17). Medical and psychiatric histories, as well as demographics, were self-reported.

All clinical assessments were administered in person by board-certified psychiatrists, a clinical psychologist, or a research nurse, each with over 10 years of experience in adult ADHD research. All clinicians were trained per standard procedures before the initiation of the study (18). All diagnostic or clinical questions were discussed with the two site principal investigators, both experts in adult ADHD (LA and JN).

Inclusion and exclusion criteria

The inclusion criteria were as follows: (1) adults aged 18–60 years meeting the DSM-5 criteria for a primary diagnosis of inattentive or combined type ADHD as diagnosed via ACDS v1.2; (2) demonstrated significant impairment, based on the norms of the BFIS; (3) the SCT+ cohort needed to have ≥5 items on the Barkley SCT Scale rated 3 (“often”) or 4 (“very often”) as well as a total SCT symptom score of ≥26 (4) and a T-score of ≥65 on the Metacognition Index Subscale of BRIEF-A; and (4) the SCT− cohort had <5 items on the Barkley SCT Scale rated 3 (“often”) or 4 (“very often”) and a total SCT symptom score <26.

Participants were excluded for the following reasons: (1) they met the DSM-5 criteria for a primary diagnosis of hyperactive-impulsive presentation ADHD as diagnosed via the ACDS v1.2; (2) they had any other current psychiatric disorder, determined via the MINI, that necessitated pharmacotherapy; (3) they had current suicidal ideation or a history of suicide attempts; (4) they had a lifetime history of bipolar disorder or any psychotic disorder (as per the MINI); (5) they were female and planning to become pregnant or were currently breast-feeding; (6) they had a positive urine drug toxicology at baseline; (7) they had any significant clinical issues that, in the opinion of the investigator, would prevent the person from participating in the study or compromise the participant's safety; and (8) they had previously used lisdexamfetamine (as this was the study drug during the treatment phase of the protocol).

The study was approved by the Institutional Review Boards at these institutions and registered at ClinicalTrials.gov (identifier: NCT02635035). The trial was conducted between January 2016 and April 2018.

Rating scales

ACDS v1.2

A diagnosis of ADHD was evaluated via v1.2 of the ACDS (11, 12), a semi-structured diagnostic interview that is extensively used in adult ADHD investigations (13, 14) (19). The ACDS v1.2 clinical interview first assesses childhood symptoms of ADHD and then an expanded set of recent (past year) adult ADHD symptoms, including all DSM-5 criteria A1 and A2 symptoms. The scale includes developmentally appropriate prompts and stems questions with modified language designed to capture DSM symptoms of ADHD as they present in childhood and adulthood. After asking all prompts and probes for each symptom, the clinician rates the symptom severity as 1 (never), 2 (mild), 3 (moderate), or 4 (severe).

The ACDS v1.2 has been expanded to include 13 additional symptoms that query executive function deficits (EFDs; 9 items) and emotional dyscontrol (ED; 4 items). EFDs contain symptoms of higher-level cognitive processes of the organization, planning, keeping things in mind (working memory), task initiation, and planning. ED includes behavioral descriptors of changeable mood, irritability, and emotional over-reactivity. Specific prompts and probes have been included for the 13 additional ACDS v1.2 EFD and ED items to help guide the rater in assessing these accessory symptoms in adulthood.

Adult ADHD investigator rating scale

At baseline, ACDS v1.2 scores were converted to AISRS scores to measure ADHD symptom severity (as has been done in prior adult ADHD trials), after verifying the similarity of symptoms in the recent past versus in the last year (20, 21). Both the ACDS v1.2 and AISRS use the same prompts and a 4-point scale (“none,” “mild,” “moderate,” and “severe”; the latter two being the cutoff for clinical impairment for each item). Previous studies have established the rationale for this transformation, which is centered on the high agreement between AISRS and ACDS v1.2 scores and the need to minimize participant burden. Therefore, all subsequent discussion of ADHD symptom rating data will refer to these scores on the expanded 31-item AISRS scale.

The AISRS provides an expanded ADHD symptom score (31 items), as well as inattentive (IA) and hyperactive-impulsive (H-I) subscale scores (9 items each). The total ADHD symptom score on the AISRS is the sum of the IA and H-I subscales (18 items), hereafter referred to as the AISRS total symptom score. As noted, the EFD subscale has nine items and the ED subscale has four items. Both the 18-item and the expanded version of the AISRS have been extensively used to evaluate baseline symptoms in both cross-sectional and treatment studies.

