Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Public Health, 21 June 2023
Sec. Children and Health

The effects of height-for-age and HIV on cognitive development of school-aged children in Nairobi, Kenya: a structural equation modelling analysis

Rachel Maina,
Rachel Maina1,2*Jia HeJia He1Amina Abubakar,Amina Abubakar3,4Miguel Perez-GarciaMiguel Perez-Garcia5Manasi KumarManasi Kumar6Jelte M. WichertsJelte M. Wicherts1
  • 1Department of Methodology and Statistics, Tilburg University, Tilburg, Netherlands
  • 2Brain and Mind Institute, Aga Khan University, Nairobi, Kenya
  • 3Neurosciences Unit, KEMRI-Wellcome Trust Research Programme, Nairobi, Kenya
  • 4Institute for Human Development, Aga Khan University, Nairobi, Kenya
  • 5Mind, Brain and Behaviour Research Center (CIMCYC), University of Granada, Granada, Spain
  • 6Department of Psychiatry, University of Nairobi, Nairobi, Kenya

Background: Empirical evidence indicates that both HIV infection and stunting impede cognitive functions of school-going children. However, there is less evidence on how these two risk factors amplify each other’s negative effects. This study aimed to examine the direct effects of stunting on cognitive outcomes and the extent to which stunting (partially) mediates the effects of HIV, age, and gender on cognitive outcomes.

Methodology: We applied structural equation modelling to cross-sectional data from 328 children living with HIV and 260 children living without HIV aged 6–14 years from Nairobi, Kenya to test the mediating effect of stunting and predictive effects of HIV, age, and gender on cognitive latent variables flexibility, fluency, reasoning, and verbal memory.

Results: The model predicting the cognitive outcomes fitted well (RMSEA = 0.041, CFI = 0.966, χ2 = 154.29, DF = 77, p < 0.001). Height-for-age (a continuous indicator of stunting) predicted fluency (β = 0.14) and reasoning (β = 0.16). HIV predicted height-for-age (β = −0.24) and showed direct effects on reasoning (β = −0.66), fluency (β = −0.34), flexibility (β = 0.26), and verbal memory (β = −0.22), highlighting that the effect of HIV on cognitive variables was partly mediated by height-for-age.

Conclusion: In this study, we found evidence that stunting partly explains the effects of HIV on cognitive outcomes. The model suggests there is urgency to develop targeted preventative and rehabilitative nutritional interventions for school children with HIV as part of a comprehensive set of interventions to improve cognitive functioning in this high-risk group of children. Being infected or having been born to a mother who is HIV positive poses a risk to normal child development.

Introduction

Stunting (a height-for-age Z score of below −2 SD) (1) affects more than 149.2 million children worldwide and is associated with cognitive impairment (24) linked to poor academic performance (5, 6). Children who are stunted are at risk of underperforming in school and consequently dropping out (7). Over time, decreased years of education may result in low intelligence or cognitive ability (8). These may further contribute to long-term effects of reduced income and increased poverty (1, 7, 9, 10). Indeed Hoddinott et al. (10) found that stunting at 2 years was associated with increased probability of poverty in adulthood. Stunting has also been associated with increased mortality, morbidity, and a vicious cycle of stunting between mothers and children if left unaddressed (3, 9). This cycle is characterized by stunted mothers who tend to have a higher probability of lower age at first birth and multiple births (10) leading to increased nutritional demands on the mother (3); if not met, may lead to undernutrition in children (11). Moreover, mothers with a history of stunting, are likely to have short stature/adult height (7) which is linked to obstetric complications during birth and having children with small gestational age (SGA) (7, 9). SGA has been associated with up to 20% of stunting in children under the age of 5 years (9). This cyclical disability effects of stunting have attracted worldwide attention with underlying factors such as poverty and hunger forming part of the amelioration efforts in the sustainable development goals (12). These two factors have been incorporated as targets of intervention given the proven association between stunting and poverty (7, 10), and hunger resulting in deficient diets that do not meet the nutritional standards needed to prevent stunting (11). These primarily nutrition-specific interventions have achieved population-wide traction and success in reducing stunting. For example, countries have already put in place measures to curtail stunting that have borne fruits, with Asian countries showing a stunting decrease from 49 to 28% between 1990 and 2010. However, in Africa stunting has remained stagnant at around 40% (3). To achieve results similar to those achieved in Asia, nutrition-specific interventions have been primarily advocated assuming that they will reverse the effects of stunting (9). However, an increase in height does not necessarily mean that the child’s cognitive function is restored and working according to age. Nutrition may increase a child’s height but not necessarily ameliorate cognitive impairment post stunting because other factors may also impair cognitive functioning in a child with stunting. Specifically, other factors such as HIV infection, poverty, and poor health may affect cognitive outcomes, particularly in low and middle-income countries (LMICs) (13). Children with short stature (having a height that is well below that of other children of the same age and sex) may exhibit poor or delayed cognitive development for various reasons. For example, stunting is highly prevalent among children with HIV (28.6% in Kenya) (14) and children who are both HIV positive and stunted could have worse cognitive outcomes. Investigating causal mechanisms between stunting and cognitive performance may inform the alignment of stunting interventions to programmatic goals for the comprehensive management of HIV for school-going children.

