- Teacher Training Department, Faculty of Psychology and Educational Sciences, “Alexandru Ioan Cuza” University of Iasi, Iași, Romania
The main purpose of this study was to examine the explanatory power of a predictive model of nomophobia consisting of rumination, fear of missing out (FoMO), mindfulness and non-pathological compulsions. The research involved a cross-sectional design exploring the prevalence of nomophobia in a Romanian university students’ cohort. The quantitative methodology was used to collect and analyse the data obtained from all the respondents. Researchers adapted and pretested the questionnaire NMP-Q, before distributing it to 194 university students. SPSS (V. 20) and Hayes’s PROCESS tool were used to analyse the data. The findings demonstrated that the above-mentioned psychological variables have a direct and significant relationship with nomophobia. Specifically, within the multidimensional mechanism that explains nomophobia, fear of missing out (FoMO), non-pathological compulsions, and rumination, included in the predictive model in this order, played the most important role, as together cover 34% of nomophobia variance. Furthermore, the fear of missing out has the highest explanatory contribution to nomophobia. The current study gives a better understanding of the dynamics of nomophobia in young people by focusing on psychological factors that play an important role in this phenomenon.
1 Introduction
Twenty years ago, probably no one would have thought that people can become addicted to their mobile phones and feel a serious state of stress when the device does not perform the expected functions. It is largely accepted that they serve purposes other than only communication, since information and entertainment are helping users to fulfill demands such as learning, developing individual skills, safety, and the need for social relationships (1). Frequent use of technology encourages recurrent and systematic monitoring of social media and texts and the consequences of this trend of frequent smartphone use could include disruptions in the current activity and a rise in anxiety (2), may have long-term effects resulting in personality disorders and perhaps exacerbating pre-existing problems e.g., “obsessive-compulsive personality disorder, social interaction anxiety, internet or smartphone addiction” [(3), p. 157]. While technology has undoubtedly improved our lives in many ways, excessive dependence on smartphones leading to nomophobia can have detrimental effects on mental health and overall wellbeing.
All these, suggests that technology has become a new source of unconscious and implicit reactions (4) giving rise to new pathologies – techno-pathologies (5). The overuse of digital devices could be conceived as a specific type of addiction and there are experts that talk about fear of missing out (6), fear of being offline and nomophobia as “forms of anxiety that border on obsession or compulsion” [(7), p. 3]. Other research has demonstrated that smartphones may result in compulsive checking behaviors, compulsive usage (8) and that smartphones can lead to addiction (9); extensive use of the mobile phone may have adverse consequences, materialized in increased depression levels, high anxiety, and poor sleep quality (10).
Some new emerging mental health problems can be added to this picture. For instance, textaphrenia or textiety (11), phantom phone signals-ringxiety (5), netlessphobia (12, 13) allows us to expand the image of 21st century digital diseases, that all are related to the use of mobile phone.
It is known that excessive dependence on smartphones (which can lead to nomophobia) can have adverse effects on mental health. It might contribute to increased stress, anxiety, and even depression due to factors like constant connectivity, social comparison, fear of missing out (FOMO), and decreased real-world interactions. Furthermore, nomophobia can significantly impact overall wellbeing. Reliance on smartphones at the expense of real-world experiences, relationships, and self-care can lead to a diminished sense of satisfaction and fulfillment in life. Excessive screen time can also affect sleep quality, physical health, and emotional balance, thus affecting overall wellbeing.
Over the last years, several authors have been concerned with identifying the psychological factors that play a significant role in explaining nomophobia. In other words, they looked to decipher the dilemma of the co-occurrence of nomophobia with certain states or stable personality traits. In this respect, some direct association between different variables and nomophobia have been observed, but also it was evaluated the strength of the interaction terms in order to explain variance beyond that can be explained by the main effects in a regression equation. Thus, several studies have tried to map direct, associative relationships between psychological factors and nomophobia discomfort, anxiety, nervousness and nomophobia (14), separation anxiety and nomophobia (15), higher levels of fear, discomfort, avoidant attachment and nomophobia (16), rumination and nomophobia (17), FoMO, and nomophobia (18), mindfulness and nomophobia (19), while others have proposed multiple regression models to capture the explanatory complexity of the phenomenon. For instance, the regression model proposed by Argumosa-Villar et al. (20) includes self-esteem, extraversion, conscientiousness and emotional stability as factors that significantly predicts nomophobia. The previous studies indicate that the set of psychological variables associated with the usage of mobile phones can be conceived as a complex relational structure.
Based on Field’s idea (21) according to which multiple hierarchical regression is the best way to build explanatory models of the outcome variables, the current study proposes a predictive model of nomophobia that includes rumination, fear of missing out (FoMO), mindfulness, and non-pathological compulsions. More than that, being a multi-faceted phenomenon, nomophobia could also be explained by indirect effects. In this respect, this study aims to investigate the mediation effect of fear of missing out (FoMO) on the relationship between rumination and nomophobia.
2 Theoretical background
2.1 Nomophobia and risk factors for problematic use of smartphone
The term nomophobia was devised in the Anglo-Saxon cultural space and comes from the expression “no mobile phone phobia’’ (22), which refers to the fear of not being in possession of your mobile phone or losing mobile phone contact. Nomophobia was described through four dimensions [(23), p. 130]:“not being able to communicate, losing connectedness, not being able to access information, giving up convenience.”
The pain or distress brought on by not having a smartphone available is known as being a situational phobia linked to agoraphobia (24). According to some authors (22, 25) the impact of ICT has favored the formation of new behaviors in people’s daily lives. Although its incorporation has streamlined and improved daily activities and provided benefits for people (26), smartphones also have caused addiction-related issues and even the emergence of psychological illnesses (nomophobia included) (27, 28). When separated from their smartphone for a short period of time and being unable to answer an incoming call, the subjects of some research (29, 30) experienced nomophobia in tandem with negative affect that leads them to elevated heart rate, blood pressure, and anxiety.
Other authors (14) connect nomophobia with a certain level of discomfort and anxiety caused by the lack of a smartphone. Recent researches proved that nomophobia positively correlates with neuroticism, attachment anxiety and loneliness (31). There are researchers (32) that place the nomophobia among the separation anxieties whereby mobile phones users experience a feeling of loss when their device is absent. Han and colleagues [(15), p. 2] suggest that separation anxiety (as described in literature) is linked to the “proximity-seeking tendency” toward the mobile device and is originated in extending one’s identity onto smartphones through the use of different ways of communication and social interaction.
As to the risk factors associated with nomophobia, different studies emphasize two main categories: socio-demographic and psychological factors. Most of the studies related to nomophobia focuses on university students (25, 33–40) and indicates that adolescents and young adults are more likely to be exposed to the negative effects of nomophobia. Emotionally dependent individuals and those that seeks for extra attention in the relationship, present higher levels of discomfort or fear. It was documented that “gender has a differential impact on the relationship between avoidant attachment and nomophobia” [(16), p. 1]. Nomophobia is predicted by several psychological factors such as: “self-esteem, extraversion, conscientiousness and emotional stability” [(20), p. 132].
Related to socio-demographic factors, in particular gender differences in the occurrence of nomophobia, some authors [(41), p. 19] highlighted that “females and young people seem to be more vulnerable to nomophobia.” In the same sense, Arpaci (42) reported statistically significant differences in nomophobia between women and men, wherein the female subjects outperformed the male subjects. Complementary, a study involving undergraduate students from Pakistan (40), explained that gender differences in NMP-Q questionnaire scores, were clearly influenced by the women subjects that demonstrates great levels of nomophobia.