Behavior rating inventory of executive function—adult version (BRIEF-A)

The BRIEF-A is a norm-referenced, self-report scale of EF with T-scores ≥65 indicating clinically significant impairment for indices and composite scores (22, 23). Nine clinical subscales measure behaviors associated with EF. The first five assess the ability to initiate behaviors (initiate), use working memory, plan and mentally organize or structure behaviors (plan/organize), judge progress on a task (task monitor), and organize objects and materials for use in tasks (organization of material); these five items refer to cognitive manifestations of symptoms. These items also contribute [together with other items (see below)] to a Metacognition Index (MI). The next four subscales [to inhibit behaviors (inhibit), to flexibly shift between tasks (shift), to control emotions (emotional control), and to monitor and regulate behaviors to adapt to situational or task demands (self-monitor)] refer to the ability to regulate behavioral responses. These items contribute to a Behavioral Regulation Index (BRI). The Global Executive Complex (GEC) score, comprising the various subscales described above, is considered a global EF measure.

MINI 7.0

The MINI was used to evaluate psychiatric co-morbidity (24). The MINI is a structured clinical interview used to assess DSM-5 psychiatric disorders and has been widely used to evaluate psychiatric co-morbidity in adult ADHD studies and in trials involving other mental health disorders.

Barkley Functional Impairment Scale (BFIS)

The BFIS is a self-report measure of perceived current impairment in 15 different major life activities rated on a 10-point Likert scale (0 = not impaired, 9 = severely impaired) (25). The activity domains include interactions at home, handling personal affairs, completing chores, activities, and work, social interactions, and relationships with friends and family. Mean ratings can be calculated to quantify impairment. The scale has been normed in a large epidemiologic survey of individuals with and without ADHD.

CGI-severity (CGI-S)

Overall impairment was assessed by the CGI-S scale, a widely used clinician-rated measure of global ADHD impairment that uses a 7-point Likert rating (1 = normal, 7 = among the most severely ill patients) (26).

Barkley SCT scale

Adult SCT was assessed using the SCT subscale of the BAARS-IV (3). This is a nine-item self-report that is scored on a 3-point rating scale (1 = “never or rarely,” 2 = “sometimes,” 3 = “often,” and “very often”). The Barkley scale cut-score thresholds are based on a large (N = 1,249) normative sample. Responses of “often” or “very often” are considered clinically relevant manifestations of SCT behaviors. The presence of four (of nine) clinically relevant items places a respondent over the 93rd percentile and in the “mildly symptomatic” range. To ensure the presence of SCT, we used the more conservative presence of five items and a cut-score of 26, which places a respondent over the 95th or 96th percentile, and in the “moderately symptomatic” range, which is above Barkley's suggested cutoff of 93rd percentile and symptom counts. The scale has been shown to have good psychometric properties, including test-retest reliability (1). The Cronbach's alpha of the Barkley SCT scale was 0.84 for this sample, which was almost identical to the 0.83 for the NYU cohort reported in the interim analyses.

Neurocognitive measures

Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale-IV (WAIS-IV)

The WAIS-IV is a standardized assessment of cognitive abilities (27). In this study, the Processing Speed Index (PSI), comprising the Coding (Cd) and Symbol Search (SS) subtests, was used to determine the speed and accuracy with which participants visually discriminate simple geometric symbols and produce manual (written) responses. Where the SS subtest requires minimal visual scanning and motor movement (drawing a line through a symbol), the cognitive (learning to associate a number with a symbol) and motor requirements (writing a number) are much greater for the Cd subtest.

The Cambridge Neuropsychological Test Automated Battery (CANTAB)

The CANTAB is a standardized, direct measure of neurocognitive function and frontal lobe dysfunction (28, 29). Tasks comprising the ADHD battery are normed with clinical thresholds and target attentional processes regulated via the prefrontal cortex. The CANTAB ADHD battery has been used to assess dopaminergic and noradrenergic pathways in ADHD treatment response and to characterize EF weaknesses among adults with ADHD (30). However, to date, there have been no studies using the CANTAB to assess SCT. The CANTAB tasks used in this study were as follows: (1) the Motor Screening Task (MOT), which discriminates between sensorimotor deficits and lack of comprehension by measuring the participant's speed of response and accuracy of pointing at the correct target; (2) the Spatial Working Memory (SWM), which assesses retention and manipulation of visuospatial information and provides measures of performance for executive function (e.g., cognitive load), strategy (e.g., the efficiency of strategies used), and errors (e.g., incorrect responses); (3) the Rapid Visual Information Processing (RVP) measures sustained visual attention and arousal, providing measures of latency, probability of false alarms, and sensitivity; (4) the Stop Signal Task (SST) assesses response inhibition by providing reaction times for “go” and “stop” signals, and the proportion of successful inhibited responses. The task also provides a measure of direction errors, since choice objects may appear either on the right or left side of the screen. The Attention Switching Task was added to the battery for this study, though it is not part of the standard CANTAB ADHD battery. This task provides a direct measure of EF as cognitive control of attention, providing a measure of attention shifting.

