- 1Dipartimento di Scienze Cognitive, Psicologiche, Pedagogiche e Degli Studi Culturali, Università di Messina, Messina, Italy
- 2Dipartimento di Filosofia e Comunicazione, Università di Bologna, Bologna, Italy
- 3Dipartimento di Medicina e Clinica Sperimentale, Università degli Studi di Messina, A.O.U. “G. Martino”, Via Consolare Valeria, Messina, Italy
Religious beliefs can shape how people process fear. Yet the psychophysiological mechanisms underlying this phenomenon remain poorly understood. We investigated fear learning and extinction processes in a group of individuals who professed a belief in God, compared to non-believers. Using a virtual reality Pavlovian fear conditioning/extinction task, we measured neurovegetative activity associated with these forms of associative learning. Our finding shows reduced fear extinction among God believers, compared to non-believers. This suggests that the general mechanism of fear extinction learning is suppressed in these individuals. Importantly, this effect was not explained by state or trait anxiety scores. These findings align with previous evidence linking religiosity and spirituality with the neural circuit of fear and suggest that religiosity may be associated with weaker inhibitory learning processes related to fear.
Introduction
Beliefs are mental representations that describe causal relationships between events or stimuli. One of the most influential and widely held beliefs worldwide is the belief in the existence of God. According to the buffering theory, belief in God and, therefore, with possibility of an afterlife helps alleviate the fear of death, by offering reassurance that death does not mark the end of conscious experiences (1, 2). This is particularly evident in Christian theology, where death takes on a series of positive connotations, such as a transition to eternal life and resurrection. Such beliefs provide comfort and assurance, helping individuals cope with uncertainties of life and the mysteries surrounding death. Moreover, belief in God can provide a sense of security and comprehension when confronting fears rooted in the uncertainties of existence.
Religiosity often serves as a source of comfort and a means of coping with fear and anxiety (3), suggesting that belief in God may act as a mechanism to mitigate fear. However, this belief can also function as a catalyst for fear. In many religious traditions, the concept of “fear of God” is frequently emphasized. For some, religious motivation stems from the fear of “divine punishment” or the consequences of failing to adhere to religious laws. Fear of hell, damnation, or negative judgment in the afterlife can be a powerful motivator for compliance with religious teachings. Additionally, religiosity may be linked to a fear of sin, which in turn can contribute to heightened stress. This connection is supported by Winterowd et al. (4) who found a positive relationship between perceived stress and spirituality, as measured with a standard scale.
In summary, the relationship between belief in God and fear is complex and multifaceted, functioning both as a source of comfort in fearful situations and as a potential amplifier of fear. Considering the significant role of fear in various psychopathological conditions such as, phobias, obsessive compulsive disorders, eating disorders and posttraumatic stress disorder [for a review see (5–7)], further research is crucial. Investigating this topic in greater depth will provide valuable insights into the psychophysiological mechanisms underlying this relationship, contributing to a more comprehensive understanding of its implications for mental health.
We contribute to this discussion by providing new insights using the Pavlovian fear conditioning/extinction task (8), a widely recognized and well-established protocol for experimentally investigating fear processing (9, 54). This paradigm is relevant for studying the link between religiosity and fear processing, as it allows for the measurement of autonomic response system (neurovegetative activity), which reflect the physiological response to stress, during two distinct types of associative—fear related—learning. The conditioning phase involves associating a neutral (e.g., visual) stimulus (CS+) with an aversive event (unconditioned stimulus—US), thereby is conditioning the stimulus to be perceived as fearful. The extinction phase, refers to a process through which the conditioned fear response diminishes or disappears over time when the feared stimulus is repeatedly presented without the aversive outcome that originally triggered the fear response [see also (10)].
If successful, individuals no longer exhibit the previously learned conditioned response following the extinction session. Interestingly, the relevance of the Pavlovian fear conditioning/extinction protocol in the study of the link fear/religiosity is further highlighted by a recent study by Ferguson et al. (11). This research documented a specific relation between religiosity/spirituality and a specific brain map focused on the Periaqueductal Gray (PAG), a brainstem region known for its role in fear conditioning (12).
The relevance of the PAG in fear processing has also been documented by Watson et al. (13), who identified extinction-susceptible and extinction-resistant cells within this area, believed to play a role to the persistence of fear memories following extinction. Prospectively, two scenarios are equally possible. On one hand, evidence suggesting that religiosity enhances coping strategies (3), a critical component of exposure therapy (3), and a factor known to promote fear extinction [e.g., (14)], leads us to hypothesize reduced fear conditioning (or improved fear extinction learning) in individuals who believe in God compared to those who do not (controls). On the other hand, consistent with in line with neural evidence linking religiosity with the fear conditioning/extinction brain network (11), we predict an amplified fear conditioning response (and/or reduced fear extinction learning) in individuals who believe in God compared to controls.
This latter scenario also aligns with evidence showing a link between religiosity and perceived threat (50), and results by Bullock et al. (15) indicating that religiosity may amplify perceived threat under certain circumstances. Given the equal plausibility of these scenarios, this study provides an exploratory investigation into this field. Additionally, considering the importance of anxiety in the Conditioning, generalization, and persistence of conditioned fear (9, 59), we also explored the role of anxiety in the fear conditioning of God believers. Religious belief has shown complex associations with anxiety [for a review, see (16)], which is itself a critical factor in fear conditioning (9).