HIV, stunting, and cognitive development

Given normal cognitive development, children’s cognitive functioning develops because of environmental factors and brain myelination among other neurological mechanisms and other factors involved in cognitive development (15, 16). HIV has been found to negatively impact cognitive function (17), and stunting (18) may partially explain this link. HIV is neurotropic meaning it directly affects the central nervous system (CNS) which may lead to cognitive impairment (19). Indirectly, HIV infection puts children at risk of undernutrition through inadequate and imperfect absorption of food, opportunistic infections, some HIV drugs, and other aetiological factors (20). Chronic undernutrition manifests itself as stunting. Children who are stunted have been found to perform poorly in receptive vocabulary and numerical ability compared to children who are not stunted, whereas children infected with HIV perform poorly in receptive and expressive language and attention compared to those without HIV (17, 21). Children with HIV are also found to have poorer cognitive performance in draw-a-person task and digit span (22), and working memory and executive functioning (23) though some studies have not found any difference in general cognitive function (24). The few earlier studies on both HIV and stunting among school-age children or lower school students used only a partial set of cognitive functions (25, 26). Stunting in children has been found to predict performance in reasoning, memory, language, executive functions, and motor ability (25, 26) while HIV predicts performance in nonverbal cognitive abilities, executive function, processing speed, memory, planning, reasoning, working memory, and visual–spatial abilities (22, 27, 28). These cognitive functions fall short of the recommended assessment domains (29, 30) of memory, language, attention, perceptual-motor, executive function, and social cognition (30). Therefore, more research using a broad battery of tests could shed light on how both HIV and stunting affect cognitive development.

We study the predictive effects (in relation to our model) of stunting and HIV on cognitive outcomes, while also considering age (3133) and gender (15, 3437) as relevant factors in predicting both stunting and cognitive outcomes. Age is central to stunting because the definition of stunting includes height-for-age ratio (1). Moreover, cognitive performance normally increases with age right from birth (16, 26, 38), although the developmental trajectories might vary over cognitive functions (34, 39) and might differ between children (40) for a host of reasons such as nutrition, exposure to HIV, parental education, and parental income (13, 22, 25, 26). Gender is relevant to our understanding of the effects of HIV and stunting on cognitive development because gender differences that vary in strength and direction have previously been found in relatively healthy children populations (15, 34, 35).

In this study, we investigate the mediation effects of stunting (as measured by height for age) of the link between HIV, age, and gender on cognitive functions recommended for assessment in the diagnostic statistical manual of mental disorders version five (DSM V) among school-age children from Kenya. The DSM V recommended classification of neurocognitive domains was preferred in this study due to the domains consistency with available knowledge on etiology of neurocognitive disorders and their impaired cognitive functions and with assessment criteria developed by experts (41, 42). We also prefer DSM V criteria because we hope that the findings can inform a holistic approach to clinical management of children with HIV. We hypothesized that stunting would partially mediate the effects of HIV on cognitive outcomes among 6-14-year-olds. The study used the Computerised Battery for Neuropsychological Evaluation of Children (BENCI) (43) – a cognitive battery that has good validity and reliability for diverse cultures including low-income settings and that can measure the cognitive functions recommended in DSM V. The outcome of this study may shed light on which cognitive domains are most impacted by stunting within a population infected with HIV to inform future interventions for improving cognitive functioning.

Methods

Design and setting

We evaluated the effects of stunting and HIV status on cognitive functions in a cross-sectional case control study among 6 to 14-year-olds within an HIV programme and three public schools. This study was part of a larger study that validated the Computerised Battery for Neuropsychological Evaluation of Children (BENCI) in Kenya (19).

The HIV uninfected sample was taken from three public primary schools in a middle-class urban setting. The schools follow the Kenyan government structured curriculum where children aged 6 years are in grade 1. The case sample was taken from a HIV programme in a middle-class urban setting. The programme provides a community based intervention to address medical, social, and economic needs of HIV positive children and their families. Both study settings are in Kenya’s capital city, Nairobi. Nairobi’s population is above the national poverty average (36.1%) and also above the national severe stunting average (11.4%) (44). Nairobi’s food consumption relies heavily on food production from other regions within the country and its inhabitants spend more on food than those in rural regions with the major food category being cereals (45).

