Skip to main content

REVIEW article

Front. Public Health, 30 May 2023
Sec. Public Mental Health
This article is part of the Research Topic Advances in the Understanding of the Affective and Cognitive Effects of Physical Activity, Exercise, and Sports View all 19 articles

Pet’s influence on humans’ daily physical activity and mental health: a meta-analysis

Catarina F. Martins
Catarina F. Martins1*Jorge P. SoaresJorge P. Soares1Antnio CortinhasAntónio Cortinhas1Luís SilvaLuís Silva1Luís Cardoso,Luís Cardoso2,3Maria A. Pires,Maria A. Pires2,3Maria P. MotaMaria P. Mota1
  • 1Research Centre in Sports Sciences, Health and Human Development (CIDESD), Department of Sport, Exercise and Health Sciences, School of Life and Environmental Sciences (ECVA), University of Trás-os-Montes and Alto Douro (UTAD), Vila Real, Portugal
  • 2Animal and Veterinary Research Centre (CECAV), Department of Veterinary Sciences, School of Agrarian and Veterinary Sciences, University of Trás-os-Montes and Alto Douro (UTAD), Vila Real, Portugal
  • 3Associate Laboratory for Animal and Veterinary Sciences, University of Trás-os-Montes and Alto Douro (UTAD), Vila Real, Portugal

Abstract: The benefits of the human-animal bond on owners’ health and quality of life have been the focus of research in recent decades. However, the results are still inconsistent. Thus, this study aims to investigate whether the presence of a pet, compared to a control group, influences daily physical activity levels and mental health using a meta-analytic method.

Methods: The PubMed, Web of Science, and Scopus databases were searched for all research articles that included pets as an object of study and related mental health and quality of life variables between pet owners and non-owners until April 2022. The PRISMA 2020 checklist was used, and the Downs and Back checklist was used to assess the methodological quality of the studies. Standardized mean differences and 95% confidence intervals were used to assess the difference between a group of pet owners and non-pet owners.

Results: An initial search located 11,389 studies, but only 49 studies fulfilled all requirements. Our results indicate that pets have a moderately significant positive effect on the physical activity of owners compared to non-pet owners. Among the moderating variables, the frequency of physical activity showed a highly significant effect, indicating that owners had a higher frequency of physical activity than non-owners. Moreover, our results indicate a significant impact but with a low effect size of pets on owners’ mental health when compared to non-pet owners.

Conclusion: Pet ownership does not seem to influence owners’ mental health, but it does influence their physical activity. Specifically, owners show a higher frequency of physical activity than non-owners.

1. Introduction

Pets play an important role in human life and human health (1). Improvements in physical, mental, psychological, and social health have been described in several works (1–7).

Physical activity (PA) is a determinant of health and quality of life and has been indicated for the prevention and treatment of various diseases (8, 9). There is also evidence that the strength of the relationship between owner and pet is strongly associated with increased PA (10, 11). Once again, dogs are the most commonly reported animals related to the increase in PA (7, 12) possibly due to social support (13), increased motivation to exercise (14, 15), or even the sense of responsibility to take care of the pet (16). Responsibility is often highlighted as a potential strategy to increase PA levels in older adult individuals (10, 17, 18) and in general population (19). However, confounding variables such as housing conditions, pet attachment, and the number of household members can modify the frequency of walks with the pet and interfere with the magnitude of the results.

Nowadays, mental health is one of the main global concerns, with an estimated 970 million people in the world having a mental disorder (20). A mental disorder is a syndrome characterized by cognitive, emotional, or behavioral dysfunction that reflects an impairment in the psychological, biological, or developmental processes underlying mental and behavioral functioning (20). These disorders not only have an impact on an individual’s daily life but also entail substantial costs to society (21). According to the OECD, up to 13% of total health spending is directed toward mental health services (22). According to Statista Research, Portugal invested approximately 136.2 million Euros in mental health hospitals in 2019 (23). As a result, several studies have investigated the possible influence of pets on human mental health, including loneliness, depression, anxiety, stress, satisfaction with life, happiness, social support, and other factors.

Interactions with pets have positive influences on the owner, with overall positive effects on mental health, such as reducing depression and anxiety (6, 24). Moreover, owning a pet may increase social connections (25–27). The human-animal bond, strengthened by the acquisition of a pet, is associated with psychological and physical benefits in children, adults, and elders (6, 10, 25, 28, 29). These aspects have a significant overall effect on the mental health of the human population, as their continued failure or dysfunction can translate into poor mental health, possibly contributing to increased morbidity and mortality (30–32). Growing evidence indicates that pets may trigger feelings of comfort, security, and emotional support, which probably have positive effects on humans by counteracting feelings of anger, sadness, anxiety, and depression (24, 33, 34). Considering the importance of social health, evidence supports that relationships with pets confer similar support to humans (35, 36), particularly in cases of mental disorders (37). Dogs have been proposed as promoters for the initiation of shared interpersonal interactions that enhance social networks (e.g., daily walks) (38). Despite the growing literature, contradictory results have been described regarding different human dimensions, namely human health variables and quality of life (15, 39). A possible explanation may rely on the value that the family or the subject gives to the pet, which may interfere with the overall mental and physical benefits of the pet’s relationship (40). Some authors have suggested a negative influence of the pet’s non-psychological parameters (41, 42) such as lower psychological well-being (18, 30), depression (41), and anxiety (41) compared to Non-Pet Owners (NPOs). Moreover, pet owners (POs) showed a lower perception of health as well as a higher prevalence of disease than non-pet owners (NPOs) of different ages, which may contribute to a worse quality of life in specific situations such as the COVID-19 pandemic (43). Although some studies have pointed out this trend, Mueller et al. (4) highlighted that POs may have adopted the pet as a way to cope with depressive symptoms or other mental disorders they were already experiencing.

As mentioned, despite a large number of studies, some results are contradictory, possibly due to the different variables considered in each study and the different study designs. To our knowledge, no meta-analysis considering this evidence has been published. Therefore, this article aims to (a) estimate the levels of physical activity (PA) of pet owners (PO) and non-pet owners (NPO) and their relation with the quality of life and human health, and (b) quantify the effect of pets on mental health and, consequently, on the quality of life of human beings. The hypotheses tested in this meta-analysis are: H1: PO tends to show higher levels of daily PA than NPO. H2: Pets have a significant and positive influence on the mental health of PO. H3: PO shows better results regarding anxiety, loneliness, depression, stress, life satisfaction and happiness, social support, quality of life, health and well-being, general mental health and resilience, and mood and self-regulation (affections, emotions, relationships) than NPO.

