- 1Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, Saarland University Hospital and Saarland University Faculty of Medicine, Homburg, Germany
- 2Technical University of Munich, TUM School of Medicine and Health, Chair of Social Pediatrics, München, Germany
Background: The use of digital media (e.g., smartphones, tablets, etc.) and the Internet have become omnipresent for every age group and are part of children’s and parents’ everyday life. Focusing on young children, the availability of media devices, their use as well as associated problems (e.g., in social, emotional and motor development) have increased in recent years. Of particular interest for prevention of these problems in early childhood is the relationship between the familial context (parental digital media use, Problematic Internet Use, school graduation, presence of siblings) and the digital media use of infants and toddlers. The present study’s goal was to describe media usage in 0–4-year-old children and to identify the potential relationship between familial context factors and child media usage.
Methods: The sample included N = 3,035 children aged 0 to 3;11 years (M = 17.37 months, SD = 13.68; 49.13% female). Recruitment took place within the framework of a restandardization study for a German developmental test. The parents of the participants answered a questionnaire on socio-demographics, on child media use, and on parental media use. Questions on parental media use included the full version of the Short Compulsive Internet Use Scale (S-CIUS).
Results: Significant increases in media usage times with child age were identified, but no significant gender differences. A multiple regression analysis revealed that increasing maternal total media usage time, a higher parental S-CIUS score, lower school leaving certificate of both mother and father, and increasing child’s age led to higher child media usage time. Having siblings diminished young children’s media usage in this study. Having more than one child and having children aged over a year was associated with a higher parental S-CIUS score.
Conclusion: Family factors such as maternal media use time, Problematic Internet Use and lower school graduation are significantly associated with young children’s digital media use. Parents should be aware of their personal influence on their children’s media use which might be due their role in terms of model learning.
1 Introduction
These days, children and adolescents grow up in homes with media like television, smartphones, computers, tablets, smart watches and gaming consoles being highly present and used, further reinforced by the COVID-19 pandemic (1–9). Due to the progressing digitization, children and adolescents as well as their parents, caregivers, teachers, therapists and doctors are being confronted with new issues and disorders arising from this development like Gaming Disorder (10–13), Internet Addiction or Problematic Internet Use (14–16). As nosology currently cannot keep up with the rapid technological development of digital hardware and applications over the last two decades, literature uses various terms for describing this clinical entity. This results in a multitude of different and partly conflicting conceptualizations of digitization-related disorders with different diagnostic criteria and test procedures.
Excessive media usage can influence a child’s or adolescent’s development in a way that prevents usual developmental tasks or milestones from being reached. The foundations for functional or dysfunctional and impairing media consumption are not laid in adolescence or childhood, but in preschool, toddlerhood and infancy. Especially social, emotional, cognitive, verbal and motor skill development as well as nutrition and sleep are negatively affected by early digital media usage (17–23). Time spent with using digital media devices can displace the time usually spent with parents or other family members (24) and result in multiple negative consequences (e.g., impaired language and executive functions, impaired caregiver-child relationship, anxiety, behavioral difficulties, cardiovascular risk) especially for infants and preschool children (25–29). Additionally, an increasing amount of parents are using mobile devices as distractions while with their children, resulting in a lack of parenting responsiveness and quality (30, 31). This leads to the assumption that „digital native“ parents are engaging in media use behaviors that affect their children’s development, as well as their own sensitivity (32, 33) toward their child, especially in the first year of life. Additionally, parental media use during parent–child-interactions (technoference) may influence the child’s externalizing and withdrawal behavior (34) and may lead to “maladaptive technological behaviors” (35).
Currently, the age at which children start using media is shifted to preschool age and infancy (24), partly because of the new interactive media devices (36) accompanied by touch screens’ simplified handling and voice control (37). As a result of market development and technical innovations, usage times have skyrocketed, with young children being specifically and more intensely targeted as consumers.
In order to be able to possibly prevent or reduce young children’s media usage, it is essential to understand which contextual conditions contribute to this problem. Models that aim to explain the development of Gaming Disorder, Problematic Internet Use or other disorders that are related to digitization are multicausal and include internal factors like structural and functional neurobiological abnormalities, executive disorders and comorbid psychological disorders as well as external (parental modeling of how to interact with media) and social factors (family’s socioeconomic situation) (12, 38–40).
Since young children are reliant on their parents for a plethora of things it makes sense to investigate the familial context when addressing influences on children’s usage of media and screen time.
Generally, children’s media usage patterns have been reported to be similar to their parents’ (41): parents who consume a large amount of media themselves are more likely to raise children who are exposed to and use media early on than parents with a more reserved approach to media usage.
Parents’ socioeconomic status has been linked to young children’s media usage: Children in lower educated, lower income families are reported to have more devices in their bedroom and spend more time using media than children whose parents have a higher socioeconomic status (41–44).
Looking at parents separately as individuals, several studies take the mother’s education into account [(e.g., 45, 46)]. Rideout and Hamel (43) report that young children with mothers who have not finished a high school education spend more time in front of a screen on a daily basis than children whose mothers have obtained a higher level of education. In line with these findings, Anand and Krosnick (47) found that mothers’ lower education resulted in more TV watching in children between 6 months and 6 years, with the same result found for fathers. Hoyos and Jago (48) report that both parents’ common education level is moderately negatively associated with screen-time while fathers’ education level shows a strong negative correlation with children’s screen-time.
While some research suggests that young children who have siblings are more likely to engage in daily media use than only children are (49), other research has not been able to replicate these effects (41). Children with siblings as well as their families might engage in more activities that are alternatives to media and screen time than families with only children do. This could imply that children who do not have siblings might spend comparatively more time using media and more time in front of screens than young children who have siblings. The effect that having siblings may or may not have on young children’s media usage is one that has been yielding inconclusive results. De Decker et al. (50) conducted a qualitative interview-study in 6 European countries and found, that parents in Bulgaria, Germany and Spain believe that siblings or friends have a major influence on children’s screen time whereas the attitudes of parents from Greece, Poland and Belgium were inconclusive. The conflicting findings found in the literature may be due to the influence of the age of siblings, as older siblings might be seen as role models and might have a stronger influence on the media usage behavior than siblings of the same age. Moreover, gender differences could also influence the relationship between siblings and digital media use, as mentioned by Bagley et al. (51).
In line with developmental progress, age overall is strongly positively associated with screen time in young children (48). Older children are reported to have a higher media consumption than younger children and it can be considered confirmed that a child’s age generally is a significant predictor of their usage of media (47).
With regard to gender differences, studies note that there is a preference for gaming among boys and a preference for social media use among girls (52, 53). Regarding younger samples, Green et al. (54) also found gendered differences in the time spend on video game usage. In a longitudinal study spanning 3 years with children of the ages 2 and 4 at the start, they found that boys spend more time playing video games than girls and that these differences increase with age. In line with this finding, a nationwide survey conducted by Ofcom in the United Kingdom in 2014 (55) showed that 30% of boys aged 3 to 4 use a games console, but only 21% of girls aged 3 to 4. The miniKIM-Studie (6) however does not find any significant gender differences in two- to five-year-old children. It is to be explored whether gender differences may not yet be so pronounced at this early age. Findings on gender differences in younger children and infants are lacking, as is research on gender differences in general digital media use time in this age group.
All these afore mentioned factors have been shown to have some effect on children’s and adolescent’s media behavior. However, it is still unclear in many ways to what extent this applies for young children as well. In addition, reciprocal relationships between children’s and parents’ media use could also be possible, in the sense that even young children could have an influence on parental media use. Obtaining more data seemed necessary to identify patterns that might result in or from young children’s media usage.
Therefore, in this study, we hypothesize that media usage in children aged 0 to 4 is predicted by familial context. More specifically, our first hypothesis is that parents’ increased media usage time and parents’ Problematic Internet Use are positively correlated with their young children’s time spent using media. An associated research question to be answered by this study is whether there is a reciprocal relationship between child characteristics and parental media use in the sense that child variables could influence parental media use, too. Hypothesis 2 states that parents level of education is a predictor for the amount of time children use media: higher level of education is associated with less time using digital media. The third hypothesis postulates that a child’s age positively predicts their media usage time: The older the child, the more it uses digital media.
Concerning the mixed results regarding siblings and their influence in research so far, a research question we aim to answer in this study is how the presence of siblings affects young children’s media usage.
2 Materials and methods
2.1 Study design
The sample was recruited within the framework of a restandardization study for the Münchner Funktionelle Entwicklungsdiagnostik (MFED), consisting of a prospective cross-sectional study. The preparation for the restandardization study project (MFED) started in 2015. The associated media study reported in this paper was prepared from January 2019, and data collection took place from May 2019 to March 2022. This study is monocentric, being conducted by the Chair of Social Pediatrics at the Technical University of Munich, and the kbo Kinderzentrum München.
The aim of the study was to carry out the investigations throughout Germany. The distribution is as follows: 58.6% Bavaria, 21.0% Berlin, 5.6% North Rhine-Westphalia, 4.9% Baden-Württemberg, 2.5% Thuringia, 2.4% Saarland, 2.0% Saxony-Anhalt, 1.3% Lower Saxony, 0.8% Bremen and 0.3% Saxony. 0.8% of the children were examined in Innsbruck (Austria).
Participating families were recruited in pediatrician’s practices, hospitals, daycare centers/preschools, and through the distribution of flyers in playgrounds, counseling centers, etc. The children, accompanied by their parents, were invited to participate in the study by the examiners.