Ethics approval

The study received ethics approval from Tilburg University’s School of Humanities Research Ethics Committee (REC# 2017/25) and the Kenyatta National Hospital/ University of Nairobi Ethical Review Committee (P556/07/2016). Additional approvals were sought from the County Government of Nairobi. Heads of the study sites authorized the study, while the caregivers gave informed consent and the children gave assent after a careful explanation of what the study entailed.

Study sample characteristics

Children who met the eligibility criteria were recruited into the study. The inclusion criteria were: all children aged 6–14 years and, for the control group, not having any medical condition as reported by the school and the students themselves, while for the experimental group, not having comorbid conditions as reported by caregivers and children themselves. We excluded children with comorbid and/or severe medical conditions associated with being HIV-positive as indicated in their medical reports. Children were recruited from four clinics within the HIV programme and three public primary schools. In the clinics, the staff helped in generating a database of children who met the inclusion criteria, and we aligned our recruitment process to their next hospital visit, which was also a play day for the children and fell on a weekend. On attending their scheduled clinic appointment, the parents of potential participants were randomly identified and informed about the study while being requested to sign up for the study. In the school setting, the same procedure was undertaken though the teachers here helped in randomly selecting the students who met inclusion criteria. Language wise, the Kenyan government obligates parents to send all children to school. The language of instruction in the schools is English, although the children prefer to use Kiswahili, Kenya’s national language, in their daily communication.

Data collection procedure

The data collection was conducted by clinical psychologists. Once consent was given, the children were immediately shown to a room in which data collectors designated them to a table with an iPad. After anthropometric measures were taken, children completed the cognitive assessment using the BENCI on the iPads, which took around 90 to 120 min. There was a 10-min break between the BENCI subtests which was scheduled right after the sustained attention subtest which can be tedious for children.

Measures

The computerised BENCI has been adapted and validated for use among Kenyan children aged 6–14 years in urban settings (19, 43) and has 17 tests that measure the following: processing speed, motor coordination, attention (sustained and selective), memory (verbal and visual), language (comprehension and production), and executive function (updating/monitoring, inhibition/impulsivity, flexibility, working memory, planning) (46). A detailed description of the tests, their administration, and scoring has been written about elsewhere (19, 43). Using the same data as reported here, BENCI has been found to have good test–retest reliability for most subtests and sufficient internal consistencies ranging from 0.50 to 0.97 (19). A four-factor model consisting of flexibility, fluency, reasoning, and verbal memory fitted the data well (RMSEA = 0.052, CFI = 0.944, TLI = 0.914) and showed metric (RMSEA = 0.040, CFI = 0.930, TLI = 0.893) and partial scalar measurement invariance (RMSEA = 0.041, CFI = 0.920, TLI = 0.884) between the HIV positive and negative groups (19). The tool has shown convergent validity with reasoning, memory, and inhibition tests from a local test battery called the Kilifi Toolkit (19). The BENCI was ideal for this study because it integrates tests that measure neurocognitive indicators recommended in DSM V and its good psychometric properties in our setting. We collected socio-demographic information about age, gender, weight, and height. Age was determined from the year of birth and calculated in terms of complete years and months since birth. Weight in kilograms was measured by a body scale as per the WHO protocol (47), and height in meters was measured by a tape measure.

Age was measured as complete years while gender was measured as either male or female.

Analyses

Stunting was calculated using the height-for-age z score (HAZ) based on 5–19 year-olds’ WHO Child Growth Reference standards where age was calculated in months (48). A WHO developed syntax was used to compute height-for-age (48). Children who were not stunted were defined as having a HAZ of ≥ −2.0 SD, those moderately stunted scored <−2.0 SD to > −3.0 SD, while those that were severely stunted scored ≤ −3.0 SD (48). The analyses used a continuous variable defined as height-for-age z score to measure stunting, with lower z values indicating more stunting.

We used maximum likelihood estimation in AMOS (49) to fit a structural equation model (SEM) as depicted in Figure 1. We used SEM as opposed to a multivariate path analysis as SEM allowed us to estimate a well-fitting complex model featuring latent cognitive variables underlying subtest scores (50). The model tests the mediating effect of height-for-age and includes all direct effects of HIV, age, and gender on the cognitive latent variables. The measurement model for the BENCI had been previously confirmed in another study using the same data (19). We adapted the model a bit due to the partial scalar invariance findings. The adaptation involved additional direct paths from HIV to Verbal Comprehension Figures CA and Visual Memory Delayed CA to accommodate the intercept differences identified in the earlier validation study.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. The BENCI measurement model with adapted partial scalar invariance and modification index paths. CA, Correct Answers.