The results indicate that the majority of studies investigating the incidence of nomophobia, primarily in teenagers and college students, are descriptive, nonexperimental, and cross-sectional in nature. These studies indicate that the current research is at an exploratory phase. An overall look upon the current researches suggests that nomophobia induces anxiety and stress and has a negative effect on personality, self-esteem and academic performance, leading to physical and mental health problems. All in all, this might be considered a health issue that has an adverse influence on the individual, leading to behavioral, physical, and psychological issues.
2.2 Rumination and nomophobia
The main psychological determinants of nomophobia namely anxiety, stress, fear, discomfort – are the triggers used by application developers and smartphones manufactures to exploit the user’s social and psychological weaknesses (43). The rapid distraction from current activities (mental disengagement) that mobile phones offer becomes a positive reinforcement that encourages the need to use smartphone because the human mind is predisposed to ruminating (44). Rumination – the tendency of an individual to think passively and repetitively about personal current feeling states, own characteristics and causes of these feelings (45) is considered to be a method of coping with negative moods that implies focusing on negative emotions (46). As a propensity to perseverate on the causes, experiences and implications of dysphoric emotions – rumination has received attention as a maladaptive emotion regulation response that is linked to prolonged dysphoric mood and depression risk (47, 48) and it was associated with anxiety (49) and with the distortion of emotional clarity (50). Because of the multiple negative consequences, it can induce (51), rumination is considered to be a major risk factor for depression and anxiety symptoms in both adolescents and adults (52, 53).
Rumination extensively predicted excessive smartphone use (44) but there is a scarcity of research examining the mechanisms and correlations that underly this relation (44). It seems that a general way of thinking based on exacerbating repetitive and intrusive negative thoughts could easily facilitate the path to nomophobia. Also, factors that could be put in relation with rumination mechanisms (e.g., “loneliness, social avoidance, and eccentricity”) significantly predicted nomophobia [(17), p. 1]. Rumination was found as a mediator for mobile phone addiction (54) and a predictor of smartphone addiction before going to sleep (55). Other individual characteristics such as emotional adaptability have an important effect on nomophobia (36).
2.3 FoMO (fear of missing out) and nomophobia
Being closely linked to the presence of the social networks, FoMO (fear of missing out) defines a state of uncertainty related to the fear of missing out on social network updates. The dual nature of the social media (on the one hand there are lots of opportunities for interaction, on the other hand these opportunities can be overwhelming in terms of time needed to pursue all the broadcast information) leads to this new type of online behavior.
FoMO is related to the desire to stay continuously online to witness to what others are doing and it was “defined as a pervasive apprehension” [(56), p. 1841] that other cyber-pals might have in terms of rewarding experiences while the subject is absent. The same authors (56) underlined that individual have low overall mood, low levels of welfare and life satisfaction. A recent study (57) has concluded that FoMO is directly related to depression, anxiety, and boredom, and a qualitative analysis on FoMO (58) reveals that the subject’s main concern is to avoid failure to be up to date with the latest news. After the massive entrance of the smartphones into our lives, the negative consequences of FoMO bring further proof that supports the existence of this type of addiction which leads people to spending a lot of time on social networks out of a fear of missing out.
The landscape of smartphone addiction is very extended and as a result, we cannot identify a definitive definition or a specific conceptualization of smartphone addiction. In this respect, there are papers that try to answer to nosology questions regarding smartphone addiction and, in this respect, the orientation is to use the non-pathological approach (59). Obviously, this is a matter of duration in using smartphones and terms such as excessive use or overuse of the smartphone, as well as problematic smartphone use have been proposed to replace the term of, “smartphone addiction” (60).
Other research findings (18) indicate that the students’ degrees of nomophobia and FoMO are above average. The above-mentioned authors show that there is “a moderately significant association and a positive direct relationship between fear of missing out and nomophobia, and that fear of missing out accounts for 30% of nomophobia” [(18), p. 16]. The results also reveal statistically significant differences in the levels of nomophobia and FoMO across students as well as variances in their demographic characteristics.
2.4 Mindfulness as positive factor to reduce nomophobia
People with high trait mindfulness tend to act consciously and attentively in their daily activities and they are generally non-reactive, non-judgmental, and conscious of their thoughts and emotions (61). In relation with nomophobia, mindfulness can act like a factor that can reduce tendencies for addictive and anxious behaviors (62).
Mindfulness is generally associated with positive emotional outcomes (62); as a consequence, a significant number of studies were produced along this line (63). There is research that suggests that while mindfulness increases, the effect of nomophobia on addictive smartphone use decreases (63, 64). The same relation was revealed by the studies of Arpaci et al.; furthermore they (16) suggested that there exists a significant negative correlation of mindfulness with nomophobia for both male and female subjects and mindfulness-based interventions can be effective in treating nomophobia, especially in women. Another study by the same authors (65) conclude that gender differences should be taken into account in the researches that studies mindfulness in relation with nomophobia.
However, the empirical research to support the positive influence of mindfulness in reducing nomophobia is somehow scarce and additional theoretical and research contributions to the study of the effects of mindfulness on smartphone addiction are needed (55, 66).
Recent investigations (19) showed that there is a significant positive correlation between mindfulness and psychological resilience, but a significant negative correlation between nomophobia and mindfulness. The same authors suggested that a significant mediating factor in the relationship between mindfulness and nomophobia is psychological resilience. These findings support the idea that the mindfulness-based interventions can increase psychological resilience and, consequently, help preventing nomophobia.
2.5 Non-pathological compulsions and nomophobia
There are not many researches that puts together the two concepts we are discussing and there is why when analyzing the relation between non-pathological compulsions and nomophobia, we can start from the observation that high levels of technology use are non-pathological unless they are linked with negative outcomes (67). The relation between nomophobia and non-pathological compulsions is very complex and it was approached differently. As some authors stated (68), in the 5th edition of DSM (69) there cannot be identified diagnostic criteria for mobile phone addiction although Internet gaming disorder is considered to be similar to gambling disorder. Starting from this, there were identified studies in this area (70) that used the diagnostic criteria for pathological gambling and/or substance dependence when analyzing nomophobia but this approach seems to be insufficiently theoretically substantiated. Other specialists in the area claim that smartphone addiction or nomophobia, is defined as a non-pathological behavioral addiction seen as extreme or problematic smartphone use (71). In this respect, it was our intention to put together the concept into the proposed model.
Additionally, in choosing this factor, non-pathological compulsions, in our study, we were inspired by work in the clinical psychology literature focusing on the association between obsessions, compulsions and nomophobia. According to this, individuals who show greater obsessive behavior (such as checking the smartphone very often) will have moderately more anxiety from “not being able to communicate with others” and/or “giving up convenience” [(9), p. 12]. Furthermore, other studies [(28), p. 7] pointed out that “sensitivity is a strong predictor of nomophobia along with obsession-compulsion, and the number of hours of smartphone use per day.” Very recent researches (72) showed that dysfunctional obsessive beliefs contribute to the occurrence of high levels of nomophobia. With all these findings in mind, involving clinical variables, we wanted to investigate the role of a non-clinical factor from the same spectrum, such as non-pathological compulsions in the occurrence of nomophobia taking into account that our study was conducted in a non-clinical setting, university environment with the participation of university students.