Data collection, preparation, and analysis

Clinical variables: SCT, ADHD, impairment BRIEF major indices ratings

Data were analyzed via logistic regressions to examine the variables that differentiated the SCT+ and SCT− cohorts. Initial analyses examined potential demographic differences, including sex, age, ethnicity, and race. Significant demographic predictors were then used as covariates in the analyses, examining the following variables as potential predictors of SCT: Barkley SCT score, AISRS IA, AISRS H-I, AISRS EFD, ASISRS ED; Brief GEC T-; Brief MCI T-; CGI-S; and BFIS MI scores. A pre-hoc decision was made to include the site as a covariate given the preferential recruitment of SCT+ participants at the ISMMS site. The analysis of ADHD symptoms in the SCT+ and SCT− groups uses the ADHD-RS with prompts total score (IA and H-A) as it reflects more recent symptoms. The AISRS total score (IA and H-I) is used as a covariate in these analyses. This avoids duplicating the analyses of the potential effects of two 18-item ADHD scales in differentiating between the SCT cohorts and distinguishing between acute and chronic symptoms.

The potential differences for the neuropsychological tests were examined, as noted below, via two-tailed t-tests, Mann–Whitney U tests, and factor analyses.

Variable distributions were examined for outliers, skew, and kurtosis. The distributions were relatively normal, although there were several somewhat kurtotic clinical-scale variables, as should be expected in a dataset consisting of only clinical cases (no healthy controls). The minor kurtosis did not need to be addressed, because the intended principal axis factoring (PAF) is robust to these violations of distribution (31).

A descriptive characterization of participants classified as with or without SCT was conducted. The neurocognitive differences between individuals with and without SCT were examined with two-tailed t-tests and Mann–Whitney U tests.

Dimension reduction was accomplished with PAF with Promax rotation (see Supplementary Text). PAF provides information about the way in which neurocognitive performance variables and clinical scales factor into SCT while respecting the structure of latent variables. The Promax rotation allows for the factors to be correlated and provides unique and shared variance estimates, as should be expected. This is desirable given that the variables are multiple measures of the same psychological constructs. In using this oblique rotation method, we first examined the pattern matrix for information about the item and factor loadings, and then the factor correlation matrix to assess correlations between the factors.

Results

Sample demographics are presented in Table 1. Most of the sample met the criteria for the combined ADHD presentation. The mean age of the whole sample was 31.9 ± 8.6 years, with the SCT+ cohort being slightly older (33.0 ± 9.7 years vs. 30.2 ± 7.1 years. Of the sample, 36.8% were men, with the percentage being somewhat lower in the SCT+ group (33.3% v. 41.86%). The sample was 50.0% (n = 53) white, 13.2% (n = 14) black, 18.9% (n = 20) Hispanic, 16.0% (n = 17) Asian, and 1.9% (n = 2) other/unknown. Table 1 shows the breakdown by SCT status. There was a greater proportion of white participants (65.1%, n = 28) in the SCT− sample compared to the SCT+ sample (39.7%, n = 25). Nine participants in the NYU cohort had a lifetime mood or anxiety disorder. Six participants in the ISMMS cohort had a lifetime mood or anxiety disorder. No participant met the criteria for a current (within the past year) mood or anxiety disorder.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Sample demographics by SCT phenotype.

Clinical variables: comparing samples of patients with and without SCT

Age was a significant predictor of SCT. This was not surprising given that the SCT+ cohort was somewhat older (×2 (1) = 3.94, p = 0.047); the site was also a significant predictor of SCT, which was also expected, as the higher proportion of SCT+ participants in the ISMMS cohort was pre-hoc established (×2 (1) = 4.28, p = 0.039). The other demographic variables were not significant (NS; sex: ×2 (1) = 0.71, NS; ethnicity: ×2 (1) = 0.01, NS; race: ×2 (1) = 1.89, NS). Therefore, the logistic regressions for the clinical variables were adjusted for the two statistically significant demographic variables (site and age) (Table 2).

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Logistic regressions predicting SCT, adjusting for site and age.

ADHD symptoms (AISRS subscales), associated EF and ED symptoms (AISRS expanded subscales), BRIEF-A measures of EF (GEC and MCI), and measures of impairment (CGI and BFIS) all significantly discriminated SCT+ from SCT− participants. These differences were due to SCT associating with higher levels of ADHD, EF, and ED symptoms and impairment. Not surprisingly, the Barkley SCT Scale significantly predicted differences between the SCT+ and SCT− cohorts, with the SCT+ sample having substantially higher scores.

