Skip to main content

SYSTEMATIC REVIEW article

Front. Public Health, 18 January 2024
Sec. Public Mental Health
This article is part of the Research Topic The Impact of Social Media and Technology on Mental Health View all 7 articles

Google effects on memory: a meta-analytical review of the media effects of intensive Internet search behavior

Chen GongChen Gong1Yang Yang,
Yang Yang2,3*
  • 1School of Journalism, Fudan University, Shanghai, China
  • 2College of Media and Arts, Shanghai University of Sport, Shanghai, China
  • 3College of Communication, Shanghai Lida University, Shanghai, China

People are increasingly using the web for fact-checking and other forms of information seeking. The “Google effects” refers to the idea that individuals rely on the Internet as a source of knowledge rather than remembering it for themselves. However, few literature review have yet comprehensively examined the media effects of this intensive Internet search behavior. In this study, by carrying out meta-analysis, we found that google effects is closely associated with cognitive load, behavioral phenotype and cognitive self-esteem. And this phenomenon is also more likely to happen while using a mobile phone to browse the Internet rather than a computer. People with a larger knowledge base are less susceptible to the consequences of Internet use than those with a smaller knowledge base. The media effect was stronger for persons who had used the Internet before than for those who had not. And meta-analyses show that participants in North America (parameter = −1.0365, 95%CI = [−1.8758, −0.1972], p < 0.05) are more susceptible to frequent Internet search behavior relative to other regions. Overall, google effects on memory challenges the way individuals seek and read information, and it may lead to changes in cognitive and memory mechanisms.

1 Introduction

People are increasingly using the web for fact-checking and other forms of information seeking. The “Google effect” (1) refers to the idea that individuals rely on the Internet as a source of knowledge rather than remembering it for themselves. Humans may be less eager to answer inquiries willingly as a result of Internet access, preferring to utilize search engines instead.

Based on Ebbinghau (2) seminal work on the memory curve, it can be observed that a significant portion of the information that an individual reads is progressively forgotten, with the most rapid decline occurring on the initial day. Nowadays, the advent of the Internet era has significantly facilitated the process by which individuals obtain information, resulting in an increased ease of access. Simultaneously, the rate at which individuals forget information has experienced a notable acceleration. Based on findings in the field of neuroscience, it has been observed that the strength of memory is directly related to the frequency of memory retrieval. This implies that the hippocampus, as the primary processor of information, plays a crucial role in storing individuals’ memories, while the capacity to access these memories remains constant (3).

Nevertheless, the advent of search engines has rendered the capacity to retrieve certain pieces of information obsolete. After individuals have acquired proficiency in the cognitive process of use the search engine “Google” to retrieve information, they can discontinue the practice of performing the remaining content to memory, resulting in a gradual reduction in their reliance on external memory for stated material. The publication of an original research article by Sparrow et al. (1) in the esteemed journal Science has garnered significant interest, resulting in a substantial number of citations thus far. In this article, Sparrow explicitly asserted that individuals exhibit a readiness to contemplate desktops when confronted with difficult problems. And when people anticipate future access to information, they have lower information memory rates and a better recall of where to obtain the information. For instance, if they need to know the price of a commonly used everyday item, learn how to perform complex computer skills, or simply want to remember the name of the singer of a song they are listening to, they only need to turn on their computer, mobile phone, or any Internet-connected device with a search function, and they can find the answer instantly. It has become so ingrained in researchers to seek answers to any problem as soon as it arises that they may experience withdrawal if they are unable to find one immediately. And it is difficult to recall how people obtained information before the Internet became a pervasive part of our daily lives, as well as how information was discovered prior to the invention of the Internet. In this way, the Internet has a type of external or transactive memory in which data is stored externally.

Based on the transactive memory system, Kahn and Martinez (4) examined the relationship between the concepts of “Google’s effect on memory” and “cognitive self-esteem” in the interpersonal contexts of snapchat and text messaging as a complement to Sparrow et al. (1). The cognitive hypothesis regarding the effect of Internet use on memory is that people use computers as “transactive memory companions.” Wegner (5) was the first to propose an interactive memory system for encoding, storing, retrieving, and communicating information from numerous fields of knowledge. This division of labor typically occurs in close proximity (6). In additional ways, external memory can serve as a source or target for cognitive offloading (7). For this reason, many experts assert that the Internet’s influence on memory and associated studies are limited to “technology partners” (811).

In the fields of brain science and neuroimage science, researchers are accumulating evidence for “Google effect.” Most directly, the cognitive effects of Internet use may result in changes to brain structure, such as the density of gray matter (12). Due to the lack of scientific literature examining the neural correlations involved in online information processing, his experiments have contributed to the advancement of brain science research on the phenomenon of memory loss caused by Internet use. In addition to long-term Internet search behavior, short-term behavior has the potential to increase Internet reliance (13). In Wang et al. (13) experiment, a comparison of post-test and pre-test data on changes in brain activation as well as a comparison of self-reported changes in neural impulses to unknown trials were obtained by setting up participants for Internet search training, further demonstrating that after training on Internet search questions (even for a short period of time), participants had a greater impulse to use the search engine again when presented with a query.