2. Methods

2.1. Search strategy

Electronic database searches were conducted in PubMed, Web of Science, and Scopus for all articles published before April 2022, following the Preferred Reporting Items for Systematic Reviews and Meta-Analyses (PRISMA) guidelines (44, 45). The search terms were “Pets” OR “Pet” OR “Pet companion” OR “Pet owner” OR “Human-animal relationship” OR “Pet-interaction” OR “Dog walking” AND “Human health” OR “Quality of life” OR “Benefit” OR “Mental health” OR “Physical health” OR “Health” OR “Life satisfaction” OR “Well-being.” The search was adapted for each database as needed, and filters were used to exclude observational studies, reviews, posters, and other studies that were not eligible for meta-analysis.

2.2. Inclusion/exclusion criteria

No studies were excluded based on the type of pet selected since the focus of this study is to investigate the influence of pet ownership on human mental health, daily physical activity, and quality of life. The inclusion criteria for article selection include reporting the impact of animals on human mental health or quality of life, having a control group without any kind of pet, statistical treatment and feasible data for meta-analysis, and writing in English, Portuguese, or Spanish. Articles are excluded if they are reviewing articles, systematic reviews, meta-analyses, or conference reports, have no focus on live animals (for example, robot pets), use pets for animal-assisted therapy, or are studies conducted with working animals. Study selection parameters were not limited to the year of publication, intra-human variability, age of participants, or sample size since the goal of this meta-analysis was to conduct a comprehensive search. However, studies that were published only in abstract form or were not accessible via inter-library loan were excluded from this meta-analysis. Study eligibility was determined individually by the group members, with each study classified as include, exclude, or unclear. Articles that were classified as “include” or “unclear” by both reviewers were included for full-text review, and any discrepancies in the determination of study eligibility were resolved through mutual consensus.

2.3. Review strategy

After the articles were searched, duplicates were removed using Zotero. The article selection process involved screening the titles and abstracts for inclusion and exclusion criteria. In cases of doubt, the articles were read in their entirety to verify if the study design was suitable for the aim of this meta-analysis. Another reviewer then checked all the excluded and included articles for validity. Subsequently, the included articles were read in their entirety by two reviewers, and any inclusion/exclusion conflicts were resolved by a third and fourth reviewer. An acceptable concordance rate of 90% was predefined. The reviewers achieved a concordance rate of 93%, resolving 19 inclusion/exclusion conflicts.

The references of the included articles were manually searched to identify possible relevant articles that were not included in the initial search, to achieve a wider scope of relevant studies and reduce publication bias. This search was conducted from December 20th, 2021 to April 3rd, 2022.

2.4. Data extraction

Data from each study included in the meta-analysis were double-extracted by two authors into a table using Microsoft Excel software. Disagreement between the extractors, which consisted mainly of small additional details, was easily resolved between all authors.

Data extracted from each study included: title, author(s), journal, year of publication, study, characteristics (date of data collection, study design), participant characteristics (age, sample sizes), outcomes, intervention description, control condition description, adverse effects, adherence, dropouts, and results.

The identified studies were divided into two groups and assigned to two pairs of reviewers, who independently conducted data extractions and assessed the quality of the studies using the Downs and Black (DB) quality assessment tool (46). The DB tool consists of 27 criteria that assess study reporting (10 items), external validity (3 items), and internal validity, including design, bias, and power (14 items). The maximum score achievable is 27 points. For cross-sectional studies, the modified version of the DB tool was used, which includes 16 criteria that assess study reporting (9 items), external validity (2 items), and internal validity, including design, bias, and power (5 items). The maximum achievable score was 16 points. Any discrepancies in the DB scoring were resolved through consensus among the reviewers (Supplementary material A).

2.5. Data analysis

The Comprehensive Meta-Analysis V2.2.057 software was used for the meta-analysis. Design-specific meta-analyses were conducted for cohort and cross-sectional studies on mental health and PA. Preference was given to the use of mean and standard deviation, and if the not possible, mean difference with a 95% CI was used in all analyses. Separate analyses were performed for PA and mental health.

The statistical heterogeneity was assessed using the Cochran Q (47) test and I2 statistics (48). We used the random effects model and set the significance level at p ≤ 0.05. We evaluated the risk of publication bias by visually inspecting the funnel plot and using the Egger test (49) and Begg’s test (50). Subgroup analyses of mental health were performed by grouping the age of the sample into three categories: children and adolescents (≤18), adults (≥18 to ≤60), and older adults (≥60). Subgroup analyses also included domain-specific analyses of mental health, such as loneliness, depression, anxiety, stress, life satisfaction and happiness, social support, quality of life, health and well-being, general mental health and resilience, and humor and self-regulation (affections, emotions, relationships). Subgroup analyses of PA considered the domain-specific of physical activity (measured by minutes, frequency, counts, and mets), as well as the form of data collection and defined age groupings. Notably, if a study reported results for more than one specific type or domain of PA separately (e.g., walking and gardening), all types of PA were included in the analysis as independent variables. However, if a study also reported on the wider spectrum of physical activity measures (e.g., total PA, total LTPA), only the broader measure was used to avoid duplication.

3. Results

3.1. Search and screening

A total of 11,389 records were identified in the electronic databases. After removing duplicates and articles irrelevant to the analysis, 289 full-text publications were assessed for eligibility. Based on the inclusion and exclusion criteria, 238 articles were also excluded. A total of 49 articles met the eligibility criteria and were included in this meta-analysis. Details of the search strategy are provided in Figure 1.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Flowchart of the search strategy to include studies.

3.2. Included study characteristics

From a total of 320,971 participants, 10,233 were children and adolescents, 79,108 adults, and 2,308 were old adults.

Of the 49 included studies that evaluated pet ownership and its influence on mental health and daily PA parameters. Regarding mental health, 27 were concerned (4, 24, 25, 30, 43, 51–73). The distribution of these articles by subcategory of mental health can be observed in Table 1. Relative to physical activity 22 studies were considered for analysis (10, 12, 13, 16, 29, 74–77, 79–90). Additionally, 5 studies were included in both analyses (13, 74–77). Descriptive data of the included studies are presented in Tables 2, 3 for PA and mental health, respectively.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Distribution of articles by subcategory of mental health.

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Studies reference to physical activity.

TABLE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. Studies concerning mental health.

3.3. Pet influence on physical activity

The main analysis showed that pets had a significant and positive effect on the PA of owners compared to non-owners, with an effect of moderate and significant magnitude (Cohen’s d = 0.554; p = 0.000; Figure 2). The studies showed high heterogeneity (I2 = 99.586%; p = 0.000). Although the asymmetry in the funnel plot indicated a likelihood of publication bias, it was not confirmed by Begg’s (p = 0.06171) and Egger’s (0.21448) tests.

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Forest plot of pet influence in physical activity ownership.