Our previous paper on adapting and validating the BENCI in Kenyan children outlines the data cleaning process, including decisions in dealing with problematic data (19). The missing data pattern was not completely at random (Little’s MCAR test χ2 = 2455.2, DF = 1725, p < 0.001) but was not significantly related to factors that may have produced a missing pattern (19). Little’s MCAR test is also sensitive to non-normality, which might also play a role in the missing data pattern. However, we used data imputation in AMOS to check for modification indexes and calculate bootstrapped indirect effects. The modification indexes were used to check whether adding some paths would improve the model through a method of forward selection. Without overfitting the model too much, we decided ad hoc to add two residual covariances based on improper estimates of negative residual variances and modification indices. Residuals of Verbal Memory Recognition and Verbal Memory Delay were positively correlated, arguably due to the use of the same items across these indicators. The other residual covariance between Visual Memory Delay and Working Memory was unexpected but implemented to improve model fit. No further adjustments in the model as shown in Figure 1 were made. Bootstrapping based on 1,000 samples was performed to determine the significance of the direct, indirect, and total effects as well as their standard errors. We also fitted a model in which effects of HIV, age, and gender were fully mediated by stunting and ran a specification search model (Figure 2) in AMOS using the model in Figure 1 to assess the robustness of the results. We compared the fit of the models to assess mediation by stunting. Model fit was evaluated using goodness of fit indicators where an excellent fitting model would have a non-significant Chi-square test, Tucker Lewis Index (TLI) ≥ 0.95, Comparative Fit Index (CFI) ≥ 0.95, and Root Mean Square Error of Approximation (RMSEA) ≤ 0.08 (51). The term ‘predictive effect’ is used in this study to refer the hypothesized direction or the arrows within the model and finding prediction does not preclude that other factors have a role in causality.

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Height-for-age mediation model: significant paths in specification search. CA, Correct Answers.

We used a sample size of 604 as calculated in our paper putting forth psychometric validity of BENCI1 (19).

Results

Socio-demographic results

The total sample mean age was 9.48 (SD = 1.31) and the mean stunting (HAZ) was −0.44 (SD = 1.38). The prevalence of stunting in the HIV-positive sample was 17.9% while in the HIV-negative sample was 3.9%. The mean height-for-age in males was −0.42 (SD = 1.30) while in females it was −0.47 (SD = 1.45). Females who were HIV positive had more stunting (mean − 0.94, SD = 1.51) than their HIV-negative counterparts and both HIV-positive and negative males2. The details of the sociodemographic indicators are presented in Table 1.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Socio-demographic indicators of the study population.

Height-for-age mediation model

We tested a full model (Figure 1) in which HIV status, age, and gender predicted the four cognitive executive functioning factors, and stunting acted as (partial) mediator of these predictions. This model showed good fit in terms of RMSEA = 0.041, CFI = 0.966, and TLI = 0.947, while the exact fit formally rejected the model (χ2 = 154.29, DF = 77, N = 604, p < 0.001), probably because of sensitivity to minor (distributional) violations and the relatively large sample size. The standardized effects and their level of significance results are presented in Table 2 and their standard errors are given in the Supplementary Appendix. We also ran a full mediation model without direct paths from age, gender, and HIV status on the cognitive latent variables, but this model showed poor fit (RMSEA = 0.074, CFI = 0.869, TLI = 0.824, χ2 = 384.22, DF = 89, N = 604, p < 0.001), highlighting that stunting does not fully mediate the effects of HIV on cognitive outcomes. A specification search yielded a more parsimonious model (Figure 2) that fitted well (RMSEA = 0.038, CFI = 0.967, TLI = 0.953, χ2 = 158.73, DF = 84, N = 604, p < 0.001) and corroborated our proposed model albeit without the significant paths.

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Height-for-age model standardized effects.

Direct effects

As expected by its effects on poor nutrition and cognitive development, HIV infection (coded as 1 = HIV+) had a significant direct effect on height-for-age (Z score with lower scores, more stunting) (β = −0.242, p < 0.002) and on all cognitive latent variables (reasoning, fluency, verbal memory, and flexibility). HIV also had a direct effect on Verbal Comprehension Figures Hits and Visual Memory Delayed Hits reflective of the uniform measurement bias we described earlier (19). Age significantly predicted height-for-age (β = −0.462, p = 0.004). Also, age showed a direct effect on reasoning (β = 0.245, p < 0.001), but we found little evidence of direct age effects on fluency, verbal memory, and flexibility.

There was no gender difference in height-for-age (β = 0.00, p = 0.927). While males averaged higher fluency scores than females (β = −0.133, p = 0.005), gender did not significantly predict performance in flexibility, verbal memory, and reasoning. As expected from earlier works on the negative impact of stunting on cognitive outcomes, height-for-age predicted both fluency (β = 0.136, p = 0.008) and reasoning (β = 0.157, p = 002).