2.6 Explanatory model of nomophobia in the current study
Based on findings from previous studies and using hierarchical regression analysis, we have proposed an explanatory model of nomophobia, in which predictors were introduced into the equation according to their importance revealed in the scientific literature (21). So, a closer review of previous research has shown that the first factors considered important in explaining nomophobia were rumination and FoMO (fear of missing out). In support of this, some authors have emphasized that rumination extensively predicted excessive smartphone use (44) but there is a scarcity of research examining the mechanisms and correlations that underly this relation. Based on the work of Arpaci & Gundogan (19), we have chosen mindfulness as the third factor we introduced into the regression equation. The above-mentioned authors concluded that nomophobia exhibits a notable adverse correlation with mindfulness, while other authors (55, 66) have indicated that the empirical research to support the positive influence of mindfulness in reducing nomophobia is somehow scarce and further theoretical frameworks and research contributions are required to enhance our understanding of how mindfulness affects smartphone addiction. The final step of the hierarchical regression equation involved non-pathological compulsions. The reasons for introducing this factor at the end of the regression equation were due to the fact that there are no direct studies that examine the relationship between non-pathological compulsions and nomophobia, but rather we drew from clinical psychology and the differential diagnostic criteria of the DSM, 5th edition. From this latter perspective, evidences are provided by Gonçalves et al. (28) and García-Masip et al. (72) who revealed that the development of nomophobia involves the impact of obsession-compulsion tendencies and dysfunctional obsessive beliefs.
3 Methodology
The study relied on a quantitative approach for data gathering, employing a questionnaire-based survey.
3.1 Hypotheses of current study
Drawing from prior research, we formulated the following hypothesis:
H1a: Rumination, fear of missing out (FoMO), and non-pathological compulsions are positively related to nomophobia, meaning that as university students have higher levels of rumination, fear of missing out FoMO, and non-pathological compulsions, they will have higher levels of nomophobia.
H1b: Mindfulness is negatively related to nomophobia, meaning that for university students, increasing the level of mindfulness decreases nomophobia.
H2: Rumination, fear of missing (FoMO), mindfulness, and non-pathological compulsions form a significant predictive model of nomophobia.
H3: There is a mediation effect of fear of missing (FoMO) on the relationship between rumination and nomophobia, in that rumination affects nomophobia both directly and indirectly through fear of missing out. Consequently, a high level of rumination is linked to a higher level of nomophobia, both directly and indirectly through the intensification of fear of missing out, which in turn is associated with an increased level of nomophobia.
H4: There are significant gender differences in nomophobia, meaning that female participants aged 19 to 25 years have a higher fear level of being left without a phone compared to male participants.
H5: Nomophobia significantly varies depending on daily time spent using smartphone by study participants. In this respect, participants who utilized their phones for over 3 h daily are expected to exhibit a greater level of nomophobia.
3.2 Population and sampling
One hundred ninety-four university students from Iasi, Romania participated in a survey specifically designed to gather information on nomophobia, rumination, fear of missing out (FOMO), mindfulness, and non-pathological compulsions. Some demographic and relevant data were collected at the end of the questionnaire: participants’ age, gender, occupation, daily time spent on smartphones (<1 h, 1–3 h, 3–5 h, > 5 h), the most commonly used activities on smartphones (social networking, WhatsApp, Internet searching, SMS text messages, phone calls, games, music, other activities) and time since they own a smartphone (<1 year, 2 years, 3 years, 4 years, > 5 years). The questionnaire was on-line accessed using Google Forms.
This study utilized convenience sampling, implying that only students who were readily accessible and available were included in the participant group. An a priori power analysis was carried out using G*Power version 3.1.9.7 (73) to ascertain the minimum sample size necessary for testing the study hypothesis. The findings revealed that a sample size of N = 84 was required to achieve 80% power for detecting a medium effect, with a significance criterion set at α = 0.05, in the context of Correlation: Bivariate normal model. Thus, the attained sample size of N = 194 effectively tests the study hypothesis. Out of the participants, 43.39% were male, while 56.61% were female. The average age of the participants was21.03 (SDage = 1.78, range = 19–25). Prior to participation, all individuals provided informed consent for inclusion in the study. This research was conducted in compliance with the Declaration of Helsinki, and the protocol received approval from the Ethics Committee of the Faculty of Psychology and Educational Sciences at the “Alexandru Ioan Cuza” University of Iasi, Romania, in March 2023.
3.3 Instrumentation
The research design encompassed five concepts measured through dedicated questionnaires that were pretested on Romanian participants. Further sections offer detailed information regarding the reliability of these instruments. Therefore, nomophobia was measured using the Nomophobia Questionnaire (NMP-Q) developed by Yildirim & Correia in 2015. This tool covers 20 items and answers were collected on a seven-step Likert-type scale, where 1 means total disagree and 7 total agree. A high level on this scale represents greater involvement in using mobile phone, a state of heightened anxiety when access to this device is not allowed.
In order to measure rumination, we used the Rumination Scale, which includes 12 items from the Rumination-Reflection Questionnaire by Trapnell and Campbell (74). This scale describes the extent to which a person tends to think excessively about past events. Items 6, 9 and 10 of this scale are inverted items. Participants were required to indicate their level of agreement with each statement on a 5-point Likert scale ranging from 1 (strongly disagree) to 5 (strongly agree). A high score on this scale is a higher occurrence of negative thoughts, more negative interpretations of information, events and the future (75).
The scale used in our study to measure mindfulness was The Mindful Attention Awareness Scale (MAAS), developed by Brown and Ryan (76). This assessment focus on dispositional mindfulness, emphasizing the presence or absence of attention and awareness concerning the present moment. It specifically targets these aspects rather than attributes such as: acceptance, trust, empathy, gratitude, or other factors typically linked with mindfulness. The tool comprises 15 items, requiring participants to rate their agreement on a scale ranging from 1 to 6: 1 (almost always) to 6 (almost never) concerning each statement. A high score on this scale means increased attention to what is happening now, a high level of well-being, better life satisfaction and the presence of positive emotions, while low-score participants show symptoms such as depression, anxiety, negative emotions, anger and loneliness (76).
To assess Fear of Missing Out, we utilized the Fear of Missing Out Scale (FOMOS) developed by Przybylski (56). The tool includes 10 items and asks participants to indicate the state of truth or falsehood for each statement, according to their experience. A high score on this scale represents an altered general mood, a lower level of well-being, a lower level of life satisfaction and mixed feelings in using social networks, a decrease in sleep quality and an increase in negative emotions, stress and physical symptoms (77).
Non-pathological compulsions were measured using Compulsiveness Inventory questionnaire developed by Kagan and Squires (78). The instrument comprises 12 items developed to measure compulsive behavior that is normal among the population. A high score on this scale represents a larger occurrence of normal compulsive behaviors (clothes and home cleaning, checking doors/windows several times), compared to a low score on this scale which refers to lack of interest in the activities listed above (78).
3.4 Reliability
To assess the internal consistency of each scale utilized in the research, a pilot study was conducted, and the questionnaires were applied to 30 students. The results indicated that Cronbach’s alpha had values ranging from 0.63 to 0.94, as follows: 0.94 for Nomophobia Questionnaire (NMP-Q), 0.88 for Rumination-Reflection Questionnaire, 0.82 for The Mindful Attention Awareness Scale (MAAS), 0.81 for Fear of Missing Out Scale (FOMOS), and 0.63 for the Compulsiveness Inventory questionnaire. These findings demonstrated that the proposed instruments are reliable and that they can be used to collect data.