Traditional social science disciplines have considered “Google effect.” Several social science researchers, in conjunction with BBC television, predicted that search engines such as Google would cause a fundamental shift in the way people search and comprehend information in the twenty-first century (14, 15), which prompted sociologists and anthropologists to study this phenomenon. Parslow (16) also discovered that when the Internet was present in the Head Start environment, it was more difficult for children to concentrate and engage with lengthy texts. However, Parslow (16) later statement that “the Internet and related technologies are actually good for the brain” lacks evidence to support it, at least not from his short article, even though there is similar evidence that (1) Only on other social groupings has the Internet reduced reliance (17); and (2) Having effective integrated skills is more valuable than biological memory (18). Moreover, it is not only the Internet that can prevent people from becoming dependent (19).

Different academics have analyzed “Google effect” on various age groups to determine whether or not it is universal across all ages. Danovitch (20) summarized youths’ understanding of the use of web devices from birth to age eight, revealing that children’s judgments about the Internet are influenced by their own personal experiences (e.g., the belief that they can only play games on the Internet) and that children are less likely to rely on the Internet for answers because they take longer to trust the information they obtain from the Internet. However, as they become adults, the younger age group is more likely to anticipate memory changes (21) because they perceive themselves as more adept Internet users, whereas the more experienced are more likely to experience a decline in their internal memory. Even if older people have excellent Internet search skills, “Google effect” is unlikely to be present, especially for those with cognitive impairments (22). Slegers et al. (22) extended the study to a group of cognitively normal older individuals with no Internet experience and found no evidence that learning Internet search skills has any cognitive effect on inexperienced older individuals. However, older people can also take advantage of the Internet’s emerging opportunities to receive new sources of cognitive stimulation, even more so than younger people (23), suggesting that “Google Effect” has different effects on older and younger individuals. Notably, there have also been ethnically specific studies, such as the study of the attention span of Finns when using the Internet (21).

In summary, the inconsistencies in previous research constitute a formidable barrier to understanding gender differences in media effects of frequent Internet search behavior. The key question addressed in this study is what factors may moderate, amplify, attenuate, or conceal the potential differences in “Google effects.” In prior findings (24), meta-analysis is an ideal technique for synthesizing group differences and identifying potential contributors to inconsistency or heterogeneity. Consequently, the present study adopted a meta-analytic approach and sought to synthesize the magnitude and direction of potential differences in effects of frequent Internet search behavior, as well as to identify potential moderators of those differences.

1.1 Potential factors related to inconsistent findings

1.1.1 Region

The presence of diverse cultures and work habits across various regions can potentially influence the manner in which individuals with varying cognitive frameworks utilize search engines for information retrieval. Consequently, this can have implications for the assessment of search engines’ influence on individuals’ perceptions (25). Therefore, we explore whether region (e.g., North America, Europe, or Asia) moderates the relationship between frequent Internet search behavior and cognitive effects.

1.1.2 Gender

The literature reviewed in this paper does not currently include any research that has specifically investigated the Google effects in relation to gender differences. Nevertheless, considering that the majority of the articles under examination provide data on the gender distribution of individuals, we have properly considered this as a plausible variable that could have impacted the outcomes. Due to significant variations in the gender distribution among the studies (26, 27), we incorporated a column indicating the percentage of males in the subsequent meta-analysis figures, in order to mitigate potential discrepancies across publications.

1.1.3 Type of sample

Different sample features may be related to conflicting findings on gender differences in google effects on memory. For instance, with an adolescent sample, Yu et al. (28) found a large gender difference in search behavior leading to cognitive change. With a community sample of adults, however, Hamilton et al. (25) and Kahn and Martinez (4) did not detect a significant gender difference. Moreover, Kamin et al. (27) and Yu et al. (28) showed that gender differences in search behavior leading to cognitive change were smaller in online databases samples than in college samples. Sample characteristics (e.g., college vs. public databases sample) speak to the strictness of a participant’s immediate situation—such as the behavioral restriction within the living community—which may affect the measurement of results. Therefore, it is imperative to investigate whether the nature of the sample or cohort might influence the association between gender and google effects on memory.

1.1.4 Experiment measurement type

The researchers conducting this meta-analysis have thoroughly examined and categorized the various types of experiments discussed in the articles pertaining to the Google effects on memory. These experiments have been classified into five distinct categories, denoted as cognitive load (cognitive load theory, CLT, is an instructional framework grounded in our understanding of human cognition. It relies on a cognitive architecture comprising a restricted working memory, which is partially isolated from processing units responsible for visual and auditory information. This working memory engages in interactions with an unlimited long-term memory) (1, 29, 30), cognitive measurement (the development of new automated data collection methods, the application of cognitive psychology concepts and methods to reduce survey measurement error) (17, 25, 31), behavioral phenotype (they are recognizable patterns of behavior-syndromes) (1, 32, 33), cognitive self-esteem (individuals with greater confidence in their own intelligence, memory and ability would hold the belief that information is easily accessible, such as through the use of a search engine) (4, 8) and psychophysiology (measurements with physiological instruments, e.g., EEG, fMRI, etc.) (13, 17). The subsequent sections of the meta-analysis further categorizes these five groups to derive more accurate and specific conclusions.

