Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Public Health, 14 June 2024
Sec. Substance Use Disorders and Behavioral Addictions
This article is part of the Research Topic Alcohol-Related Brain Injury: Diagnosis, Treatment, and Rehabilitation View all 4 articles

Sex-related differences in young binge drinkers on the neurophysiological response to stress in virtual reality

  • 1Facultad de Educación y Psicología, Universidad Francisco de Vitoria, Madrid, Spain
  • 2Departamento de Psicología Biológica y de la Salud, Facultad de Psicología, Universidad Autónoma de Madrid, Madrid, Spain

Background: Stress is one of the main environmental factors involved in the onset of different psychopathologies. In youth, stressful life events can trigger inappropriate and health-damaging behaviors, such as binge drinking. This behavior, in turn, can lead to long-lasting changes in the neurophysiological response to stress and the development of psychological disorders late in life, e.g., alcohol use disorder. Our aim was to analyze the pattern of neurophysiological responses triggered with the exposition to a stressful virtual environment in young binge drinkers.

Methods: AUDIT-3 (third question from the full AUDIT) was used to detect binge drinking (BD) in our young sample (age 18–25 years). According to the score, participants were divided into control (CO) and BD group. Next, a standardized virtual reality (VR) scenario (Richie’s Plank) was used for triggering the stress response while measuring the following neurophysiological variables: brain electrical activity by electroencephalogram (EEG) and cortisol levels through saliva samples both measurements registered before and after the stressful situation. Besides, heart rate (HR) with a pulsometer and electrodermal response (EDA) through electrodes placed on fingers were analyzed before, during and after the VR task.

Results: Regarding the behavior assessed during the VR task, BD group spent significantly less amount of time walking forward the table and a tendency toward more time walking backwards. There was no statistically significant difference between the BD and the CO group regarding time looking down, but when we controlled the variable sex, the BD women group displayed higher amount of time looking down than the rest of the groups. Neurophysiological measurements revealed that there was not any statistically significant difference between groups in any of the EEG registered measures, EDA response and cortisol levels. Sex-related differences were found in HR response to VR scenario, in which BD women displayed the highest peak of response to the stressor. Also, the change in heartbeat was higher in BD women than men.

Conclusion: Unveiling the neurophysiological alterations associated with BD can help us to prevent and detect early onset of alcohol use disorder. Also, from our data we conclude that participants’ sex can modulate some stress responses, especially when unhealthy behaviors such as BD are present. Nevertheless, the moment of registration of the neurophysiological variables respect to the stressor seems to be a crucial variable.

Introduction

Excessive alcohol intake is considered the most pervasive harmful habit in the world and has been identified as a social problem (1), causing 5.3% of all deaths worldwide (3 million deaths annually) according to the World Health Organization (WHO). This percentage rises among younger people (20–39 years of age), accounting for 13.5% of all fatalities in this age group, besides the effect on public health that this entails (2).

In recent years, college students have started to be considered a risk population for problematic alcohol use (35). The university stage is a time when the student builds a new social network and creates the foundation of their adult identity (6). Since most young people use alcohol as a recreational substance, alcohol use is quite prevalent at this period of life (7, 8). Additionally, the beginning of university studies also marks the onset of the legal drinking age, which allows them easy access and purchase of alcohol. A frequent pattern of alcohol consumption among young people is characterized by episodes of intense and compulsive drinking over a short period of time which is known as binge drinking (BD) and it has been correlated with high-risk behavior, such as aggression, reckless driving, or unprotected sex (9, 10). This represents a harmful alcohol-consumption pattern, and people who engage in this behavior may experience a variety of short- and long-term impacts, such as issues with their physical and mental health, violent behavior, or difficulty adjusting to family or school life (11, 12). Although quantitative definitions of BD differ significantly among nations, the most common threshold for defining BD is the intake of four to five alcohol doses (for men and women, respectively), in 2 h, at least once per month over a period of 6 months (13).

There is a great need for identification of biological markers associated with early alcohol-related brain damage. Concerning to this, BD is known to alter the frontal pathways (fronto-limbic) of white matter in adolescents and young adults (14, 15). The connections between the prelimbic and infralimbic regions, as well as those between the infralimbic region and the orbitofrontal cortex, appear to be affected by this diminished fronto-limbic connectivity (16). Besides, reduced or compromised inhibitory control seems to be associated with this decline in fronto-striatal connection (17). On the other hand, high alcohol consumption in university students has also been linked to a loss of gray matter in the prefrontal areas (18, 19) and cholinergic and serotonergic neurons persisting into adulthood (20). However, little is known about the impact of BD on physiological stress-related responses despite the relevance of this factor on alcohol abuse (21, 22). Some studies have pointed that young people and adolescents may be motivated to drink alcohol by their desire for new experiences and feelings, but, in general, they frequently drink alcohol to cope with the stress or unpleasant feelings they may have throughout their first year of college (due to the perception of responsibility, greater freedom, study, work, or the creation of new social networks) (23, 24). In comparison to the general population, university students often exhibit higher levels of anxiety and worst mental health (25, 26), and the primary source of this distress appears to be anxiety about academic success (27). Thus, lack of adequate strategies to deal with situations of stress can lead to the adoption of inappropriate or harmful coping mechanisms, such as excessive alcohol use.

Emotions include a psychophysiological component that can be studied using measurements of skin conductance (SC) and heart rate indices (HR) (28). SC is mainly influenced by the amount of moisture produced by the eccrine sweat glands under the control of the sympathetic branch of the autonomic nervous system (ANS) (29). HR is regulated by both the sympathetic and parasympathetic branches of the ANS, and it reflects stress reactivity in psychosocial and cognitive circumstances (30). Salivary cortisol is also frequently utilized as a biomarker of anxiety, and salivary cortisol levels seem an accurate indicator of the hypothalamus—pituitary–adrenal axis (HPA) response to stress (31). In summary, these three peripheral responses (SC, HR, and level of cortisol) are frequently used as measures of ANS reactivity (32) and, additionally, they are useful measurements for examining how drinking is related to emotional functioning (33, 34). Physiological arousal levels have been positively correlated with dependence and alcohol consumption levels (35), and gender differences in these responses also have been reported (36, 37).