Mediation effects

As shown in Table 2 and Supplementary Appendix, we found three significant indirect effects due to stunting between HIV grouping and verbal memory (β = −0.024, SE = 0.013, p = 0.047), fluency (β = −0.033, SE = 0.013, p = 0.005), and reasoning (β = −0.038, SE = 0.013, p = 0.001). An additional analysis captured in Supplementary Appendix suggested that the non-significant indirect path for flexibility (β = 0.020, SE = 0.012, p = 0.100) could be due to low power.

Given the failure of gender as a variable to predict height-for-age, height-for-age did not mediate the relationship between gender and any of the cognitive latent variables. In addition, an exploratory analysis captured in the Supplementary Appendix highlighted that females who were HIV positive showed more severe stunting, but adding the interaction between gender and HIV status rendered the direct path of gender on fluency non-significant. We deliberate on this finding in the Supplementary Appendix section on interaction effect.

Height-for-age mediated the relationship of age with fluency (β = −0.063, SE = 0.023, p = 0.009) and reasoning (β = −0.072, SE = 0.023, p = 0.002). Total effects are reported in Table 2, while their standard errors are reported in Supplementary Table A2 of the Supplementary Material. Figure 2 reports the results of the specification search. Sensitivity analyses that checked for specification errors in the model are reported in Supplementary Table A1 in the Supplementary Material.

Discussion

We studied the mediating effects of stunting and the predictive effects of HIV, age, and gender on cognitive outcomes in a sample of 604 Kenyan children, and found that fluency, verbal memory, and reasoning are functions that may need to be targeted for intervention in children who were stunted and HIV positive. Next, we discuss these findings in detail.

Height-for-age effects

Similar effects of height-for-age on language were found in a recent study in Kenya (25). Our study confirms this earlier study in showing the persistent nature of cognitive impairment among children who are stunted. Children who are stunted when aged 2 and who later recovered from stunting, remain underperforming on cognitive tests aged 5 compared to children who were never stunted (38). Two cross-sectional studies among children older than 5 years have found that children with better HAZ have better performance in the cognitive tests (25, 26) and found that height-for-age mediates the prediction with age of reasoning, memory, language, executive functions, and motor ability.

Our finding that HIV directly contributes to stunting has also been found in other studies within Sub-Saharan Africa (26, 53, 54). Children living with HIV infection and those exposed to HIV yet uninfected have a higher prevalence of stunting than children who are neither infected nor exposed to HIV. Hence, being infected or having been born to a mother who is HIV positive poses a risk to normal child development (22, 32, 55). Moreover, children who are stunted and living with HIV or exposed to HIV are likely to have persistent stunting as they age (32).

Our findings of stunting increasing with age have been found in other studies (25) and are consistent with the notion that stunting often persists over age. A study looking at changes in height-for-age among children living with HIV and started ART around 8 years found that stunting reached its peak at 13 years for boys and 12 years for girls (36). After this age, stunting declined though more slowly in boys (at 13 years 50%, 15 years 48% and 18 years 31% stunted) than in girls (at 13 years 35%, 15 years 25% and 18 years 15% stunted) (36). Though there is a dearth of studies and indeed consensus on the exact age when HIV most directly leads to stunting, there are variable suggestions such as a study that showed male and females do not differ by age and stunting at ART initiation when aged around 8 years (36). However, stunting z scores start to dip as early as the first year of life among children living with HIV than those exposed but not infected (33). Another explanation for the strong effect is how stunting is calculated, i.e., as height relative to age z score.

HIV effects

We found children without HIV to outperform their HIV infected counterparts in all domains of cognitive functioning, with up to 44% of reasoning performance variation due to HIV. Our findings are consistent with those of earlier studies showing that children living with HIV score poorly in tests of nonverbal cognitive abilities, executive function, processing speed, memory, planning, reasoning, working memory, and visual–spatial abilities (22, 27, 28), especially in advanced stages of the disease (27). Suboptimal cognitive functioning significantly impedes the wellbeing of children. For instance, adherence requires memory capabilities for learning new information, encoding, storing and retrieving it when required (56). Similarly, for teenagers negotiating for healthy lifestyles, reasoning becomes an important asset. Deficits in these cognitive domains caused by HIV thus hinder psychosocial, learning processes including wading through routine functions and activities of daily life.

Mediation by stunting

An earlier study in Kenya found that stunting mediated the effect of age and years in school on executive function, language, and motor skills, but not on verbal memory (25). These results are consistent with the current study findings related to language comprehension.