1.1.5 Age

Previous studies have demonstrated that the extent and orientation of differences in search behavior leading to cognitive change may vary throughout the stages of adolescence, young adulthood, and middle age. For instance, Yu et al. (28) conducted a study examining problematic Internet use among a sample of adolescents (M = 15.33, SD = 0.47). Similarly, Sanchiz et al. (34) conducted a study investigating the impact of frequent search engine usage on human memory cognition. They employed a cognitive aging-related experimental design, which included both an experimental group (M = 66.00 years old, SD = 3.45) and a control group (M = 21.28 years old, SD = 1.78). There exists a substantial disparity in the effect sizes observed in the outcomes of the two previously mentioned experiments. Therefore, age may also influence the media effects brought by Internet search behavior.

2 Methods

2.1 Selection of studies

A literature search was conducted for studies that had examined google effects on memory published up to June 2023. A computer-based search was conducted using ACM Digital library, PsycINFO, Web of Science, IEEE Xplore, and Scopus. To mitigate the possibility of unintentionally excluding literature, the entire search procedure was replicated three times at one-month intervals in these databases. Records containing the search string in their titles or abstracts were located and the search string can be found in https://osf.io/b8d94/ (see Supplementary material S1 as well). For the search string, it was divided by the author into three distinct sections: the object or phenomenon, the method or tool employed, and the journal type. The most frequently searched keywords were entered into each section, employing logical linkers such as “AND” and “OR” and wildcards like “*” to facilitate the search for multiple forms of the term throughout the screening procedure. For instance, the input “memor*” may return results for terms such as “memory,” “memorize,” and so on, but not words like “distributed memory.” To link each input, an “OR” link was necessary due to their relatively similar meanings. The initial search identified 908 records and 35 studies in 22 different articles were finally considered. The meta-analysis incorporated studies that satisfied the following set of criteria: (1) The decision to use English as the language of publication for this study was based on previous research findings that indicated a lack of substantial evidence suggesting any systematic bias resulting from the restriction to English language (35) in meta-analyses; (2) was published in peer-reviewed journals or conferences; (3) provided comparisons between various experiments in terms of google effects on memory, and also reported or made it possible to calculate the effect size (Cohen’s d) for these comparisons; (4) Experiments must be well designed methodologically. We refer to the criteria related to assessing experiments quality in Chen et al. (36), for example, “was the study population clearly specified and defined” and “well developed with reporting validity and/or reliability.” And experiments with qualitative or quantitative analysis of evidence to support conclusions, and if part of the experiment involves a moral and ethical test, only ethically certified experiments will be considered.

We did not include unpublished findings for the following reasons: (1) Only original research and quantitative literature review could be selected, and it should not be a novel, communication letters or editorial report, etc.; (2) Studies that do not focus directly on the methods used for memory or cognition change on Internet use, but merely refer to the various effects of the Internet; (3) Studies not discussing the effect of using search engines but only focusing on the different methods to use them; (4) Reports on the same subject that have been published more than once (37). So in this research, the most comprehensive iteration of the study was included when multiple reports of the study were found in various journals; (5) other spontaneous surveys in the literature (e.g., no defined research objectives; no defined search process; no defined data collection process).

2.2 Coding of studies

The discrepancies among the raters were resolved through engaging in discussions with the first and second authors. The following information was extracted from each of the included studies: paper ID and study ID; authors; publication year; regions (north America, Europe, Asia); age; male, female and percentage of males; treatment and control group sample size; experimental measurement indicators; research type (cognitive load, cognitive measurement, behavioral phenotype, cognitive self-esteem, psychophysiology); and Cohen’s d or the information for computing Cohen’s d. The two coders were trained to code a set of articles that had been expertcoded for another study prior to beginning formal coding. Once the coders had acquired experience in coding articles for meta-analyses, they commenced the formal coding process. The reliability of the final intercoder varied between 0.91 and 1.00 (Krippendorff’s alpha). Discrepancies were resolved through discussions. For included studies that did not directly report Cohen’s d, means and standard deviations of experiment and control groups for were extracted and used to calculate d. Where necessary, we changed the sign of the published Cohen’s d to ensure that all effect sizes were consistent.

2.3 Data analyses

The data analyses were performed using R (38) and the metafor package (39). We conducted mean effect size analyses, heterogeneity tests and other required analysis. Initially, to calculate a comprehensive mean effect size, we employed a random-effects model as we anticipated substantial variability in effect sizes across studies. And the determination of the effect size of Cohen’s d was derived from (40) guidelines, which established 0.20 as the threshold for a small effect, 0.50 for a medium effect, and 0.80 for a large effect.