Methods
Fifty-three participants (26 Catholic God believers, 12 males, and 27 non-God believers, 12 males) were examined with a Pavlovian fear conditioning protocols used in previous investigations (17, 56). The sample size was established through a priori power analysis using G*Power. Assuming a conservative scenario, the analysis indicated a required total sample size of ~54 participants (27 per group) considering the study design including two groups (God believers, no believers), 2 stimuli (CS+, CS-) and 3 conditions (habituation, Conditioning, extinction), with measurements taken within subjects, to detect a medium effect size (f = 0.25) with an alpha level of 0.05 and a desired power level of 0.80. All participants were students recruited from the University of Messina. All individuals were white Caucasian, and without a history of mental disease, according to their self-assessment. Like previous investigations in the field (18) participants provided information about their belief in God by simply responding “yes” or “no” to the question: “Do you believe in the existence of God?” They also clarified to be Catholics.
Written informed consent was obtained from all participants before inclusion, and the protocol was approved by the local ethics committee of the Department of Cognitive, Psychological, Educational, and Cultural Studies (Approval n. COSPECS_4_2021; COSPECS_07_2022), University of Messina, Italy. The experimental procedures were conducted according to the Declaration of Helsinki principles and subsequent updated versions (19). These data are secondary to a main project exploring the link between ideological thinking and fear processing. Descriptive details are provided in the Table 1.
Table 1. Descriptive statistics of the variables of interest for individuals who believe and do not believe in God.
Instruments
State-trait anxiety inventory
To examine the relationships between anxiety and fear conditioning and extinction, we employed both the state (STAI-Y1) and trait (STAI-Y2) anxiety subscales of the State-Trait Anxiety Inventory [STAI; (20)]. We used the Italian version of the STAI, which was created by Pedrabissi and Santinello (21). This inventory comprises 20 items for assessing state anxiety and 20 items for assessing trait anxiety. Participants responded to each item on a 4-point scale ranging from 1 (“Almost Never”) to 4 (“Almost Always”). The total score for each subscale ranges from 20 to 80 points. An illustrative item from the Italian version is: “mi sento bene” (I feel well). The internal consistency for the state and trait anxiety subscales is robust, with Cronbach's alpha coefficients reported between 0.91–0.95 and 0.85–0.90, respectively.
Virtual reality Pavlovian fear conditioning/extinction task
To study fear conditioning and extinction, we used a Pavlovian fear conditioning protocol previously created by our group (22). This task improves upon traditional non-VR-based paradigms by addressing key limitations, such as inconsistencies in eliciting universal fear responses and challenges with stimulus calibration. By utilizing a simple, controlled environment, the protocol minimizes external influences and ensures precise trial replication, effectively addressing concerns tied to the “replication crisis” (23). Furthermore, unlike classical paradigms that rely on electrical shocks, the VR-based approach eliminates discomfort and enhances ecological validity by examining fear responses in realistic scenarios. These advancements collectively enhance the accuracy, applicability, and interpretability of fear conditioning studies [for further insights on this topic refer to Lucifora et al. and Nucera (24, 25)]. Our experimental setup used the Unity engine 3D and the Oculus Rift, a VR headset equipped with two Pentile OLED displays, each with a resolution of 1,080 × 1,200 pixels, a 90 Hz refresh rate, and a 110° field of view. The device also includes position tracking, rotation capabilities, and integrated headphones that provide a 3D sound effect (26, 27). We used a graphics workstation with an NVIDIA Titan X graphics card to run simulation, ensuring consistent high-resolution rendering of the virtual environment projected to the VR headset.
Our VR paradigm, previously utilized successfully in our group's research [e.g., (17, 57)] comprised three sessions: habituation, Conditioning, and extinction. The protocol involved two differently colored doors as stimuli. During the habituation session (duration about 4 min), the participants viewed the two different doors (blue and red) in a total of eight trials (i.e., four trials for each door) presented in a randomized order. Each door was individually displayed for 12 s in a random order; the door remained closed for the first 9 s after it appeared and then opened for the following 3 s. No stimuli were presented while the door was open in this phase. The inter-trial interval varied between 6 and 20 s.
After a 60-s break, participants proceeded to fear conditioning stage (~10 min) when they were ready. Each door was presented for a total of 12 s per trial. During the first 9 s, the door remained closed. In this session, the blue color served as the conditioning stimulus (CS+). It was paired with a threatening stimulus (a monster) in 80% of trials (eight out of 10), acting as the unconditioned stimulus (US). The US appeared during the final 3 s of the trial, immediately after the door opened. The monster then jumped toward the participant and screamed (80 dB) to elicit a fear response. The red door, in contrast, was not paired with the US after the door opened and served as the safety signal (CS-; Figure 1). The conditioning session comprised two blocks: early and late Conditioning lasting ~10 min in total.
Figure 1. A screenshot from the conditioning phase. The upper panels depict the CS– (red door), while the lower panels show the CS+ paired with the US (blue door with the fearful stimulus). Each door remains closed for 9 s and opens for 3 s during the task.