Regarding the analysis of moderating variables by owner’s age group there was a significant and positive influence but of low magnitude in adults (Cohen’s d = 0.009; 95% CI [0.001–0.016]; p = 0.000), and older adults (Cohen’s d = 0.009; 95% CI [0.135–0.184]; p = 0.000). No significant values were observed in children. The results indicated a moderate and high heterogeneity (I2 = 99.932%, I2 = 99.063%, respectively, for adults and older adults).

Considering the analysis of the PA moderator variables, the frequency of performing physical activity showed a high magnitude (Cohen’s d = 1.386; 95% CI [1.297–1.476]; p = 0.000), with high heterogeneity (I2 = 99.574%). The moderator variables of PA counts (Cohen’s d = 0.423; 95% CI [0.295–0.551]; p = 0.000) and Met (Cohen’s d = 0.147; 95% CI [0.124–0.171]; p = 0.000) showed low but significant effect magnitude. Duration did not show a significant value. The heterogeneity presented in the significant variables was high (I2 = 99.917%, I2 = 72.678%, respectively for met and counts).

The analysis of the moderating variables regarding the instruments used for measuring PA revealed a small effect magnitude for the objective (Cohen’s d = 0.180; 95% [0.136–0.224)]; p = 0.000) and subjective measure (Cohen’s d = 0.018; 95% [0.010–0.025]; p = 0.000), but significant. The heterogeneity presented was high for both objective (I2 = 81.523%) and subjective (I2 = 99.923%) moderating variables.

3.4. Pet influence on mental health

In the main analyses, it was found that pets have a significant and positive effect on owners. Additionally, a significant and positive effect on owners’ mental health was reported, albeit of low magnitude (p = 0.021; Cohen’s d = 0.068; Figure 3). The studies showed high heterogeneity (I2 = 95.987%; p = 0.000). However, the symmetric funnel plot analysis revealed a low risk of publication bias, as evidenced by Begg’s (p = 0.11060) and Egger’s tests (p = 0.34245) (Supplementary material B).

FIGURE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 3. Forest plot of pet influence on mental health.

Owner’s age group as a moderating variable has a significant and positive influence but of low magnitude in children (Cohen’s d = 0.030; 95% CI [0.009–0.05; p = 0.005]), adults (Cohen’s d = 0.037; 95% CI [0.0024–0.05]; p = 0.000), and older adults (Cohen’s d = 0.091; 95% CI [0.061–0.121]; p = 0.000). The results indicated a moderate and high heterogeneity of moderating variables by owner’s age group (I2 = 98.397%, I2 = 77.605%, I2 = 60.934%, respectively for children, adults, and older adults).

Considering the analysis of moderating variables by mental health, all showed a low effect magnitude but with a significant and positive influence on the humor and self-regulation (affections, emotions, relationships; Cohen’s d = 0.241; 95% CI [0.203–0.280]; p = 0.000), social support (Cohen’s d = 0.100; 95% CI [0.064–0.137]; p = 0.000) life satisfaction and happiness (Cohen’s d = 0.063; 95% CI [0.044–0.081]; p = 0.000). Loneliness had a significant influence, but a negative effect (Cohen’s d = −0.036; 95% CI [−0.088–0.017]; p = 0.005), anxiety, loneliness depression, stress, life satisfaction and happiness, social support, quality of life, health and well-being, general mental health and resilience showed low magnitude and non-significant values. Despite exploration among the moderating variables, heterogeneity remained moderate to high (I2 = 0.000%, I2 = 82.205%, I2 = 65.479%, I2 = 66.963%, I2 = 83.883%, I2 = 67.735%, I2 = 740,739%, I2 = 76.147%, I2 = 97.371%, respectively for anxiety, loneliness depression, stress, life satisfaction and happiness, social support, quality of life, health and well-being, general mental health and resilience, humor and self-regulation (affections, emotions, relationships).

4. Discussion

To our knowledge, this is the first comprehensive meta-analysis to investigate the influence of pet ownership on owners’ daily PA levels and mental health. The main findings indicate a moderate positive impact of pets on PA compared to non-pet owners (NPO).

Among the PA moderating variables, frequency showed a highly significant effect, indicating that pet owners have a higher frequency of physical activity than NPO. However, no significant impact of pet ownership on mental health compared to NPO was found. One of the most promising results of this meta-analysis is the evidence that pet owners are more physically active than NPO, which may be related to pet care such as walking and going outside. A previous study (19) found that dog owners walk moderately more than non-dog owners. Of all the analyzed studies, five were conducted in dogs and other different species (13, 29, 77, 87, 90). The analysis of these studies showed that the benefits related to PA were more evident in dog owners than in owners of other pets. These findings led us to analyze PA specifically regarding dog owners DO and NDO.

About the moderating variables of PA (frequency, duration, counts, and mets), PO significantly had a higher frequency of walking. However, duration was not significant, as found in a previous meta-analysis (19). Owners with a stronger attachments to their dogs were more likely to walk with them, but at a shorter distance than owners with weaker pet attachments (17). These results may be based on the owner’s commitment to meet the pet’s needs, which may lead to an increase in the owner’s willingness and frequency to take a walk, even if it is not perceived, as opposed to non-owners. On the other hand, it is possible that dog characteristics, such as age and breed, could influence the relationship between physical activity and mental health outcomes (92). For example, younger dogs may require more physical activity than older dogs, which could demand more attention and owners’ general activity. Moreover, it is known that some breeds have higher exercise requirements than others (92). Future studies could take into account factors such as age, breed, and dogs’ physical activity needs, and how they influence health outcomes of pet owners.

Objective and subjective measurement methods revealed a significant, but low effect size. For the assessment of owners’ PA, most previous studies have used subjective physical activity measures, with only eight studies using objective assessment measures (10, 74, 76, 77, 80, 81, 84, 89). One of the limitations of subjective measures is that they are based on the perception or recall of PA performed before the date of completing the questionnaire, which may generate bias in the results or overestimation compared to objective measures (93). Comparative studies comparing the two measurement methods have found that the results obtained through the IPAQ are not reflected in the PA measurements with the accelerometer (77, 79). Despite our results covering both methodologies, subjective measures were the most commonly reported method among studies, similar to the meta-analysis conducted in 2013 (19). Although studies conducted with objective measures have been increasing, more studies that examine the application of these methods are needed, particularly studies that quantify both pet and owner levels of PA. Moreover, such an analysis would allow us to establish a more reliable role of pets in owners’ PA and possibly define guidelines for the population that can benefit the most from owning a pet.

Besides the relationship between pet ownership and physical health, there may also be a relationship with mental health. There is some evidence that suggest that physical activity and pet ownership can both have positive effects on mental health outcomes (11). Engaging in regular physical activity has been shown to be associated with improved mental health, including decreased symptoms of depression and anxiety, as well as increased feelings of well-being and self-esteem (32). Thus, it is possible that physically active pet owners may have better mental health outcomes compared to non-pet owners, as they may benefit from both the physical activity and the social support and companionship provided by their pets (28, 94). However, more research is needed to fully understand the complex relationship between physical activity, pet ownership, and mental health outcomes.