In recent years, new research approaches have explored more ecological contexts and reliable measurements of stress-related responses. In this context, virtual reality (VR) has been increasingly employed because it is able to induce stress reactivity despite the virtual context (38). Currently, there is a dearth of studies that have investigated stress reactivity and anxiety-related behaviors in young individuals with substance use through exposure to virtual scenarios, being specially focus on treatment, craving or cognition (3941). In a previous study of our group with young people with problematic alcohol consumption, without BD, we observed more anxious-related behaviors when they were exposed to our virtual environment, as well as a different pattern of physiological responses compared to the healthy youths (42). In consequence, we are interested in analyzing similar physiological responses when alcohol is consumed in BD way.

Therefore, our main goal was to analyze the physiological responses induced by a stressful VR environment in young people with BD. Along with this, we will consider the influence of participants’ sex in our results owing to both stress reactivity and regulation have been shown to differ between men and women, despite the neural networks supporting these processes remain unclear (43). We expect that these variables can constitute possible biomarkers that allow us to detect young people at risk of developing alcohol use disorder in the future.

Methods

Study sample

Volunteers were recruited from Francisco Vitoria University and Autonomous University of Madrid. Subjects included in the study signed the informed consent, accompanied by an informative note, and they created an alphanumeric code based on the first letter of their first name, the last letter of your first surname followed by your date of birth (e.g., PO1304) to guarantee privacy during the data processing and analysis phases. To collect information about alcohol use, anxiety trait, and depression symptoms on-line standardized questionnaires were administered using Qualtrics XM (Qualtrics, Provo, UT) by snowball sampling [Alcohol use disorders identification test (AUDIT, Spanish adaptation: (44))]; State–Trait Anxiety Inventory [STAI-t (45)]; Beck Depression Inventory-II [BDI-II (46)] (Table 1). Based on AUDIT-3 scores (“How often do you have 6 or more drinks on one occasion?”), we divided the sample into 2 groups: control group [CO, n = 23 (12 men and 11 women)]; BD group [n = 20 (8 men and 12 women)]. Inclusion criteria were: (1) aged 18–25 years; (2) an absence of diagnosis of substance use disorder; (3) an absence of comorbid disease, such as anxiety-related disorders, depression, psychotic disorder, or Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity-Disorder (ADHD). Moreover, healthy controls should not present a history of drug abuse, including nicotine and alcohol. All volunteers were excluded if they presented severe difficulties in understanding the test instructions, altered consciousness or agitation, presence of fear of heights or claustrophobia, a score in the AUDIT above 19 points and if they consumed prescription drugs affecting the central nervous system (mainly anxiolytics and antidepressants). Additionally, during the initial interview, we asked them about possible phobias and individuals with fear of heights or claustrophobia were excluded from the study (Table 1). After the online assessment, a representative subgroup of participants was contacted by email to manage the VR session. In the case that some participant informed of nausea, dizziness, or blurred vision during the VR session, we would stop the session and he/she would be excluded from the study.

Table 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Descriptive results for sociodemographic and emotional-related variables.

The study received approval from the Ethics Committee of the Autonomous University of Madrid (Code: CEI-122-2490) and Universidad Francisco de Vitoria (UFV 23-2022) in accordance with the Ethical Principles for Medical Research Involving Human Subjects adopted in the Declaration of Helsinki by the World Medical Association (64th WMA General Assembly, Fortaleza, Brazil, October 2013), Recommendation No. R (97) 5 of the Committee of Ministers to Member States on the Protection of Medical Data (1997), and the Spanish Data Protection Act (Ley Orgánica 15/1999 de Protección de Datos, LOPD). All participants were informed about the study prior to their inclusion in the study and then provided their written informed consent.

VR setup

Participants received a VR headset (Meta Quest 2, 128 Gb, Meta Platforms Technologies Ireland Ltd.) and after checking the vision of participants, our VR EPM started. The immersive VR scenario chosen was Richie’s Plank on Oculus Quest 2, used successfully in previous research for evoking both stress response (47) and brain activity (4749). Richie’s Plank Experience steam game is a VR game that makes the participant enter an elevator, takes them to the top floor of a skyscraper, and allows them to walk over and finally jump off a plank extending over the edge of the building. To increase the sensorial experience of the plank, we used a real wooden plank with 50 cm in diameter, the height 15 cm, and the length 160 cm. The VR scenario lasted approximately 5 min with 4 subsequent levels combining innate stressors, such as heights and spiders. We requested the consent of the participants to record their performance inside the task by casting the experience to a laptop. Behavioral variables registered were the following: percentage of time spent walking forward and backward, latency to walk forward (s) and time spent looking down in the edge of the plank (s). Behavioral variables registered were analyzed manually on the VR recordings by Raton Time program (42). Also, the different measures were taken by two independent observers and inter-rater reliability was determined by correlation analysis higher than 0.9.

Psychophysiological measurements

EEG data were collected before (T1) and after (T2) the VR experience using Bitbrain’s Versatile 32 cap (model E32.A1), which is comprised of 32 electrodes (plus ground and reference) set in the international 10–10 system (Bitbrain Technologies 2018, Zaragoza, Spain). After informing them of what the task entailed, participants were seated in a comfortable chair during the EEG set-up, where the experimenter placed the EEG cap and ensured that all electrode impedances were kept below 5 Ω. Brain electrical activity was registered before and after the VR stressful situation [5 min eyes open (EO) and 5 min eyes closed (EC)] while staring at a fixation dot painted on the wall 90 cm from them. The conditions were counterbalanced so that half of the participants started with the EC condition and the other half started with the EO condition. The EEG signals were preprocessed using a combination of manual and automated procedures. First, all EEG recordings were visually examined by one of the authors in order to reject the most notable portions of noisy data. Then, the EEGlab (50) pipeline provided by Hatlestad-Hall et al. (51) was used to automatically reject the more subtle instances of noise in the data. This pipeline starts by iteratively referencing the data, searching for noisy channels, and excluding them from the computation of the average reference, which increases the robustness of the referenced data. Afterwards, a 1 Hz high-pass filter is applied, along with the removal of the power line noise using the nt_zapline function from the NoiseTools toolbox (52). Then, muscle and eye artifacts are identified using the SOBI algorithm (53) and subsequently removed using the IClabel function (54). Next, bad channels are interpolated, and a 40 Hz low-pass filter is applied. Finally, the continuous data is segmented into non-overlapping 4 s epochs for posterior analysis, and the two runs of the EO and EC conditions are, respectively, appended.