The extinction phase, aimed at extinguishing the conditioned fear response. In this phase, the doors were again presented for a total of 12 s per trial, following the same structure: 9 s with the door closed and 3 s with the door open. The blue door (CS+), when open, was no longer paired with the US (i.e., the jumping and screaming monster), ensuring the CS+ (closed blue door) was dissociated from it over repeated trials. The extinction session comprised two blocks: early and late extinction lasting ~10 min in total. This session included 20 trials with the blue door and the red door each, presented in randomized order, similar to the conditioning phase. For conditioning and extinction phases, the closed-door represents the CS, while the open-door serves as the outcome. Both phases together create the temporal structure necessary for effective Pavlovian conditioning.
Skin conductance response
During the task, skin conductance response (SCR) was measured using the eSense device (Mindfield Biosystems, Inc. Berlin. Germany) connected to a MEIZU M5C M710H. Electrodes were attached to the middle and index fingers with Velcro straps and connected to the device via the audio connection input. The eSense recorded data at a sampling rate of 5 Hz, which were then exported as csv files via email. SCR for the CS+ and CS– was calculated in microSiemens (μS) by subtracting the mean SCL during the 2 s prior to stimulus onset from the maximum SCL during the 12 s of stimulus presentation (3 s with the door closed and 9 s with the door open). Data extraction was performed using a MATLAB script developed in our lab specifically for this purpose.
Fear stimulus rating
At the end of each session, participants were asked to rate the perceived level of fear associated with the presented stimuli (i.e., the red and the blue doors). This fear stimulus rating (FSR) was assessed using a 10-point Likert scale, with 1 represented “not scary at all” and 10 indicated “extremely scary.”
Procedure
Timeline of the experimental procedure
After participants provided informed consent, they completed the STAI-Y1, the STAI-Y2 questionnaires. They were then connected to the GSR Amp (eSense), with two ring-shaped skin conductance electrodes placed over the middle and index fingers of the right hand to measure SCR. Next, the VR helmet (Oculus Rift) was placed on the head, and the fear conditioning/extinction task was carried out. During the experimental sessions, we also collected fear stimulus ratings (FSR) to assess/monitor contingency awareness. At the conclusion of the experiment participants were debriefed and asked to indicate their belief in the existence of God by responding with either “yes, I believe in the existence of God” or “no, I do not believe in the existence of God.”
Data analysis
Statistical analyses were conducted to test the hypothesis of a difference in fear conditioning and extinction learning between individuals stating to believe and individuals not believing in God. The SCR amplitude was determined off-line by subtracting baseline SCR (measured 2 s prior to CS presentation) from the peak skin conductance level during each CS presentation. This calculation was performed individually for each participant. To analyze the SCR data a square root transformation was applied to reduce variability, following the methodology used in previous studies of our group [e.g., (17, 28, 56)].
Separate ANOVAs involving SCR as dependent variable were conducted for each session (habituation, Conditioning, extinction) in line with previous investigations in the field [e.g., (56)]. Data were analyzed using separate mixed-model ANOVAs with a 2 (Groups: God believers, not God believers) × 2 (Stimuli: CS+, CS-) × 2 (Block: early, late) x Trials (the number of trials varied between stages) design. FSR were analyzed using a mixed model ANOVA with a 2 (Groups: God believers, not God believers) × 3 (Sessions: habituation, Conditioning, extinction) x 2 (Stimuli: CS+, CS-) factorial design. In cases of significant ANOVA results, post-hoc comparisons were conducted using Bonferroni-corrected t-tests to control for Type I errors (false positives). Effect sizes for the mixed-model ANOVAs are reported as partial-eta squared (). Correlation analyses, either parametric or non-parametric, depending on the data distribution, were performed to investigate associations between the variables of interest (STAI, SCR, FSR) for each session separately. For the correlation analyses involving SCR and FSR, we calculated the differential mean (CS+ minus CS-) for each condition and session. A significant threshold of α= 0.05 was applied. Statistical analyses were performed using STATISTICA (StatSoft. Inc., Tulsa, OK, USA) version 7.0.
Results
Demographics
No significant difference was found for gender (X2 = 0.074, p = 0.784), age [t(52) = 1.322, p = 0.192], STAI-Y1 [t(52) = 0.721, p = 0.473] and STAI-Y2 [t(52) = 0.199, p = 0.842] when comparing individuals who believe and non-believe in God.
Skin conductance response
Habituation
A significant main effect of the factor trials was found [F(3, 156) = 17.51, p < 0.001 ηp2 = 0.252]. No further significant results were identified for habituation (refer to Supplementary Table 1 for details).
Conditioning
We identified a significant main effect of the factor CS [F(1, 52) =52.35, p < 0.001 ηp2 = 0.252], with higher score for the CS+ condition (M = 0.387, SE = 0.032) compared to CS- (M = 0.210, SE = 0.018. More details on this pattern of results are provided in Figure 2). Furthermore, the main effect of the factor Block was significant [F(1, 52) = 13.91, p < 0.001 ηp2 = 0.211], with higher scores in the early (M = 0.325, SE = 0.024) compared to the late block (M = 0.272, SE = 0.023). No further significant results were identified for Conditioning (refer to Supplementary Table 2 for details).
Figure 2. The mean SCR for CS+ and CS- during habituation, conditioning, and extinction sessions is shown for participants who state they believe in God and those who do not. An asterisk (*) indicates significant results. Vertical bars denote the standard error of the means.