4 Data analysis procedure
The data was analyzed using the SPSS software program, Version 20.0. The study involved distributing the Google Form link to 250 students from various faculties at Iasi University, Romania. Out of the 250 completed questionnaires, only 194 responses that successfully passed the SPSS screening and cleaning procedures and were included in the current analysis.
In this investigation, hypotheses were formulated to explore potential statistically significant relationships among the variables. Consequently, methods like Pearson’s correlation, hierarchical regression, and mediation analysis employing Hayes’s PROCESS tool are deemed suitable for testing these hypotheses. According to Chan et al. (79) for samples which fall between 50 and 300, we need a z range between −3.29 and +3.29 in order to have a normal distribution. As shown in Table 1, the statistical data indicate a normal distribution.
5 Findings
Firstly, we hypothesized that it is possible to identify a set of relationships between rumination, fear of missing out (FoMO), mindfulness, non-pathological compulsions and nomophobia. More precisely, we suggested that rumination, fear of missing out (FoMO), and non-pathological compulsions are positively related to nomophobia, while mindfulness is negatively related to nomophobia.
As shown in Table 2, statistical data revealed the existence of a significant positive correlation between rumination and nomophobia, r(194) = 0.343, **p < 0.01, and a significant positive correlation between the fear of missing out and nomophobia, r(194) = 0.539, **p < 0.01. In other words, young people aged 19 to 25 who get high scores on rumination and fear of missing out tend to get high scores on nomophobia, too. Our findings are consistent with previous research conducted by Khoo and Yang (44), Lian et al. (54), and Cheng et al. (55) who found that there is a direct, positive and significant relationship between rumination and nomophobia. Additionally Hoşgör and Hoşgör (18), demonstrated a noteworthy moderate positive and direct relationship between nomophobia and fear of missing out. Their study revealed that fear of missing out accounted for 30% of nomophobia. Regarding the relationship between non-pathological compulsions and nomophobia, the Pearson correlation analysis indicated a significant negative correlation between non-pathological compulsions and nomophobia, r(194) = −0.251, **p < 0.01, which is an unexpected result, meaning that Romanian students who get high scores on non-pathological compulsions tend to get low scores on nomophobia. In this respect, we consider that the empirical research to support the positive relationship of non-pathological compulsions and nomophobia is somehow scarce and additional theoretical and research contributions are needed to study the impact of non-pathological compulsions on smartphone addiction.
The results concerning relationship between mindfulness and nomophobia pointed out a significant positive correlation between mindfulness and nomophobia, r(194) = 0.147, *p = 0.041. This means that the students participating in this research with high scores on mindfulness are more likely to have high scores on nomophobia; this result is in contrast with several earlier studies. Existing research suggests that while mindfulness increases, the effect of nomophobia on addictive smartphone use decreases (63, 64). The same relation was revealed by the studies of Arpaci et al. (65); furthermore they (65) suggested that there exists a significant negative correlation between mindfulness and nomophobia for both male and female subjects.
To examine the efficacy of explanatory models concerning nomophobia involving rumination, fear of missing out, mindfulness, and non-pathological compulsion, a three-stage hierarchical regression analysis was carried out (Figure 1).
Therefore, rumination and fear of missing out were entered at stage one of regression. Mindfulness was added at stage two and non-pathological compulsion at stage three. Variables were introduced in this order due to their importance revealed in previous studies. The significant values of standardized coefficients and changes in R2 were examined to assess the interaction terms’ capacity to elucidate variance beyond what is already explained by the main effects in the equation. Intercorrelations between the multiple regression variables were reported in Table 2 and the regression statistics are in Table 3.
The hierarchical multiple regression revealed that at stage one, rumination and fear of missing out contributed in a significant manner to the regression model, F(2,191) = 41.92, **p < 0.01, and described for 31% of the variation in nomophobia. On introducing mindfulness, variables explained an additional 0.4% in the variation in nomophobia and this change in R2 was significant, F(3,190) = 28.28, **p < 0.01. Adding non-pathological compulsion to the regression equation, the model explained an additional 3% of the variation in nomophobia and this change in R2 was significant, F(4,189) = 24.14, **p < 0.01.
The findings suggested that Model 3, comprising rumination, fear of missing out, mindfulness, and non-pathological compulsions, stands as the most accurate explanatory model. It displayed an adjusted coefficient of determination R2 = 0.34, indicating that the variables within Model 3 collectively account for 34% of the variance in nomophobia. The highest explanatory contribution for nomophobia is given by fear of missing out, followed by non-pathological compulsions and rumination. Additionally, the effect size indicators for significant predictors were: rsp = 0.41 for fear of missing out, rsp = −0.17 for non-pathological compulsions, and rsp = 0.12 for rumination. Therefore, the effects are medium (fear of missing out) and weak (non-pathological compulsions and rumination). Nomophobia is equal to 82 + 1.55*fear of missing out +0.37*rumination – 58.53*non-pathological compulsions, where fear of missing out is coded in dichotomous units 1 = true and 2 = false, non-pathological compulsions is also coded in dichotomous units 1 = true and 2 = false, and rumination is coded in units ranging from 5 = agree to 1 = disagree. These values indicate that as fear of missing out increases by one unit, nomophobia increases by 1.55 units. Also, as rumination increases by one unit, nomophobia increases by 0.37 units. Finally, as non-pathological compulsion increases by one unit, nomophobia decreases 58.53 units.
The nomophobia explanatory model that we have examined so far has unveiled a direct relationship between rumination, fear of missing out, mindfulness, and non-pathological compulsions (the predictors) and nomophobia (the outcome). Taking into consideration the complexity of nomophobia and starting from previous evidence (17, 18, 44, 54, 55), it’s obvious that the relationships among the factors explored in the study are multifaceted. Consequently, we hypothesized that the relationship between rumination and nomophobia is not only a direct effect, but it operates through an increase in fear of missing out (FoMO). The hypothesis was examined through a mediation analysis using Hayes’s PROCESS tool (Figure 2).
According to study findings, there was highlighted a significant indirect effect of rumination on nomophobia through fear of missing out, b = 0.598, Bca CI [0.378, 0.865], and similarly K2 = 0.206, 95% Bca CI [0.132, 0.285]. K2 is bounded between 0 and 1, so we can interpret this as the indirect effect being about 20.6% of the maximum value that it could have been, which is a medium and meaningful mediation effect. A high level of rumination leads to a high level of nomophobia, both directly and indirectly through increasing the fear of missing out, which leads to a higher level of nomophobia. Sobel mediation test values (z = 4.89, **p < 0.01) reconfirmed the importance of the fear of missing out factor as a mediator of the relationship between rumination and nomophobia. As we can see in Figure 2, the statistical analysis has demonstrated the presence of partial mediation within the proposed conceptual model. Specifically, it indicates that fear of missing out acts as a partial mediator in the relationship between rumination and nomophobia. It’s encouraging to note that these relationships are in line with the predicted direction.
As gender has been taken into account in previous works (65) in relation to some intervention models in treating nomophobia, showing their effectiveness especially in women, the study examined the difference in the intensity of nomophobia for man and woman participants (Table 4). We hypothesized that nomophobia is more present in young females compared to males. The outcomes from the Independent-Samples T Test indicate significant differences in nomophobia based on gender, t(192) = −4.555, **p < 0.01. Young female aged 19 to 25 years obtained higher scores on nomophobia (MD = 79.38) compared to young males of the same age (MD = 64.18).