HR was registered with a pulsometer (Polar H10®) and EDA response through electrodes placed on fingers (eSense Skin Response®) before (T1), during (T2) and after (T3) the VR experience. Saliva samples were recollected (12 p.m. to 18 p.m.) through salivettes (Salivette®, Sarstedt, S.A., Barcelona, Spain) to analyze cortisol response before the start of the VR task (T1) and immediately after the end (T2). Saliva samples were coded and stored in the refrigerator at −20°C. Then, they were centrifuged at 1,500 rpm for 15 min, resulting in a clear supernatant of low viscosity that was stored at −80°C until the analysis of the salivary cortisol response (sCORT). Salivary cortisol levels were measured by an enzyme immunoassay kit (functional sensitivity 0.018 μg/dL) following the manufacturer’s instructions (Salimetrics, Stage College, PA, United States) (42).

Statistical analysis

Statistical analyses were performed using SPSS 25 (IBM SPSS Statistics, Corporate headquarters, New Orchard Road, Armonk, New York 10504-1722, United States). All p values were two-tailed, and the level of significance was taken as p ≤ 0.05. Descriptive analyses (mean ± SEM) were performed on the demographic variables, information related to drug use and online questionnaires (more details are displayed in Table 1). GROUP (CO/BD) and SEX (men/women) were considered between-factors with two levels each and then, two-way repeated measures ANOVA (comparing T1/T2/T3) was performed for all the measures taken throughout the study (EEG, HR, EDA and salivary cortisol). Appropriate post hoc comparisons were conducted when significant differences were found. Additionally, given the small sample size, together with p value above mentioned (p ≤ 0.05), the critical value of each statistical analysis was used as the decision criterion, calculated Post hoc with G*Power based on the number of groups and number of measurements (55, 56).

Results

Behavioral results

First, with concern to the scores in the different tests administered, we observed no difference in STAI-T and BDI-II scores between groups (neither control vs. BD nor men vs. women; Figures 1A,B), but statistically significant high values in AUDIT scores in BD group [t (41)= − 2.19, p = 0.03, critical t = 1.64; Figure 1A] and a tendency toward significance in women [t (41) = 1.86; p = 0.07; critical t = 1.64; Figure 1B]. Regarding the behavior assessed during the VR task (Figure 1C), we observed that the BD group spent significant less amount of time walking forward the table [t (32)=2.44; p = 0.02] and a tendency toward more time walking backwards [t (32)= −1.79; p = 0.08; critical t = 1.44; Figure 1C]. There was no statistically significant difference between the BD and the control group regarding time looking down, but when we included gender, the BD women group displayed higher amount of time looking down than the rest of the groups [Group × Sex effect: F (1, 28) = 4.29, p = 0.04; critical F = 2.85; Figure 1D]. Interestingly, we found a significant positive correlation between AUDIT scores and time looking down in women group (r = 0.44; p = 0.03; Figure 1D).

Figure 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Psychological and behavioral variables of sample. (A) Means of AUDIT, STAI-T and BDI-II scores among groups (control and BD). (B) Scores in the same questionnaires separating men and women. (C) Behavior analysis in VR task (time walking forward, latency to walk forward and time walking backwards). (D) BD women group displayed higher amount of time looking down and this index correlates positively with AUDIT score.

Physiological results

With concern to the physiological response to VR stress, we found mixed results in different areas, all compiled in Figure 2. First, we did not observe any significant difference in the EDA response during the task when comparing for group or sex (Figure 2A). Secondly, there was no significant change of VR task in sCORT in the two measures taken (before the task and immediately after, Figure 2B). We did observe a significant effect of time in heart rate parameter. In fact, repeated-measures ANOVA revealed significant sex-related changes in HR response to VR [time × group × sex, F (3, 117) = 3.86; p = 0.01; critical F = 2.68; Figure 2C], in which BD women displayed the highest peak of response to the stressor, and BD men showed lower response. Also, the change in heartbeat was higher in women than men (sex effect; Figure 2C).

Figure 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Sex-related differences in physiological variables measured in BD and control groups. (A) Skin conductance during the task. (B) Salivary cortisol levels before and after the VR task. (C) Interaction effect between group, time and sex in heart rate along the different measures (before, during and after VR stressor). (D) EEG parameters analyzed previous and after the stressful situation induced by VR.

Finally, the differential EEG signal between BD and CO group was analyzed before and after the VR. At this point, there was not any statistically significant difference between groups in any of the EEG measures determined (Figure 2D). However, we observed significant differences between T1 and T2 in beta power (dB) of both EEG conditions, eyes open (EO) and eyes closed (EC) (Figure 2D).

Discussion

In the current study, we explored the behavioral and neuropsychophysiological responses of young individuals with BD during the exposition to a stressful VR environment compared to a healthy CO group. Our stress-related variables, registered at different moments of the VR task (behavioral pattern of ambulation, EEG, HR, and EDA response, as well as salivary cortisol), could be possible used as possible biomarkers that allow us to detect young people at risk of developing alcohol use disorder in the future, as well as anxiety-related disorders (5759). As expected, our results showed several behavioral and physiological differences between groups, highlighting the variable sex as an important factor modulating our results.