Extinction
The factor Group x Stimulus was significant [F(1, 52) = 4.58, p = 0.036, ηp2 = 0.081]. Post-hoc comparison shows a significant difference between CS+ (M = 0.172, SE = 0.020) and CS- (M = 0.124, SE = 0.017) in God believers (p = 0.041, Figure 2). On the other hand, no significant difference was reported for no God believers between CS+ and CS- (p = 1.000). No further significant results were identified for extinction (refer to Supplementary Table 4 for details).
Finally, no correlations were found for both groups between State and Trait anxiety with the mean differential SCRs associated to the three sessions (i.e., habituation, conditioning, and extinction). See Table 2 for the details.
Table 2. Spearman correlation rank outputs between STAI scores and SCR scores across the three sessions of the Pavlovian conditioning task for the two groups of participants.
Fear stimulus ratings
A significant main effect of the factor Session was found [F(2, 104) = 13.29, p < 0.001 ηp2 = 0.203]. Post-hoc comparisons show a significant difference (p < 0.001) between Habituation (M = 4.185, SE = 0.314) and Conditioning (M = 5.407, SE = 0.226), as well as (p < 0.001) between Conditioning and Extinction (M = 4.481, SE = 0.337). Furthermore, the main effect of the factor Stimulus was significant [F(1, 52) = 21.91, p < 0.001 ηp2 = 0.296], with an higher score (M = 5.209, SE = 0.279) in response to the CS+ compared to the CS- (M = 4.172, SE = 0.307). Moreover, a significant Stimulus x Session interaction was revealed [F(2, 104) = 22.70, p < 0.001 ηp2 = 0.303]. Post-hoc comparisons document a significant difference between CS+ (M = 6.555, SE = 0.305) and CS- (M = 4.259, SE = 0.357) in the Conditioning session (p < 0.001). Additionally, a significant difference was found between CS+ (M = 4.963, SE = 0.363) and CS- (M = 4.00, SE = 0.348) and CS- for the extinction session (p = 0.004). No significant difference was found between CS+ and CS- was found for the habituation session (p = 1.000). No further significant results were found (see Supplementary Table 5 for details). Finally, no correlations were found for both groups between State and Trait anxiety with the mean differential FSR associated to the three sessions (i.e., habituation, conditioning, and extinction). See Table 3 for the details.
Table 3. Spearman correlation rank outputs between STAI scores and FSR scores across the three sessions of the Pavlovian conditioning task for the two groups of participants.
Discussion
In this study, we aimed to investigate whether belief in the existence of God can affect the ability to process fear. We employed a VR-version of Pavlovian fear conditioning paradigm, a widely used translational protocol for the experimental study of mechanisms that underlie pathological fear and anxiety (9, 28–30). A strength of our protocol is that, unlike physical stimuli such as electrical shocks, VR-based stimuli offer the advantage of not requiring individual calibration to accommodate participants' physical differences. This allows researchers to investigate fear responses without inducing discomfort, thereby improving the reliability and interpretability of the findings. In contrast, paradigms employing electrical shocks as the US often face challenges due to variability in physical discomfort sensitivity among participants. Our protocol avoids these limitations by eliminating physical discomfort-inducing stimuli altogether [see also (22) for a discussion]. Moreover, the lower volume of the screaming voice [i.e., 80 dB instead of the traditional 95 dB; see (31)] reduces the aversive impact of the loud noise itself. This demonstrates that the VR paradigm can effectively induce fear conditioning even without traditional unconditioned stimuli (US), such as electrical shocks or loud audio stimuli, as used in Shechner et al. (31). Finally, the SCR pattern in the current study can be interpreted as a “fear response per se,” as the persistence of higher SCR to the CS+ (even when the US is omitted) is indicative of fear memory and challenges in inhibitory learning, not just heightened arousal.
Our findings suggest a reduced capacity to extinguish fear in individuals who believe in God compared to non-believers. This conclusion is supported by the higher SCR to the CS+ compared to CS- in God believers during the extinction session. In contrast, no group differences were observed in SCR during the fear conditioning session. This indicates that both groups effectively acquired the association between the CS+ and the US. Moreover, our study found no significant differences in state or trait anxiety between the two groups, nor any correlation between these anxiety measures and the implicit (SCR) and explicit (FSR) measures of fear conditioning. These results suggest that the observed group differences are independent of participants' trait or state anxiety levels. Evidence suggests that anxiety is psychologically and neuronally distinct from fear processing (32, 33), although some overlap between the two has been documented (32–34). This suggests that belief in the existence of God may specifically involve the neural circuitry associated with fear (11), with only minimal involvement of the neural network responsible for anxiety (33). Additionally, no group difference was found in explicit measures (i.e., FSR), with both groups showing higher ratings for the CS+ compared to CS- in both the Conditioning and extinction sessions. This indicates that the influence of belief in God on fear conditioning appears to be limited to the neurovegetative component, rather than affecting conscious, explicit fear responses.
In a Pavlovian fear conditioning task, a neutral stimulus (the CS) is repeatedly paired with an aversive stimulus, such as a shock (the US). Over time, the individual learns to associate the CS with the US, leading to a fear response (such as an increased SCR), whenever the CS is presented. During the extinction phase, the individual is exposed to the CS without the US, indicating that the CS no longer predicts the US. If the fear response persists despite repeated presentations of the CS without the US, it suggests that the fear remains, indicating that the association has not been fully extinguished. The preservation of the neurovegetative fear response in the extinction session could be interpreted as a challenge for God believers in relearning inhibitory process that is expected to occur during fear extinction (35).