To examine the potential contribution of ADHD scores to differences between the SCT+ and SCT− groups, logistic regressions were conducted adjusting for total AISRS scores (IA + H-I), in addition to age and site. These analyses found that EF deficits and ED symptoms (as elicited on AISRS subsets and the BRIEF-A) still differentiated the SCT+ and SCT− groups, as did the Barkley SCT scores. The SCT cohort maintained significantly greater impairment on both the CGI and BFIS (Table 3) after correcting for total ADHD symptoms. Thus, the significantly greater impairment seen in the SCT+ cohort was not accounted for by their higher ADHD scores. Rather, the greater impairment seen in participants with SCT was due to the presence of EFD and possibly also ED. The AISRS IA and H-I subscales were not significantly different between the SCT+ and SCT− cohorts.

TABLE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. Logistic regressions predicting SCT, adjusting for ADHD severity, site, and age.

Neuropsychological measures

Of the 104 participants who completed valid CANTAB MOT and SST subtests, there were no significant differences between those with and without SCT in the speed of motor skills without cognitive load (MOT Mean Latency). Nor was there a difference in their ability to perform the task correctly without a cognitive load (MOT Mean Errors). There were no significant differences in any of the measures of response inhibition with minimal cognitive load (Table 4).

TABLE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Table 4. Clinical and neuropsychological assessments, differences ADHD + SCT and ADHD-SCT.

Among participants with valid RVP subtest scores on the CANTAB (N = 104), those with SCT detected fewer targets [RVP A’: SCT+ = 0.888 ± 0.060; SCT− = 0.915 ± 0.048; t(102) = −2.468; p = 0.015]. They also took longer to make decisions and respond [RVP mean latency: SCT+ = 503.80 ± 127.67; SCT− = 461.01 ± 78.9; t(102) = 2.865; p = 0.005].

Among the participants who completed the WAIS subtests to provide a measure of processing speed (N = 101), overall processing speed (PSI) was significantly slower among participants with SCT (N = 61, mean = 99.48 ± 16.49) than without SCT (N = 40, mean = 106.38 ± 11.70; t(98) = −2.274; p = 0.02). On the Symbol Search Subtest, participants with SCT (mean = 9.64 ± 3.54) demonstrated a significantly slower speed for visually scanning a page and matching novel geometric designs [t(99) = −2.024; p = 0.046] compared to the participants without SCT (mean = 10.93 ± 2.33). On the Coding subtest, the participants with SCT (mean = 10.30 ± 2.99) trended slower in their ability to associate a simple geometric symbol with numbers, and to draw the shape when prompted by the number [t(99) = −1.988; p = 0.05] than those without SCT (mean = 11.46 ± 2.63).

Among the 103 participants with valid AST subtest scores on the CANTAB, the measures that were significantly different indicated that participants with SCT paid a higher cognitive cost when discriminating between stimuli [AST Congruency Cost Median: SCT+ mean = 63.10 ± 60.84; SCT− mean = 36.80 ± 48.42; t(101) = 2.35; p = 0.021]. They took longer to make correct responses [AST mean correct latency: SCT+ = 603.14 ± 131.01; SCT− = 551.78 ± 106.49; t(101) = 2.117; p = 0.037]. They took longer to correctly respond to incongruent stimuli [AST mean correct latency (incongruent): SCT+ = 634.67 ± 140.82; SCT− = 566.06 ± 128.87; t(101) = 2.529; p = 0.013 (mean correct latency incongruent, Mann–Whitney U test, p = 0.031)]. They spent more time determining correct responses when task stimuli did not switch [AST mean correct latency (blocks 3,5) (non-switching blocks): SCT+ = 472.52 ± 140.12; SCT− = 422.38 ± 74.98; t(101) = 2.695; p = 0.008 (p = 0.042)]. There was also a trend for a lower proportion of trials correctly completed [AST percent correct trials: SCT+ mean = 92.68 ± 6.47; SCT− mean = 94.92 ± 4.64; t(101) = −1.938; p = 0.055].

Participants with SCT performed significantly more poorly on the CANTAB SWM subtest, using a less efficient cognitive strategy [SCT+ mean = 17.43 ± 3.52; SCT− mean = 14.60 ± 3.76; t(102) = 3.908; p = 0.000]. They also made significantly more errors [SCT+ mean = 18.38 ± 10.43; SCT− mean = 12.67 ± 10.15; t(102) = 2.776; p = 0.007].

The planned Forced 5 Factor model explained 49.83% of the variance in SCT; however, the factor correlation matrix (Table 5) revealed a very strong correlation between factors 4 and 1, and the proportion of variance explained dropped precipitously between factors 3 and 4 and remained reasonably and consistently low with subsequent factors. As such, we interpreted the more conservative Forced 3 Factor model that explained 41.23% of the variance (Tables 68). The factor correlation matrix indicated that Factors 1 and 3 were moderately correlated (r = 0.372), and Factors 2 and 3 were moderately correlated (r = 0.327) as would be expected with related traits. However, Factors 1 and 2 shared very little overlap (r = 0.102) and an examination of the items comprising the factors explains why. The pattern matrix is displayed in Table 6, the pattern structure matrix is displayed in Table 7, and the unrotated matrix is displayed in Table 8. Items with factor loadings above 0.320 are shown.