Concerning the behavioral performance during the VR task, our results showed that the BD group spent significantly less time walking forward the table with respect to the CO group, whereas the time spent looking down was similar in both groups. Nevertheless, when we controlled the variable sex, the BD women group displayed higher amount of time looking down than BD men. As we know, this is the first study that has employed the software Richie’s Plank Experience to analyze the behavioral pattern of exploration of young participants with alcohol consumption in BD way. Studies in animal models (60, 61) and in humans (42, 62, 63) have shown that the higher the level of anxiety, the more we tend to avoid unprotected open spaces. Hence, our BD participants did not spend much of the experience walking the plank, let alone getting to the end of it. On the contrary, although it was not significant, they showed a tendency to walk backwards toward the protected space without turning around as an indicator of anxiety and fear of falling. Previous study of our group using another VR experience revealed similar results with a more anxious pattern of exploration in young subjects with problematic alcohol use (42). It results interesting that BD women displayed higher time looking down during the experience respect to BD men. This result could be understood in two ways. On the one hand, it is possible that the women from the experimental group exhibited more risk-taking and sensation seeking-related behavior, or on the other hand, this behavior could have reflected an attempt to check that they were not going to fall while walking slowly on the board. Regarding the first interpretation, some studies have observed that women in the mid- and late-20s usually, as in our study, usually show higher scores in sensation seeking behaviors (64, 65) which, in turn, is related to seek out novel and exciting stimuli, including risk-taking behavior (66). Further research on this matter is necessary due to the clinical implications that this variable presents as a risk factor (67).

EEG measurements were not different between groups, as well as EDA response and the salivary cortisol levels. It is possible that the lack of differences in salivary cortisol levels were due to methodological errors associated with the pattern of release of this hormone. We recollected the T2 sample immediately after the end of the VR task, whereas cortisol peak occurs between 20 and 30 min after stressor onset and can continue to be elevated for an hour or more (68, 69). A previous study of our group also found no differences in EDA response between young participants with problematic alcohol consumption and the CO group (42). Some studies have suggested that blunted reactivity is a general characteristic of alcohol dependency, particularly in adults with a long-standing history of consumption, rather than in individuals with time-limited excessive alcohol use as in our studies (70). Regarding EEG registered variables, recent literature has described that, similar to individuals with alcohol addiction, BD also displayed increased resting-state EEG signal (59). For instance, high-BD subjects presented greater spectral power over the frontal, central and parietal regions in the delta (0–4 Hz), and fast-beta (20–35 Hz) frequency bands, suggesting that high-BDs display an EEG spectral pattern similar to that observed in subjects with alcohol use disorder (70, 71). Similarly, Affan et al. (58) found a slower Alpha Peak Frequency as well as increased frontal theta and beta power in the BD group. Hence, resting-state EEG studies seem to indicate that BDs present abnormal spontaneous EEG signal, mainly characterized by increased power in slow (delta/theta) and fast (beta) frequency bands which could be associated with a brain overactivity eventually caused by an excitatory-inhibitory imbalance (72). In our study, the CO and BD group were similar in Alpha power which is not consistent with previous studies. However, reduced alpha power was observed mainly when anxiety symptoms are present in the BD sample (73, 74). This aspect was controlled in our study (see STAI and BDI-II results in Table 1) and could have contributed to the non-significant difference between groups. On the other hand, the short history of alcohol use in our BD sample could be also related to the lack of differences with the CO group (72, 75).

Sex-related differences were found in HR response to VR scenario, in which BD women displayed the highest peak of response to the stressor. Also, the change in heartbeat was higher in BD women than men. This enhanced HR response in BD women could be related to their risky behaviors during the exploration of the VR environment. Thus, it has been widely described that to cope with stressful stimuli our body releases adrenaline, a hormone that temporarily causes higher heart rate to speed up and your blood pressure to rise (76). Hence, it could be a normal biomarker of their pattern of ambulation during the VR environment. These findings are consistent with the theory that increased HR reflects engagement of self-regulatory efforts to cope with an emotional or stressful situation (77). Considering the historical under-representation of women in alcohol abuse research, the identification of HR as a potential biomarker for addiction and relapse risk (78), and the close relationship between stress, alcohol abuse, and relapse in women (79), these issues hold significant research and clinical relevance.

In general, VR is a technological tool which can open new avenues to better understand the neurobiological mechanisms of emotional responding in healthy and pathological conditions (42). It results attractive to young people and, while its employment is more prevalent in the intervention context, it could be employed as a tool to identify behavioral patterns or physiological responses related to future clinical conditions. Moreover, it is notorious to highlight the role of sex variable in addiction related studies in which girls usually show a tendency toward increased alcohol consumption associated with negative mood states (80). Additionally, their different psychophysiological responses to a stressor may be useful as a future predictor of severity. As limitations of our study, we would like to mention the reduced sample size. Moreover, variables such as the menstrual phase, as a variable influencing stress response or drug-related variables, as the quantity of alcohol consumed by the participants, should have been considered in our study.

Conclusion

The present study focused on examining the behavioral signs of anxiety through a VR task and investigating the psychophysiological responses associated with a virtual stressful context. Exploring biomarkers provides crucial insights into the intricate relationship between the body and mind, and VR appears to be a useful tool for investigating anxiety in humans. Therefore, understanding and addressing the biological mechanisms involved in stress-related responses holds the promise of improving treatments, predicting problematic use, and provide personalized care to address the specific needs of women.

Data availability statement

The raw data supporting the conclusions of this article will be made available by the authors, without undue reservation.

Ethics statement

The study received approval from the Ethics Committee of the Autonomous University of Madrid (Code: CEI-122-2490) and Universidad Francisco de Vitoria (UFV 23-2022) in accordance with the Ethical Principles for Medical Research Involving Human Subjects adopted in the Declaration of Helsinki by the World Medical Association (64th WMA General Assembly, Fortaleza, Brazil, October 2013), Recommendation No. R (97) 5 of the Committee of Ministers to Member States on the Protection of Medical Data (1997), and the Spanish Data Protection Act (Ley Orgánica 15/1999 de Protección de Datos, LOPD). All participants were informed about the study prior to their inclusion in the study and then provided their written informed consent. The studies were conducted in accordance with the local legislation and institutional requirements. The participants provided their written informed consent to participate in this study.