Difficulties in inhibiting fear memory traces and in extinguishing fear have been linked to poor stress management (36). As anticipated, some evidence identifies religiosity and spirituality as valuable coping resource (37). However, the poor stress management hypothesis also aligns with studies suggesting that religiosity and spirituality can amplify perceived threat and stress (4, 52).
Although we did not directly measure the degree of participants' religiosity, which is a limitation, our results are line with the interpretation proposed by Winterowd et al. (4) and by Carlozzi et al. (52). Specifically, the failure in fear extinction observed in God believing group supports the hypothesis of a heightened perceived threat and stress in these individuals [for a comprehensive review on the detrimental effects of stress on fear extinction learning see also Maren (38)]. Moreover, our results are aligned with the recent result by Ferguson et al. (11) linking religiosity and spirituality with the neural circuit of fear conditioning and extinction learning (13), and with evidence that religiosity increases under threat (39). Finally, the current results may reflect differences in the cognitive profiles of the two groups of participants. For example, Lindeman and Lipsanen (53) found lower analytical thinking in religious, compared to non-religious individuals. However, to the best of our knowledge, there is no evidence associating this cognitive skill with fear extinction learning. Nonetheless, it might be plausible to expect that a person who can think analytically might be better able to understand and predict when a fear-inducing stimulus is no longer present, potentially aiding in the fear extinction process.
Given that the difficulty in extinguishing fear can be a risk and/or predisposing factor in the genesis of several psychopathological conditions such as post-traumatic stress disorder (40) and anxiety (51), the current results suggest a potentially detrimental role of religiosity in the context of these conditions. This implies that religiosity could be a relevant variable affecting the process of fear recovery following traumatic experiences. While existing literature highlights the potential benefits of religiosity in alleviating mental health disorders (41), particularly by providing emotional support and coping mechanisms, there is also evidence suggesting that certain religious ideologies may be linked to mental distress (42). Our findings align more closely with the latter perspective, indicating that religiosity could pose challenges for exposure-based therapies. Given these mixed outcomes, a systematic investigation that not only explores the potential benefits and drawbacks of religiosity but also considers the differences across various religious traditions is crucial for advancing our understanding in this area. Such research would help identify specific factors that might influence how religiosity interacts with therapeutic approaches.
Based on these findings, future investigations could build upon this study by exploring the connection between fear conditioning and religiosity using questionnaires-based methods. For instance, it would be relevant incorporating widely accepted belief-in-God scales, which reflect a spectrum from atheism to theism [e.g., (43, 58)]. Including these scales would provide a test for the curvilinear relationship between religiosity and fear processing, a well-established finding in relation to mental wellbeing (44) and prejudice (45). Furthermore, it would be intriguing to investigate potential differences between various types of religions, as fear can be perceived both as a source of comfort in fearful situations and as a potential enhancer of fear, and a limitation of our study is our focus on Catholic believers. In this regard, it would be interesting to explore the extent to which the current results apply to individuals of other religious affiliations and to the various facets of religiosity, including fear of crime (46), fear of death, and fear of the unknown. Moreover, it would be intriguing to explicitly investigate the tendency to forgiving, which is encouraged in Catholic spirituality, as the current study reports a difficulty of God believers in extinguishing fear, which might reflect interaction between religious beliefs and existential fears.
Since our participants were tested using a Pavlovian fear conditioning protocol that did not include stimuli and conditions explicitly relevant to religiosity, future investigations could incorporate these aspects into the protocols. This would help assess the generalizability of these results within the context of religiosity. Moreover, it would be of interest extending the current investigation by including the extinction recall and fear renewal phases of this task and including neuroimaging methods [e.g., see (47)] to outline respective neural correlates. Adding extinction recall would allow for assessing whether such difference in fear extinction between God believers and non-believers is maintained over time. This could reinforce the conclusion that belief in God is associated with sustained fear responses, highlighting its implications for exposure-based therapies. Moreover, adding fear renewal phases would provide valuable insights into the context dependency and persistence of fear extinction differences between the two examined groups of participants. This would provide a more comprehensive picture of the stability and context sensitivity of these differences over time.
Finally, exploring variables related to personality traits, emotion regulation, alexithymia, coping styles, or cultural factors could provide additional insights, given their relevance to risk taking behavior (48, 55), fear conditioning (17), mood (60), and ideological thought (49). Examining these factors could deepen our understanding of the psychophysiological mechanisms underlying the complex relationship between religiosity and fear processing.
Data availability statement
The data analyzed in this study is subject to the following licenses/restrictions: Data will be provided following motivated request. Requests to access these datasets should be directed to Y3ZpY2FyaW9AdW5pbWUuaXQ=.
Ethics statement
The studies involving humans were approved by Internal Ethics Committee, COSPECS Department, University of Messina. The studies were conducted in accordance with the local legislation and institutional requirements. The participants provided their written informed consent to participate in this study.
Author contributions
CV: Conceptualization, Data curation, Formal analysis, Funding acquisition, Methodology, Software, Supervision, Validation, Visualization, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing. LC: Data curation, Project administration, Writing – review & editing. CL: Conceptualization, Supervision, Writing – review & editing. FF: Data curation, Formal analysis, Methodology, Writing – review & editing. SM: Supervision, Validation, Visualization, Writing – review & editing. GM: Supervision, Validation, Writing – review & editing. FT: Supervision, Validation, Visualization, Writing – review & editing. AF: Supervision, Validation, Writing – review & editing, Visualization.