The questionnaire was completed by the parents at home or during the child’s developmental examination. All participants’ parents were informed and asked for written consent for participation in the study.
2.2 Participants
The sample used for our study included children aged from a few days postnatal age to 3 years and 11 months whose development had been normal up to that point.
Exclusion criteria were as follows: prematurity (birth weight under 1,500 g), a mother tongue different from German, medication impacting children’s cognitive or verbal performance as well as sensory or motor disabilities. Illnesses with a heightened risk of developmental disorders or genetic disorders were also excluded. No people who in any way were dependent on the director of studies or doctor/scientist responsible for this study were included.
The original ad-hoc sample consisted of 3,126 children. 12 (0.38%) had to be excluded because of missing values in total media usage time, 27 (0.86%) had missing values in S-CIUS and therefore became ineligible for further analysis and 24 (0.77%) children had to be excluded because of missing values in the total media usage time of their mother and father. Lastly, 28 (0.90%) data sets were excluded as outliers (participants were excluded as outliers in the multiple regression analysis (3 SD or more, based on standardized residuals)), so the final sample consists of 3,035 participants, 97.09% of the original sample. The participants flow can also be found in Figure 1.
Figure 1. Flow of participants. MV, missing value. Participants were excluded as outliers in the multiple regression analysis (3 SD or more, based on standardized residuals).
2.3 Measures
The questionnaire on socio-demographics and on media usage, times of use and contexts of use of children aged 0–4 years and their parents is a questionnaire developed by the Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, Saarland University Hospital in cooperation with the Technical University of Munich in 2019. The questionnaire contains 57 items and was designed to assess general information, such as demographic information, school leaving graduation of mother and father as well as leisure activities and contexts of use and times of use of electronic media (e.g., television, computer/laptop, smartphone, smartwatch, tablet, game console) in children and parents.
Information about the child (10 items; e.g., gender, number of siblings, position of the child in the family, illnesses) and the child’s living circumstances (1 item, single-choice; e.g., living with both biological parents; see Table 1) are asked. Furthermore, the questionnaire asks whether the child attends a nursery/kindergarten and whether digital media are used there (dichotomous response format; yes/no), whether the child is in a club, and which activities the child likes (open response format). The questionnaire asks which media devices are available in the household (e.g., Smarttoy; see Table 2), which devices the child uses on a daily basis (open response format) and looks at how much time is spent with them (on average per day; indicated in minutes; see Table 3) as well as in which contexts (e.g., for the child’s occupation, at mealtimes, during waiting times, etc.; see Table 4). The questionnaire also asks whether the child can freely dispose of his or her media time, whether he or she has free access to the internet and whether safety locks have been installed (dichotomous response format; yes/no).
Table 3. Frequency and percentage of children’s media use and child average daily media usage time by age, gender, and siblings present or missing.
Table 4. Frequency and percentage of children’s use of electronic media in different contexts (overall and separated by the first 4 years of life) in parental judgment.
Information about the highest school-leaving graduation (e.g., High School, University; see Table 1) of the biological parents are asked.
Additionally, the parents’ media consumption is recorded in detail, in particular how much time mother and father spend per day on average with different screen media (see Table 5). The Short Compulsive Internet Use Scale (S-CIUS) (56) is a short form of the Compulsive Internet Use Scale (CIUS) (57) and embedded in the above-mentioned questionnaire. It’s a screening tool to assess Problematic Internet Use (PIU). It consists of 5 of the original 14 items rated with a five-point Likert scale. The items are as follows: “How often do you find it difficult to stop using the internet when you are online?’’, “How often do other people (parents, friends) say you should use the internet less?’’, “How often do you sleep too little because of the internet?’’, “How often do you neglect your daily obligations because you prefer to go online?’’ and “How often do you go online when you are feeling down?’’. The response options for each are “0 = never, 1 = seldom, 2 = sometimes, 3 = frequent, 4 = very frequent’’. Its reliability of 0.77 (Cronbach’s Alpha) is adequate. At a cut-off of 7 which was shown to perform best in case detection, it yields a sensitivity of 0.95 and a specificity of 0.87 (58). In all these psychometric properties it is no worse than its full-length version.
Table 5. Frequency and percentage of media use by mother and father and average daily media usage times.
2.4 Statistical analysis
The contents of the study were evaluated descriptively and via inferential statistics. A multiple regression analysis was conducted to predict the child’s total media usage time. To compare the effect of age group (children) on Problematic Internet Use (S-CIUS total score) (parents) a one-way ANOVA was performed. Because of the violation of the preconditions (such as homoscedasticity and normal distribution) and unequal group sizes, a Brown-Forsythe ANOVA was calculated. Additional t-tests were performed for group analysis of continuous variables. Since the requirements for a t-test for independent samples were not met, a Mann Whitney U Test was calculated.
Data were analyzed using the IBM SPSS Statistics version 26. A significance level of 0.05 was used for all statistical tests.
3 Results
3.1 Descriptive analysis
3.1.1 Sample
The participants’ mean age was 17.37 months (SD = 13.68, Min. = 0, Max. = 47) with 1,544 (50.87%) of them being male and 1,491 (49.13%) being female. The average maternal age at birth was approximately 32 years (SD = 4.73, Min. = 14, Max. = 52). The average paternal age at birth 35.16 years (SD = 5.76, Min. = 15, Max. = 67). 93.40% of the questionnaires were filled out by the mother alone or by the mother with another person (e.g., father/new partner). At the time of data collection 2,843 (93.67%) children lived with both of their biological parents. In case of the biological parents being split up, most children lived with their mothers and not their fathers. Only 10 (0.33%) lived in foster families or with adoptive parents. 1,690 (55.68%) had at least one sibling while 1,345 (44.32%) were only children. 2038 of the mothers (57.15%) had graduated university or a finished high school education, 31.25% finished secondary or intermediate secondary school, and 0.92% had none or a special school certificate. Among fathers, 62.27% had graduated university or a finished high school education, 34.96% finished secondary or intermediate secondary school, and 1.25% had none or a special school certificate (see Table 1).
3.1.2 Media characteristics by household and child
The media devices available in the household can be found in Table 2. The most owned items used for consuming electronic media among families were smartphones (93.44%), televisions (87.55%), laptops (84.09%) and tablets (61.48%). Consoles were present in 24.65% of households.
Tables 2, 3 show the children’s media behaviors, such as total daily media usage time in minutes as well as daily media usage time categorized by media type. Out of 3,035 children, about half of them were reported not to be media-users (= parents indicated a daily use of 0 min for their child for all digital media devices indicated) (48.01%). The other 51.99% of children use electronic media for an average of 39.84 min (SD = 34.30, Min. = 1, Max. = 300) per day, of which about half is spend on screens (20.65 min; total screen time is defined as the sum of screen time spend with (Video-)Calling/Skype, Internet, movies/series, digital games, apps and digital books and newspaper). The following usage times occur among the media users: Most popularly used by children are music and audiobooks (19.19 min/day), followed by movies and series (16.88 min/day). Least used were the Internet (0.09 min/day) and digital picture books (0.33 min/day). As can be seen in Table 3, a children’s average total media usage time per day increases with their age. There were no missing values for the media characteristics by child.
Only 0.13% of children have free access to the Internet and 20.89% of households have a child safety lock installed.
If we look at media use in the nursery/kindergarten, we see that on average 6.07% of those children who attend a nursery/kindergarten also consume media there. However, children under 1 year of age do not consume media in the nursery/kindergarten, the number of consumers then increases across the age groups and reaches 8.86% among the 3- to 4-year-olds.
3.1.2.1 Age differences
Among children who use digital media, total digital media usage time averages 28.51 min per day in the first year of life, 35.13 min per day in the second year of life, 43.71 min per day for 2- to 3-year-olds and 46.99 min per day for 3- to 4-year-olds. Due to the presence of heteroskedasticity [Levene’s F (3, 3,031) = 184.83, p ≤ 0.001], lack of a normal distribution and unequal group sizes, a Brown-Forsythe ANOVA was performed. This showed that there was a statistically significant difference in children’s overall media use time between at least two age groups [Brown-Forsythe- F (3, 3,031) = 360.69, p ≤ 0.001, n = 3,035]. The estimated ω2 = 0.26 indicates a large effect. Games-Howell post-hoc procedure showed that the mean value of children’s media use differed significantly between all age groups.
The number of those who use digital games is increasing rapidly with age: while no digital games are used among the under-one-year-olds, the number of users doubles from the third to the fourth year of age (2- to 3-year-olds: 3.36%; 3- to 4-year-olds: 6.28%). While under-one-year-old media users watch movies/series an average of 4.39 min per day, the 1-2-year-olds increase it to 9.89 min, the 2-3-year-olds to 21.23 min and the 3-4-year-olds to 26.41 min.
A similar increase is seen in total screen time (up to 1 year old: 7.20 min; 1 to 2 years old: 14.38 min; 2 to 3 years old: 24.90 min; 3 to 4 years old: 30.14 min). Due to the presence of heteroskedasticity [Levene’s F (3, 3,031) = 338.50, p ≤ 0.001], lack of normal distribution and unequal group sizes, a Brown-Forsythe ANOVA was performed. This showed that there was a statistically significant difference in children’s screen time between at least two age groups [Brown-Forsythe- F (3, 3,031) = 438.02, p ≤ 0.001, n = 3,035]. The estimated ω2 = 0.30 suggests a large effect. Games-Howell post-hoc procedure showed that the mean value of children’s screentime differed significantly between all age groups.