TABLE 5
www.frontiersin.org

Table 5. Factor correlation matrix.

TABLE 6
www.frontiersin.org

Table 6. Three-factor pattern matrixa (β).

TABLE 7
www.frontiersin.org

Table 7. Three-factor structure matrix (zero-order correlations).

TABLE 8
www.frontiersin.org

Table 8. Three-factor matrixa (r—unrotated).

Each of the three factors represented a separate latent construct (see Supplementary Text). Factor 1 comprised clinical and neurocognitive measures associated specifically with SCT on the BAARS. Factor 2 represented executive function problems characterized by poor cognitive flexibility, worse response inhibition, and worse performance with an increased cognitive burden. Factor 3 represented better processing speed, poor task performance (more errors), and more problems with working memory.

Discussion

The current finding, compatible across both the clinical ratings and neurocognitive assessments, shows that SCT represents a distinct subset within ADHD and that individuals with ADHD + SCT have higher ratings and neuropsychological measures suggestive of EF deficits, and higher impairment due to the EF deficits (since those were not explained by ADHD severity). Importantly, contrary to the preliminary findings presented in the interim analysis, this held true regardless of whether EF problems were measured via self or clinician report.

The observation of greater impairment in adults with ADHD and SCT vs. those with ADHD alone has been reported previously (1) and was also seen in our interim report (15). All ADHD symptom sets (IA, H-I, EFD, and ED) on the BRIEF and ADHD symptom scales were seen to be significantly higher in the SCT+ cohort in these analyses of the full baseline cohort. When a smaller number of SCT+ participants were analyzed in the interim analysis, similar findings were seen; however, significantly higher scores of IA and not H-I symptoms on the AISRS in the SCT+ cohort were observed. It should be noted that the regression analyses conducted on this full sample are consistent with the data analyses suggested in the publication of the interim data (15).

Neurocognitive testing supported and amplified these findings; there was a distinct pattern of EF deficits among individuals with SCT in this sample of adults with ADHD. SCT was associated with difficulty processing information rapidly in the context of increasing cognitive load, and less effective strategies on working memory tasks. These deficits are indicative of problems with planning and strategy use (e.g., set-shifting and maintenance tasks), focus (interference control), and working memory. In this sample, SCT did not associate with simple motor speed or response inhibition above and beyond the deficits already associated with ADHD. Taken together, these findings suggest that SCT represents a phenotype of executive dysfunction that adds additional burden to those EF deficits associated with ADHD. This is consistent with the finding of significantly greater self-reported EF deficits on the BRIEF-A in the SCT+ versus. SCT− cohorts. While it should be noted that this sample was somewhat loaded for EF deficits in requiring a significant score on the BRIEF MCI subscale on enrollment, the majority of participants who met that threshold did not have SCT on a more comprehensive assessment.

This is the first study to perform CANTAB among adults with ADHD + SCT. It is the second study to examine WAIS processing speed among adults with ADHD + SCT (9), and the first to explore the relative contributions of cognitive processing load and psychomotor coordination compared to reaction time in task performance. While some isolated findings on the CANTAB were not confined to SCT, the more specific profile of deficits on the CANTAB converged with self-report and clinician ratings of EF and SCT. The CANTAB findings are of interest, as they illustrate the linkage between neurocognitive performance and behavioral manifestations of EF dysfunction. The objective measures of working memory problems and poor response inhibition manifest in behaviors captured on the clinical reports. Moreover, the objective measures of difficulty with cognitive loads and mental flexibility manifest as difficulty with shifting attention, monitoring behavior during tasks, and flexibly updating mental sets on rating scale reports. The scales, however, may be more meaningful in translating those neuropsychological deficits to problems in life functions and may be more suitable to clinical practice because of testing time and financial demands.

The latent factors elucidated by PAF explain much of the variance traditionally seen in the assessment of ADHD and provide insight into the neurocognitive phenotypes of ADHD and the nature of SCT. All factors show that EF problems are pervasive among adults with ADHD and SCT, and that specific neurocognitive processes may sub-serve the EF problems associated with SCT. The results of the extant literature regarding EF deficits in the context of SCT and ADHD seem to concur. ADHD is more often found through response inhibition, but SCT is more often found in other EF domains [e.g., in studies by Barkley (5), Sergeant (8), and Wood et al. (9)]. In effect, when SCT is added to ADHD, individuals have significantly more issues in processing their environments and responding to task demands especially when cognitive loads are high.