Funding
The author(s) declare financial support was received for the research, authorship, and/or publication of this article. The current work was supported by Ministero Istruzione Università e Ricerca (PRIN 2022, NextGenerationEU. Project code: 2022L3AALJ).
Conflict of interest
The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.
Generative AI statement
The author(s) declare that no Gen AI was used in the creation of this manuscript.
Publisher's note
All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.
Supplementary material
The Supplementary Material for this article can be found online at: https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fpubh.2024.1509388/full#supplementary-material
References
1. Rose BM, O' Sullivan M. Afterlife beliefs and death anxiety: an exploration of the relationship between afterlife expectations and fear of death in an undergraduate population. Omega J Death Dying. (2002) 45:229–43. doi: 10.2190/RV40-6NBR-66GF-UJMU
2. Wink P, Scott J. Does religiousness buffer against the fear of death and dying in late adulthood? findings from a longitudinal study. J Gerontol. (2005) 60B:207–14. doi: 10.1093/geronb/60.4.P207
3. Sen HE, Colucci L, Browne DT. Keeping the faith: religion, positive coping, and mental health of caregivers during COVID-19. Front Psychol. (2022) 12:805019. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2021.805019
4. Winterowd C, Harrist S, Thomason N, Worth S, Carlozzi B. The relationship of spiritual beliefs and involvement with the experience of anger and stress in college students. J Coll Stud Dev. (2005) 46:515–29. doi: 10.1353/csd.2005.0057
5. Vicario CM. Altered insula response to sweet taste processing in recovered anorexia and bulimia nervosa: a matter of disgust sensitivity? Am J Psychiatry. (2013) 170:1497. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2013.13060748
6. Culicetto L, Ferraioli F, Lucifora C, Falzone A, Martino G, Craparo G, et al. Disgust as a transdiagnostic index of mental illness: a narrative review of clinical populations. Bullet Menninger Clinic. (2023) 87(Suppl.A):53–91. doi: 10.1521/bumc.2023.87.suppA.53
7. Ferraioli F, Culicetto L, Cecchetti L, Falzone A, Tomaiuolo F, Quartarone A, et al. Virtual reality exposure therapy for treating fear of contamination disorders: a systematic review of healthy and clinical populations. Brain Sci. (2024) 14:510. doi: 10.3390/brainsci14050510
8. Pavlov I. Conditioned reflexes: an investigation of the physiological activity of the cerebral cortex. Ann Neurosci. (1927) 17:136–41. doi: 10.5214/ans.0972-7531.1017309
9. Vicario CM, Salehinejad MA, Felmingham K, Martino G, Nitsche MA. A systematic review on the therapeutic effectiveness of non-invasive brain stimulation for the treatment of anxiety disorders. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. (2019) 96:219–31 doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2018.12.012
10. Lonsdorf TB, Menz MM, Andreatta M, Merz CJ. Don't fear 'fear conditioning': methodological considerations for the design and analysis of studies on human fear conditioning, extinction, and return of fear. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. (2017) 77:247–85. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2017.02.026
11. Ferguson MA, Schaper FLWVJ, Cohen A, Siddiqi S, Merrill SM, Nielsen JA, et al. A neural circuit for spirituality and religiosity derived from patients with brain lesions. Biol Psychiatry. (2022) 91:380–8. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2021.06.016
12. Kim JJ, Rison RA, Fanselow MS. Effects of amygdala, hippocampus, and periaqueductal gray lesions on short- and long-term contextual fear. Behav Neurosci. (1993) 107:1093–8. doi: 10.1037/0735-7044.107.6.1093
13. Watson TC, Cerminara NL, Lumb BM, Apps R. Neural correlates of fear in the periaqueductal gray. J. Neurosci. (2016) 36:12707–19. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1100-16.2016
14. Paredes D, Morilak DA. A rodent model of exposure therapy: the use of fear extinction as a therapeutic intervention for PTSD. Front Behav Neurosci. (2019) 13:46. doi: 10.3389/fnbeh.2019.00046
15. Bullock J, Lane JE, Mikloušić I, Shults FL. Modeling nationalism, religiosity, and threat perception: during the COVID-19 pandemic. PLoS ONE. (2023) 18:e0281002. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0281002
16. Kay AC. Gaucher D, McGregor I, Nash K. Religious belief as compensatory control. Personal Soc Psychol Rev. (2010) 14:37–48. doi: 10.1177/1088868309353750
17. Lucifora C, Grasso GM, Nitsche MA, Vicario CM. Enhanced fear conditioning in individuals with evening chronotype. A virtual reality fear conditioning/extinction study. J Affect Disord. (2022) 311:344–52. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2022.05.033
18. Nash K, Kleinert T, Leota J, Scott A, Schimel J. Resting-state networks of believers and non-believers: an EEG microstate study. Biol Psychol. (2022) 169:108283. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2022.108283
19. World Medical Association. World Medical Association Declaration of Helsinki: ethical principles for medical research involving human subjects. J Am Med Assoc. (2013) 310:2191–4. doi: 10.1001/jama.2013.281053
20. Spielberger CD, Gorsuch R, Lushene RE. Manual for the State-Trait Anxiety Inventory. Palo Alto, CA: Consulting Psychologist Press (1970).