Time spent with music and audio books decreases with age (up to 1 year old: 21.31 min; 3 to 4 years old: 16.85 min). The time spent with ((Video-)Calling/Skype), internet and digital games hardly changes over the age range considered here. Children under the age of one do not use the internet. If we look at single children versus siblings, children with siblings are more often electronic media users (57.05%) than single children (45.65%). A chi-squared test confirmed that the percentage of electronic media users did differ by existence of siblings χ2(1, 3,035) = 38.93, p ≤ 0.001. We see in particular that siblings spend more time watching films and series (19.59 min versus single children, 12.61 min).
3.1.2.2 Gender differences
In our sample, there are no gender differences between users of electronic media: 51.75% of male and 52.25% of female children are users. Looking at gender differences (media users only), boys (M = 0.59 min, SD = 4.23) played on average longer games than girls (M = 0.31 min, SD = 2.33). According to Mann Whitney U test, however, this was not a significant difference (U(n boys =799, n girls = 779) = 308644.00, z = −0.96, p = 0.34).
Girls (M = 0.44, SD = 3.21) read more digital picture books than boys (M = 0.23, SD = 2.90). However, according to Mann Whitney U test, this was not a significant difference (U(n boys = 799, n girls = 779) = 309810.00, z = −0.62, p = 0.53).
Girls (M = 21.18, SD = 23.49) had a higher screen time than boys (M = 20.13, SD = 23.06). Again, according to Mann Whitney U test, this was not significant U(n boys = 799, n girls = 779) = 299664.00, z = −1.29, p = 0.20. Girls (M = 40.29, SD = 34.43) also had higher average daily media usage times than boys (M = 39.40, SD = 34.19) but also, the difference was not significant according to Mann Whitney U test (U(n boys = 799, n girls = 779) = 310476.00, z = −0.81, p = 0.94).
3.1.3 Media characteristics by parents
The vast majority of parents use digital media (mothers: 99.11%; to a slightly lesser extent fathers: 92.36%; see Table 5). The following usage times occur among the media users. Mothers spend on average 192.68 min a day using various media. Looking more closely at the mothers’ usage time, on an average day 159.57 min are spent on screen media, 53.20 min on watching films and series and 47.51 min on the internet. Fathers spend a daily average of 268.92 min using media, 240.46 of those on screens. Leading among fathers were movies and series as well as the Internet, each with about 57 min per day.
Looking at the S-CIUS scores (see Table 6), in total, 356 parents (11.73%) had a result above the cut-off 7, which implies Problematic Internet Use. Overall, the average total value was 3.03 (see Table 6).
Table 6. Descriptive statistics of S-CIUS values (parents) total and by child age, child gender, and siblings present or missing.
3.1.4 Reciprocal relationship between familial factors and media usage
Parents of multiple children (M = 3.15, SD = 2.80) scored higher in S-CIUS than parents of only children (M = 2.89, SD = 2.76). A Mann Whitney U Test indicated that this difference was statistically significant (U(n multiple children = 1,690, n single child = 1,345) = 1069973.50, z = −2.80, p ≤ 0.01). The effect size according to Cohen (59) is Pearson r = 0.05 and is below a small effect (r = 0.10).
The S-CIUS score of parental media use also differed depending on the age of the child. In the first year of life, the parents’ S-CIUS total score was 2.65 (8.93% above the Problematic Internet Use PIU cut-off), in the second year of life 3.25 (13.37% PIU), in the third year of life 3.05 (14.58% PIU) and in the fourth year of life 3.38 (14.66% PIU). An ANOVA with the 4-fold stepped factor age was calculated on the S-CIUS total values. Due to heteroskedasticity [Levene’s F (3, 3,031) = 8.45 p ≤ 0.001], lack of normal distribution and unequal group sizes a Brown-Forsythe-ANOVA was performed. This revealed that there was a statistically significant difference in terms of Problematic Internet Use (S-CIUS total score) between at least two groups [Brown-Forsythe-F(3, 3,031) = 17.53, p ≤ 0.001, n = 3,035]. The estimated ω2 = 0.02 suggests a small effect. Games-Howell post-hoc procedure revealed that the mean S-CIUS total score differed significantly between parents of infants up to 1 year old and parents of all other age groups [compared to 1–2 year old infants p ≤ 0.001, 95% C.I. = (−0.94;-0.27); compared to 2–3 year old infants p ≤ 0.001, 95% C.I. = (−1.24;-0.47); compared to 3–4 year old infants p ≤ 0.001, 95% C.I. = (−1.13;-0.34)]. Otherwise, there were no statistically significant differences between the older age groups (comparison of 1-2-year-olds with 2-3-year-olds p = 0.45; comparison of 1-2-year-olds with 3-4-year-olds p = 0.88; comparison of 2-3-year-olds with 3-4-year-olds p = 0.92). Further descriptive data concerning the S-CIUS are found in Table 6.
Parents were asked about the contexts of electronic media use using predefined categorie (see Table 4). In the total sample of media-using children, 44.78% of children were allowed to use electronic media when parents did not have time, 37.59% of children were occupied with electronic media to calm them down, 28.36% before going to sleep, 19.12% during waiting times, 14.3% with other children and 6.06% at mealtimes. Specifically in the first year of life, media are used to occupy and calm the child (52.25%) in contrast to the following 3 years of life (42.81, 34.25, 32.02%). Additionally, the reason ‘lack of time’ shows an increase with age (16.22% in the first, 35.97% in the second, 46.79% in the third and 58.71% in the fourth year of life).
3.2 Multiple contextual influences
A multiple regression analysis (method enter) was used to predict total media usage time of all children (media and no media users) from total media usage time of all mothers, total media usage time of all fathers, parental S-CIUS-Total-score, school graduation mother, school graduation father, child gender, child age and single child versus child with siblings (Table 7). The model explained a statistically significant amount of variance in total media usage time of child, F (8,2,888) = 170.05, p < 0.001, R2 = 0.32, R2adjusted = 0.32. Significant predictors were: total media usage time of mother (ß = 0.11, t = 5.78, p≤ 0.001), S-CIUS-Total-score (ß = 0.05, t = 3.43, p < 0.001), school graduation mother (ß = −0.05, t = −2.45, p = 0.01), school graduation father (ß = −0.09, t = −4.46, p < 0.001), child age (ß = 0.56, t = 34.20, p < 0.001.) and siblings (ß = −0.04, t = −2.65, p < 0.01). Therefore, the final predictive model was: Total media usage time of child = 11.40 + 0.03 (total media usage time of mother) + 0.60 (S-CIUS-Total-score) −1.41 (school graduation mother) −2.28 (school graduation father) + 1.28 (child age) −2.64 (siblings). Increasing maternal total media usage time, Problematic Internet Use, lower school leaving certificate of mother, lower school leaving certificate of father, increasing age and being an only child lead to higher child media usage time. The R2 for the overall model indicates a substantial goodness of fit according to Cohen (59), f2 = 0.47 (large effect). Child’s gender and total media usage time of father were no significant predictors of child’s electronic media usage time.
Table 7. Multiple linear regression analysis results (n = 3,035) with “total media usage time of child” (averaged over media and no media users) as criterion.
4 Discussion
The present study examined the digital media use and media availability in the first 4 years of life of more than 3,000 children. Young children’s media use was examined in relation to the media use of their parents, their parents’ Problematic Internet Use, the educational attainment of their parents and family composition.
The hypotheses put forward at the beginning were partly confirmed, with mothers’ media usage, level of education of mother and father and children’s age being relevant predictors in the assumed capacities. There are no significant gender differences in the media use times of children at this early age. Siblings in this study are a factor that significantly diminishes young children’s media usage rather than increase it. In the first 4 years of their children’s lives, electronic screen media are used by parents comprehensively and depending on children’s age in different contexts (eating, falling asleep) and with different functions (to occupy/calm the child). In addition, we observed that parents of siblings had a higher S-CIUS score than parents of only children and that there was an increase in S-CIUS scores between parents of children under 1 year and parents of children aged 1 to 3 years.
4.1 Media characteristics by child
According to the results of our study, more than half of the 0- to 4-year-old children spend approximately 40 min using electronic media per day. However, of these 40 min of daily electronic media use, the use of music/audiobook with over 19 min makes up the largest part (main share). Listening, singing and dancing are highly-encouraged activities, which parents can offer to their children from an early age, either in person or through electronic media. Nevertheless, an average total screen time of 20.65 min per day remains for the first 4 years of life.
Ferjan Ramírez et al. (60) report 58 min of daily electronic media exposure in 6- to 24-month-old children and results of the miniKim-Studie (61) show comparable results to the present study, reporting that 2–3-year-olds spend 34 min watching TV. Additionally, 4% of 2–3-year-old use computer, console or online games at this early age. This is in line with results of the present study, reporting that about 3% of 2–3-year-olds and 6% of 3–4-year-olds use digital games. However, the results of our study reveal a deviation of the current practice from the recommendations of the American Academy of Pediatrics (AAP) (37, 62). Our study shows 18.30% media users in the first year of life and 61.45% media users in the second year (Table 3). If we look at the media usage times of only the media users, we find an average of 4.93 min daily for “child movies/series time” already in the first year of life and an average of 9.89 min daily in the second year of life. As well as an average daily total screen time of 7.20 min in the first year of life and 14.38 min in the second year of life. Even if we take into account that the total screen time in the first year of life includes an average of 2.25 min of (Video-)Calling/Skype daily and in the second year of life an average of 2.62 min of (Video-)Calling/Skype daily, these descriptive results deviate from the recommendations of the AAP. The AAP recommends completely avoiding the use of digital media (with the exception of video-chatting) for children younger than 18 months. If children between the ages of 18 and 24 months are to be introduced to digital media, it should always be with a caregiver and with quality educational digital media content (37).