Author contributions

RM-F: Conceptualization, Formal analysis, Investigation, Methodology, Project administration, Supervision, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing. EB-B: Investigation, Writing – original draft. MC-C: Investigation, Methodology, Writing – original draft. FB-S: Formal analysis, Writing – original draft. PS-P: Conceptualization, Funding acquisition, Investigation, Methodology, Project administration, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing.

Funding

The author(s) declare that financial support was received for the research, authorship, and/or publication of this article. This study was funded by the Spanish Ministry of Health (Government Delegation for the National Plan on Drugs, code 2022I004 to PS-P), Spanish Ministry of Science and Innovation (MCIN/AEI/10.13039/501100011033/FEDER, UE, code PID2022-137601OA-I00 to PS-P) and Universidad Francisco de Vitoria (project reference: UFV2022-41 to RM-F).

Acknowledgments

We are grateful to the Faculty of Experimental Sciences of Universidad Francisco de Vitoria for kindly providing the laboratory equipment for cortisol analysis, to Miriam Granado for the idea of Richie’s Plank, to Daniel Rodríguez Espinosa and Andrea Rodero Herrera for their help in EEG processing, and to Gabriel Peñas, David García León and Next Level Studio for the ongoing collaboration with virtual reality.

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher’s note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

References

1. Moss, HB. The impact of alcohol on society: a brief overview. Soc Work Public Health. (2013) 28:175–7. doi: 10.1080/19371918.2013.758987

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

2. World Health Organization. Appendix: draft action plan (2022–2030) to effectively implement the global strategy to reduce the harmful use of alcohol as a public health priority In: World Health Organization. Political declaration of the third high-level meeting of the General Assembly on the prevention and control of non-communicable diseases. Executive Board 150th session, 11 January 2022, Provisional agenda item 7. Geneva: WHO (2022)

Google Scholar

3. Herrero-Montes, M, Alonso-Blanco, C, Paz-Zulueta, M, Pellico-López, A, Ruiz-Azcona, L, Sarabia-Cobo, C, et al. Excessive alcohol consumption and binge drinking in college students. PeerJ. (2022) 10:e13368. doi: 10.7717/peerj.13368

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

4. Chow, MSC, Poon, SHL, Lui, KL, Chan, CCY, and Lam, WWT. Alcohol consumption and depression among university students and their perception of alcohol use. East Asian Arch Psychiatr. (2021) 31:87–96. doi: 10.12809/eaap20108

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

5. Sanchez, A, Gainza Perez, M, and Field, CA. The role of resilience in alcohol use, drinking motives, and alcohol-related consequences among Hispanic college students. Am J Drug Alcohol Abuse. (2022) 48:100–9. doi: 10.1080/00952990.2021.1996584

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

6. Lorant, V, Nicaise, P, Soto, VE, and d'Hoore, W. Alcohol drinking among college students: college responsibility for personal troubles. BMC Public Health. (2013) 13:615. doi: 10.1186/1471-2458-13-615

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

7. Verhoog, S, Dopmeijer, JM, de Jonge, JM, van der Heijde, CM, Vonk, P, Bovens, RHLM, et al. The use of the alcohol use disorders identification test - consumption as an indicator of hazardous alcohol use among university students. Eur Addict Res. (2020) 26:1–9. doi: 10.1159/000503342

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

8. Llamosas-Falcón, L, Manthey, J, and Rehm, J. Changes in alcohol consumption in Spain between 1990 and 2019. Adicciones. (2022) 34:61–72. doi: 10.20882/adicciones.1400

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

9. Hingson, RW, and Zha, W. Binge drinking above and below twice the adolescent thresholds and health-risk behaviors. Alcohol Clin Exp Res. (2018) 42:904–13. doi: 10.1111/acer.13627

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

10. Chung, T, Creswell, KG, Bachrach, R, Clark, DB, and Martin, CS. Adolescent binge drinking. Alcohol Res. (2018) 39:5–15.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

11. Coleman, LG Jr, Crews, FT, and Vetreno, RP. The persistent impact of adolescent binge alcohol on adult brain structural, cellular, and behavioral pathology: a role for the neuroimmune system and epigenetics. Int Rev Neurobiol. (2021) 160:1–44. doi: 10.1016/bs.irn.2021.08.001

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

12. Welch, AE, Caramanica, K, Maslow, CB, Cone, JE, Farfel, MR, Keyes, KM, et al. Frequent binge drinking five to six years after exposure to 9/11: findings from the world trade center health registry. Drug Alcohol Depend. (2014) 140:1–7. doi: 10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2014.04.013

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

13. Hasselgård-Rowe, J, Senchyna, A, Broers, B, and Haller, DM. Heterogeneity of definitions and measurements of binge drinking in research on adolescents and young adults. Drug Alcohol Depend. (2022) 241:109650. doi: 10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2022.109650

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

14. Knoff, AA, Knight, AR, Salat, DH, Bedi, A, Currao, A, Fonda, JR, et al. Early onset adolescent binge drinking is associated with reduced white matter integrity in post-9/11 adult veterans. Alcohol Alcohol. (2023) 58:662–71. doi: 10.1093/alcalc/agad062

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

15. Morris, LS, Dowell, NG, Cercignani, M, Harrison, NA, and Voon, V. Binge drinking differentially affects cortical and subcortical microstructure. Addict Biol. (2018) 23:403–11. doi: 10.1111/adb.12493

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

16. Broadwater, MA, Lee, SH, Yu, Y, Zhu, H, Crews, FT, Robinson, DL, et al. Adolescent alcohol exposure decreases frontostriatal resting-state functional connectivity in adulthood. Addict Biol. (2018) 23:810–23. doi: 10.1111/adb.12530

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

17. Cohen-Gilbert, JE, Nickerson, LD, Sneider, JT, Oot, EN, Seraikas, AM, Rohan, ML, et al. College binge drinking associated with decreased frontal activation to negative emotional distractors during inhibitory control. Front Psychol. (2017) 8:1650. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2017.01650