21. Pedrabissi L, Santinello M. Verifica della validità dello STAI forma Y di Spielberger [Verification of the validity of the STAI, Form Y, by Spielberger]. Giunti Organizzazioni Speciali. (1989) 11–4, 191–2.
22. Lucifora C, Gangemi A, D'Italia G, Culicetto L, Ferraioli F, Grasso G, et al. PanicRoom: a virtual reality-based Pavlovian fear conditioning paradigm. Front Psychol. (2024) 15:1432141. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2024.1432141
23. Stroebe W, Strack F. The alleged crisis and the illusion of exact replication. Perspect Psychol Sci. (2014) 9:59–71. doi: 10.1177/1745691613514450
24. Lucifora C, Schembri M, Poggi F, Grasso GM, Gangemi A. Virtual reality supports perspective taking in cultural heritage interpretation. Comput Hum Behav. (2023) 148:107911. doi: 10.1016/j.chb.2023.107911
25. Nucera S. A Brief Analysis of the Educational Implications of Virtual Reality. Preliminary Reports and Negative Results in Life Science and Humanities. (2024). p. 1. doi: 10.13129/3035-062X/prnr-4222
26. Daher K, Capallera M, Lucifora C, Casas J, Meteier Q, El Kamali M, et al. Empathic interactions in automated vehicles. In: CHI Conference on Human Factors in Computing Systems (CHI EA '21). Yokohama (2021). p. 1–4. doi: 10.1145/3411763.3441359
27. Lucifora C, Grasso GM, Perconti P, Plebe A. Moral dilemmas in self-driving cars [Dilemmi morali nelle automobili a guida autonoma]. Rivista Internazionale di Filosofia e Psicologia. (2020) 11:238–50. doi: 10.4453/rifp.2020.0015
28. Ney LJ, Vicario CM, Nitsche MA, Felmingham KL. Timing matters: transcranial direct current stimulation after extinction learning impairs subsequent fear extinction retention. Neurobiol Learn Mem. (2021) 177:107356. doi: 10.1016/j.nlm.2020.107356
29. Milad MR, Quirk GJ. Fear extinction as a model for translational neuroscience: ten years of progress. Annu Rev Psychol. (2012) 63:129–51. doi: 10.1146/annurev.psych.121208.131631
30. Vicario CM, Nitsche MA, Felmingham K. Forgetting fear associations through tES: which memory process might be critical? Transl Psychiatry. (2017) 7:e1046. doi: 10.1038/tp.2017.26
31. Shechner T, Britton JC, Ronkin EG, Jarcho JM, Mash JA, Michalska KJ, et al. Fear conditioning and extinction in anxious and nonanxious youth and adults: examining a novel developmentally appropriate fear-conditioning task. Depr Anxiety. (2015) 32:277–88. doi: 10.1002/da.22318
32. Hur J, Smith JF, DeYoung KA, Anderson AS, Kuang J, Kim HC, et al. Anxiety and the neurobiology of temporally uncertain threat anticipation. J Neurosci. (2020) 40:7949–64. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0704-20.2020
33. Öhman A. Fear and anxiety: overlaps and dissociations. In:Lewis M, Haviland-Jones JM, Barrett LF, , editors. Handbook of Emotions. New York, NY: The Guilford Press (2008). p. 709–28.
34. Jafari E, Alizadehgoradel J, Pourmohseni Koluri F, Nikoozadehkordmirza E, Refahi M, Taherifard M, et al. Intensified electrical stimulation targeting lateral and medial prefrontal cortices for the treatment of social anxiety disorder: a randomized, double-blind, parallel-group, dose-comparison study. Brain Stimul. (2021) 14:974–86. doi: 10.1016/j.brs.2021.06.005
35. Myers KM, Davis M. Mechanisms of fear extinction. Mol Psychiatry. (2007) 12:120–50. doi: 10.1038/sj.mp.4001939
36. Maren S, Holmes A. Stress and fear extinction. Neuropsychopharmacology. (2016) 41:58–79. doi: 10.1038/npp.2015.180
37. Perera CK, Pandey R, Srivastava AK. Role of religion and spirituality in stress management among nurses. Psychol Stud. (2018) 63:187–99. doi: 10.1007/s12646-018-0454-x
38. Maren S. Unrelenting fear under stress: Neural circuits and mechanisms for the immediate extinction deficit. Front Syst Neurosci. (2022) 16:888461. doi: 10.3389/fnsys.2022.888461
39. Koenig HG. In the Wake of Disaster: Religious Responses to Terrorism & Catastrophe. London: Templeton Foundation Press (2006).
40. Lokshina Y, Sheynin J, Vogt GS, Liberzon I. Fear extinction learning in posttraumatic stress disorder. Curr Top Behav Neurosci. (2023) 64:436. doi: 10.1007/7854_2023_436
41. Koenig HG, King DE, Carson VB. Handbook of Religion and Health. 2nd ed. Oxford: Oxford University Press (2012).
42. Exline JJ, Rose E. Religious and spiritual struggles. In: Paloutzian RF, Park CL, , editors. Handbook of the Psychology of Religion and Spirituality. New York, NY: The Guilford Press (2005). p. 315-30.