Regarding the relationship between mental health and pet ownership, this meta-analysis included 135 results from 32 studies, whose methodological quality ranged from good to excellent. Despite the significant influence of pets on owners’ mental health, it must be considered that the effect size was low. The high heterogeneity of the sample characteristics, the variables used to measure mental health, and other methodological issues might explain this low effect size. Nevertheless, this result has already been described in previous systematic reviews (91, 95).

To better understand the heterogeneity found, various moderating variables related to mental health were considered (95). Among these moderating variables, only loneliness, social support, life satisfaction, happiness, and mood and self-regulation were significantly related to pet ownership.

Regarding loneliness and social support, this meta-analysis suggests that PO are more likely to experience loneliness than NPO, but owning a pet can confer greater social support to the owner. Despite the differences between these concepts, they complement each other. Loneliness refers to the discrepancy between actual and desired social relationships. Social isolation (included in the social support moderating variable of this meta-analysis) arises in the absence of such contact with society (96). Kretzler (3) suggests that a pet tends to be associated with an increased frequency of social interactions, allowing for the increase of social and community ties (57), which may favor increased feelings of belonging and decreased loneliness and social isolation (70). Among the activities shared between pets and owners, walking and visits to parks appear to be most strongly associated with the social support felt by owners (13). Dogs are the most frequently reported pets in these activities (13, 57, 75) and seem to produce better results compared to other pets (65, 96). However, these differences between species are mostly dependent on the type of activities performed with the pet. Therefore, it may be important to consider other types of daily life activities with pets in future studies, as they may also promote greater social support for owners and decrease feelings of loneliness.

The literature suggests that pets may play a beneficial role in providing social support and companionship, particularly for older adult individuals who live alone (24, 51). However, while pets may serve as a form of social support, they cannot completely alleviate feelings of loneliness. Despite these findings, the low effect size of the relationship between pet ownership and mental health can be attributed to the high heterogeneity of the samples. Studies with individuals with chronic low back pain (75), members of the LGBT community (64), and those experiencing the COVID-19 pandemic (43, 78) have further demonstrated the high variability of samples. Certain contexts may even contribute to increased inconsistencies and incoherence in the role of pets in social isolation and feelings of loneliness. For instance, during the COVID-19 pandemic, social isolation was not significantly associated with pet ownership, but loneliness may have been reduced (3). Further research is needed to fully understand the impact of pets on social support and levels of loneliness.

This meta-analysis also indicates that pets may promote greater life satisfaction and happiness in their owners. The concept of life satisfaction is subjective and may depend on individuals’ experiences. Curl (57) reported that pet owners experience greater social engagement and life satisfaction, especially in the older adult population. Additionally, pet owners who experienced the death of a pet during the previous year were significantly less happy and satisfied compared to those who did not have a pet and those who did not experience the death of a pet (54). On the other hand, it is also plausible to consider that owners’ personalities, conditions of the pets’ presence, as well as expenses associated with the pet’s care, might influence owners’ life satisfaction. Therefore, it is important to understand the relationship between pet ownership and life satisfaction and happiness, considering different variables such as life satisfaction before and after the adoption of the pet.

This meta-analysis also shows that the presence of a pet may lead to better mood, coping skills, affection, and relationships, particularly regarding humor and emotions. Moreover, having a pet throughout life was predictive of more positive relationships (56). However, high heterogeneity was found, which could be explained by the attachment to the pet, as the human-animal bond may differ among members of the same family, influencing their responses to the same questionnaire. Most analyzed studies did not control for this variable, which makes it difficult to understand the possible influence of the human-animal bond. Therefore, in future studies, it will be important to consider this variable.

Concerning the remaining moderator variables, no significant effect of pets was found, and the magnitude of the effect was low. These results may be due, in part, to the diversity of instruments and methodological procedures used in the included studies. In fact, in the different studies analyzed the variable mental health and resilience were quantified by using different scales, which most frequently was the Short Form Health Survey with 36 items (43, 59, 61), with Whight (73) using a reduced version with 12 items, the Moca (53), Patient Health Questionnaire (66), Psychological Symptoms (67), and BSI- Brief Symptom Inventor (70) were also used, along with 3 other studies (43, 59, 61) that assessed resilience. This fact may cause bias and variability, as it depends on the reading and interpretation of the self-administered questionnaire. Therefore, the high variety of instruments used to measure the same or different mental health variables was probably the main reason for the high heterogeneity observed. Similar results were observed for the depression and anxiety variables.

Therefore, 12 studies were included in this meta-analysis that explored the effect of pet presence on depressive symptoms (13, 24, 43, 52, 53, 59, 60, 70, 71, 74–76), and 8 studies on anxiety (24, 43, 52, 59, 70–72, 76). However, no significant effect was observed, which is consistent with other reported literature (91, 95). Symptoms of anxiety and depression are frequently analyzed together since they are highly comorbid and share common etiological processes (97). In this meta-analysis, only one study (72) analyzed anxiety independently of depression. In the remaining studies, the authors used the same instrument to analyze both variables: the Depression, Anxiety, and Stress scale (24, 43, 52, 59, 70) and the Hospital Anxiety and Depression Scale (76).

Regarding the influence of pets on the owner’s quality of life, health, and well-being, no significant results were found, contradicting a previous systematic review that reported the potential benefits of pets to impact owner well-being (28). Once again, different scales and procedures were used in the reviewed studies. Quality of life was measured in 17 studies using subjective health items (57, 96), the European Health Interview Surveys-Quality of Life questionnaire (43), the Warwick-Edinburgh Mental Well-Being Scale (59), the Kidscreen-10 index (67), items from the SF-36 scale (vitality, pain, and role physical) (30, 76, 77), and items from the shortened version SF-12 (physical health) (66, 73). The physical health scales were used in studies on physical illnesses (52, 65, 75, 96). The World Health Organization Quality of Life Instrument (70) and the Well-being Index (58) was also used to measure well-being.

Quality of life, health, and well-being questionnaires may be subject to bias due to the subjectivity of interpretation inherent in the different dimensions evaluated. Moreover, the confounding factors considered by the authors, depending on the study goal, are different in each study, which could also be a limitation of our analysis.

Overall, although this meta-analysis did not aim to understand the effect of different pet species on mental health, it is worth mentioning that they seem to have an influence. Pruchno (65) found a higher positive association between quality-of-life outcomes and dog ownership than cat ownership, while Hajek (96) found a similar association regarding social isolation and loneliness. In another study with Portuguese adolescents, pets were associated with a better perception of well-being, more life satisfaction, and overall mental health. However, when analyzed by species, dogs showed more evident results (67). This may also contribute to explaining the high heterogeneity of the obtained results.