Looking at total screen time, half of the 0- to 4-year-old children in the present study spend approximately 20 min in front of a screen per day. Trinh et al. (63) show an average screen time of 30 min for toddlers and 2 h for 3-year-olds. Kracht et al. (64) report 1 h of screen time per day for 3-month-olds, 1.1 h for 12-month-olds and 1.7 h for 2-year-olds. Tandon et al. (65) found much higher numbers, whereby weekday screen time for preschool children was 4 h per day, in line with the findings of Cheng et al. (66). Tandon et al. (65) points out that the usage times in the nursery/kindergarten and especially in home-based childcare should not be underestimated. As we can see in the present study, 6.07% of the children already use media in the nursery/kindergarten. Since we did not measure the time spent with media in the nursery/kindergarten, we cannot compare it with the results of the study by Tandon et al. (65). However, this shows that media time in the nursery/kindergarten cannot be neglected as it could be one of the reasons for the observed lower usage times in our sample and has the potential to become a significant additional source in the cumulative daily screen time of young children in the future.
By the end of the first year of life, approximately one fifth (18.3% of the children in our sample) are already media users. Durham et al. (67) find much higher frequencies with 45% of children already interacting with digital media in their first year of life. Kiliç et al. (68) report an average age of 12 months for the first use of mobile devices. In the present study, the frequency of media use increases sharply in the second (61.45%) and third (91.58%) years of life and reaches almost full coverage in the fourth year of life at 97.59%. There are considerable increases especially in the second year of life (by more than 40%) and in the third year of life (by about 30%). Significant course settings in media use seem to take place in the early childhood years.
With increasing age, 0- to 4-year-old children in this study are reported to use media for an increasing amount of time per day, confirming previous findings of age being a predictor of media usage time [(e.g., 47, 69)]. Certain and Kahn (70) found that 83% of 0 to 11-month-olds spend less than an hour a day watching TV while 48% of 12- to 23-month-olds spend at least 1 h every day watching TV. Among the 24 to 35 months old, 16% were reported to watch 5 or more hours of TV every day, while 41% of this age group were reported to watch at least 3 or more hours daily. This finding is in line with Duch et al. (24) noting that older children (about 36 months old) have a higher screen time than younger children. As children grow older, they gain more autonomy and independence, possibly to use media by themselves as well as more fine and gross motor skills that facilitate specific and extensive media usage.
Comparisons between our data and existing studies (and between existing studies themselves) are limited by different methodological approaches (e.g., how media use is measured in the different studies or how representative the sample is).
Regarding the varying media characteristics reported in the literature, cultural differences in policy and the different policies on internet use in different countries play an important role (71, 72). In Germany, for example, internet use policies take on a crucial role, as the digitalization campaign by the German government lays the framework for a substantial increase in the use of digital media, especially in the context of schools (73).
Parents were asked in which contexts electronic media are used in the first 4 years of life. Results show that media are mainly used to occupy the child, especially when parents do not have time or want to calm the child down, but also before falling asleep, during waiting times, with other children and at mealtimes. This is supported by the findings of Kabali et al. (74) and consistent with findings by Vandewater et al. (9). The results of the present study contrast again with recommendations from the AAP (37), emphasizing that media should not be used to distract the child. In addition, screens are to be turned off at least 1 hour before bedtime (75). Furthermore, mealtimes and parent–child times should be media-free times. Ventura et al. (76) raise the question of whether maternal use of digital media during infant feeding has a negative impact and found that there was a negative association with some aspects of the quality of feeding interaction. In our study, during the first years of life, electronic media are used especially to occupy and calm the child. As children get older, media were used more often when parents do not have time. When it comes to media use during mealtime, we found an inverted U-shaped relationship. It seems that at the time of learning to eat independently (second and third year of life), electronic media are used particularly intensively.
In the present study, no significant gender differences were found with regard to screen and media use time. Consistent with previous research on older children [(e.g., 53, 77)], this sample of younger children also indicates a tendency for boys to spend more time playing digital games. Girls, on the other hand, spent more time with digital picture books, which is in line with the finding of Jabbar & Warraich (78) reporting that girls are more frequent readers than boys. As girls get older, there is a higher preference for Social Media use in adolescence than in boys (77), and some studies also report more Problematic Internet Use in girls than in boys (16). To summarize, on the one hand, there is a tendency and direction toward these gender differences known from studies of older children. On the other hand, these gender-specific findings in the present study are not statistically significant. Thus, one could conclude that gender differences are not yet so pronounced at this early age. However, there is a lack of research and comparable studies on infants and young children on this topic.
4.2 Family context factors
The aim of the present study was to find predictors for the media use of young children, looking more closely at family factors, as there is a lack of studies for infants and toddlers in this research area. In line with prior research identifying parental media usage as a strong predictor for children’s digital media use [(e.g., 41, 79, 80)], it was found that one of the major predictors in the sample of 0- to 4-year-old children was maternal media usage. Children spend more time watching television, playing video games and generally using screens, when their parents have a higher media consumption themselves [(e.g., 42, 81)]. Woodard and Gridina (82) note that this applies especially for those parents who are heavier media users. If, for instance, a parent spends more than 2 h per weekday watching television, young children have been found to be at least 3.4 times more likely to also spend more than 2 h watching TV (83). Durham et al. (67) also point out that family TV time is a major predictor of infant screen time. In previous studies specifically mothers’ screen time (e.g., watching TV) has been found to predict the time young children spend in front of screens or engaging in media (24). The positive association between maternal media use and children’s media usage might be attributed to the fact that children learn their behavior by observing their caregivers’ interaction with the world (41, 84), as described in Bandura’s theory of social learning (85). In addition, parents’ attitudes toward the effects of media use also play a major role here, as these affect and shape the way in which parents value media in their homes (84). Parents who perceive media use as less harmful to their children may also be more inclined to expose them to more media devices more often.
In our study, the extent of paternal media usage was not a significant predictor of the child’s screen time. So far there has been little to no research finding comparable results for fathers when looked at outside of a parental dyad and their media usage. This relative lack of literature investigating the paternal influence on children’s media usage might be caused by mothers spending more time caring for, interacting with and even just being in the presence of their child than fathers, whose time with their children is often mediated by the presence of the mother (86). This is especially true for the first 2 years of life, when the mother plays a very significant role in parent–child interaction and - at least in the traditional model still predominant in Germany - fathers are less involved. The mother’s media consumption seems to be a significant influencing factor for the child’s media use in the first years of life, while the father’s is not. This implies that mothers are an important target group for early prevention. Kiliç et al. (68) for example showed that there is a great lack of knowledge about the effect of mobile devices: 95% of the parents who participated in their study reported that they have not been informed about the effect of mobile devices on their children by a doctor. Universal prevention programs for mothers during pregnancy and the newborn period could be implemented to share information about possible adverse effects of maternal media use. At the same time, for mothers as the main caregivers of very young children in most Western societies, there are also opportunities in the use of screens, namely to counteract the dangers of social exclusion (87) through the use of social networks. However, it is certainly favorable if this does not happen during mother–child interaction.
In addition to the mother’s screen time, the parents’ Problematic Internet Use (PIU) also plays a significant role in its effects on the child’s media use time, which is in line with the findings of Hefner et al. (88). The positive prediction power of parents’ PIU possibly indicates that parents who use the internet problematically also fail to see the dangerous consequences of digital media use for their young children, which is why their children’s media usage time might not be a (big) concern for them.
Looking at the difference in S-CIUS scores between parents with more than one child and parents with only children, in the present study we found that parents with more than one child have higher S-CIUS scores, indicating more PIU. To the best of our knowledge there are no studies on this topic in the current literature. Possible explanations could be that parents of multiple children have more time to use digital media because the children are engaged with each other and require less attention from parents. It could also be that the use of digital media serves as an emotion regulation strategy (89, 90) or as a coping mechanism (89, 91) due to for example increased stress caused by multiple children [(e.g., 92)]. Another explanation might be that parents of several children have less opportunities for activities outside the home.
In addition, the present study found, that the S-CIUS score of parents of 1- to 4-year-olds increases sharply compared to the score of parents of under one-year olds. PIU of parents was significantly lower in the first year of life than in the 2 to 4 years of life of their children, increasing sharply in the second year of life. It can be speculated that there is less time and/or need to develop a PIU in the first year of life. Further studies are warranted to confirm this finding and to investigate possible mechanisms and explanations.