Furthermore, the evaluation of unexplained heterogeneity is an essential component in the process of conducting a meta-analysis. Both random sampling error, which refers to the variability observed within a study, and systematic study features, such as the characteristics of the samples, the selection of measurement instruments, and the type of publication, have the potential to contribute to the heterogeneity observed among studies. The outcomes of the heterogeneity test have a direct impact on the choice of statistical models, specifically fixed-effects versus random-effects, when conducting an analysis of effect sizes. The Cochrane Q statistic and the I2 index are frequently employed in the evaluation of the statistical significance of unexplained heterogeneity (41). Based on the I2 statistic, mild heterogeneity is indicated by a range of 0–40%, moderate heterogeneity is within the range of 40–60%, greater heterogeneity is represented by a range of 50–90%, and great heterogeneity is denoted by a range of 75–100%.

Thirdly, we conducted moderator analyses to investigate the potential factors that may have contributed to the variability observed in previous research findings. Several potential moderators were examined (e.g., publication year, region, age etc.). We suspected that if there is a statistically significant moderating effect was found for a categorical moderator comprising more than two levels, a post-hoc comparisons to examine the gaps between the levels within the moderator would be processed. To reduce the risk of Type one errors, we employed Bonferroni corrections during the post-hoc pairwise comparisons.

Publication bias for articles is generally illustrated by a funnel plot (42), which estimated that if there is a certain relative symmetry between the studies on both sides of the vertical line of the combined effect size, and with a non-significant p-value (i.e., greater than 0.050) indicating insufficient evidence for publication bias (43).

3 Results

3.1 Study selection

The selection of studies for this meta-analysis is illustrated graphically in Figure 1. The initial search identified 908 records from the five (ACM Digital library, PsycINFO, Web of Science, IEEE Xplore and Scopus) databases. After removing duplicates, 879 studies remained. After excluding studies based on their titles and abstracts in accordance with the criteria described in the method section, 69 studies were included in the full-text review. Twenty two out of 69 full texts were determined to meet the meta-analysis selection criteria. In addition, the reference lists of all the chosen articles were examined for additional relevant studies, but no additional records were found. Among these 22 articles, seven contained multiple independent sub-studies with samples (multiple tasks in the same sample or overlap of participant sample) (1, 17, 25, 31, 44, 45), resulting in 35 independent comparisons of google effects on memory. More details of initial records and considered articles can be found in Supplementary materials S3, S4.

Figure 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. PRISMA study selection flowchart.

3.2 Descriptive characteristics of the studies included

The 22 selected articles involved a total of 30,889 participants with their ages ranging from 12 to 89 years old. The dates of publications ranged from 2011 to 2021. The reported effect size (Cohen’s d) of cognitive effects by frequent Internet search behavior ranged from −0.85 (31) to 4.38 (46), with a positive effect size indicating higher levels of google effects on memory. The descriptive characteristics of the studies are shown in https://osf.io/b8d94/.

3.3 Overall analysis

Using a random-effects model, the average effect size and its 95% confidence interval were estimated. The pooled d indicated a moderate but statistically significant effect size. The forest plot is described in appendix in our open data platform.

3.4 Subgroup analysis

Since the experiments included in the meta-analysis were mainly classified into five categories of research type: cognitive load, cognitive measurement, behavioral phenotype, cognitive self-esteem and psychophysiology. we did subgroup analyses of the included literature and the results are shown in Figure 2. For the cognitive load subgroup, observed outcome = 0.73, 95%CI = [0.22, 1.24], which shows the estimates of cognitive load are positively influenced, indicating that this factor has a significant positive effect on the variables analyzed. And this is similar to behavioral phenotype subgroup and cognitive self-esteem subgroup, with observed outcome = 0.39, 95%CI = [0.16, 0.61] and observed outcome = 0.91, 95%CI = [0.23, 1.59] respectively. For the cognitive measurement subgroup, observed outcome = 0.56, 95%CI = [−0.15, 1.28], although the estimates for the cognitive measures were positive, 95%CI included zero, so it was not possible to conclude whether this factor had a significant effect on the variables analyzed. And this is similar to psychophysiology subgroup, with observed outcome = 1.01, 95%CI = [−0.12, 2.13] (for more details, see Figure 3).

Figure 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Funnel plot for checking publication bias. (1) Is the funnel plot for overall publication bias, (2)~(6) are plots for subgroups publication bias.

Figure 3
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 3. Subgroup analysis.

3.5 Moderator analyses

The results of moderator analyses are shown in Table 1. Overall, results indicated that only one variable contributed significantly to the heterogeneity of effect sizes: region (Q = 6.1997, p < 0.050) accounting for 13.18% of the total heterogeneity. We argued that there are other omitted variables that are not included in the regression and are therefore absorbed into the intercept term, resulting in the intercept being significant but some variables not being significant. Specifically, for region, the pooled d from Europe (d = −1.1063, 95%CI = [−2.3157, 0.103]) was larger (take an absolute value) than that from North America (d = −1.0365, 95%CI = [−1.8758, −0.1972], p < 0.05). One possible hypothesis posits that there exists a positive correlation between the frequency of Internet search engine utilization and both the availability of broadband connections in the area and the local GDP (47). Furthermore, this discovery is consistent with the results of a recent study (48) that examined populations across 21 regions and countries and discovered that the impact of social media fatigue and Internet addiction was more pronounced in Europe compared to the America and Asia. For research type, the pooled d from cognitive self-esteem (d = −0.5262, 95%CI = [−2.1533, 1.1009]) was the greatest (take an absolute value) and the parameter from psychophysiology (d = 0.1297, 95%CI = [−1.5356, 1.7950]) was the smallest (see Table 1 for more details).