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

18. Doallo, S, Cadaveira, F, Corral, M, Mota, N, López-Caneda, E, and Holguín, SR. Larger mid-dorsolateral prefrontal gray matter volume in young binge drinkers revealed by voxel-based morphometry. PLoS One. (2014) 9:e96380. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0096380

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

19. Crews, FT, Vetreno, RP, Broadwater, MA, and Robinson, DL. Adolescent alcohol exposure persistently impacts adult neurobiology and behavior. Pharmacol Rev. (2016) 68:1074–109. doi: 10.1124/pr.115.012138

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

20. Goldfarb, EV, Scheinost, D, Fogelman, N, Seo, D, and Sinha, R. High-risk drinkers engage distinct stress-predictive brain networks. Biol Psychiatry Cogn Neurosci Neuroimaging. (2022) 7:805–13. doi: 10.1016/j.bpsc.2022.02.010

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

21. Zorrilla, EP, Logrip, ML, and Koob, GF. Corticotropin releasing factor: a key role in the neurobiology of addiction. Front Neuroendocrinol. (2014) 35:234–44. doi: 10.1016/j.yfrne.2014.01.001

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

22. Hagan, MJ, Modecki, K, Moctezuma Tan, L, Luecken, L, Wolchik, S, and Sandler, I. Binge drinking in adolescence predicts an atypical cortisol stress response in young adulthood. Psychoneuroendocrinology. (2019) 100:137–44. doi: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2018.10.002

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

23. Hallihan, H, Bing-Canar, H, Paltell, K, and Berenz, EC. Negative urgency, PTSD symptoms, and alcohol risk in college students. Addict Behav Rep. (2023) 17:100480. doi: 10.1016/j.abrep.2023.100480

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

24. Benzerouk, F, Djerada, Z, Naassila, M, Barrière, S, Kaladjian, A, and Gierski, F. Role of impulsivity and emotion dysregulation dimensions on core characteristics of binge drinking among university students. Psychol Belg. (2022) 62:286–96. doi: 10.5334/pb.1167

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

25. Sheldon, E, Simmonds-Buckley, M, Bone, C, Mascarenhas, T, Chan, N, Wincott, M, et al. Prevalence and risk factors for mental health problems in university undergraduate students: a systematic review with meta-analysis. J Affect Disord. (2021) 287:282–92. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2021.03.054

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

26. Auerbach, RP, Alonso, J, Axinn, WG, Cuijpers, P, Ebert, DD, Green, JG, et al. Mental disorders among college students in the World Health Organization world mental health surveys. Psychol Med. (2016) 46:2955–70. doi: 10.1017/S0033291716001665

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

27. Beiter, R, Nash, R, McCrady, M, Rhoades, D, Linscomb, M, Clarahan, M, et al. The prevalence and correlates of depression, anxiety, and stress in a sample of college students. J Affect Disord. (2015) 173:90–6. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2014.10.054

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

28. Jentsch, VL, and Wolf, OT. The impact of emotion regulation on cardiovascular, neuroendocrine, and psychological stress responses. Biol Psychol. (2020) 154:107893. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2020.107893

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

29. Sequeira, H, Hot, P, Silvert, L, and Delplanque, S. Electrical autonomic correlates of emotion. Int J Psychophysiol. (2009) 71:50–6. doi: 10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2008.07.009

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

30. Immanuel, S, Teferra, MN, Baumert, M, and Bidargaddi, N. Heart rate variability for evaluating psychological stress changes in healthy adults: a scoping review. Neuropsychobiology. (2023) 82:187–202. doi: 10.1159/000530376

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

31. Hellhammer, DH, Wüst, S, and Kudielka, BM. Salivary cortisol as a biomarker in stress research. Psychoneuroendocrinology. (2009) 34:163–71. doi: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2008.10.026

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

32. Reinhardt, T, Schmahl, C, Wüst, S, and Bohus, M. Salivary cortisol, heart rate, electrodermal activity and subjective stress responses to the Mannheim multicomponent stress test (MMST). Psychiatry Res. (2012) 198:106–11. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2011.12.009

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

33. Starcke, K, van Holst, RJ, van den Brink, W, Veltman, DJ, and Goudriaan, AE. Physiological and endocrine reactions to psychosocial stress in alcohol use disorders: duration of abstinence matters. Alcohol Clin Exp Res. (2013) 37:1343–50. doi: 10.1111/acer.12103

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

34. Boschloo, L, Vogelzangs, N, Licht, CM, Vreeburg, SA, Smit, JH, van den Brink, W, et al. Heavy alcohol use, rather than alcohol dependence, is associated with dysregulation of the hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal axis and the autonomic nervous system. Drug Alcohol Depend. (2011) 116:170–6. doi: 10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2010.12.006

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

35. Kearns, NT, Blumenthal, H, Contractor, AA, Guillot, CR, and Rafiuddin, H. Effects of bodily arousal on desire to drink alcohol among trauma-exposed college students. Alcohol. (2021) 96:15–25. doi: 10.1016/j.alcohol.2021.07.002

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

36. Chaplin, TM, Hong, K, Bergquist, K, and Sinha, R. Gender differences in response to emotional stress: an assessment across subjective, behavioral, and physiological domains and relations to alcohol craving. Alcohol Clin Exp Res. (2008) 32:1242–50. doi: 10.1111/j.1530-0277.2008.00679.x

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

37. Peltier, MR, Verplaetse, TL, Mineur, YS, Petrakis, IL, Cosgrove, KP, Picciotto, MR, et al. Sex differences in stress-related alcohol use. Neurobiol Stress. (2019) 10:100149. doi: 10.1016/j.ynstr.2019.100149

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

38. Dammen, LV, Finseth, TT, McCurdy, BH, Barnett, NP, Conrady, RA, Leach, AG, et al. Evoking stress reactivity in virtual reality: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. (2022) 138:104709. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2022.104709

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

39. Wiebe, A, Kannen, K, Selaskowski, B, Mehren, A, Thöne, AK, Pramme, L, et al. Virtual reality in the diagnostic and therapy for mental disorders: a systematic review. Clin Psychol Rev. (2022) 98:102213. doi: 10.1016/j.cpr.2022.102213