A final refining of the analysis of nomophobia involved the time spent by students on mobile phone. Statistical data (Table 5) indicated that there is a significant difference between participants who use mobile phone more than 3 h a day and those who use it less than 3 h a day regarding the level of nomophobia, t(192) = −4.276, **p < 0.01. As the time of using mobile phone increases to more than 3 h a day, the level of nomophobia also increases (MD = 78.52), compared to the situation when time spent on the mobile phone is less than 3 h a day (MD = 64.06).
6 Discussion
Nowadays, in the empire of modern technologies, it has become really challenging to decipher the codes of human behavior. We can easily notice a variety of contrasting effects in using mobile phones, computers, the Internet and all the applications. All these are supposed to make our lives easier, but on the contrary, sometimes we experience undesirable effects on us, such as restlessness, fear and worry. In other words, there are times when, for example, using the phone leads to negative cognitive and affective reactions in terms of personal characteristics and achievements, social integration, effective interaction with the world around us and the possibility of being in continuous development. Such a negative affective reaction is nomophobia. In the scientific literature, several studies (15, 22, 27–30, 32, 80) have examined nomophobia as a factor underlying behavioral changes or negative affective reactions, and far fewer have explored what actually determines people to develop such a phobia. In light of these facts, our study wanted to highlight some triggers of nomophobia and to clarify the nature of the relationships between those predictors and nomophobia.
Our current research aimed to elucidate specific psychological factors that contribute to the presence of nomophobia among students within the Romanian university context. On this multidimensional phenomenon of nomophobia, fear of missing out (FoMO), non-pathological compulsions, and rumination, included in a regression model, in this order, played the most important role, together explaining 34% of nomophobia variance. Furthermore, the fear of missing out (FoMO) has the greatest explanatory power of nomophobia. In terms of the interpretation of these connections, there existed a direct, positive, and significant correlation between fear of missing out, rumination, and nomophobia, and a direct, negative, and significant association for non-pathological compulsions and nomophobia. This means that as fear of missing out (FoMO) and rumination increase, nomophobia also increases, and as non-pathological compulsions increase, nomophobia decreases. In other words, among Romanian students, the greater the uncertainty related to fear of missing out (FoMO) a call, a message (6) or social network updates, the greater fear of being out of mobile phone access will be. Complementary, the more students show a tendency to think passively and repetitively about personal current feeling states, own characteristics and the possible causes and consequences of these symptoms (45), the higher the level of nomophobia will be. These results confirm the hypotheses formulated in this study, and are also consistent with previous literature.
In their study, Gezgin et al. (80) identified a moderately positive relationship between nomophobia and levels of fear of missing out (FoMO). As well, Hoşgör and Hoşgör (18) showed that there is a moderately significant association and a positive direct relationship between fear of missing out and nomophobia, and that fear of missing out accounts for 30% of nomophobia. Indeed, in a study involving Chinese college students (81), highlighted that smartphone use for entertainment purposes and concerns about missing out on enjoyable experiences with friends were linked to susceptibility to nomophobia. Concerning the relationship between rumination and nomophobia, it involves a double sense. On one side, a closer review of previous studies indicates that rumination extensively predicts excessive smartphone use (44), but on the other side, in modern societies, there’s a shift in understanding rumination–it’s not only seen through a cognitive lens but also through the lens of human-device interaction. This expanded view, acknowledges how our contemplations intersect with and are influenced by our interactions with technology (82).
.Looking to map out the concepts and relationships underlying nomophobia, we have recognized that our explanatory model, focusing on the direct impact of predictors on the dependent variable, only addresses one facet of the issue. Based on previous studies (17, 18, 44, 54, 55, 83), we tested a mediator model in which the relationship between rumination and nomophobia is mediated by fear of missing out. So, we found a significant indirect effect of rumination on nomophobia through fear of missing out, which means that a high level of rumination predicts a high level of nomophobia, both directly and indirectly through increasing the fear of missing out, which, in turn, predicts a higher level of nomophobia.
Given the extent to which nomophobia has taken hold in the last few years, a comparative analysis of the variability of this phenomenon along relevant structural dimensions can improve our understanding of the concept. In the current study we have confirmed a prevalence of nomophobia in young females aged 19–25 years in comparison with young males of the same age. These findings are in line with the hypotheses formulated and are also supported by previous studies, some authors suggesting that nomophobia is present in both men and women, but specific intervention models in treating nomophobia may be more effective especially in women (16, 65).
The current study’s results align with Gonçalves, Dias, and Correia’s findings (28), indicating that the number of hours spent using smartphones daily serves as a strong predictor of nomophobia. Specifically, our study suggests that young individuals who use mobile phones for more than 3 h per day tend to exhibit higher levels of nomophobia. Basically, we are dealing here with a matter of duration in using smartphones which has been associated with terms such as excessive use or overuse of smartphones and smartphone addiction (60).
Finally, we can state that, based on the current study findings, the ideas, implicitly, the research hypotheses have been confirmed. Thus, we can emphasize the existence of a predictive model of nomophobia, in which the factors fear of missing out (FoMO), non-pathological compulsions, and rumination, included in a regression model, in this order, played the most important role. Additionally, this research has demonstrated the significant mediating effect of factor fear of missing out (FoMO) on the relationship between rumination and nomophobia. Given that we did not identify studies that put together the above-mentioned psychological factors into explanatory model of nomophobia, we believe that present results help to a better understanding of nomophobia and could guide a potential intervention to dilute this type of phobia.
7 Limitations of the study
The initial premise was rooted in previous studies indicating the prevalence of nomophobia, primarily among adolescents and university students. However, the significant limitation of the current study stemmed from the use of a study group confined to one university’s subjects. However, this does not diminish the value of the data obtained on the selected sample of participants. The results found in the present study may express a pattern that can certainly be replicated in other groups of university students. Indeed, the methodology’s reliance on a Google Form questionnaire might have impacted the objectivity and potentially introduced desirability biases in the participants’ responses to the questions. The adaptation of the questionnaires to the cultural context could be an extra limitation of the study, despite the fact that the scales showed good psychometric properties. Nevertheless, the outcomes of this study have provided significant insights and have led to comprehensive conclusions regarding potential explanations for nomophobia among young individuals.
8 Recommendations for future research
This study paves the way for future research endeavors and offers opportunities for further interpretation and exploration concerning nomophobia, for example, detailing the impact of mindfulness on nomophobia using of a multidimensional scale covering the facets of the concept of mindfulness. Furthermore, it is essential that in a future methodological investigation on nomophobia, the role of other predictor/mediator variables should also be taken into account. It would be interesting to test and clarify the relationships between depression, anxiety, psychological resilience, boredom, panic disorder, nervousness, welfare satisfaction with life and nomophobia. Ultimately, further research efforts will be essential to explore and understand nomophobia more comprehensively, using a longitudinal design with multiple measurements to more accurately capture the dynamics of this phenomenon.
9 Conclusion
According to the current research findings, fear of being without a mobile phone or losing mobile phone contact especially found in young people is predicted at 34% by rumination, fear of missing out, mindfulness, and non-pathological compulsion. Among these factors, the highest explanatory contribution for nomophobia is given by fear of missing out, which has a direct impact on the phenomenon, but also mediates the relationship between rumination and nomophobia. In other words, on one hand, a high level of rumination leads to a high level of nomophobia, both directly and indirectly through increasing the fear of missing out, which, on the other hand, leads to a higher level of nomophobia. In addition, the study indicates the prevalence of nomophobia in women, and draws attention to time duration in using smartphones daily.