There is a dearth of evidence in form of comparative studies for such mediation findings among lower school students/school-age children. Among younger and older cohorts, age of stunting onset and gender impact cognitive development among children with HIV (36) and without HIV infection (33). A study that followed up children from birth till 5 years found significant lower cognitive scores among those with early stunting onset (1–6 months) compared to those who were never stunted (at 60 months). The effect of stunting on cognitive performance, however, was no longer significant among those with late stunting onset (7–24 months after birth) although this might have been due to low power (31). We would therefore expect indirect effects of age and HIV on cognition among lower school students/school-age children due to persistent stunting.

The statistically non-significant findings of the indirect effect of HIV on flexibility via height-for-age in our study could be attributed to low power. However, we note that there are additional underlying factors that determine good cognitive functioning among children who are stunted, such as lack of parental stimulation and few learning opportunities, which could contribute to cognitive deficiencies (38, 55). Indeed parental stimulation among children who are stunted has been seen to improve performance in language and IQ tests (57), and such factors warrant more research in the future.

Age differences

The prediction of reasoning based on age was expected as reasoning increases with age and height-for-age reflects a history of stunted growth. However, other studies among a community samples have not found age differences related to reasoning and memory in 6–8 year-olds though the narrow age range could lower correlations with age (26). Such age differences in cognitive functions are expected because some functions such as inhibition develop earlier and rapidly more than others that appear later on in development (39). In other cases, late school onset and repeating a grade may create spurious age differences in cognitive performance (58) or obscure aging effects. Repeating children either improve academic achievement (59) or experience a decline in cognitive performance (60, 61) depending on how long they were retained though repeating may also reflect an existing low cognitive ability (62) among other persistent psychosocial and academic challenges.

The age-wise trend for cognitive performance should be steep but we found non- significant age differences in some of the cognitive indicators. Aging effects on cognitive performance could have been obscured by other risk factors that were not included and controlled for in our analysis. The few risk factors may have underpowered the findings resulting in non-significant correlations with age.

Gender differences

Gender has been found to be associated with risk of stunting among children aged below 5 years where having female gender was protective against stunting (31). Though our study did not reflect the same findings, the direction of our outcome is seen in other studies where girls were found to be more stunted than boys [although Intiful, Abdulai (63) found this difference to be non-significant].

Males in our study outperformed the females in fluency function, but no other gender gaps emerged. Earlier studies documented that males outperformed females in other cognitive functions such as visual–spatial ability though females have better scores than males in memory (64). Gender differences in cognitive function have been linked to school achievement with females performing better in languages though some studies have not found any differences in some subjects (37, 65). Whereas such outcomes may bring up confusion on which gender is need of a certain cognitive intervention, such findings should be interpreted with caution because studies have shown age related sex differences in cognitive maturation (34).

A study on underlying factors in gender differences may contribute to giving boys and girls equal opportunities in development may it be in improved school performance and increased earning potential.

Limitations

Our study interrogated a few independent variables while additional socio-economic factors and other confounding factors could affect cognitive functioning alongside stunting, HIV, age, and gender. Including additional factors such as poverty caregiver socioeconomic status, children schooling and related factors, and children’s familiarity with technology such as iPads in future studies might shed further light on the mechanisms causing lower cognitive functioning in populations infected by HIV. There is a dearth of studies evaluating the interplay between technology familiarity and cognition in children living with HIV and stunting. However, technological tools have been associated with cognitive development depending on exposure and pre-existing cognitive deficits (66). Moreover, though the Kenyan government obligates all parents to send children to school, our findings on the level of cognitive performance could be confounded by factors such as absentia and repeating grades among other factors. Our study did not control for such educational factors.

Our study used a cross-sectional design that is less able to uncover when and how effects emerge. A longitudinal study would point out the exact point where the severity of HIV strongly predicts cognitive deficiency in interaction with other determinants of stunting.

Our cross-sectional study design and study assessments do not allow us to uncover cognitive development trends within the children, or their ability to cope with early functional deficits (67). In addition, in cross-sectional studies, we cannot see whether the older children at an earlier time- point differ from the younger children in our sample. Of note, is that even longitudinal studies may miss out on this learning/coping confounding effect. Therefore, in situations where a child may be seen as underperforming, for example, in reasoning, they may have developed alternate ways of making sense of their environment such as through memorization. Indeed, children of the same age group have been found to have different patterns of developing reasoning functions (40). We may also not adequately explain differences in cognitive functioning of children of the same age who are brought up in different cognitively stimulating environments. With age, it is important to consider differences between following up the same cohort over time (33, 36, 61) and studying at one time-point (34, 38). Another limitation encompasses the cohort we used. These study findings and implications were drawn from a community sample and school factors such as student-to-teacher ratio and resources available in public vs. private schools may not have been matched to the sample. A hospital sample may present different findings hence the implications should not be overgeneralised.