The present study shows that maternal and paternal education level significantly predicts children’s media usage time. Children of parents with higher education levels spend less time using media than children of non-academic parents, which is in line with findings of Anand and Krosnick (47) and Kiliç et al. (68). More educated parents reported less leisure media usage (81) and higher family income was negatively associated with parental media use as well (93). This is in line with the findings of Rey-López et al. (94), noting that not only parental education but also occupation influences time spent watching television. Looking specifically at maternal education, almost double the amount of mothers who had not graduated high school than of mothers who were college graduates reported that their 2 year olds watched at least 3 h of television a day (70). A woman who had not graduated from high school was almost 4 times as likely as a woman who had graduated from college to report that her 0- to 11-month-old watched at least 1 h of television per day. Overall, families with young children who have a comparatively high or even very high media intake are significantly lower educated and have a lower annual income than families who report a moderate or low usage of media. This could stem from parents with a higher level of education being more knowledgeable and educated about adverse effects of early life media usage and also being more likely to seek advice from doctors (95). Therefore, parents with a higher socioeconomic status might establish more and stricter rules regulating their children’s media and screen time and might develop these rules in a participatory joint conversation with their children (96), which could lead to a sustainable pursuit of these rules. Sebre et al. (97) highlight the importance of rules regarding social media use noting that reported rules for internet use by children were linked to lower ratings of problematic use of the internet. Additionally, parents with a higher socioeconomic status appear to be providing a reduced availability of media devices to their children compared to lower educated parents as Nikken and Schols already found in 2015 (42). Kabali et al. (74) report that young children in an urban, low-income, minority community had almost universal exposure to mobile devices, and most had their own device by the age 4. Furthermore Tandon et al. (44) note that children from lower income households are provided with a greater access to media in their bedroom and at the same time have lower access to other play equipment which promote physical activities, such as for example bikes.
In contrast Mollborn et al. (98) found that higher-socioeconomic children spend a similar amount of time with digital media devices to other groups and at the same time do not have more rules, than children from socio-economically disadvantaged families, regarding the use of digital media. This contradicts previous findings. One possible explanation the authors refer to is a theroretical perspective stating that more “advantaged” parents tend to follow an “individualistic parenting approach” (99).
When looking at specific types of media usage, Anand and Krosnick (47) found that children with fathers who either had some college education or who were college graduates were shown to spend more time using computers than children whose fathers had no high school education. This pattern is also evident in relation to playing video games or watching DVDs/videos. This could raise the question if and how various types of media usage differ and how they might be predicted or influenced by varying factors to varying degrees.
The results of our study, like those of many others state that low parental education and a low socioeconomic status are associated with children spending more time watching TV [(e.g., 70, 100)]. Mollborn et al. (98) confirm this finding and note that children brought up by a college-educated primary caregiver spend less time watching TV, but more time with non-TV technology. In conclusion, one could assume that a poorer level of education could be passed on transgenerationally to the children of these families through more intensive exposure to screen media.
The present study found that siblings turned out to be a protective factor regarding media usage time, having siblings decreased the daily average time spend on media. As for why this is the case, it can be speculated that children who are and have siblings spend part of their leisure time with their sibling(s) instead of using media. This is in line with the findings of Bagley et al. (51) and Davies and Gentile (101). However, there are also other studies that show the opposite: Hardy et al. (102) for example found that the presence of siblings increased the time spend watching TV. The presence of other people, including siblings, during screen time is a contextual feature and thus a situational influence that could affect young children’s media-related behavior (103). As children often spend a lot of time with their siblings, even more so than with their parents [McHale and Crouter (104) as quoted in Davies and Gentile (101)] it is highly relevant to conduct further research on topics such as the potential function of the sibling as a role model, the effects of age differences between siblings, their impact on media use, and the effects of sharing digital devices (101).
5 Strengths and limitations
The inclusion of a relatively large data set of infants and toddlers and their parents offered the possibility to relate the children’s media use to that of their parents. In addition, other family factors such as the parents’ level of education and the number of siblings were included. The recording of media time for the child as well as for the mother and father was not done as a total value, but very differentiated according to individual categories [(e.g., Video-)Calling/Skype, internet, movies, games, picture books, audiobook]. These are all strengths of this study and extend the current literature.
As for limitations, the present study is a cross-sectional study identifying correlations, but ultimately no causal relationships. Nevertheless, we consider it more likely that family factors such as parents’ media use time, income or parental PIU score have an impact on the very young child’s media use time rather than vice versa. However, it seems that eventually there are reciprocal relationships, thus the parental PIU is lower in the first year of the child’s life than in the 2nd-4th year of the child’s life.
Critically it was only recorded whether children watch television and whether there is a television in the house, but not how much television is watched (only generally “watch movies/series”). Therefore, it is problematic to distinguish whether the time spent watching films and series is spent on the television or perhaps on the computer, smartphone or tablet, which makes it difficult to compare the present results with other studies. Similarly, the item “playing digital games” did not distinguish between educational and non-educational games. However, at present there is little research distinguishing „high-quality” (37) educational games versus non-educational games.
With 49% of both parents having a university degree and over 90% of the children living with their mother and father, the question arises as to the representativeness of the sample studied (even though it is very large). It could be assumed that more educationally distant family systems would tend to result in higher media use times.
The questionnaires used in the present study were self-report questionnaire which could lead to response biases such as under- and over-statements, as well as socially desirable answers. Furthermore, only the parents filled in the questionnaire, so there are no other data sources. Parents’ perceptions of their children’s time spent using media may be biased, inaccurate and underestimated, especially for parents with high S-CIUS scores as they may have no insight into their own or their children’s problematic behavior. Additionally, PIU was assessed only using 5 out of the 14 original items with the short version of the CIUS (S-CIUS) (56). Another issue is that the socio-economic status of the parents was only measured through educational attainment and not through further factors such as income, profession, or resources in the household.
6 Conclusion and outlook
In conclusion, this study yields indications for a possible problematic media consumption in early childhood in respect of the high percentage of media use in early childhood (51.99%), the average total daily screen time (20.65 min) and the context (e.g., pacifying in absence of parental resources even before bedtime) of media use. In light of the results of the present study, it is important to keep the plentiful adverse effects of media consumption in very early childhood in mind, such as negative repercussions on social, emotional, cognitive, verbal and motor skill development as well as nutrition and sleep (17–22, 81, 104–106). Excessive use of digital media can also lead to the neglect and abandonment of activities like physical exercise (107). However, some studies fail to find a negative effect (108, 109). As for the possible positive effects of using digital media at a young age, there is currently little evidence (110, 111). The AAP recommends parental interaction with the child during media use in order to provide support and guidance and to prevent excessive digital media use (37).
Because of the high educational level of the study population and the fact, that low education is correlated with high media consumption this study is very likely to underestimate the situation in the normal population. Preventive efforts to reduce the use of digital media especially among infants and toddlers seem mandatory, as early life is potentially highly relevant for further media socialization, as well as the family. There is a risk that kindergartens are playing an increasingly important part in digital socialization, however, they could also be targeted as a starting point for prevention. From the data of our study, first conclusions for prevention strategies may be drawn. The role model function of parents has to play a central role, access by the less educated population has to be assured and communication programs through pediatric practitioners should be established. Overall, this seems to be of particular relevance in order to compensate for the plethora of adversities encountered by socially disadvantaged children. Recognizing that media are a potential mediator for the transgenerational transmission of educational attainment (and ultimately Socioeconomic status SES) offers further starting points for specifically tailored indicated prevention programs.
Future research should focus on longitudinal studies to examine possible reciprocal relationships between parental PIU and the age of the child, as well as consider age of the child as a moderating factor in the relationship between parental PIU and child media use. In addition, a broader range of participants with a more diverse parental educational background as well as different living circumstances (e.g., lives with the mother and her new partner, lives with father) is needed. Regarding the contradictory findings on siblings, more research is needed on topics such as the influence of siblings’ age, the impact of sharing digital devices and also possible gender effects. In general, there is a need for more studies on infants and toddlers on the topic of digital media.
The qualitative criteria mentioned for PIU or IA or GD of adults are not transferable to toddlers and infants, for whom primarily quantitative time criteria are recorded. However, pure screen time, which was used as a quantitative measure in this study and in many other studies, does not appear to be sufficient. Future research should develop the qualitative structure and criteria of dysfunctional and disturbed media consumption in infants and children beyond the time of use.
A next step would then be to include corresponding age-appropriate criteria in the DC:0–5 (112). [The Diagnostic Classification of Mental Health and Developmental Disorders of Infancy and Early Childhood (DC:0–5) is a multiaxial classification system for mental disorders in early childhood providing a framework for standardizing clinical practice and research (113)].
Qualitative criteria for screen use such as educationally valuable applications, age appropriateness of the programs and level of quality of the programs need to be considered and researched more extensively. Only then will we be able to better understand what really happens during children’s screen time and how screens ultimately affect children’s development and parent–child interactions.
Data availability statement
The datasets presented in this article are not readily available because the data that support the findings of this study are available from VM (Munich) upon reasonable request. Requests to access the datasets should be directed to VM, Vm9sa2VyLk1hbGxAa2JvLmRl.
Ethics statement
The studies involving humans were approved by Ethics committee of the Technical University of Munich (No. 278/18S-AS; date 16.08.2018). The studies were conducted in accordance with the local legislation and institutional requirements. The participants provided their written informed consent to participate in this study.
Author contributions
FP: Conceptualization, Data curation, Methodology, Supervision, Visualization, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing. JJ: Data curation, Formal analysis, Methodology, Software, Visualization, Writing – original draft. AF: Data curation, Investigation, Project administration, Resources, Validation, Writing – review & editing. TF: Data curation, Investigation, Project administration, Resources, Writing – review & editing. EM: Supervision, Writing – review & editing. VM: Conceptualization, Investigation, Methodology, Project administration, Supervision, Writing – review & editing.
Funding
The author(s) declare that no financial support was received for the research, authorship, and/or publication of this article.
Acknowledgments
We would particularly like to thank Ida Gerstner (Saarland University Hospital) for contributing to the literature search, and for proofreading.