Data availability statement
The raw data supporting the conclusions of this article will be made available by the authors, without undue reservation.
Author contributions
CC, AM, and RA contributed to conception and design of the study and wrote sections of the manuscript. AM organized the database and performed the statistical analysis. CC wrote the first draft of the manuscript. All authors contributed to manuscript revision, read, and approved the submitted version.
Acknowledgments
Authors are thankful to Romanian Ministry of Research, Innovation and Digitization, within Program 1 – Development of the national RD system, Subprogram 1.2 – Institutional Performance – RDI excellence funding projects, Contract no. 11PFE/30.12.2021, for financial support.
Conflict of interest
The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.
Publisher’s note
All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.
References
1. Kang, S , and Jung, J . Mobile communication for human needs: a comparison of smartphone use between the US and Korea. Comput Hum Behav. (2014) 35:376–87. doi: 10.1016/j.chb.2014.03.024
2. Mendoza, JS , Pody, BC , Lee, S , Kim, M , and McDonough, IM . The effect of cellphones on attention and learning: the influences of time, distraction, and nomophobia. Comput Hum Behav. (2018) 86:52–60. doi: 10.1016/j.chb.2018.04.027
3. Bragazzi, NL , and del Puente, G . A proposal for including nomophobia in the new DSM-V. Psychol Res Behav Manag. (2014) 7:155–60. doi: 10.2147/PRBM.S41386
4. Greenwald, AG , and Banaji, MR . The implicit revolution: reconceiving the relation between conscious and unconscious. Am Psychol. (2017) 72:861–71. doi: 10.1037/amp0000238
5. Frissen, T . Phantom cell phone signals/Ringxiety In: J Bulk , editor. The international encyclopedia of media psychology. Hoboken, NJ: Wiley (2020). 1–8.
6. Lai, C , Altavilla, D , Ronconi, A , and Aceto, P . Fear of missing out (FOMO) is associated with activation of the right middle temporal gyrus during inclusion social cue. Comput Hum Behav. (2016) 61:516–21. doi: 10.1016/j.chb.2016.03.072
7. Rosen, L , and Samuel, A . Conquering digital distraction. Harv Bus Rev. (2015) 45:12. doi: 10.2469/dig.v45.n8.12
8. Oulasvirta, A , Rattenbury, T , Ma, L , and Raita, E . Habits make smartphone use more pervasive. Pers Ubiquit Comput. (2012) 16:105–14. doi: 10.1007/s00779-011-0412-2
9. Lee, S , Kim, M , Mendoza, JS , and McDonough, IM . Addicted to cellphones: exploring the psychometric properties between the nomophobia questionnaire and obsessiveness in college students. Heliyon. (2018) 4:e00895. doi: 10.1016/j.heliyon.2018.e00895
10. Graben, K , Doering, BK , Jeromin, F , and Barke, A . Problematic Mobile phone use: validity and reliability of the problematic use of Mobile phone (PUMP) scale in a German sample. Addict Behav Rep. (2020) 12:100297. doi: 10.1016/j.abrep.2020.100297
11. Verma, RK , Rajiah, K , Cheang, A , and Barua, A . Textaphrenia: an emerging silent pandemic. Afr J Psychiatry (South Africa). (2014) 17:510–1. doi: 10.4172/psychiatry.1000e103
12. Alt, D , and Boniel-Nissim, M . Links between adolescents’ deep and surface learning approaches, problematic internet use, and fear of missing out (FoMO). Internet Interv. (2018) 13:30–9. doi: 10.1016/j.invent.2018.05.002
13. Kanbay, Y , Fırat, M , Akçam, A , Çınar, S , and Özbay, Ö . Development of Fırat Netlessphobia scale and investigation of its psychometric properties. Perspect Psychiatr Care. (2022) 58:1258–66. doi: 10.1111/ppc.12924
14. Lin, IH , Ko, CH , Chang, YP , Liu, TL , Wang, PW , Lin, HC, et al. The association between suicidality and internet addiction and activities in Taiwanese adolescents. Compr Psychiatry. (2014) 55:504–10. doi: 10.1016/j.comppsych.2013.11.012
15. Han, S , Kim, KJ , and Kim, JH . Understanding nomophobia: structural equation modeling and semantic network analysis of smartphone separation anxiety. Cyberpsychol Behav Soc Netw. (2017) 20:419–27. doi: 10.1089/CYBER.2017.0113
16. Arpaci, I , Baloğlu, M , Kozan, HİÖ , and Kesici, Ş . Individual differences in the relationship between attachment and nomophobia among college students: the mediating role of mindfulness. J Med Internet Res. (2017) 19:e404–13. doi: 10.2196/jmir.8847
17. Lu, X , Liu, T , Liu, X , Yang, H , and Elhai, JD . Nomophobia and relationships with latent classes of solitude. Bull Menn Clin. (2022) 86:1–19. doi: 10.1521/BUMC.2022.86.1.1
18. Hoşgör, H , and Hoşgör, DG . The relationship among nomophobia, fear of miss-ing out and demographic variables: example of the healthcare manager candidates. Curr Addict Res. (2019) 3:1–24. doi: 10.5455/car.105-1562758780
19. Arpaci, I , and Gundogan, S . Mediating role of psychological resilience in the relationship between mindfulness and nomophobia. Br J Guid Couns. (2020) 50:782–90. doi: 10.1080/03069885.2020.1856330
20. Argumosa-Villar, L , Boada-Grau, J , and Vigil-Colet, A . Exploratory investigation of theoretical predictors of nomophobia using the Mobile phone involvement questionnaire (MPIQ). J Adolesc. (2017) 56:127–35. doi: 10.1016/J.ADOLESCENCE.2017.02.003
22. King, ALS , Valença, AM , Silva, AC , Sancassiani, F , Machado, S , and Nardi, AE . “Nomophobia”: impact of cell phone use interfering with symptoms and emotions of individuals with panic disorder compared with a control group. Clin Pract Epidemiol Ment Health. (2014) 10:28–35. doi: 10.2174/1745017901410010028
23. Yildirim, C , and Correia, AP . Exploring the dimensions of nomophobia: development and validation of a self-reported questionnaire. Comput Hum Behav. (2015) 49:130–7. doi: 10.1016/j.chb.2015.02.059
24. King, ALS , Valença, AM , and Nardi, AE . Nomophobia: the mobile phone in panic disorder with agoraphobia: reducing phobias or worsening of dependence? Cogn Behav Neurol. (2010) 23:52–4. doi: 10.1097/WNN.0b013e3181b7eabc
25. Rodríguez-García, AM , Belmonte, JL , and Moreno-Guerrero, AJ . Nomophobia: an individual’s growing fear of being without a smartphone—a systematic literature review. Int J Environ Res Public Health. (2020) 17:580. doi: 10.3390/ijerph17020580
26. Dai, C , Tai, Z , and Ni, S . Smartphone use and psychological well-being among college students in China: a qualitative assessment. Front Psychol. (2021) 12:970. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2021.708970
27. Adawi, M , Zerbetto, R , Re, TS , Bisharat, B , Mahamid, M , Amital, H, et al. Psychometric properties of the brief symptom inventory in nomophobic subjects: insights from preliminary confirmatory factor, exploratory factor, and clustering analyses in a sample of healthy Italian volunteers. Psychol Res Behav Manag. (2019) 12:145–54. doi: 10.2147/PRBM.S173282