Limitations

Several factors should be considered when interpreting these data, including the following: (1) weighting the sample toward executive dysfunction by recruiting people who had SCT and a BRIEF MCI of >65; (2) including only adults with ADHD—we are not able to extrapolate these findings to individuals with SCT but without ADHD; and (3) the unbalanced enrollment nature between the two sites. Future studies require larger sample sizes and the inclusion of people without executive function deficits to confirm the deficits reported above, because SCT may be associated with EF deficits, and our recruitment strategy may have a bias toward amplifying this relationship. Notwithstanding these potential limitations, this study highlights the importance of EF deficits, described clinically and neuropsychologically in defining SCT in adults with ADHD; the overlap of clinical and neuropsychological observations reinforces the importance of observing EF deficits in the phenomenology of SCT. Furthermore, the persistence of heightened impairment when controlling for ADHD symptoms highlights the need for further investigations into understanding the impact of SCT in adults.

Conclusion

To summarize, in this sample of well-characterized adults with ADHD, patient- and clinician-rated cognitive, behavioral, and functional status were objectively supported by neurocognitive performance. These measures were discrete and separable from those usually associated with ADHD. Further, the neurocognitive and clinical reports, when taken together, defined multiple constructs representing cognitive phenotypes. SCT was associated with distinct, measurable, and objective neurocognitive dysfunctions that directly relate to the functional impairments frequently reported by patients. The results of this study extend the scientific basis of SCT as a neurocognitive syndrome within ADHD and provide reassurance that clinically obtained measures are valid and useful when neurocognitive testing is not feasible. For clinical practice, the data suggest that the evaluation of SCT in patients with ADHD can add important information for understanding the patients’ challenges and for treatment planning.

Data availability statement

The authors will make their data available as is necessary, acceptable, and reasonable on request.

Ethics statement

The studies involving human participants were reviewed and approved by The Program for Protection of Human Subjects at the Icahn School of Medicine at Mount Sinai and the IRB at NYU Medical School. The patients/participants provided written informed consent to participate in this study.

Author contributions

The authors made substantial contributions to the conception or design of the work; the acquisition, analysis, or interpretation of data for the work; drafting or revising the work for important intellectual content; providing approval for publication of the content; agree to be accountable for all aspects of the work in ensuring that questions related to the accuracy or integrity of any part of the work are appropriately investigated and resolved. All authors contributed to the article and approved the submitted version.

Funding

Funding for this trial was provided by an investigator-initiated grant (NCT02635035 to LAA and JN) from Shire Pharmaceuticals awarded to New York University. Mt. Sinai was funded through a subcontract from New York University.

Conflict of interest

In the last three years, BK has served as a consultant to Hippo T&C. In the last three years, LAA has received grant/research support from Shire/Takeda Pharmaceuticals, Otsuka; has served as a consultant to Bracket/Signant, Shire/Takeda Pharmaceuticals, Otsuka Pharmaceuticals, SUNY, Supernus Pharmaceuticals, the National Football League, and Major League Baseball; and has received royalty payments (as the inventor) since 2004 from NYU for the license of adult ADHD scales and training materials. In the last three years, JN was a consultant/advisory board member for Adlon Therapeutics, Cingulate Therapeutics, Corium, Hippo T&C, Ironshore, Lumos, Lundbeck, Medice, Mind Tension, Myriad, NLS, OnDosis, Otsuka, Rhodes, Shire/Takeda, and Supernus; he received research support from Adlon, Otsuka, Shire, Supernus; honoraria for disease state lectures from Otsuka and Shire; and served as a consultant for the US National Football League. In the last three years, SF has received support from Otsuka Grant Support and Speakers Bureau, Supernus Advisory Board, Grant Support, Research Support, Speakers Bureau, and Travel Support for participation in another meeting; Aardvark Consultant and Stock or Equity; Aardwolf Consultant and Stock or Equity; Atentiv Consultant; Axsome Consultant; Genomind Consultant and Stock or Equity; Ironshore Consultant, Stock or Equity and Travel Support for participation in another meeting; Corium Consultant; Sky Therapeutics Consultant and Stock or Equity; Medice Consultant; Noven Consultant and Research Support; Rhodes Consultant; Tris Consultant and Speakers Bureau; Vallon Consultant and Research Support; Shire Takeda Grant Support; Sandoz Research Support and Speakers Bureau; Akili Stock or Equity; Elsevier Royalties; Guilford Press Royalties; Oxford University Press Royalties.

BK declared that they were an editorial board member of Frontiers at the time of submission. This had no impact on the peer review process and the final decision.