Conflict of interest
The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.
Publisher’s note
All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.
References
1. Auhuber, L , Vogel, M , Grafe, N , Kiess, W , and Poulain, T . Leisure activities of healthy children and adolescents. Int J Environ Res Public Health. (2019) 16:2078. doi: 10.3390/ijerph16122078
2. Bitkom . Jung und vernetzt-Kinder und Jugendliche in der digitalen Gesellschaft. (2014). Available at: https://www.bitkom.org/Bitkom/Publikationen/Jung-und-vernetzt-Kinder-und-Jugendliche-in-der-digitalen-Gesellschaft.html (Accessed December 16, 2022).
3. DAK . DAK-Studie: Gaming, Social-Media and Corona. (2020). Available at: https://www.dak.de/dak/gesundheit/dak-studie-gaming-social-media-und-corona-2295548.html (Accessed December 16, 2022).
4. Medienpädagogischer Forschungsverbund Südwest . JIM-Studie 2020 - Jugend, Information, Medien. (2021). Available at: https://www.mpfs.de/fileadmin/files/Studien/JIM/2020/JIM-Studie-2020_Web_final.pdf (Accessed December 16, 2022).
5. Medienpädagogischer Forschungsverbund Südwest . KIM-Studie 2020–Kindheit, Internet, Medien. (2020). Available at: https://www.mpfs.de/fileadmin/files/Studien/KIM/2020/KIM-Studie2020_WEB_final.pdf (Accessed December 16, 2022).
6. Medienpädagogischer Forschungsverbund Südwest . miniKIM-Studie 2020 – Kleinkinder und Medien. (2020). Available at: https://www.mpfs.de/fileadmin/user_upload/lfk_miniKIM_2020_211020_WEB_barrierefrei.pdf (Accessed December 16, 2022).
7. Pedrotti, BG , Mallmann, MY , Almeida, CRS , Marques, FM , Vescovi, G , Riter, H, et al. Infants’ and toddlers’ digital media use and mothers’ mental health: a comparative study before and during the COVID-19 pandemic. Infant Ment Health J. (2022) 43:24–35. doi: 10.1002/imhj.21952
8. Rideout, VJ , Foehr, UG , and Roberts, DF . Generation m2. Media in the Lives of 8- to 18-year-olds. A Kaiser Family Foundation study. (2010). Available at: https://files.eric.ed.gov/fulltext/ED527859.pdf (Accessed December 16, 2022).
9. Vandewater, EA , Rideout, VJ , Wartella, EA , Huang, X , Lee, JH , and Shim, M . Digital childhood: electronic media and technology use among infants, toddlers, and preschoolers. Pediatrics. (2007) 119:e1006–15. doi: 10.1542/peds.2006-1804
10. King, DL , Delfabbro, PH , Perales, JC , Deleuze, J , Király, O , Krossbakken, E, et al. Maladaptive player-game relationships in problematic gaming and gaming disorder: a systematic review. Clin Psychol Rev. (2019) 73:101777. doi: 10.1016/j.cpr.2019.101777
11. Kuss, DJ , and Lopez-Fernandez, O . Internet addiction and problematic internet use: a review of clinical research. World J Psychiatry. (2016) 6:143–76. doi: 10.5498/wjp.v6.i1.143
12. Paulus, FW , Ohmann, S , Von Gontard, A , and Popow, C . Internet gaming disorder in children and adolescents: a systematic review. Dev Med Child Neurol. (2018) 60:645–59. doi: 10.1111/dmcn.13754
13. WHO . ICD-11. (2022). Available at: https://www.who.int/news/item/11-02-2022-icd-11-2022-release (Accessed December 21, 2022).
14. Jorgenson, AG , Hsiao, RCJ , and Yen, CF . Internet addiction and other behavioral addictions. Child Adolesc Psychiatr Clin N Am. (2016) 25:509–20. doi: 10.1016/j.chc.2016.03.004
15. Kamolthip, R , Chirawat, P , Ghavifekr, S , Gan, WY , Tung, SE , Nurmala, I, et al. Problematic internet use (PIU) in youth: a brief literature review of selected topics. Curr Opin Behav Sci. (2022) 46:101150. doi: 10.1016/j.cobeha.2022.101150
16. Paulus, FW , Joas, J , Gerstner, I , Kühn, A , Wenning, M , Gehrke, T, et al. Problematic internet use among adolescents 18 months after the onset of the COVID-19 pandemic. Children. (2022) 9:1724. doi: 10.3390/children9111724
17. Cespedes, EM , Gillman, MW , Kleinman, K , Rifas-Shiman, SL , Redline, S , and Taveras, EM . Television viewing, bedroom television, and sleep duration from infancy to mid-childhood. Pediatrics. (2014) 133:e1163–71. doi: 10.1542/peds.2013-3998
18. Dore, RA , Logan, J , Lin, TJ , Purtell, KM , and Justice, LM . Associations between Children’s media use and language and literacy skills. Front Psychol. (2020) 11:1734. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2020.01734
19. Hill, MM , Gangi, D , Miller, M , Rafi, SM , and Ozonoff, S . Screen time in 36-month-olds at increased likelihood for ASD and ADHD. Infant Behav Dev. (2020) 61:101484. doi: 10.1016/j.infbeh.2020.101484
20. Kahn, M , Barnett, N , Glazer, A , and Gradisar, M . Sleep and screen exposure across the beginning of life: deciphering the links using big-data analytics. Sleep. (2021) 44:zsaa158. doi: 10.1093/sleep/zsaa158
21. Paulus, FW , Möhler, E , Recktenwald, F , Albert, A , and Mall, V . Electronic media and early childhood: a review. Klin Padiatr. (2021) 233:157–72. doi: 10.1055/a-1335-4936
22. Van den Heuvel, M , Ma, J , Borkhoff, CM , Koroshegyi, C , Dai, DWH , Parkin, PC, et al. Mobile media device use is associated with expressive language delay in 18-month-old children. J Dev Behav Pediatr. (2019) 40:99–104. doi: 10.1097/DBP.0000000000000630
23. Zhang, G , Wu, L , Zhou, L , Lu, W , and Mao, C . Television watching and risk of childhood obesity: a meta-analysis. Eur J Pub Health. (2016) 26:13–8. doi: 10.1093/eurpub/ckv213
24. Duch, H , Fisher, EM , Ensari, I , and Harrington, A . Screen time use in children under 3 years old: a systematic review of correlates. Int J Behav Nutr Phys Activity. (2013) 10:102. doi: 10.1186/1479-5868-10-102
25. Aglipay, M , Vanderloo, LM , Tombeau Cost, K , Maguire, JL , and Birken, CS, TARGet Kids! Collaborative . The digital media environment and cardiovascular risk in children. Can J Cardiol. (2020) 36:1440–7. doi: 10.1016/j.cjca.2020.04.015
26. Anderson, DR , and Subrahmanyam, K . Cognitive impacts of digital media workgroup. Digital screen media and cognitive development. Pediatrics. (2017) 140:S57–61. doi: 10.1542/peds.2016-1758C
27. Bozzola, E , Spina, G , Ruggiero, M , Memo, L , Agostiniani, R , Bozzola, M, et al. Media devices in pre-school children: the recommendations of the Italian pediatric society. Ital J Pediatr. (2018) 44:69. doi: 10.1186/s13052-018-0508-7
28. Hoge, E , Bickham, D , and Cantor, J . Digital media, anxiety, and depression in children. Pediatrics. (2017) 140:S76–80. doi: 10.1542/peds.2016-1758G
29. Poulain, T , Vogel, M , Neef, M , Abicht, F , Hilbert, A , Genuneit, J, et al. Reciprocal associations between electronic media use and behavioral difficulties in preschoolers. Int J Environ Res Public Health. (2018) 15:814. doi: 10.3390/ijerph15040814
30. Abels, M , Vanden Abeele, MMP , Van Telgen, T , and Van Meijl, H . Nod, nod, ignore: an exploratory observational study on therelation between parental mobile media use and parental responsive-ness towards young children In: EM Luef and MM Marin, editors. The talking species: Perspectives on the evolutionary, neuronal, and cultural foundations of language. Graz: Uni-Press (2018). 195–228.
31. McDaniel, BT . Parent distraction with phones, reasons for use, and impacts on parenting and child outcomes: a review of the emerging research. Human Behav Emerg Technol. (2019) 1:72–80. doi: 10.1002/hbe2.139
32. Braune-Krickau, K , Schneebeli, L , Pehlke-Milde, J , Gemperle, M , Koch, R , and Von Wyl, A . Smartphones in the nursery: parental smartphone use and parental sensitivity and responsiveness within parent-child interaction in early childhood (0-5 years): a scoping review. Infant Ment Health J. (2021) 42:161–75. doi: 10.1002/imhj.21908
33. Wolfers, LN , Kitzmann, S , Sauer, S , and Sommer, N . Phone use while parenting: an observational study to assess the association of maternal sensitivity and smartphone use in a playground setting. Comput Hum Behav. (2020) 102:31–8. doi: 10.1016/j.chb.2019.08.013
34. McDaniel, BT , and Radesky, JS . Technoference: longitudinal associations between parent technology use, parenting stress, and child behavior problems. Pediatr Res. (2018) 84:210–8. doi: 10.1038/s41390-018-0052-6
35. Komanchuk, J , Toews, AJ , Marshall, S , Mackay, LJ , Hayden, KA , Cameron, JL, et al. Impacts of parental Technoference on parent-child relationships and child health and developmental outcomes: a scoping review. Cyberpsychol Behav Soc Netw. (2023) 26:579–603. doi: 10.1089/cyber.2022.0278
36. Rideout, V . The common sense census: media use by Kids age zero to eight in America, a common sense media research study, [United States], 2013, 2017: version 2 (version v2) [data set]. ICPSR–Interuniversity Consortium for Political and Social Research (2019).