28. Gonçalves, S , Dias, P , and Correia, AP . Nomophobia and lifestyle: smartphone use and its relationship to psychopathologies. Comput Hum Behav Rep. (2020) 2:100025. doi: 10.1016/j.chbr.2020.100025
29. Csibi, S , Griffiths, MD , Cook, B , Demetrovics, Z , and Szabo, A . The psychometric properties of the smartphone application-based addiction scale (SABAS). Int J Ment Health Addict. (2018) 16:393–403. doi: 10.1007/s11469-017-9787-2
30. Elhai, JD , Levine, JC , Dvorak, RD , and Hall, BJ . Fear of missing out, need for touch, anxiety and depression are related to problematic smartphone use. Comput Hum Behav. (2016) 63:509–16. doi: 10.1016/j.chb.2016.05.079
31. Sun, Y , Yang, J , Li, M , and Liu, T . The association between neuroticism and nomophobia: chain mediating effect of attachment and loneliness. Int J Ment Heal Addict. (2022) 2022:1–18. doi: 10.1007/S11469-022-00897-9/METRICS
32. Cheever, NA , Rosen, LD , Carrier, LM , and Chavez, A . Out of sight is not out of mind: the impact of restricting wireless mobile device use on anxiety levels among low, moderate and high users. Comput Hum Behav. (2014) 37:290–7. doi: 10.1016/j.chb.2014.05.002
33. Bartwal, J , and Nath, B . Evaluation of nomophobia among medical students using smartphone in North India. Med J Arm Forces India. (2020) 76:451–5. doi: 10.1016/j.mjafi.2019.03.001
34. Gurbuz, IB , and Ozkan, G . What is your level of nomophobia? An investigation of prevalence and level of nomophobia among Young people in Turkey. Community Ment Health J. (2020) 56:814–22. doi: 10.1007/s10597-019-00541-2
35. Gutiérrez-Puertas, L , Márquez-Hernández, V , São-Romão-Preto, L , Granados-Gámez, G , Gutiérrez-Puertas, V , and Aguilera-Manrique, G . Comparative study of nomophobia among Spanish and Portuguese nursing students. Nurse Educ Pract. (2019) 34:79–84. doi: 10.1016/j.nepr.2018.11.010
36. Luo, J , Ren, S , Li, Y , and Liu, T . The effect of college students’ adaptability on nomophobia: based on lasso regression. Front Psych. (2021) 12:1417. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2021.641417
37. Márquez-Hernández, V , Gutiérrez-Puertas, L , Granados-Gámez, G , Gutiérrez-Puertas, V , and Aguilera-Manrique, G . Problematic mobile phone use, nomophobia and decision-making in nursing students mobile and decision-making in nursing students. Nurse Educ Pract. (2020) 49:102910. doi: 10.1016/j.nepr.2020.102910
38. Moreno-Guerrero, AJ , Hinojo-Lucena, FJ , Trujillo-Torres, JM , and Rodríguez-García, AM . Nomophobia and the influence of time to REST among nursing students. A descriptive, correlational and predictive research. Nurse Educ Pract. (2021) 52:103025. doi: 10.1016/j.nepr.2021.103025
39. Moreno-Guerrero, AJ , López-Belmonte, J , Romero-Rodríguez, JM , and Rodríguez-García, AM . Nomophobia: impact of cell phone use and time to rest among teacher students. Heliyon. (2020) 6:e04087. doi: 10.1016/j.heliyon.2020.e04084
40. Schwaiger, E , and Tahir, R . Nomophobia and its predictors in undergraduate students of Lahore, Pakistan. Heliyon. (2020) 6:e04837. doi: 10.1016/j.heliyon.2020.e04837
41. León-Mejía, AC , Gutiérrez-Ortega, M , Serrano-Pintado, I , and González-Cabrera, J . A systematic review on nomophobia prevalence: surfacing results and standard guidelines for future research. PLoS One. (2021) 16:e0250509. doi: 10.1371/JOURNAL.PONE.0250509
42. Arpaci, I . Gender differences in the relationship between problematic internet use and nomophobia. Curr Psychol. (2022) 41:6558–67. doi: 10.1007/s12144-020-01160-x
43. Eyal, N , and Hoover, R . Hooked: How to build habit-forming products. London: Penguin Group (2014).
44. Khoo, SS , and Yang, H . Mental disengagement mediates the effect of rumination on smartphone use: a latent growth curve analysis. Comput Hum Behav. (2021) 120:106757. doi: 10.1016/j.chb.2021.106757
45. Nolen-Hoeksema, S , and Morrow, J . Effects of rumination and distraction on naturally occurring depressed mood. Cognit Emot. (1993) 7:561–70. doi: 10.1080/02699939308409206
46. Lyubomirsky, S , and Nolen-Hoeksema, S . Effects of self-focused rumination on negative thinking and interpersonal problem solving. J Pers Soc Psychol. (1995) 69:176–90. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.69.1.176
47. Joormann, J , and Stanton, CH . Examining emotion regulation in depression: a review and future directions. Behav Res Ther. (2016) 86:35–49. doi: 10.1016/j.brat.2016.07.007
48. Yaroslavsky, I , Napolitano, SC , and France, CM . Ruminative responses to interpersonal precipitants mediate borderline personality disorder features’ effects on distress reactivity and recovery in daily life. J Clin Psychol. (2019) 75:2188–209. doi: 10.1002/JCLP.22839
49. Stone, LB , Lewis, GM , and Bylsma, LM . The autonomic correlates of dysphoric rumination and post-rumination savoring. Physiol Behav. (2020) 224:113027. doi: 10.1016/j.physbeh.2020.113027
50. Lask, LS , Moyal, N , and Henik, A . Rumination, emotional intensity and emotional clarity. Conscious Cogn. (2021) 96:103242. doi: 10.1016/j.concog.2021.103242
51. Watkins, ER , and Roberts, H . Reflecting on rumination: consequences, causes, mechanisms and treatment of rumination. Behav Res Ther. (2020) 127:103573. doi: 10.1016/j.brat.2020.103573
52. Michl, LC , McLaughlin, KA , Shepherd, K , and Nolen-Hoeksema, S . Rumination as a mechanism linking stressful life events to symptoms of depression and anxiety: longitudinal evidence in early adolescents and adults. J Abnorm Psychol. (2013) 122:339–52. doi: 10.1037/A0031994
53. Wilkinson, PO , Croudace, TJ , and Goodyer, IM . Rumination, anxiety, depressive symptoms and subsequent depression in adolescents at risk for psychopathology: a longitudinal cohort study. BMC Psychiatry. (2013) 13:1–9. doi: 10.1186/1471-244X-13-250/TABLES/5
54. Lian, SL , Sun, XJ , Niu, GF , Yang, XJ , Zhou, ZK , and Yang, C . Mobile phone addiction and psychological distress among Chinese adolescents: the mediating role of rumination and moderating role of the capacity to be alone. J Affect Disord. (2021) 279:701–10. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2020.10.005
55. Cheng, S-S , Zhang, C-Q , and Wu, J-Q . Mindfulness and smartphone addiction before going to sleep among college students: the mediating roles of self-control and rumination. Clocks Sleep. (2020) 2:354–63. doi: 10.3390/clockssleep2030026
56. Przybylski, AK , Murayama, K , Dehaan, CR , and Gladwell, V . Motivational, emotional, and behavioral correlates of fear of missing out. Comput Hum Behav. (2013) 29:1841–8. doi: 10.1016/j.chb.2013.02.014