Using longitudinal case–control designs, future studies could consider trends in different cognitive functions as factors of the environment they grown in, compensatory mechanisms for deficits and neurological mechanisms. Whereas our study takes a cross-sectional approach with few predictors, it is equally important in reviewing pediatric HIV programmes and setting up stunting and cognitive interventions.

Conclusion

As strides are made to mitigate and better manage HIV in children while reducing new infections, addressing stunting as well as its cognitive effects remain crucial, especially with the added burden of HIV (3). Stunting appears to play a role in the effects of HIV on cognitive domains. Our results point to the importance of integrating interventions that target reasoning, fluency, and verbal memory cognitive functions among children suffering from HIV infection and stunting. Nutrition programmes looking into reversing the effects of HIV on cognitive outcomes among lower school children in LMIC can tailor interventions targeting stunting. This is by targeting reasoning, fluency, and verbal memory and a wider set of cognitive functions that may need to be rehabilitated based on future research findings.

Data availability statement

The raw data supporting the conclusions of this article will be made available by the authors, without undue reservation.

Ethics statement

The studies involving human participants were reviewed and approved by Tilburg University’s School of Humanities Research Ethics Committee (REC# 2017/25) and the Kenyatta National Hospital/University of Nairobi Ethical Review Committee (P556/07/2016). Written informed consent to participate in this study was provided by the participants' legal guardian/next of kin.

Author contributions

RM, JH, AA, MP-G, MK, and JW significantly contributed to the study conceptualization, analysis, and manuscript preparation. RM, AA, JH, and MK conceptualized the study. RM conducted the data collection, entry, and clean up as JH, AA, MP-G, MK, and JW verified the dataset. Data analysis was done by RM, JH, and JW as MP-G, AA, MK, JH, and JW reviewed the findings and gave suggestions on how best the study objectives could be met through analysis. RM wrote the first manuscript draft that was read and reviewed by JH, AA, MK, MP-G, and JW. All authors contributed to the article and approved the submitted version.

Funding

This study was part of a larger study that was funded through a seed grant for early career researchers organized by Partnerships for Mental Health Development in Sub-Saharan Africa (PaM-D) (NIMH award number U19MH98718) and a 2017 institutional award by the Kenyatta National Hospital’s Research & Programs Department (KNH/R&P/23F/55/13). The funding institutions had no role in the study conceptualization, data collection, analysis, data interpretation and manuscript preparation. AA and RM are also supported by the Office Of The Director, National Institutes Of Health (OD), the National Institute Of Biomedical Imaging And Bioengineering (NIBIB), the National Institute Of Mental Health (NIMH), and the Fogarty International Center (FIC) of the National Institutes of Health under award number U54TW012089 (AA and Waljee AK). The content is solely the responsibility of the authors and does not necessarily represent the official views of the National Institutes of Health.

Acknowledgments

The authors acknowledge the invaluable support of staff in the HIV clinics and schools most especially the organization heads, Sister Owen and Roseann Maina, respectively.

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher’s note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

Supplementary material

The Supplementary material for this article can be found online at: https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fpubh.2023.1171851/full#supplementary-material

Footnotes

1. ^Using the sample in the validity paper (19) we determined the power of our model given the sample size of 604, RMSEA of 0.08 to assess misfit (if a model does not fit, RMSEA >0.08) and a model with 77 degrees of freedom (df). Using the function SemPower.PostHoc in R (52), a sample size of N = 604 is associated with a power larger than >99.9% to reject a wrong model with DF = 77 with an amount of misspecification corresponding to RMSEA = 0.08 using Alpha = 0.05 (52).

2. ^Females who were HIV positive had more stunting (mean − 0.94, SD = 1.51) than females who were HIV negative (Mean − 0.02, SD = 1.24), males who were HIV positive (Mean − 0.68, SD = 1.23) and males who were HIV negative (Mean − 0.23, SD = 1.32) (F (1, 503) = 3.89, p = 0.049).

References

1. United Nations Children's Fund, World Health Organization, The World Bank. UNICEF-WHO-the World Bank: Joint Child Malnutrition Estimates, Levels and Trends. New York, Geneva and Washington, DC: UNICEF, WHO and The World Bank (2021).