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

40. Park, MJ, Kim, DJ, Lee, U, Na, EJ, and Jeon, HJ. A literature overview of virtual reality (VR) in treatment of psychiatric disorders: recent advances and limitations. Front Psych. (2019) 10:505. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2019.00505

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

41. Hone-Blanchet, A, Wensing, T, and Fecteau, S. The use of virtual reality in craving assessment and cue-exposure therapy in substance use disorders. Front Hum Neurosci. (2014) 8:844. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2014.00844

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

42. Moreno-Fernández, RD, García-León, D, Peñas, G, Martín-Romero, R, Buades-Sitjar, F, and Sampedro-Piquero, P. Immersive virtual plus-maze to examine behavior and psychophysiological-related variables in young people with problematic alcohol and cannabis consumption. Neurobiol Stress. (2023) 26:100564. doi: 10.1016/j.ynstr.2023.100564

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

43. Goldfarb, EV, Seo, D, and Sinha, R. Sex differences in neural stress responses and correlation with subjective stress and stress regulation. Neurobiol Stress. (2019) 11:100177. doi: 10.1016/j.ynstr.2019.100177

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

44. Motos-Sellés, P, Cortés-Tomás, MT, and Giménez-Costa, JA. Evaluation of AUDIT consumption items new adaptation to improve the screening of college students binge drinking. Adicciones. (2020) 32:255–64. doi: 10.20882/adicciones.1145

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

45. Spielberger, CD, Gorsuchm, RL, and Lushene, RE. Manual for the state-trait anxiety inventory. Palo Alto: Consulting Psychologist Press (1970).

Google Scholar

46. Beck, AT, Steer, RA, and Brown, G. Beck depression inventory–II (BDI-II). APA PsycTests (1996). doi: 10.1037/t00742-000,

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

47. Fadeev, KA, Smirnov, AS, Zhigalova, OP, Bazhina, PS, Tumialis, AV, and Golokhvast, KS. Too real to be virtual: autonomic and eeg responses to extreme stress scenarios in virtual reality. Behav Neurol. (2020) 2020:5758038. doi: 10.1155/2020/5758038

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

48. Wang, H, Wang, Q, and Hu, F. Are you afraid of heights and suitable for working at height? Biomed Signal Process Control. (2019) 52:23–31. doi: 10.1016/j.bspc.2019.03.011

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

49. Aspiotis, V, Miltiadous, A, Kalafatakis, K, Tzimourta, KD, Giannakeas, N, Tsipouras, MG, et al. Assessing electroencephalography as a stress indicator: a vr high-altitude scenario monitored through EEG and ECG. Sensors (Basel). (2022) 22:5792. doi: 10.3390/s22155792

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

50. Delorme, A, and Makeig, S. EEGLAB: an open-source toolbox for analysis of single-trial EEG dynamics including independent component analysis. J Neurosci Methods. (2004) 134:9–21. doi: 10.1016/j.jneumeth.2003.10.009

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

51. Hatlestad-Hall, C, Rygvold, TW, and Andersson, S. BIDS-structured resting-state electroencephalography (EEG) data extracted from an experimental paradigm. Data Brief. (2022) 45:108647. doi: 10.1016/j.dib.2022.108647

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

52. de Cheveigné, A. ZapLine: a simple and effective method to remove power line artifacts. NeuroImage. (2020) 207:116356. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2019.116356

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

53. Belouchrani, A, Abed-Meraim, K, Cardoso, J, and Moulines, É. Second order blind separation of temporally correlated sources. Digit Signal Process. (1993)

Google Scholar

54. Pion-Tonachini, L, Kreutz-Delgado, K, and Makeig, S. ICLabel: an automated electroencephalographic independent component classifier, dataset, and website. NeuroImage. (2019) 198:181–97. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2019.05.026

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

55. Faul, F, Erdfelder, E, Lang, AG, and Buchner, A. G*power 3: a flexible statistical power analysis program for the social, behavioral, and biomedical sciences. Behav Res Methods. (2007) 39:175–91. doi: 10.3758/bf03193146

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

56. Kang, H. Sample size determination and power analysis using the G*power software. J Educ Eval Health Prof. (2021) 18:17. doi: 10.3352/jeehp.2021.18.17

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

57. Biedermann, SV, Biedermann, DG, Wenzlaff, F, Kurjak, T, Nouri, S, Auer, MK, et al. An elevated plus-maze in mixed reality for studying human anxiety-related behavior. BMC Biol. (2017) 15:125. doi: 10.1186/s12915-017-0463-6

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

58. Affan, RO, Huang, S, Cruz, SM, Holcomb, LA, Nguyen, E, and Marinkovic, K. High-intensity binge drinking is associated with alterations in spontaneous neural oscillations in young adults. Alcohol. (2018) 70:51–60. doi: 10.1016/j.alcohol.2018.01.002

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

59. Almeida-Antunes, N, Crego, A, Carbia, C, Sousa, SS, Rodrigues, R, Sampaio, A, et al. Electroencephalographic signatures of the binge drinking pattern during adolescence and young adulthood: a PRISMA-driven systematic review. NeuroImage. (2021) 29:102537. doi: 10.1016/j.nicl.2020.102537

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

60. Sampedro-Piquero, P, Moreno-Fernández, RD, Begega, A, López, M, and Santín, LJ. Long-term consequences of alcohol use in early adolescent mice: focus on neuroadaptations in GR, CRF and BDNF. Addict Biol. (2022) 27:e13158. doi: 10.1111/adb.13158

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

61. Lee, KM, Coehlo, M, McGregor, HA, Waltermire, RS, and Szumlinski, KK. Binge alcohol drinking elicits persistent negative affect in mice. Behav Brain Res. (2015) 291:385–98. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2015.05.055

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

62. Biedermann, SV, Roth, L, Biedermann, D, and Fuss, J. Reliability of repeated exposure to the human elevated plus-maze in virtual reality: behavioral, emotional, and autonomic responses. Behav Res Methods. (2022) 56:187–98. doi: 10.3758/s13428-022-02046-5