57. Holte, AJ , and Ferraro, FR . Anxious, bored, and (maybe) missing out: Evaluation of anxiety attachment, boredom proneness, and fear of missing out (FoMO). Comput Hum Behav. (2020) 112:106465. doi: 10.1016/j.chb.2020.106465
58. Gezgin, D , Hamutoglu, NB , Sezen Gultekin, G , and Yildirim, S . Preservice teachers’ metaphorical perceptions on smartphone, no Mobile phone phobia (nomophobia) and fear of missing out (FoMO). Bartın Üniv Eğitim Fakült Derg. (2019) 8:733–83. doi: 10.14686/buefad.516540
59. Bae, SM . Smartphone addiction of adolescents, not a smart choice. J Korean Med Sci. (2017) 32:1563–4. doi: 10.3346/JKMS.2017.32.10.1563
60. Ryding, FC , and Kuss, DJ . Passive objective measures in the assessment of problematic smartphone use: a systematic review. Addict Behav Rep. (2020) 11:100257. doi: 10.1016/J.ABREP.2020.100257
61. Baer, RA , Smith, GT , Hopkins, J , Krietemeyer, J , and Toney, L . Using self-report assessment methods to explore facets of mindfulness. Assessment. (2006) 13:27–45. doi: 10.1177/1073191105283504
62. Spears, CA , Hedeker, D , Li, L , Wu, C , Anderson, NK , Houchins, SC, et al. Mechanisms underlying mindfulness-based addiction treatment versus cognitive behavioral therapy and usual care for smoking cessation. J Consult Clin Psychol. (2017) 85:1029–40. doi: 10.1037/CCP0000229
63. Liu, QQ , Zhou, ZK , Yang, XJ , Kong, FC , Niu, GF , and Fan, CY . Mobile phone addiction and sleep quality among Chinese adolescents: a moderated mediation model. Comput Hum Behav. (2017) 72:108–14. doi: 10.1016/j.chb.2017.02.042
64. Regan, T , Harris, B , Van Loon, M , Nanavaty, N , Schueler, J , Engler, S, et al. Does mindfulness reduce the effects of risk factors for problematic smartphone use? Comparing frequency of use versus self-reported addiction. Addict Behav. (2020) 108:106435. doi: 10.1016/j.addbeh.2020.106435
65. Arpaci, I , Baloğlu, M , and Kesici, Ş . A multi-group analysis of the effects of individual differences in mindfulness on nomophobia. Inf Dev. (2019) 35:333–41. doi: 10.1177/0266666917745350
66. Koppel, S , Stephens, AN , Kaviani, F , Peiris, S , Young, KL , Chambers, R, et al. It’s all in the mind: the relationship between mindfulness and nomophobia on technology engagement while driving and aberrant driving behaviours. Transport Res F: Traffic Psychol Behav. (2022) 86:252–62. doi: 10.1016/j.trf.2022.03.002
67. Charlton, JP , and Danforth, IDW . Distinguishing addiction and high engagement in the context of online game playing. Comput Hum Behav. (2007) 23:1531–48. doi: 10.1016/j.chb.2005.07.002
68. Yang, Z , Asbury, K , and Griffiths, MD . An exploration of problematic smartphone use among Chinese university students: associations with academic anxiety, academic procrastination, self-regulation and subjective wellbeing. Int J Ment Health Addict. (2019) 17:596–614. doi: 10.1007/s11469-018-9961-1
69. American Psychiatric, Association . (2013). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (5th ed.).
70. Kuss, J , Griffiths, M , Karila, L , and Billieux, J . Internet addiction: a systematic review of epidemiological research for the last decade. Curr Pharm Des. (2014) 20:4026–52. doi: 10.2174/13816128113199990617
71. Billieux, J , Maurage, P , Lopez-Fernandez, O , Kuss, DJ , and Griffiths, MD . Can disordered Mobile phone use be considered a behavioral addiction? An update on current evidence and a comprehensive model for future research. Curr Addict Rep. (2015) 2:156–62. doi: 10.1007/s40429-015-0054-y
72. García-Masip, V , Sora, B , Serrano-Fernandez, MJ , Boada-Grau, J , and Lampert, B . Personality and nomophobia: the role of dysfunctional obsessive beliefs. Int J Environ Res Public Health. (2023) 20:4128. doi: 10.3390/ijerph20054128
73. Faul, F , Erdfelder, E , Lang, AG , and Buchner, A . G*power 3: a flexible statistical power analysis program for the social, behavioral, and biomedical sciences. Behav Res Methods. (2007) 39:175–91. doi: 10.3758/BF03193146/METRICS
74. Trapnell, PD , and Campbell, JD . Private self-consciousness and the five-factor model of personality: distinguishing rumination from reflection. J Pers Soc Psychol. (1999) 76:284–304. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.76.2.284
75. Nolen-Hoeksema, S , Wisco, BE , and Lyubomirsky, S . Rethinking rumination. Perspect Psychol Sci. (2008) 3:400–24. doi: 10.1111/j.1745-6924.2008.00088.x
76. Brown, KW , and Ryan, RM . The benefits of being present: mindfulness and its role in psychological well-being. J Pers Soc Psychol. (2003) 84:822–48. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.84.4.822
77. Milyavskaya, M , Saffran, M , Hope, N , and Koestner, R . Fear of missing out: prevalence, dynamics, and consequences of experiencing FOMO. Motiv Emot. (2018) 42:725–37. doi: 10.1007/S11031-018-9683-5
78. Kagan, DM , and Squires, RL . Measuring nonpathological compulsiveness. Psychol Rep. (1985) 57:559–63. doi: 10.2466/PR0.1985.57.2.559
79. Chan, MCY , Aryadoust, V , and Nie, Y . CORE 2018: a quantitative study of teaching and learning in Singapore classrooms. (2022). Available at: https://repository.nie.edu.sg/handle/10497/24456.
80. Gezgin, D , Hamutoglu, NB , Sezen-Gultekin, G , and Gemikonakli, O . Relationship between nomophobia and fear of missing out among Turkish university students. Cypriot J Educ Sci. (2018) 13:1. doi: 10.18844/cjes.v13i4.3464
81. Wen, J , Huang, Y , Liu, G , and Miao, M . The nature of nomophobia and its associations with contents of smartphone use and fear of missing out: a network perspective. Telematics Inform. (2023) 82:102011. doi: 10.1016/j.tele.2023.102011
82. Wong, SMY , Chen, EYH , Lee, MCY , Suen, YN , and Hui, CLM . Rumination as a Transdiagnostic phenomenon in the 21st century: the flow model of rumination. Brain Sci. (2023) 13:1041. doi: 10.3390/brainsci13071041
Keywords: techno phobias, nomophobia, rumination, fear of missing out, non-pathological compulsions, mindfulness
Citation: Ceobanu CM, Marian AL and Apostolache R (2023) Glimpse on 21st century new phobias; a predictive model of nomophobia. Front. Public Health. 11:1252099. doi: 10.3389/fpubh.2023.1252099
Edited by:
Maj Britt Dahl Nielsen, University of Southern Denmark, DenmarkReviewed by:
Filiz Adana, Adnan Menderes University, TürkiyeGustavo Cunha de Araujo, Federal University of North Tocantins, Brazil
Karolina Appelt, Adam Mickiewicz University, Poland
Copyright © 2023 Ceobanu, Marian and Apostolache. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.
*Correspondence: Ciprian Marius Ceobanu, Y2lwcmlhbkB1YWljLnJv
†These authors have contributed equally to this work and share first authorship