Table 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Moderator analyses.

3.6 Publication bias and sensitivity analysis

The analysis of publication bias for these articles is typically illustrated by a funnel plot, as shown in Figure 2. This funnel plot demonstrates that there is a certain degree of relative symmetry between the studies on both sides of the vertical line of the combined effect size, leading to the conclusion that publication bias is insignificant.

4 Discussion

Using meta-analytic techniques, this study provided a quantitative summary of the literature on media effects brought by frequent Internet search behavior prior to Jun 2023. Based on a random-effects model, our findings indicated that frequent Internet search behavior may lead to the change of cognitive load, behavioral phenotype and cognitive self-esteem. It was also discovered that one significant moderator (region) contributed to the heterogeneity of previous findings.

This paper will address two primary issues: How do people use their human memory when working on Internet-based projects? (2) How does the use of the Internet affect human cognition (this could be memory or a related concept)? These two questions are now answerable. To answer the first question, it is possible for people to forget where their thoughts end up and what is stored internally versus what is stored online when they use the internet to access information. Because the Internet is an example of a repository for interactive memory, people who operate on the Internet can easily rely on this memory. Since the advent of the Internet, people’s cognitive memories have changed. Even if the cognitive mechanisms that comprise the interactive memory system do not change, Internet users may begin to model characteristics of the Internet into their own self-perceptions, and they may believe that they are particularly adept at thinking and remembering information, despite the fact that the Internet is increasingly responsible for “remembering” information (1, 49). Moreover, when people search for information on the Internet while working on the Internet, they are more likely to use the Internet rather than their brain the next time they encounter that issue, and they retain pertinent information in an interesting way: they remember the Internet address where the pertinent information is stored (e.g., domain name, database, etc.). People who have searched the Internet for a solution to a problem, for example, will remember the website where they found the solution more vividly when they encounter the problem again (50), even if they have forgotten the precise essence of the problem for which they were searching.

To answer the second question, the effect of working on the Internet is an essential-to-phenomenal process that first may have caused some changes in brain structure or function, e.g., the act of using the Internet creates new connections in the brain’s synapses, causing further structural changes in the brain’s gray matter layer, and then these changes in brain plasticity cause individuals to misinterpret the content of transactive memories. People are more likely to return to the Internet and repeat the search process when confronted with a similar issue if they remember the location where the information was saved. Moreover, people’s perceptions of findability roughly predict the amount of time it will take them to find the information they need on the Internet, which increases their reliance on it laterally (38). Moreover, when searching for solutions on the Internet, rapid responses (answers that are obtained more quickly) are more likely to be convincing. People feel more confident when working in an Internet-accessible environment, and “Google effect” is stronger for those who have previously used the Internet. Race and age influence the “Google effect” differently.

Academic perspectives on this phenomenon range from support to concern, but the vast majority have considered ways to coexist with it. Basic research on the Google effect has been completed, and for future research directions, the primary focus will be on close integration with the social sciences (experimental psychology, situational memory, cognitive psychology, etc.); expanding the range of experimental subjects (sample size, expanding the age range of the population, e.g., the experimental phenomenon of memory effects caused by Internet use in the older adult is still unclear), and establishing universal e-learning environments. Determine the extent to which memory is affected by different Internet devices and the actual Internet environment (i.e., the presence of misleading information on the Internet) on human memory.

Over thousands of years, humans have demonstrated a remarkable capacity to employ new technology to grow and enhance their cognitive abilities; therefore, fear of new technologies is not new. Academics believe that the Internet’s benefits will ultimately outweigh any disadvantages it may have, even if the current era is unique and the Internet’s features are vastly different from those of previous technologies. In addition, by examining the potential costs and limitations of the Internet, individuals are in a better position to develop and modify the technology so that it is potentially more productive, less disruptive, and more consistent with the everyday goals and functions of human cognition.

There are some limitations on our present study. Due to the lack of solicitation for ongoing or unpublished studies from google effect researchers, there is a potential for the missing of certain relevant research that were not included in the databases searched. Second, while the current study identified significant moderators, it is possible that some moderators may have been neglected because they may not be highlighted during our coding process. Third, in moderators’ analysis, we found that the pooled d from Europe was larger than that from North America, we considered the region analysis could be optimized further (e.g., by including country-specific data). Fourth, funnel plots were employed to investigate publication bias in our current study. However, potential sources of bias may still exist. Subsequent related research could employ additional tests [e.g., Trim-and-Fill test (51, 52)] for further study.