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

63. Krupic, D, Zuro, B, and Corr, PJ. Anxiety and threat magnification in subjective and physiological responses of fear of heights induced by virtual reality. Pers Individ Differ. (2021) 169:109720. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2019.109720

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

64. Evans-Polce, RJ, Schuler, MS, Schulenberg, JE, and Patrick, ME. Gender- and age-varying associations of sensation seeking and substance use across young adulthood. Addict Behav. (2018) 84:271–7. doi: 10.1016/j.addbeh.2018.05.003

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

65. de Haan, L, Egberts, AC, and Heerdink, ER. The relation between risk-taking behavior and alcohol use in young adults is different for men and women. Drug Alcohol Depend. (2015) 155:222–7. doi: 10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2015.07.013

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

66. Zhang, L, Zhang, C, and Shang, L. Sensation-seeking and domain-specific risk-taking behavior among adolescents: risk perceptions and expected benefits as mediators. Pers Individ Differ. (2016) 101:299–305. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2016.06.002

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

67. Doumas, DM, Miller, R, and Esp, S. Impulsive sensation seeking, binge drinking, and alcohol-related consequences: do protective behavioral strategies help high risk adolescents? Addict Behav. (2017) 64:6–12. doi: 10.1016/j.addbeh.2016.08.003

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

68. Kudielka, BM, Hellhammer, DH, and Wüst, S. Why do we respond so differently? Reviewing determinants of human salivary cortisol responses to challenge. Psychoneuroendocrinology. (2009) 34:2–18. doi: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2008.10.004

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

69. Joseph, NT, Jiang, Y, and Zilioli, S. Momentary emotions and salivary cortisol: a systematic review and meta-analysis of ecological momentary assessment studies. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. (2021) 125:365–79. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2021.02.042

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

70. Bibbey, A, Phillips, AC, Ginty, AT, and Carroll, D. Problematic internet use, excessive alcohol consumption, their comorbidity and cardiovascular and cortisol reactions to acute psychological stress in a student population. J Behav Addict. (2015) 4:44–52. doi: 10.1556/2006.4.2015.006

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

71. Courtney, KE, and Polich, J. Binge drinking effects on EEG in young adult humans. Int J Environl Res Public Health. (2010) 7:2325–36. doi: 10.3390/ijerph7052325

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

72. López-Caneda, E, Cadaveira, F, Correas, A, Crego, A, Maestú, F, and Rodríguez, HS. The brain of binge drinkers at rest: alterations in Theta and Beta oscillations in first-year college students with a binge drinking pattern. Front Behav Neurosci. (2017) 11:168. doi: 10.3389/fnbeh.2017.00168

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

73. Enoch, MA, Rohrbaugh, JW, Davis, EZ, Harris, CR, Ellingson, RJ, Andreason, P, et al. Relationship of genetically transmitted alpha EEG traits to anxiety disorders and alcoholism. Am J Med Genet. (1995) 60:400–8. doi: 10.1002/ajmg.1320600510

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

74. Chandler, DA, Roach, A, Ellison, A, Husid Burton, E, and Jelsone-Swain, L. Symptoms of depression together with trait anxiety increase the ability to predict alpha power change between attention and resting states. Int J Psychophysiol. (2022) 182:57–69. doi: 10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2022.09.010

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

75. Sampedro-Piquero, P, Buades-Sitjar, F, Capilla, A, Zancada-Menéndez, C, González-Baeza, A, and Moreno-Fernández, RD. Risky alcohol use during youth: impact on emotion, cognitive networks, and resting-state EEG activity. Prog Neuro-Psychopharmacol Biol Psychiatry. (2024) 132:110994. doi: 10.1016/j.pnpbp.2024.110994

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

76. Prell, R, Opatz, O, Merati, G, Gesche, B, Gunga, HC, and Maggioni, MA. Heart rate variability, risk-taking behavior and resilience in firefighters during a simulated extinguish-fire task. Front Physiol. (2020) 11:482. doi: 10.3389/fphys.2020.00482

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

77. Balzarotti, S, Biassoni, F, Colombo, B, and Ciceri, M. Cardiac vagal control as a marker of emotion regulation in healthy adults: a review. Biol Psychol. (2017) 130:54–66. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2017.10.008

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

78. Beauchaine, TP, and Thayer, JF. Heart rate variability as a transdiagnostic biomarker of psychopathology. Int J Psychophysiol. (2015) 98:338–50. doi: 10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2015.08.004

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

79. Guinle, MIB, and Sinha, R. The role of stress, trauma, and negative affect in alcohol misuse and alcohol use disorder in women. Alcohol Res. (2020) 40:5. doi: 10.35946/arcr.v40.2.05

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

80. Tobaldini, E, Carandina, A, Toschi-Dias, E, Erba, L, Furlan, L, Sgoifo, A, et al. Depression and cardiovascular autonomic control: a matter of vagus and sex paradox. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. (2020) 116:154–61. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2020.06.029

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: alcohol, binge drinking, stress, neurophysiology, virtual reality, youth

Citation: Moreno-Fernández RD, Bernabéu-Brotons E, Carbonell-Colomer M, Buades-Sitjar F and Sampedro-Piquero P (2024) Sex-related differences in young binge drinkers on the neurophysiological response to stress in virtual reality. Front. Public Health. 12:1348960. doi: 10.3389/fpubh.2024.1348960

Received: 03 December 2023; Accepted: 06 June 2024;
Published: 14 June 2024.

Edited by:

Annie Olivia Campbell, Queen’s University Belfast, United Kingdom

Reviewed by:

Jose Manuel Lerma-Cabrera, University of Almeria, Spain
Signe Tomsone, Riga Stradiņš University, Latvia

Copyright © 2024 Moreno-Fernández, Bernabéu-Brotons, Carbonell-Colomer, Buades-Sitjar and Sampedro-Piquero. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Román D. Moreno-Fernández, romandario.moreno@ufv.es; Patricia Sampedro-Piquero, patricia.sampedro@uam.es

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.