The remaining authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher's note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

Supplementary material

The Supplementary Material for this article can be found online at: https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/frcha.2023.1188901/full#supplementary-material

References

1. Barkley RA. Distinguishing sluggish cognitive tempo from attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder in adults. J Abnorm Psychol. (2012) 121(4):978–90. doi: 10.1037/a0023961

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

2. Becker SP, Leopold DR, Burns GL, Jarrett MA, Langberg JM, Marshall SA, et al. The internal, external, and diagnostic validity of sluggish cognitive tempo: a meta-analysis and critical review. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. (2016) 55(3):163–78. doi: 10.1016/j.jaac.2015.12.006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

3. Barkley RA. Barkley sluggish cognitive tempo scale—children and adolescents (BSCTS-CA). New York: Guilford Publications (2018). p. 120.

4. Becker SP, Willcutt EG, Leopold DR, Fredrick JW, Smith ZR, Jacobson LA, et al. Report of a work group on sluggish cognitive tempo: key research directions and a consensus change in terminology to cognitive disengagement syndrome. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. (2022) 62(6):629–45. doi: 10.1016/j.jaac.2022.07.821

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

5. Barkley RA. Distinguishing sluggish cognitive tempo from ADHD in children and adolescents: executive functioning, impairment, and comorbidity. J Clin Child Adolesc Psychol. (2013) 42(2):161–73. doi: 10.1080/15374416.2012.734259

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

6. Becker SP, Barkley RA. Sluggish cognitive tempo. In: Banaschewski T, Coghill D, Zuddas A, editors. Oxford textbook of attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. Oxford, United Kingdom: Oxford University Press (2018). p. 147–53.

7. Lunsford-Avery JR, Kollins SH, Mitchell JT. Sluggish cognitive tempo in adults referred for an ADHD evaluation: a psychometric analysis of self- and collateral report. J Atten Disord. (2021) 25(3):322–31. doi: 10.1177/1087054718787894

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

8. Sergeant J. The cognitive-energetic model: an empirical approach to attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. (2000) 24(1):7–12. doi: 10.1016/S0149-7634(99)00060-3

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

9. Wood WLM, Lewandowski LJ, Lovett BJ, Antshel KM. Executive dysfunction and functional impairment associated with sluggish cognitive tempo in emerging adulthood. J Atten Disord. (2017) 21(8):691–700. doi: 10.1177/1087054714560822

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

10. Jarrett MA, Rapport HF, Rondon AT, Becker SP. ADHD Dimensions and sluggish cognitive tempo symptoms in relation to self-report and laboratory measures of neuropsychological functioning in college students. J Atten Disord. (2017) 21(8):673–83. doi: 10.1177/1087054714560821

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

11. Leikauf JE. Corrigendum to sluggish cognitive tempo, internalizing symptoms, and executive function in adults with ADHD. J Atten Disord. (2020) 24(10):1472. doi: 10.1177/1087054720927868

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

12. Becker SP, Burns GL, Garner AA, Jarrett MA, Luebbe AM, Epstein JN, et al. Sluggish cognitive tempo in adults: psychometric validation of the adult concentration inventory. Psychol Assess. (2018) 30(3):296–310. doi: 10.1037/pas0000476

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

13. Fredrick JW, Burns GL, Langberg JM, Becker SP. Examining the structural and external validity of the adult concentration inventory for assessing sluggish cognitive tempo in adults. Assessment. (2022) 29(8):1742–55. doi: 10.1177/10731911211027224

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

14. Gaur S, Pallanti S. Treatment outcomes in an adult attention deficit hyperactivity disorder clinic with a focus on executive functioning and sluggish cognitive tempo. Cureus. (2020) 12(8):e9814. doi: 10.7759/cureus.981

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

15. Silverstein MJ, Leon TL, Krone B, Faraone SV, Newcorn JH, Adler LA. The characteristics and unique impairments of comorbid adult ADHD and sluggish cognitive tempo: an interim analysis. Psychiatr Ann. (2019) 49(10):457–65. doi: 10.3928/00485713-20190905-01

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

16. Adler LA, Leon TL, Sardoff TM, Krone B, Faraone SV, Silverstein MJ, et al. A placebo-controlled trial of lisdexamfetamine in the treatment of comorbid sluggish cognitive tempo and adult ADHD. J Clin Psychiatry. (2021) 82(4):20m13687. doi: 10.4088/JCP.20m13687

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

17. Posner K, Brown GK, Stanley B, Brent DA, Yershova KV, Oquendo MA, et al. The Columbia-suicide severity rating scale: initial validity and internal consistency findings from three multisite studies with adolescents and adults. Am J Psychiatry. (2011) 168(12):1266–77. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2011.10111704

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

18. Adler LA, Spencer T, Faraone SV, Reimherr FW, Kelsey D, Michelson D, et al. Training raters to assess adult ADHD: reliability of ratings. J Atten Disord. (2005) 8(3):121–6. doi: 10.1177/1087054705277168