Despite these limitations, our findings have various theoretical and practical implications. Regarding the former, this is possibly the first meta-analytical review on google effects, and it makes a significant academic contribution to the evidence base. This study is also carried out during the time that people may have more media and search engines exposure (53). Regarding practical implications, our findings may be useful for local government and teachers. The regional differences in susceptibility to frequent Internet search behavior may prompt policymakers to think about adapting some measures to reduce google effects on memory by influencing the way local people think if possible. One plausible strategy may involve a record of application usage durations and the distribution of individualized security alerts to particular user groups. Our research holds significant implications for a broad classroom teachers’ group, given the prevalence of mobile device usage (e.g., tablets) in the classroom today. While such devices are used, their extent of student usage must be strictly regulated. As illustrated by Hochberg et al. (26), we cannot simply infer if a higher learning efficacy can be achieved by using video analysis with tablets in comparison with traditional physics classes. So we also highly recommend the implementation of campaigns to raise public awareness of intensive Internet use behavior and promote media literacy. To mitigate the negative effects of this, future research may investigate some applicable psychological therapies such as mindfulness and positive thinking therapy. Additionally, correlations between specific search engines’ characteristics and individual differences, such as personality traits, could be investigated further to determine which population is most susceptible to google effects.

In sum, this is the first meta-analytical research review for google effects on memory. Frequent Internet search behavior was found to be closely associated with cognitive load, behavioral phenotype and cognitive self-esteem. And this phenomenon is also more likely to happen while using a mobile phone to browse the Internet rather than a computer. People with a larger knowledge base are less susceptible to the consequences of internet use than those with a smaller knowledge base. The media effect was stronger for persons who had used the Internet before than for those who had not. And meta-analyses show that participants in North America (parameter = −1.0365, 95%CI = [−1.8758, −0.1972], p < 0.05) are more susceptible to frequent Internet search behavior relative to other regions. Overall, google effects on memory challenges the way individuals seek and read information, and it may lead to changes in cognitive and memory mechanisms. A future update of the current meta-analysis can then be conducted when more research collecting and reporting year, type, method, gender, region, and subgroups differences becomes available.

Data availability statement

The datasets presented in this study can be found in online repositories. The names of the repository/repositories and accession number(s) can be found in the article/Supplementary material.

Author contributions

CG: Conceptualization, Data curation, Formal analysis, Methodology, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing. YY: Validation, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing.

Funding

The author(s) declare that no financial support was received for the research, authorship, and/or publication of this article.

Acknowledgments

Thanks the reviewers and editors for their suggestions on this manuscript.

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher’s note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

Supplementary material

The Supplementary material for this article can be found online at: https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fpubh.2024.1332030/full#supplementary-material

References

1. Sparrow, B, Liu, J, and Wegner, DM. Google effects on memory: cognitive consequences of having information at our fingertips. Science. (2011) 333:776–8. doi: 10.1126/science.1207745

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

2. Ebbinghaus, Hermann. (1885). Memory: a contribution to experimental psychology. Transl. H.A. Ruger and C.E. Bussenius. Teachers College, Columbia University, New York.

Google Scholar

3. Burgess, N, Maguire, EA, and O’Keefe, J. The human Hippocampus and spatial and episodic memory. Neuron. (2002) 35:625–41. doi: 10.1016/S0896-6273(02)00830-9

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

4. Kahn, AS, and Martinez, TM. Text and you might miss it? Snap and you might remember? Exploring ‘Google effects on memory’ and cognitive self-esteem in the context of snapchat and text messaging. Comput Hum Behav. (2020) 104:106166. doi: 10.1016/j.chb.2019.106166

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

5. Wegner, DM. Transactive memory: a contemporary analysis of the group mind. Theor Group Behav. (1987):185–208. doi: 10.1007/978-1-4612-4634-3_9

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

6. Brandon, DP, and Hollingshead, AB. Transactive memory systems in organizations: matching tasks, expertise, and people. Organ Sci. (2004) 15:633–44. doi: 10.1287/orsc.1040.0069

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

7. Risko, EF, and Gilbert, SJ. Cognitive offloading. Trends in cognitive sciences. (2016) 20:676–688.

Google Scholar

8. Hamilton, KA, and Yao, MZ. Blurring boundaries: effects of device features on metacognitive evaluations. Trends Cogn.Sci. (2018) 89:213–20. doi: 10.1016/j.chb.2018.07.044

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

9. Hollan, J, Hutchins, E, and Kirsh, D. Distributed cognition: toward a new foundation for human-computer interaction research. ACM Trans Comput Hum Interact. (2000) 7:174–96. doi: 10.1145/353485.353487

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

10. Ward, AF. Supernormal: how the internet is changing our memories and our minds. Psychol Inq. (2013) 24:341–8. doi: 10.1080/1047840X.2013.850148

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

11. Yacci, Michael, and Rozanski, Evelyn P.. (2012). Student information consumption strategies: implications of the Google effect ACM International Conference Proceeding Series, Toronto, ON, 248–253.