5. Limitations

This meta-analysis has identified several limitations and methodological issues that limit the generalizability of the results. These flaws include the absence of randomized controlled trials and a small number of longitudinal studies. Additionally, there is a lack of studies that compare health-related variables before and after pet ownership. It is also important to differentiate between participants, distinguish between the main and secondary responsible owners, and to sure attachment to the pet. Furthermore, it would be valuable to include other moderating variables such as age group, gender, economic factors, social status, ethnicity, and pet species to reduce the heterogeneity of the analysis. Finally, the use of diverse instruments to assess mental health and physical activity increases the heterogeneity of the results.

6. Conclusion

In general, pet ownership has been found to have a positive influence on owners’ physical activity, with pet owners showing a higher frequency of physical activity than non-owners. However, pets do not seem to have a significant impact on owners’ mental health. There were some moderating variables related to mental health, such as loneliness, social support, life satisfaction, happiness, mood, and self-regulation, which were significantly associated with pet ownership but with low effect sizes. This suggests that pet owners may have higher levels of social support, life satisfaction, happiness, mood, and self-regulation and lower levels of loneliness than non-owners.

The results of this meta-analysis provide a nuanced understanding of the potential impact of pets on owners’ mental health and physical activity from a one health perspective.

We suggest that future researchers explore theoretical frameworks and methodological approaches that can explain the uniqueness of the relationships between pets and people, and how these influence them.

Author contributions

CM, JS, MM, MP, and LC: the conception of the research, the design of the research protocol, and review of the final draft of the manuscript. CM, JS, and MM: literature review and manuscript drafting. CM and MM: publication search. CM, JS, MM, and AC: publication screening and data extraction. LC and LS: third and fourth reviews. AC: statistical analysis. CM, AC, JS, and LS: data analysis and interpretation of results. All authors contributed to the article and approved the submitted version.

Funding

This work was funded by the R&D&I project “oneHcancer – One health approach in animal cancer,” operation no.: NORTE-01-0145-FEDER-000078, co-funded by the European Regional Development Fund (ERDF) through NORTE 2020 (North Portugal Regional Operational Program 2014/2020).

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher’s note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

Supplementary material

The Supplementary material for this article can be found online at: https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fpubh.2023.1196199/full#supplementary-material

References

1. Day, MJ. Human-animal health interactions: the role of one health. Am Fam Physician. (2016) 93:345–6.

Google Scholar

2. Fine, AH. Handbook on animal-assisted therapy: Foundations and guidelines for animal-assisted interventions. Fourth ed. Amsterdam; Boston: Elsevier Academic Press (2015). 427 p.

Google Scholar

3. Kretzler, B, König, HH, and Hajek, A. Pet ownership, loneliness, and social isolation: a systematic review. Soc Psychiatry Psychiatr Epidemiol. (2022) 57:1935–57. doi: 10.1007/s00127-022-02332-9

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

4. Mueller, MK, Gee, NR, and Bures, RM. Human-animal interaction as a social determinant of health: descriptive findings from the health and retirement study. BMC Public Health. (2018) 18:305. doi: 10.1186/s12889-018-5188-0

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

5. Powell, L, Chia, D, McGreevy, P, Podberscek, AL, Edwards, KM, Neilly, B, et al. Expectations for dog ownership: perceived physical, mental and psychosocial health consequences among prospective adopters. PLoS One. (2018) 13:e0200276. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0200276

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

6. Purewal, R, Christley, R, Kordas, K, Joinson, C, Meints, K, Gee, N, et al. Companion animals and child/adolescent development: a systematic review of the evidence. Int J Environ Res Public Health. (2017) 14:234. doi: 10.3390/ijerph14030234

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

7. Westgarth, C, Christley, RM, and Christian, HE. How might we increase physical activity through dog walking? A comprehensive review of dog walking correlates. Int J Behav Nutr Phys Act. (2014) 11:83. doi: 10.1186/1479-5868-11-83

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

8. Gualdi-Russo, E, and Zaccagni, L. Physical activity for health and wellness. Int J Environ Res Public Health. (2021) 18:7823. doi: 10.3390/ijerph18157823

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

9. Pedersen, BK, and Saltin, B. Evidence for prescribing exercise as therapy in chronic disease. Scand J Med Sci Sports. (2006) 16:3–63. doi: 10.1111/j.1600-0838.2006.00520.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

10. Dall, PM, Ellis, SLH, Ellis, BM, Grant, PM, Colyer, A, Gee, NR, et al. The influence of dog ownership on objective measures of free-living physical activity and sedentary behaviour in community-dwelling older adults: a longitudinal case-controlled study. BMC Public Health. (2017) 17:496. doi: 10.1186/s12889-017-4422-5

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

11. WHO. Guidelines on physical activity and sedentary behaviour. WHO. Geneva: Genebra: World Health Organization (2020).

Google Scholar

12. Richards, EA. Does dog walking predict physical activity participation: results from a national survey. Am J Health Promot. (2016) 30:323–30. doi: 10.1177/0890117116646335

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

13. Taniguchi, Y, Seino, S, Nishi, M, Tomine, Y, Tanaka, I, Yokoyama, Y, et al. Physical, social, and psychological characteristics of community-dwelling elderly Japanese dog and cat owners. PLoS One. (2018) 13:e0206399. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0206399

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

14. Cutt, H, Giles-Corti, B, Knuiman, M, and Burke, V. Dog ownership, health and physical activity: a critical review of the literature. Health Place. (2007) 13:261–72. doi: 10.1016/j.healthplace.2006.01.003

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

15. Matchock, RL. Pet ownership and physical health. Curr Opin Psychiatry. (2015) 28:386–92. doi: 10.1097/YCO.0000000000000183

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

16. Brown, SG, and Rhodes, RE. Relationships among dog ownership and leisure-time walking in western Canadian adults. Am J Prev Med. (2006) 30:131–6. doi: 10.1016/j.amepre.2005.10.007

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

17. Curl, A, Bibbo, J, and Johnson, R. Dog walking, the human-animal bond and older adults’ physical health. Gerontologist. (2017) 57:930–9. doi: 10.1093/geront/gnw051

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

18. Toohey, AM, Hewson, JA, Adams, CL, and Rock, MJ. Pets, social participation, and aging-in-place: findings from the Canadian longitudinal study on aging. Can J Aging. (2018) 37:200–17. doi: 10.1017/S0714980818000107

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

19. Christian, HE, Westgarth, C, Bauman, A, Richards, EA, Rhodes, RE, Evenson, KR, et al. Dog ownership and physical activity: a review of the evidence. J Phys Act Health. (2013) 10:750–9. doi: 10.1123/jpah.10.5.750

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

20. WHO (2022). World mental health report: Transforming mental health for all [Internet]. Report No.: ISBN: 9789240049338. Available from: https://www.who.int/publications-detail-redirect/9789240049338 [Accessed December 12, 2022].