37. AAP Council on Communications and Media . Media and Young minds. Pediatrics. (2016) 138:e20162591. doi: 10.1542/peds.2016-2591
38. Brand, M , Young, KS , Laier, C , Wolfling, K , and Potenza, MN . Integrating psychological and neurobiological considerations regarding the development and maintenance of specific internet-use disorders: an interaction of person-affect-cognition-execution (I-PACE) model. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. (2016) 71:252–66. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2016.08.033
39. Dreier, M , Wolfling, K , and Müller, KW . Psychological research and a sociological perspective on problematic and addictive computer game use in adolescence. Int J Child Adolesc Health. (2013) 6:421–35.
40. Konca, AS . Digital technology usage of Young children: screen time and families. Early Childhood Educ J. (2021) 50:1097–108. doi: 10.1007/s10643-021-01245-7
41. Nikken, P . Implications of low or high media use among parents for young children’s media use. Cyberpsychology. (2017) 11:3. doi: 10.5817/CP2017-3-1
42. Nikken, P , and Schols, M . How and why parents guide the media use of Young children. J Child Fam Stud. (2015) 24:3423–35. doi: 10.1007/s10826-015-0144-4
43. Rideout, VJ , and Hamel, E . The media family: Electronic Media in the Lives of infants, toddlers, Preschoolers and Their Parents. San Francisco, CA: The Henry J. Kaiser Family Foundation (2006).
44. Tandon, PS , Zhou, C , Sallis, JF , Cain, KL , Frank, LD , and Saelens, BE . Home environment relationships with children’s physical activity, sedentary time, and screen time by socioeconomic status. Int J Behav Nutr Phys Act. (2012) 9:88. doi: 10.1186/1479-5868-9-88
45. Pons, M , Bennasar-Veny, M , and Yañez, AM . Maternal education level and excessive recreational screen time in children: a mediation analysis. Int J Environ Res Public Health. (2020) 17:8930. doi: 10.3390/ijerph17238930
46. Wijtzes, AI , Jansen, W , Kamphuis, CBM , Jaddoe, VWV , Moll, HA , Tiemeier, H, et al. Increased risk of exceeding entertainment-media guidelines in preschool children from low socioeconomic background: the generation R study. Prev Med. (2012) 55:325–9. doi: 10.1016/j.ypmed.2012.07.023
47. Anand, S , and Krosnick, JA . Demographic predictors of media use among infants, toddlers and preschoolers. Am Behav Sci. (2005) 48:539–61. doi: 10.1177/0002764204271512
48. Hoyos Cillero, I , and Jago, R . Systematic review of correlates of screen-viewing among young children. Prev Med. (2010) 51:3–10. doi: 10.1016/j.ypmed.2010.04.012
49. Cingel, DP , and Krcmar, M . Predicting media use in very Young children: the role of demographics and parent attitudes. Commun Stud. (2013) 64:374–94. doi: 10.1080/10510974.2013.770408
50. De Decker, E , De Craemer, M , De Bourdeaudhuij, I , Wijndaele, K , Duvinage, K , Koletzko, B, et al. Influencing factors of screen time in preschool children: an exploration of parents’ perceptions through focus groups in six European countries. Obes Rev. (2012) 13:75–84. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-789X.2011.00961.x
51. Bagley, S , Salmon, J , and Crawford, D . Family structure and children’s television viewing and physical activity. Med Sci Sports Exerc. (2006) 38:910–8. doi: 10.1249/01.mss.0000218132.68268.f4
52. Rehbein, F , and Mößle, T . Video game and internet addiction: is there a need for differentiation? Sucht. (2013) 59:129–42. doi: 10.1024/0939-5911.a000245
53. Winds, K , Aebi, M , and Plattner, B . Problematic internet use among adolescent male and female psychiatric inpatients: a gender perspective. Child Psychiatry Hum Dev. (2022). doi: 10.1007/s10578-022-01408-6
54. Green, SB , Huston, AC , Marquis, J , and Wright, JC . How Young children spend their time: television and other activities. Dev Psychol. (1999) 35:912–25. doi: 10.1037/0012-1649.35.4.912
55. Ofcom . Children and Parents: Media Use and Attitudes Report. (2014). Available at: http://stakeholders.ofcom.org.uk/binaries/research/media-literacy/media-use-attitudes14/Childrens_2014_Report.pdf.
56. Bischof, G , Bischof, A , Besser, B , and Rumpf, HJ . Problematische und pathologische Internetnutzung: Entwicklung eines Kurzscreenings (PIEK). In: Abschlussbericht an das Bundesministerium für Gesundheit. Lübeck: Universität zu Lübeck, Klinik für Psychiatrie und Psychotherapie. Berlin, Germany: Bundesministerium für Gesundheit, (2016).
57. Meerkerk, G-J , Van Den Eijnden, RJJM , Vermulst, AA , and Garretsen, HFL . The compulsive internet use scale (CIUS): some psychometric properties. Cycberpsychol Behav. (2009) 12:1–6. doi: 10.1089/cpb.2008.0181
58. Besser, B , Rumpf, H-J , Bischof, A , Meerkerk, G-J , Higuchi, S , and Bischof, G . Internet-related disorders: development of the short compulsive internet use scale. Cyberpsychol Behav Soc Netw. (2017) 20:709–17. doi: 10.1089/cyber.2017.0260
59. Cohen, J . Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences. Hillsdale, NJ: L. Erlbaum Associates (1988).
60. Ferjan Ramírez, N , Hippe, DS , and Shapiro, NT . Exposure to electronic media between 6 and 24 months of age: an exploratory study. Infant Behav Dev. (2021) 63:101549. doi: 10.1016/j.infbeh.2021.101549
61. Feierabend, S , Plankenhorn, T , Rathgeb, T , and Kim, M . Kleinkinder und Medien. Basisuntersuchung zum Medienumgang 2- bis 5-jähriger in Deutschland. Stuttgart: Medienpädagogischer Forschungsverbund Südwest, pp. 1–34. (2014).
62. American Academy of Pediatrics AAP . Media use by children younger than 2 years: policy statement of the AAP. Pediatrics. (2011) 128:1040–5. doi: 10.1542/peds.2011-1753
63. Trinh, M-H , Sundaram, R , Robinson, SL , Lin, T-C , Bell, EM , Ghassabian, A, et al. Association of trajectory and covariates of Children’s screen media time. JAMA Pediatr. (2020) 174:71–8. doi: 10.1001/jamapediatrics.2019.4488
64. Kracht, CL , Redman, LM , Bellando, J , Krukowski, RA , and Andres, A . Association between maternal and infant screen time with child growth and development: a longitudinal study. Pediatr Obes. (2023) 18:e13033. doi: 10.1111/ijpo.13033
65. Tandon, PS , Zhou, C , Lozano, P , and Christakis, DA . Preschoolers’ total daily screen time at home and by type of child care. J Pediatr. (2011) 158:297–300. doi: 10.1016/j.jpeds.2010.08.005
66. S ChengMaeda, T , Yoichi, S , Yamagata, Z , and Tomiwa, K, Japan Children's Study Group . Early television exposure and children’s behavioral and social outcomes at age 30 months. J Epidemiol. (2010) 20:S482–9. doi: 10.2188/jea.je20090179
67. Durham, K , Wethmar, D , Brandstetter, S , Seelbach-Göbel, B , Apfelbacher, C , Melter, M, et al. Digital media exposure and predictors for screen time in 12-month-old children: a cross-sectional analysis of data from a German birth cohort. Front Psych. (2021) 12:737178. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2021.737178
68. Kiliç, AO , Sari, E , Yucel, H , Oğuz, MM , Polat, E , Acoglu, EA, et al. Exposure to and use of mobile devices in children aged 1-60 months. Eur J Pediatr. (2019) 178:221–7. doi: 10.1007/s00431-018-3284-x
69. Rideout, VJ . Zero to eight: children's media use in America 2013. San Francisco, CA: Common Sense Media (2013).
70. Certain, LK , and Kahn, RS . Prevalence, correlates, and trajectory of television viewing among infants and toddlers. Pediatrics. (2002) 109:634–42. doi: 10.1542/peds.109.4.634
71. Davies, B , and Blake, E . Evaluating existing strategies to limit video game playing time. IEEE Comput Graph Appl. (2016) 36:47–57. doi: 10.1109/MCG.2016.25
72. King, DL , Delfabbro, PH , Doh, YY , Wu, AMS , Kuss, DJ , Pallesen, S, et al. Policy and prevention approaches for disordered and hazardous gaming and internet use: an international perspective. Prevent Sci. (2018) 19:233–49. doi: 10.1007/s11121-017-0813-1
73. Bundesministerium für Bildung und Forschung . Der DigitalPakt Schule kommt. (2023). Available at: https://www.bildung-forschung.digital/digitalezukunft/de/bildung/der-digitalpakt-schule-kommt.html?nn=249244 (accessed on 20 September 2023).