Google Scholar

12. Loh, KK, and Kanai, R. How has the internet reshaped human cognition? Neuroscientist. (2016) 22:506–20. doi: 10.1177/1073858415595005

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

13. Wang, Y, Wu, L, Luo, L, Zhang, Y, and Dong, G. Short-term internet search using makes people rely on search engines when facing unknown issues. PLoS One. (2017) 12:e0176325. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0176325

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

14. Nicholas, D, Rowlands, I, Clark, D, and Williams, P. Google generation II: web behaviour experiments with the BBC. ASLIB Proc. (2011) 63:28–45. doi: 10.1108/00012531111103768

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

15. Rowlands, I, Nicholas, D, Williams, P, Huntington, P, Fieldhouse, M, Gunter, B, et al. The Google generation: the information behaviour of the researcher of the future. ASLIB Proc. (2008) 60:290–310. doi: 10.1108/00012530810887953

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

16. Parslow, GR. Commentary: how the internet is changing the way we think, read and remember. Biochem Mol Biol Educ. (2011) 39:228. doi: 10.1002/bmb.20514

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

17. Fisher, M, Goddu, MK, and Keil, FC. Searching for explanations: how the internet inflates estimates of internal knowledge. J Exp Psychol Gen. (2015) 144:674–87. doi: 10.1037/xge0000070

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

18. Heersmink, R. The internet, cognitive enhancement, and the values of cognition. Mind Mach. (2016) 26:389–407. doi: 10.1007/s11023-016-9404-3

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

19. Tamir, DI, Templeton, EM, Ward, AF, and Zaki, J. Media usage diminishes memory for experiences. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology. (2018) 76:161–168.

Google Scholar

20. Danovitch, JH. Growing up with Google: how children’s understanding and use of internet-based devices relates to cognitive development. Hum Behav Emerg Technol. (2019) 1:81–90. doi: 10.1002/hbe2.142

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

21. Näsi, M, and Koivusilta, L. Internet and everyday life: the perceived implications of internet use on memory and ability to concentrate. Cyberpsychol Behav Soc Netw. (2013) 16:88–93. doi: 10.1089/cyber.2012.0058

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

22. Slegers, K, van Boxtel, M, and Jolles, J. Effects of computer training and internet usage on cognitive abilities in older adults: a randomized controlled study. Aging Clin Exp Res. (2009) 21:43–54. doi: 10.1007/BF03324898

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

23. McElroy, E, Shevlin, M, Murphy, S, Roberts, B, Makhashvili, N, Javakhishvili, J, et al. The “online brain”: how the internet may be changing our cognition. World Psychiatry. (2019) 18:236–7. doi: 10.1002/wps.20638

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

24. Bornstein, DB, Beets, MW, Byun, W, and McIver, K. Accelerometer-derived physical activity levels of preschoolers: a meta-analysis. J Sci Med Sport. (2011) 14:504–511.

Google Scholar

25. Hamilton, KA, McIntyre, KP, and Hertel, PT. Judging knowledge in the digital age: the role of external-memory organization: judging knowledge in the digital age. Appl Cogn Psychol. (2016) 30:1080–7. doi: 10.1002/acp.3277

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

26. Hochberg, K, Becker, S, Louis, M, Klein, P, and Kuhn, J. Using smartphones as experimental tools-a follow-up: cognitive effects by video analysis and reduction of cognitive load by multiple representations. J Sci Educ Technol. (2020) 29:303–17. doi: 10.1007/s10956-020-09816-w

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

27. Kamin, ST, Seifert, A, and Lang, FR. Participation in activities mediates the effect of internet use on cognitive functioning in old age. Int Psychogeriatr. (2021) 33:83–8. doi: 10.1017/S1041610220003634

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

28. Yu, JJ, Kim, H, and Hay, I. Understanding adolescents’ problematic internet use from a social/cognitive and addiction research framework. Comput Hum Behav. (2013) 29:2682–9. doi: 10.1016/j.chb.2013.06.045

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

29. Canale, N, Vieno, A, Doro, M, Mineo, ER, Marino, C, and Billieux, J. Emotion-related impulsivity moderates the cognitive interference effect of smartphone availability on working memory. Sci Rep. (2019) 9:18519. doi: 10.1038/s41598-019-54911-7

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

30. Shin, MS, Oh, SJ, Seo, S, Lee, JH, and Song, MJ. Effects of smartphone-based memory training for older adults with subjective memory complaints. Eur Psychiatry. (2017) 41:S176–6. doi: 10.1016/j.eurpsy.2017.01.2075

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

31. Frost, P, Donahue, P, Goeben, K, Connor, M, Cheong, HS, and Schroeder, A. An examination of the potential lingering effects of smartphone use on cognition. Appl Cogn Psychol. (2019) 33:1055–67. doi: 10.1002/acp.3546

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

32. Ackerman, R, and Lauterman, T. Taking reading comprehension exams on screen or on paper? A metacognitive analysis of learning texts under time pressure. Comput Hum Behav. (2012) 28:1816–28. doi: 10.1016/j.chb.2012.04.023

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

33. Dong, G, and Potenza, MN. Behavioural and brain responses related to internet search and memory. Eur J Neurosci. (2015) 42:2546–54. doi: 10.1111/ejn.13039

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

34. Sanchiz, M, Chin, J, Chevalier, A, Fu, WT, Amadieu, F, and He, J. Searching for information on the web: impact of cognitive aging, prior domain knowledge and complexity of the search problems. Inf Process Manag. (2017) 53:281–94. doi: 10.1016/j.ipm.2016.09.003