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

19. Kessler RC, Adler L, Ames M, Demler O, Faraone S, Hiripi E, et al. The world health organization adult ADHD self-report scale (ASRS): a short screening scale for use in the general population. Psychol Med. (2005) 35(2):245–56. doi: 10.1017/S0033291704002892

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

20. Spencer TJ, Adler LA, Meihua Q, Saylor KE, Brown TE, Holdnack JA, et al. Validation of the adult ADHD investigator symptom rating scale (AISRS). J Atten Disord. (2010) 14(1):57–68. doi: 10.1177/1087054709347435

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

21. Silverstein MJ, Faraone SV, Alperin S, Leon TL, Biederman J, Spencer TJ, et al. Validation of the expanded versions of the adult ADHD self-report scale v1.1 symptom checklist and the adult ADHD investigator symptom rating scale. J Atten Disord. (2019) 23(10):1101–10. doi: 10.1177/1087054718756198

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

22. Roth RM, Isquith PK, Gioia GA. Assessment of executive functioning using the behavior rating inventory of executive function (BRIEF). In: Goldstein S, Naglieri JA, editors. Handbook of executive functioning. New York: Springer New York (2014). p. 301–31.

23. Roth RM, Lance CE, Isquith PK, Fischer AS, Giancola PR. Confirmatory factor analysis of the behavior rating inventory of executive function-adult version in healthy adults and application to attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Arch Clin Neuropsychol. (2013) 28(5):425–34. doi: 10.1093/arclin/act031

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

24. Sheehan DV, Lecrubier Y, Harnett Sheehan K, Janavs J, Weiller E, Keskiner A, et al. The validity of the Mini International Neuropsychiatric Interview (MINI) according to the SCID-P and its reliability. Eur Psychiatry. (1997) 12(5):232–41. doi: 10.1016/S0924-9338(97)83297-X

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

25. Barkley RA. Barkley functional impairment scale (BFIS). New York: Guilford Press (2011).

26. Berk M, Ng F, Dodd S, Callaly T, Campbell S, Bernardo M, et al. The validity of the CGI severity and improvement scales as measures of clinical effectiveness suitable for routine clinical use. J Eval Clin Pract. (2008) 14(6):979–83. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2753.2007.00921.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

27. Wechsler D. Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale–Fourth Edition (WAIS-IV) [Database record]. APA PsycTests (2008). doi: 10.1037/t15169-000

28. Robbins TW, James M, Owen AM, Sahakian BJ, McInnes L, Rabbitt P. Cambridge neuropsychological test automated battery (CANTAB): a factor analytic study of a large sample of normal elderly volunteers. Dementia. (1994) 5(5):266–81. doi: 10.1159/000106735

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

29. Robbins TW, James M, Owen AM, Sahakian BJ, Lawrence AD, McInnes L, et al. A study of performance on tests from the CANTAB battery sensitive to frontal lobe dysfunction in a large sample of normal volunteers: implications for theories of executive functioning and cognitive aging. Cambridge neuropsychological test automated battery. J Int Neuropsychol Soc. (1998) 4(5):474–90. doi: 10.1017/S1355617798455073

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

30. Fried R, DiSalvo M, Kelberman C, Biederman J. Can the CANTAB identify adults with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder? A controlled study. Appl Neuropsychol Adult. (2021) 28(3):318–27. doi: 10.1080/23279095.2019.1633328

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

31. Costello AB, Osborne J. Best practices in exploratory factor analysis: four recommendations for getting the most from your analysis. Pract Assess Res Evaluation. (2005) 10(1):7. doi: 10.7275/jyj1-4868

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder (ADHD), Sluggish Cognitive Tempo (SCT), Cognitive Disengagement Syndrome (CDS), Adult ADHD, Executive Function, Neurocognitive Phenotyping, Cambridge Neuropsychological Test Automated Battery (CANTAB), Barkley Adult SCT Scale (BAARS)

Citation: Krone B, Adler LA, Anbarasan D, Leon T, Gallagher R, Patel P, Faraone SV and Newcorn JH (2023) Characteristics of Sluggish Cognitive Tempo among adults with ADHD: objective neurocognitive measures align with self-report of executive function. Front. Child Adolesc. Psychiatry 2:1188901. doi: 10.3389/frcha.2023.1188901

Received: 18 March 2023; Accepted: 26 June 2023;
Published: 24 July 2023.

Edited by:

Sara Calderoni, Stella Maris Foundation (IRCCS), Italy

Reviewed by:

Antonella Gagliano, Magna Græcia University, Italy
Peter K. Isquith, Dartmouth College, United States
Scott Decker, University of South Carolina, United States

© 2023 Krone, Adler, Anbarasan, Leon, Gallagher, Patel, Faraone and Newcorn. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Beth Krone beth.krone@mssm.edu

These authors share first authorship

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.