Google Scholar

21. Knapp, M, and Wong, G. Economics and mental health: the current scenario. World Psychiatry. (2020) 19:3–14. doi: 10.1002/wps.20692

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

22. OECD (2018). Health at a glance: Europe 2018: state of health in the EU cycle [internet]. OECD. (Health at a Glance: Europe). Available at: https://www.oecd-ilibrary.org/social-issues-migration-health/health-at-a-glance-europe-2018_health_glance_eur-2018-en [Accessed November 14, 2021].

Google Scholar

23. Statista Research Department (2022). Health care expenditure for mental health hospitals in Portugal from 2010 to 2019 [internet]. Statista. Available at: https://www.statista.com/statistics/1298445/portugal-health-care-expenditure-mental-health-hospital/ [Accessed December 19, 2022].

Google Scholar

24. Bennett, P, Trigg, J, Godber, T, and Brown, C. An experience sampling approach to investigating associations between pet presence and indicators of psychological wellbeing and mood in older Australians. Anthrozoös. (2015) 28:403–20. doi: 10.1080/08927936.2015.1052266

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

25. Black, K. The relationship between companion animals and loneliness among rural adolescents. J Pediatr Nurs. (2012) 27:103–12. doi: 10.1016/j.pedn.2010.11.009

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

26. Hawkins, RD, Hawkins, EL, and Tip, L. “I can’t give up when i have them to care for”: People’s experiences of pets and their mental health. Anthrozoös. (2021) 34:543–62. doi: 10.1080/08927936.2021.1914434

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

27. Turner, DC, Rieger, G, and Gygax, L. Spouses and cats and their effects on human mood. Anthrozoös. (2003) 16:213–28. doi: 10.2752/089279303786992143

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

28. Gee, NR, and Mueller, MK. A systematic review of research on pet ownership and animal interactions among older adults. Anthrozoös. (2019) 32:183–207. doi: 10.1080/08927936.2019.1569903

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

29. Thorpe, R, Kreisle, R, Glickman, L, Simonsick, E, Newman, A, and Kritchevsky, S. Physical activity and pet ownership in year 3 of the health ABC study. J Aging Phys Act. (2006) 14:154–68. doi: 10.1123/japa.14.2.154

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

30. Amiot, CE, Gagné, C, and Bastian, B. Pet ownership and psychological well-being during the COVID-19 pandemic. Sci Rep. (2022) 12:6091. doi: 10.1038/s41598-022-10019-z

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

31. Hughes, AM, Braun, L, Putnam, A, Martinez, D, and Fine, A. Advancing human–animal interaction to counter social isolation and loneliness in the time of COVID-19: a model for an interdisciplinary public health consortium. Animals. (2021) 11:2325. doi: 10.3390/ani11082325

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

32. Merkouri, A, Graham, TM, O’Haire, ME, Purewal, R, and Westgarth, C. Dogs and the good life: a cross-sectional study of the association between the dog–owner relationship and owner mental wellbeing. Front Psychol. (2022) 13:903647. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2022.903647

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

33. Hui Gan, GZ, Hill, AM, Yeung, P, Keesing, S, and Netto, JA. Pet ownership and its influence on mental health in older adults. Aging Ment Health. (2020) 24:1605–12. doi: 10.1080/13607863.2019.1633620

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

34. Kogan, LR, Currin-McCulloch, J, Bussolari, C, Packman, W, and Erdman, P. The psychosocial influence of companion animals on positive and negative affect during the COVID-19 pandemic. Animals. (2021) 11:2084. doi: 10.3390/ani11072084

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

35. Brooks, H, Rushton, K, Walker, S, Lovell, K, and Rogers, A. Ontological security and connectivity provided by pets: a study in the self-management of the everyday lives of people diagnosed with a long-term mental health condition. BMC Psychiatry. (2016) 16:409. doi: 10.1186/s12888-016-1111-3

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

36. Headey, B, and Grabka, MM. Pets and human health in Germany and Australia: national longitudinal results. Soc Indic Res. (2007) 80:297–311. doi: 10.1007/s11205-005-5072-z

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

37. Zimolag, U, and Krupa, T. Pet ownership as a meaningful community occupation for people with serious mental illness. Am J Occup Ther. (2009) 63:126–37. doi: 10.5014/ajot.63.2.126

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

38. Bradshaw, J. The animals among us: The new science of anthrozoology. First ed. London: Allen Lane (2017).

Google Scholar

39. Herzog, H. The impact of pets on human health and psychological well-being: fact, fiction, or hypothesis? Curr Dir Psychol Sci. (2011) 20:236–9. doi: 10.1177/0963721411415220

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

40. Crawford, EK, Worsham, NL, and Swinehart, ER. Benefits derived from companion animals, and the use of the term ‘attachment’. Anthrozoös. (2006) 19:98–112. doi: 10.2752/089279306785593757

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

41. Fraser, G, Huang, Y, Robinson, K, Wilson, MS, Bulbulia, J, and Sibley, CG. New Zealand pet owners’ demographic characteristics, personality, and health and wellbeing: more than just a fluff piece. Anthrozoös. (2020) 33:561–78. doi: 10.1080/08927936.2020.1771060

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

42. Maharaj, N, and Haney, CJ. A qualitative investigation of the significance of companion dogs. West J Nurs Res. (2015) 37:1175–93. doi: 10.1177/0193945914545176

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

43. Phillipou, A, Tan, EJ, Toh, WL, van Rheenen, T, Meyer, D, Neill, E, et al. Pet ownership and mental health during COVID-19 lockdown. Aust Vet J. (2021) 99:423–6. doi: 10.1111/avj.13102

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

44. Hutton, B, Salanti, G, Caldwell, DM, Chaimani, A, Schmid, CH, Cameron, C, et al. The PRISMA extension statement for reporting of systematic reviews incorporating network meta-analyses of health care interventions: checklist and explanations. Ann Intern Med. (2015) 162:777–84. doi: 10.7326/M14-2385

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

45. Page, MJ, McKenzie, JE, Bossuyt, PM, Boutron, I, Hoffmann, TC, Mulrow, CD, et al. The PRISMA 2020 statement: an updated guideline for reporting systematic reviews. BMJ. (2021) 372:n71. doi: 10.1136/bmj.n71

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

46. Downs, SH, and Black, N. The feasibility of creating a checklist for the assessment of the methodological quality both of randomised and non-randomised studies of health care interventions. J Epidemiol Community Health. (1998) 52:377–84. doi: 10.1136/jech.52.6.377

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

47. Cohen, J. The statistical power of abnormal-social psychological research: a review. J Abnorm Soc Psychol. (1962) 65:145–53. doi: 10.1037/h0045186