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

35. Cudo, A, and Zabielska-Mendyk, E. Cognitive functions in internet addiction—a review. Psychiatr Pol. (2019) 53:61–79. doi: 10.12740/PP/82194

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

36. Chen, X, Cai, Z, He, J, and Fan, X. Gender differences in life satisfaction among children and adolescents: a meta-analysis. J Happiness Stud. (2020) 21:2279–307. doi: 10.1007/s10902-019-00169-9

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

37. Kuribayashi, K, Imamura, K, Watanabe, K, Miyamoto, Y, Takano, A, Sawada, U, et al. Effects of an internet-based cognitive behavioral therapy (iCBT) intervention on improving depressive symptoms and work-related outcomes among nurses in Japan: a protocol for a randomized controlled trial. BMC Psychiatry. (2019) 19:245. doi: 10.1186/s12888-019-2221-5

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

38. Risko, EF, Ferguson, AM, and McLean, D. On retrieving information from external knowledge stores: feeling-of-findability, feeling-of-knowing and internet search. Comput Hum Behav. (2016) 65:534–43. doi: 10.1016/j.chb.2016.08.046

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

39. R Core Team. R: A Language and Environment for Statistical Computing. Vienna: R Foundation for Statistical Computing (2018) https://www.R-project.org

Google Scholar

40. Storm, BC, Stone, SM, and Benjamin, AS. Using the internet to access information inflates future use of the internet to access other information. Memory. (2017) 25:717–23. doi: 10.1080/09658211.2016.1210171

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

41. Suranata, K, Rangka, IB, and Permana, AAJ. The comparative effect of internet-based cognitive behavioral counseling versus face to face cognitive behavioral counseling in terms of student’s resilience. Cogent Psychol. (2020) 7:1751022. doi: 10.1080/23311908.2020.1751022

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

42. Viechtbauer, W. Conducting meta-analyses in R with the metafor package. J. Stat. Softw. (2010) 36:1–48.

Google Scholar

43. Lera-López, F, Billon, M, and Gil, M. Determinants of internet use in Spain. Econ Innov New Technol. (2011) 20:127–52. doi: 10.1080/10438590903378017

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

44. Su, W, Han, X, Yu, H, Wu, Y, and Potenza, MN. Do men become addicted to internet gaming and women to social media? A meta-analysis examining gender-related differences in specific internet addiction. Comput Hum Behav. (2020) 113:106480. doi: 10.1016/j.chb.2020.106480

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

45. Cohen, J. Statistical power analysis. Curr. Dir. Psychol. Sci. (1992) 1:98–101.

Google Scholar

46. Nakagawa, S, Lagisz, M, Jennions, MD, Koricheva, J, Noble, DW, Parker, TH, et al. Methods for testing publication bias in ecological and evolutionary meta-analyses. Methods Ecol Evol. (2022) 13:4–21. doi: 10.1111/2041-210X.13724

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

47. Huedo-Medina, TB, Sánchez-Meca, J, Marín-Martínez, F, and Botella, J. Assessing heterogeneity in meta-analysis: Q statistic or I² index? Psychol. Methods. (2006) 11:193.

Google Scholar

48. Stanley, TD. Beyond publication bias. J Econ Surv. (2005) 19:309–45. doi: 10.1111/j.0950-0804.2005.00250.x

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

49. Duval, S, and Tweedie, R. Trim and fill: a simple funnel‐plot–based method of testing and adjusting for publication bias in meta‐analysis. Biometrics. (2000) 56:455–63.

Google Scholar

50. Ahmed, S. Examining public perception and cognitive biases in the presumed influence of deepfakes threat: empirical evidence of third person perception from three studies. Asian J Commun. (2023) 33:308–31. doi: 10.1080/01292986.2023.2194886

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

51. Egger, M, Smith, GD, Schneider, M, and Minder, C. Bias in meta-analysis detected by a simple, graphical test. BMJ. (1997) 315:629–34.

Google Scholar

52. Ferguson, M, Brandreth, M, Brassington, W, and Wharrad, H. Information retention and overload in first-time hearing aid users: An interactive multimedia educational solution. Am. J. Audiol. (2015) 24:329–32.

Google Scholar

53. Giebl, S, Mena, S, Storm, BC, Bjork, EL, and Bjork, RA. Answer first or Google first? Using the Internet in ways that enhance, not impair, one’s subsequent retention of needed information. Psychol. Learn. Teach. (2021) 20:58–75.

Google Scholar

Keywords: Google effects, meta-analysis, Internet use, cognition and memory, media effects

Citation: Gong C and Yang Y (2024) Google effects on memory: a meta-analytical review of the media effects of intensive Internet search behavior. Front. Public Health. 12:1332030. doi: 10.3389/fpubh.2024.1332030

Received: 02 November 2023; Accepted: 05 January 2024;
Published: 18 January 2024.

Edited by:

Georgios D. Floros, Aristotle University of Thessaloniki, Greece

Reviewed by:

Corine S. M. Wong, The University of Hong Kong, Hong Kong SAR, China
Yuan-Wei Yao, Free University of Berlin, Germany

Copyright © 2024 Gong and Yang. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Yang Yang, UmlvOTUyNkAxNjMuY29t

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.