43. Dawkins R. The God Delusion. London: Bantam Press; New York, NY: Houghton Mifflin Company (2006).
44. Galen LW, Kloet JD. Mental well-being in the religious and the non-religious: evidence for a curvilinear relationship. Ment Health Relig Cult. (2011) 14:673–89. doi: 10.1080/13674676.2010.510829
45. Steinmann JP, Pickel G. Explaining the Relationship Between Religiosity and Anti-diversity Attitudes Among Christians in Western Germany. Cambridge: Politics and Religion (2024).
46. Steinmann J-P. Faithful and fearful: does religion promote or reduce fear of crime in Germany? Eur J Criminol. (2024) 21:631–64. doi: 10.1177/14773708241226824
47. Garfinkel SN, Abelson JL, King AP, Sripada RK, Wang X, Gaines LM, et al. Impaired contextual modulation of memories in PTSD: an fMRI and psychophysiological study of extinction retention and fear renewal. J Neurosci. (2014) 34:13435–43. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4287-13.2014
48. Massimino S, Rinella S, Buscemi A, Similia E, Perciavalle V, Perciavalle V, et al. Digit ratio, personality and emotions in skydivers. Biomed Rep. (2019) 10:39–46. doi: 10.3892/br.2018.1174
49. Schlenker BR, Chambers JR, Le BM. Conservatives are happier than liberals, but why? political ideology, personality, and life satisfaction. J Res Personal. (2012) 46:127–46. doi: 10.1016/j.jrp.2011.12.009
50. Beller J, Kröger C. Religiosity, religious fundamentalism, and perceived threat as predictors of Muslim support for extremist violence. Psycholog Relig Spiritual. (2018) 10:345–55. doi: 10.1037/rel0000138
51. Bryant RA. Is fear extinction impairment central to psychopathology? Curr Top Behav Neurosci. (2023) 64:439. doi: 10.1007/7854_2023_439
52. Carlozzi BL, Winterowd C, Harrist RS, Thomason N, Bratkovich K. Spirituality, anger, and stress in early adolescents. J Relig Health. (2010) 49:445–59. doi: 10.1007/s10943-009-9295-1
53. Lindeman M, Lipsanen J. Diverse cognitive profiles of religious believers and nonbelievers. Int J Psychol Relig. (2016) 26:185–92. doi: 10.1080/10508619.2015.1091695
54. Marković V, Vicario CM, Yavari F, Salehinejad MA. A systematic review on the effect of transcranial direct current and magnetic stimulation on fear memory and extinction. Front Hum Neurosci. (2021) 15:655947. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2021.655947
55. Rinella S, Buscemi A, Massimino S, Perciavalle V, Tortorici MM, Tomaselli DG, et al. Risk-taking behavior, the second-to-fourth digit ratio and psychological features in a sample of cavers. Peer J. (2019) 7:e8029. doi: 10.7717/peerj.8029
56. Vicario CM., Salehinejad MA, Felmingham K, Martino G, Nitsche MA, Hoysted I. A nodal transcranial direct current stimulation over the ventromedial prefrontal cortex enhances fear extinction in healthy humans: A single blind sham-controlled study. Brain Stimul. (2020) 13:489–91. doi: 10.1016/j.brs.2019.12.022
57. Vicario CM, Makris S, Culicetto L, Lucifora C, Falzone A, Martino G, et al. Evidence of altered fear extinction learning in individuals with high vaccine hesitancy during COVID-19 pandemic. Clin Neuropsychiat. (2023) 20:364–9. doi: 10.36131/cnfioritieditore20230417
58. Shenhav A, Rand DG, Greene JD. Divine intuition: cognitive style influences belief in God. J Exp Psychol Gen. (2012) 141:423–8. doi: 10.1037/a0025391
59. Torrents-Rodas D, Fullana MA, Bonillo A, Caseras X, Andión O, Torrubia R. No effect of trait anxiety on differential fear conditioning or fear generalization. Biol Psychol. (2013) 92:185–90. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2012.10.006
Keywords: God belief, religious ideology, Pavlovian fear conditioning, reduced fear extinction learning, weaker inhibitory learning process
Citation: Vicario CM, Culicetto L, Lucifora C, Ferraioli F, Massimino S, Martino G, Tomaiuolo F and Falzone AM (2025) The power of belief? Evidence of reduced fear extinction learning in Catholic God believers. Front. Public Health 12:1509388. doi: 10.3389/fpubh.2024.1509388
Received: 10 October 2024; Accepted: 10 December 2024;
Published: 07 January 2025.
Edited by:
M. Walid Qoronfleh, Q3 Research Institute, United StatesReviewed by:
Jony Sheynin, Texas A and M University, United StatesAlexandru I. Tiba, University of Oradea, Romania
Giuseppe Craparo, Kore University of Enna, Italy
Copyright © 2025 Vicario, Culicetto, Lucifora, Ferraioli, Massimino, Martino, Tomaiuolo and Falzone. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.
*Correspondence: Carmelo Mario Vicario, Y3ZpY2FyaW9AdW5pbWUuaXQ=; Chiara Lucifora, Y2hpYXJhLmx1Y2lmb3JhQHVuaWJvLml0; Simona Massimino, c2ltb25hLm1hc3NpbWlub0B1bmltZS5pdA==; Francesca Ferraioli, ZnJhbmNlc2NhLmZlcnJhaW9saUB1bmltZS5pdA==