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

48. Higgins, JPT, Thompson, SG, Deeks, JJ, and Altman, DG. Measuring inconsistency in meta-analyses. BMJ. (2003) 327:557–60. doi: 10.1136/bmj.327.7414.557

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

49. Egger, M, Smith, GD, Schneider, M, and Minder, C. Bias in meta-analysis detected by a simple, graphical test. BMJ. (1997) 315:629–34. doi: 10.1136/bmj.315.7109.629

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

50. Begg, CB, and Mazumdar, M. Operating characteristics of a rank correlation test for publication bias. Biometrics. (1994) 50:1088–101. doi: 10.2307/2533446

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

51. Antonacopoulos, N. A longitudinal study of the relation between acquiring a dog and loneliness. Soc Anim. (2017) 25:319–40. doi: 10.1163/15685306-12341449

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

52. Bradley, L, and Bennett, P. Companion-animals’ effectiveness in managing chronic pain in adult community members. Anthrozoös. (2015) 28:635–47. doi: 10.1080/08927936.2015.1070006

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

53. Branson, SM, Boss, L, Padhye, NS, Gee, NR, and Trötscher, TT. Biopsychosocial factors and cognitive function in cat ownership and attachment in community-dwelling older adults. Anthrozoös. (2019) 32:267–82. doi: 10.1080/08927936.2019.1569908

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

54. Brkljačić, T, Sučić, I, Lučić, L, Glavak Tkalić, R, and Kaliterna Lipovčan, L. The beginning, the end, and all the happiness in between: pet owners’ wellbeing from pet acquisition to death. Anthrozoös. (2020) 33:71–87. doi: 10.1080/08927936.2020.1694313

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

55. Canady, B, and Sansone, A. Health care decisions and delay of treatment in companion animal owners. J Clin Psychol Med Settings. (2019) 26:313–20. doi: 10.1007/s10880-018-9593-4

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

56. Cloutier, A, and Peetz, J. Relationships’ best friend: links between pet ownership, empathy, and romantic relationship outcomes. Anthrozoös. (2016) 29:395–408. doi: 10.1080/08927936.2016.1181361

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

57. Curl, A, Bibbo, J, and Johnson, R. Neighborhood engagement, dogs, and life satisfaction in older adulthood. J Appl Gerontol. (2021) 40:1706–14. doi: 10.1177/0733464820953725

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

58. Endo, K, Yamasaki, S, Ando, S, Kikusui, T, Mogi, K, Nagasawa, M, et al. Dog and cat ownership predicts adolescents’ mental well-being: a population-based longitudinal study. Int J Environ Res Public Health. (2020) 17:884. doi: 10.3390/ijerph17030884

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

59. Grajfoner, D, Ke, GN, and Wong, RMM. (2021). The effect of pets on human mental health and wellbeing during COVID-19 lockdown in Malaysia. Animals (Basel). 11:2689. doi: 10.3390/ani11092689

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

60. Hajek, A, and König, HH. How do cat owners, dog owners and individuals without pets differ in terms of psychosocial outcomes among individuals in old age without a partner? Aging Ment Health. (2020) 24:1613–9. doi: 10.1080/13607863.2019.1647137

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

61. Hill, L, Winefield, H, and Bennett, P. Are stronger bonds better? Examining the relationship between the human–animal bond and human social support, and its impact on resilience. Aust Psychol. (2020) 55:729–38. doi: 10.1111/ap.12466

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

62. Kim, J, and Chun, BC. Association between companion animal ownership and overall life satisfaction in Seoul, Korea. PLoS One. (2021) 16:e0258034. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0258034

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

63. Muldoon, J, Williams, J, Lawrence, A, and Currie, C. The nature and psychological impact of child/adolescent attachment to dogs compared with other companion animals. Soc Anim. (2019) 27:55–74. doi: 10.1163/15685306-12341579

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

64. Muraco, A, Putney, J, Shiu, C, and Fredriksen-Goldsen, K. Lifesaving in every way: the role of companion animals in the lives of older lesbian, gay, bisexual, and transgender adults age 50 and over. Res Aging. (2018) 40:859–82. doi: 10.1177/0164027517752149

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

65. Pruchno, R, Heid, AR, and Wilson-Genderson, M. Successful aging, social support, and ownership of a companion animal. Anthrozoös. (2018) 31:23–39. doi: 10.1080/08927936.2018.1406199

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

66. González Ramírez, MT, and Landero Hernández, R. Benefits of dog ownership: comparative study of equivalent samples. J Vet Behav. (2014) 9:311–5. doi: 10.1016/j.jveb.2014.08.002

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

67. Reis, M, Ramiro, L, Camacho, I, Tomé, G, Brito, C, and Gaspar de Matos, M. Does having a pet make a difference? Highlights from the HBSC Portuguese study. Eur J Dev Psychol. (2018) 15:548–64. doi: 10.1080/17405629.2017.1317242

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

68. le Roux, MC, and Wright, S. The relationship between pet attachment, life satisfaction, and perceived stress: results from a south African online survey. Anthrozoös. (2020) 33:371–85. doi: 10.1080/08927936.2020.1746525

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

69. Serpell, J. Beneficial effects of pet ownership on some aspects of human health and behaviour. J R Soc Med. (1991) 84:717–20. doi: 10.1177/014107689108401208

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

70. Teo, JT, and Thomas, SJ. Psychological mechanisms predicting wellbeing in pet owners: Rogers’ core conditions versus bowlby’s attachment. Anthrozoös. (2019) 32:399–417. doi: 10.1080/08927936.2019.1598660

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

71. Watson, N, and Weinstein, M. Pet ownership in relation to depression, anxiety, and anger in working women. Anthrozoös. (1993) 6:135–8. doi: 10.2752/089279393787002295

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

72. Wright, H, Hall, S, Hames, A, Hardiman, J, Mills, R, PAWS Project Team,, et al. Pet dogs improve family functioning and reduce anxiety in children with autism spectrum disorder. Anthrozoös. (2015) 28:611–24. doi: 10.1080/08927936.2015.1070003

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

73. Wright, MM, Schreiner, P, Rosser, BRS, Polter, EJ, Mitteldorf, D, West, W, et al. The influence of companion animals on quality of life of gay and bisexual men diagnosed with prostate cancer. Int J Environ Res Public Health. (2019) 16:4457. doi: 10.3390/ijerph16224457

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

74. Ballin, M, Antonsson, O, Rosenqvist, V, Nordström, P, and Nordström, A. Association of dog ownership with accelerometer-measured physical activity and daily steps in 70-year-old individuals: a population-based cross-sectional study. BMC Public Health. (2021) 21:2313. doi: 10.1186/s12889-021-12401-4

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

75. Carr, ECJ, Wallace, JE, Pater, R, and Gross, DP. Evaluating the relationship between well-being and living with a dog for people with chronic low back pain: a feasibility study. Int J Environ Res Public Health. (2019) 16:1472. doi: 10.3390/ijerph16081472