74. Kabali, HK , Irigoyen, MM , Nunez-Davis, R , Budacki, JG , Mohanty, SH , Leister, KP, et al. Exposure and use of mobile media devices by young children. Pediatrics. (2015) 136:1044–50. doi: 10.1542/peds.2015-2151
75. Canadian Paediatric Society, D. H. T. F., Ottawa, Ontario . Screen time and young children: promoting health and development in a digital world. Paediatr Child Health. (2017) 22:461–8. doi: 10.1093/pch/pxx123
76. Ventura, AK , Levy, J , and Sheeper, S . Maternal digital media use during infant feeding and the quality of feeding interactions. Appetite. (2019) 143:104415. doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2019.104415
77. Schou Andreassen, C , Billieux, J , Griffiths, MD , Kuss, DJ , Demetrovics, Z , Mazzoni, E, et al. The relationship between addictive use of social media and video games and symptoms of psychiatric disorders: a large-scale cross-sectional study. Psychol Addict Behav. (2016) 30:252–62. doi: 10.1037/adb0000160
78. Jabbar, A , and Warraich, N . Gender differences in leisure reading habits: a systematic review of literature. Glob Knowl Mem Commun. (2022) 72:572–92. doi: 10.1108/GKMC-12-2020-0200
79. Bleakley, A , Jordan, AB , and Hennessy, M . The relationship between parents’ and children’s television viewing. Pediatrics. (2013) 132:e364–71. doi: 10.1542/peds.2012-3415
80. Jago, R , Stamatakis, E , Gama, A , Carvalhal, IM , Nogueira, H , Rosado, V, et al. Parent and child screen-viewing time and home media environment. Am J Prev Med. (2012) 43:150–8. doi: 10.1016/j.amepre.2012.04.01282
81. Choe, DE , Lawrence, AC , and Cingel, DP . The role of different screen media devices, child dysregulation, and parent screen media use in Children’s self-regulation. Arlington, VA: Psychology of Popular Media (2022).
82. Woodard, E , and Gridina, N . Media in the home: 2000. Philadelphia, PA: University of Pennsylvania, Annenberg Public Policy Center (2000).
83. Jago, R , Thompson, JL , Sebiore, SJ , Wood, L , Pool, L , Zahra, J, et al. Cross-sectional associations between the screen-time of parents and young children: differences by parent and child gender and day of the week. Int J Behav Nutr Phys Act. (2014) 11:54. doi: 10.1186/1479-5868-11-54
84. Lauricella, AR , Wartella, E , and Rideout, VJ . Young children’s screen time: the complex role of parent and child factors. J Appl Dev Psychol. (2015) 36:11–7. doi: 10.1016/j.appdev.2014.12.001
86. Craig, L . Does father care mean fathers share? A comparison of how mothers and fathers in intact families spend time with children. Gend Soc. (2006) 20:259–81. doi: 10.1177/0891243205285212
87. Shin, E , Choi, K , Resor, J , and Smith, CL . Why do parents use screen media with toddlers? The role of child temperament and parenting stress in early screen use. Infant Behav Dev. (2021) 64:101595. doi: 10.1016/j.infbeh.2021.101595
88. Hefner, D , Knop, K , Schmitt, S , and Vorderer, P . Rules? Role model? Relationship? The impact of parents on their Children’s problematic Mobile phone involvement. Media Psychol. (2019) 22:82–108. doi: 10.1080/15213269.2018.1433544
89. Wolfers, LN , and Schneider, FM . Using Media for Coping: a scoping review. Commun Res. (2021) 48:1210–34. doi: 10.1177/0093650220939778
90. Wadley, G , Smith, W , Koval, P , and Gross, JJ . Digital emotion regulation. Curr Dir Psychol Sci. (2020) 29:412–8. doi: 10.1177/0963721420920592
91. Wolfers, LN , and Utz, S . Social media use, stress, and coping. Curr Opin Psychol. (2022) 45:101305. doi: 10.1016/j.copsyc.2022.101305
92. Qian, G , Mei, J , Tian, L , and Dou, G . Assessing mothers’ parenting stress: differences between one- and two-child families in China. Front Psychol. (2021) 11:609715. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2020.609715
93. Kaur, N , Gupta, M , Malhi, P , and Grover, S . Screen time in under-five children. Indian Pediatr. (2019) 56:773–88. doi: 10.1007/s13312-019-1638-8
94. JP Rey-LópezTomas, C , Vicente-Rodriguez, G , Gracia-Marco, L , Jiménez-Pavón, D , Pérez-Llamas, F, et al. Sedentary behaviours and socio-economic status in Spanish adolescents: the AVENA study. Eur J Pub Health. (2011) 21:151–7. doi: 10.1093/eurpub/ckq035
95. Auxier, B , Anderson, M , Perrin, A , and Turner, E . Parenting children in the age of screens. (2020). Available at: https://www.pewresearch.org/internet/2020/07/28/parenting-children-in-the-age-of-screens.
96. Lareau, A . Unequal childhoods: Class, race, and family life. 2nd ed. Berkely: University of California Press (2011).
97. Sebre, SB , Pakalniškiene, V , Jusiene, R , Wu, JC-L , Miltuze, A , Martinsone, B, et al. Children’s problematic use of the internet in biological and social context: a one-year longitudinal study. J Child Fam Stud. (2023). doi: 10.1007/s10826-022-02527-3
98. Mollborn, S , Limburg, A , Pace, J , and Fomby, P . Family socioeconomic status and Children’s screen time. J Marriage Fam. (2022) 84:1129–51. doi: 10.1111/jomf.12834
100. Hofferth, SL . Home media and Children’s achievement and behavior. Child Dev. (2010) 81:1598–619. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2010.01494.x
101. Davies, JJ , and Gentile, DA . Responses to Children’s media use in families with and without siblings: a family development perspective. Fam Relat. (2012) 61:410–25. doi: 10.1111/j.1741-3729.2012.00703.x
102. Hardy, LL , Baur, LA , Garnett, SP , Crawford, D , Campbell, KJ , Shrewsbury, VA, et al. Family and home correlates of television viewing in 12–13 year old adolescents: the Nepean study. Int J Behav Nutr Phys Act. (2006) 3:24. doi: 10.1186/1479-5868-3-24
103. Comstock, G , and Scharrer, E . The use of television and other screen media In: DG Singer and JL Singer, editors. Handbook of children and the media. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage Publications, Inc (2012). 13–44.
104. Hale, L , and Guan, S . Screen time and sleep among school-aged children and adolescents: a systematic literature review. Sleep Med Rev. (2015) 21:50–8. doi: 10.1016/j.smrv.2014.07.007
105. Beyens, I , and Nathanson, AI . Electronic media use and sleep among preschoolers: evidence for time-shifted and less consolidated sleep. Health Commun. (2019) 34:537–44. doi: 10.1080/10410236.2017.1422102
106. Mall, V , and Paulus, FW . Elektronische Medien und frühe Kindheit. Pädiatrie. (2018) 13:119–34. doi: 10.1055/s-0043115286
107. K ManzSchlack, R , Poethko-Müller, C , Mensink, G , Finger, J , Lampert, T, et al. Physical activity and electronic media use in children and adolescents: results of the KiGGS study: first follow-up (KiGGS wave 1). Bundesgesundheitsblatt Gesundheitsforschung Gesundheitsschutz. (2014) 57:840–8. doi: 10.1007/s00103-014-1986-4
108. Jing, M , Ye, T , Kirkorian, HL , and Mares, M-L . Screen media exposure and young children’s vocabulary learning and development: a meta-analysis. Child Dev. (2023) 94:1398–418. doi: 10.1111/cdev.13927
109. Yang, S , Saïd, M , Peyre, H , Ramus, F , Taine, M , Law, EC, et al. Associations of screen use with cognitive development in early childhood: the ELFE birth cohort. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. (2023). doi: 10.1111/jcpp.13887
110. Kirkorian, HL , Choi, K , and Pempek, TA . Toddlers’ word learning from contingent and noncontingent video on touch screens. Child Dev. (2016) 87:405–13. doi: 10.1111/cdev.12508
111. Roseberry, S , Hirsh-Pasek, K , and Golinkoff, RM . Skype me! Socially contingent interactions help toddlers learn language. Child Dev. (2014) 85:956–70. doi: 10.1111/cdev.12166
112. Bödeker, K , Watrin-Avino, LM , Martin, A , Schlensog-Schuster, F , Janssen, M , Friese, L, et al. Assessment and diagnostic classification using DC:0-5 in early childhood mental health clinics: the protocol for the developmental psychiatry diagnostic challenges study (DePsy). Children. (2023) 10:11. doi: 10.3390/children10111770
Keywords: parental media usage, children’s media usage, family factors, Problematic Internet Use, familial context, young children
Citation: Paulus FW, Joas J, Friedmann A, Fuschlberger T, Möhler E and Mall V (2024) Familial context influences media usage in 0- to 4-year old children. Front. Public Health. 11:1256287. doi: 10.3389/fpubh.2023.1256287
Edited by:
Margarete Ingrid Bolten, University of Basel, SwitzerlandReviewed by:
Melody Mary Terras, University of the West of Scotland, United KingdomNevena Dimitrova, Haute École de Travail Social et de la Santé Lausanne, Switzerland
Copyright © 2024 Paulus, Joas, Friedmann, Fuschlberger, Möhler and Mall. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.
*Correspondence: Frank W. Paulus, ZnJhbmsucGF1bHVzQHVrcy5ldQ==