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

76. Feng, Z, Dibben, C, Witham, M, Donnan, P, Vadiveloo, T, Sniehotta, F, et al. Dog ownership and physical activity in later life: a cross-sectional observational study. Prev Med. (2014) 66:101–6. doi: 10.1016/j.ypmed.2014.06.004

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

77. Mičková, E, Machová, K, Daďová, K, and Svobodová, I. Does dog ownership affect physical activity, sleep, and self-reported health in older adults? Int J Environ Res Public Health. (2019) 16:3355. doi: 10.3390/ijerph16183355

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

78. Mueller, MK, Richer, AM, Callina, KS, and Charmaraman, L. Companion animal relationships and adolescent loneliness during covid-19. Animals. (2021) 11:1–14. doi: 10.3390/ani11030885

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

79. Brown, BB, and Jensen, WA. Dog ownership and walking: perceived and audited walkability and activity correlates. Int J Environ Res Public Health. (2020) 17:1385. doi: 10.3390/ijerph17041385

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

80. Byers, C, Wilson, C, Stephens, M, Goodie, J, Netting, F, and Olsen, C. Owners and pets exercising together: canine response to veterinarian-prescribed physical activity. Anthrozoös. (2014) 27:325–33. doi: 10.2752/175303714X14036956449224

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

81. Coleman, KJ, Rosenberg, DE, Conway, TL, Sallis, JF, Saelens, BE, Frank, LD, et al. Physical activity, weight status, and neighborhood characteristics of dog walkers. Prev Med. (2008) 47:309–12. doi: 10.1016/j.ypmed.2008.05.007

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

82. Corrigan, VK, Pierce, BJ, and Hosig, K. Dog ownership, physical activity, and health-related quality of life in veterinary students: a cross-sectional study. J Vet Med Educ (2018) 45:51–63. doi: 10.3138/jvme.0616-106r1 1943-7218.

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

83. Garcia, DO, Wertheim, BC, Manson, JE, Chlebowski, RT, Volpe, SL, Howard, BV, et al. Relationships between dog ownership and physical activity in postmenopausal women. Prev Med. (2015) 70:33–8. doi: 10.1016/j.ypmed.2014.10.030

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

84. Koohsari, MJ, Shibata, A, Ishii, K, Kurosawa, S, Yasunaga, A, Hanibuchi, T, et al. Dog ownership and adults’ objectively-assessed sedentary behaviour and physical activity. Sci Rep. (2020) 10:17487. doi: 10.1038/s41598-020-74365-6

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

85. Lail, P, McCormack, GR, and Rock, M. Does dog-ownership influence seasonal patterns of neighbourhood-based walking among adults? A longitudinal study. BMC Public Health. (2011) 11:148. doi: 10.1186/1471-2458-11-148

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

86. Machová, K, Daďová, K, Chaloupková, H, and Svobodová, I. Does having a pet influence the physical activity of their young female owners? BMC Public Health. (2019) 19:1672. doi: 10.1186/s12889-019-7962-z

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

87. Oka, K, and Shibata, A. Dog ownership and health-related physical activity among Japanese adults. J Phys Act Health. (2009) 6:412–418, doi: 10.1123/jpah.6.4.412 1543-5474.

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

88. Schofield, G, Mummery, K, and Steele, R. Dog ownership and human health-related physical activity: an epidemiological study. Health Promot J Austr. (2005) 16:15–9. doi: 10.1071/HE05015

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

89. Westgarth, C, Christley, RM, Jewell, C, German, AJ, Boddy, LM, and Christian, HE. Dog owners are more likely to meet physical activity guidelines than people without a dog: an investigation of the association between dog ownership and physical activity levels in a UK community. Sci Rep. (2019) 9:5704. doi: 10.1038/s41598-019-41254-6

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

90. Yabroff, K, Troiano, R, and Berrigan, D. Walking the dog: is pet ownership associated with physical activity in California? J Phys Act Health. (2008) 5:216–28. doi: 10.1123/jpah.5.2.216

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

91. Hughes, MJ, Verreynne, ML, Harpur, P, and Pachana, NA. Companion animals and health in older populations: a systematic review. Clin Gerontol. (2020) 43:365–77. doi: 10.1080/07317115.2019.1650863

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

92. Hielscher-Zdzieblik, B, Froboese, I, Serpell, J, and Gansloßer, U. Impact of dog’s age and breed on dog owner’s physical activity: a German longitudinal study. Animals. (2022) 12:1314. doi: 10.3390/ani12101314

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

93. Lee, PH, Macfarlane, DJ, Lam, T, and Stewart, SM. Validity of the international physical activity questionnaire short form (IPAQ-SF): a systematic review. Int J Behav Nutr Phys Act. (2011) 8:115. doi: 10.1186/1479-5868-8-115

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

94. Goh, YX, Tan, JSQ, Syn, NL, Tan, BSW, Low, JY, Foo, YH, et al. Association between pet ownership and physical activity levels, atopic conditions, and mental health in Singapore: a propensity score-matched analysis. Sci Rep. (2020) 10:19898. doi: 10.1038/s41598-020-76739-2

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

95. Scoresby, KJ, Strand, EB, Ng, Z, Brown, KC, Stilz, CR, Strobel, K, et al. Pet ownership and quality of life: a systematic review of the literature. Vet Sci. (2021) 8:332. doi: 10.3390/vetsci8120332

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

96. Hajek, A, Kretzler, B, and König, HH. Multimorbidity, loneliness, and social isolation: a systematic review. Int J Environ Res Public Health. (2020) 17:8688. doi: 10.3390/ijerph17228688

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

97. Garber, J, Brunwasser, SM, Zerr, AA, Schwartz, KTG, Sova, K, and Weersing, VR. Treatment and prevention of depression and anxiety in youth: test of crossover effects. Depress Anxiety. (2016) 33:939–59. doi: 10.1002/da.22519

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: active lifestyle, health, pet ownership, physical activity, quality of life

Citation: Martins CF, Soares JP, Cortinhas A, Silva L, Cardoso L, Pires MA and Mota MP (2023) Pet’s influence on humans’ daily physical activity and mental health: a meta-analysis. Front. Public Health 11:1196199. doi: 10.3389/fpubh.2023.1196199

Received: 29 March 2023; Accepted: 28 April 2023;
Published: 30 May 2023.

Edited by:

Yasuhiro Mochizuki, Waseda University, Japan

Reviewed by:

Susanna Toivanen, Mälardalen University, Sweden
Marija Jevtic, University of Novi Sad, Serbia

Copyright © 2023 Martins, Soares, Cortinhas, Silva, Cardoso, Pires and Mota. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Catarina F. Martins, YW5hX2NhdGFyaW5hXzk1QGhvdG1haWwuY29t

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.