Google Scholar

2. Crookston, BT, Dearden, KA, Alder, SC, Porucznik, CA, Stanford, JB, Merrill, RM, et al. Impact of early and concurrent stunting on cognition. Matern Child Nutr. (2011) 7:397–409. doi: 10.1111/j.1740-8709.2010.00255.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

3. Prendergast, AJ, and Humphrey, JH. The stunting syndrome in developing countries. Paediatr Int Child Health. (2014) 34:250–65. doi: 10.1179/2046905514Y.0000000158

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

4. Ekholuenetale, M, Barrow, A, Ekholuenetale, CE, and Tudeme, G. Impact of stunting on early childhood cognitive development in Benin: evidence from demographic and health survey. Egypt Pediatric Association Gaz. (2020) 68:31. doi: 10.1186/s43054-020-00043-x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

5. Cortés Pascual, A, Moyano Muñoz, N, and Quílez Robres, A. The relationship between executive functions and academic performance in primary education: review and Meta-analysis. Front Psychol. (2019) 10:1582. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2019.01582

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

6. Pellicano, E, Kenny, L, Brede, J, Klaric, E, Lichwa, H, and McMillin, R. Executive function predicts school readiness in autistic and typical preschool children. Cogn Dev. (2017) 43:1–13. doi: 10.1016/j.cogdev.2017.02.003

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

7. Victora, CG, Adair, L, Fall, C, Hallal, PC, Martorell, R, Richter, L, et al. Maternal and child undernutrition: consequences for adult health and human capital. Lancet. (2008) 371:340–57. doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736(07)61692-4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

8. Ritchie, SJ, and Tucker-Drob, EM. How much does education improve intelligence? Psychol Sci Public Interest. (2018) 29:1358–69. doi: 10.1177/0956797618774253

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

9. Leroy, JL, and Frongillo, EA. Perspective: what does stunting really mean? A critical review of the evidence. Adv Nutr. (2019) 10:196–204. doi: 10.1093/advances/nmy101

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

10. Hoddinott, J, Behrman, JR, Maluccio, JA, Melgar, P, Quisumbing, AR, Ramirez-Zea, M, et al. Adult consequences of growth failure in early childhood. Am J Clin Nutr. (2013) 98:1170–8. doi: 10.3945/ajcn.113.064584

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

11. Black, RE, Allen, LH, Bhutta, ZA, Caulfield, LE, de Onis, M, Ezzati, M, et al. Maternal and child undernutrition: global and regional exposures and health consequences. Lancet. (2008) 371:243–60. doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736(07)61690-0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

12. UN General Assembly. Transforming our world: the 2030 agenda for sustainable development Geneva. (2015). Available at: https://www.refworld.org/docid/57b6e3e44.html

Google Scholar

13. Musindo, O, Krabbendam, L, Mutahi, J, García, MP, Bangirana, P, and Kumar, M. Neurocognitive deficits and socioeconomic risk factors among children and adolescents living with HIV in sub-Saharan Africa: a systematic review. Child Adolesc Psychiatry Ment Health. (2022) 16:31. doi: 10.1186/s13034-022-00465-y

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

14. Abate, BB, Aragie, TG, and Tesfaw, G. Magnitude of underweight, wasting and stunting among HIV positive children in East Africa: a systematic review and meta-analysis. PLoS One. (2020) 15:e0238403. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0238403

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

15. Ardila, A, Rosselli, M, Matute, E, and Inozemtseva, O. Gender differences in cognitive development. Dev Psychol. (2011) 47:984–90. doi: 10.1037/a0023819

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

16. Jäncke, L. Sex/gender differences in cognition, neurophysiology, and neuroanatomy. F1000Research. (2018) 7:7. doi: 10.12688/f1000research.13917.1

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

17. Wedderburn, CJ, Yeung, S, Rehman, AM, Stadler, JAM, Nhapi, RT, Barnett, W, et al. Neurodevelopment of HIV-exposed uninfected children in South Africa: outcomes from an observational birth cohort study. Lancet Child Adolesc Health. (2019) 3:803–13. doi: 10.1016/S2352-4642(19)30250-0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

18. McDonald, CM, Manji, KP, Kupka, R, Bellinger, DC, Spiegelman, D, Kisenge, R, et al. Stunting and wasting are associated with poorer psychomotor and mental development in HIV-exposed Tanzanian infants. J Nutr. (2013) 143:204–14. doi: 10.3945/jn.112.168682

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

19. Maina, R, He, J, Abubakar, A, Perez-Garcia, M, Kumar, M, and Wicherts, J. Psychometric evaluation of the computerized battery for neuropsychological evaluation of children (BENCI) among school aged children in the context of HIV in an urban Kenyan setting. BMC Psychiatry. (2022) 23:373. doi: 10.1186/s12888-023-04880-z

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

20. Vangal, KS, and Rajneesh, T. Malnutrition in HIV/AIDS - Aetiopathogenesis In: D Nancy, editor. Nutrition and HIV/AIDS. Rijeka: IntechOpen (2020)

Google Scholar

21. Woldehanna, T, Behrman, JR, and Araya, MW. The effect of early childhood stunting on children's cognitive achievements: evidence from young lives Ethiopia. Ethiop J Health Dev. (2017) 31:75–84.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

22. Sherr, L, Hensels, IS, Tomlinson, M, Skeen, S, and Macedo, A. Cognitive and physical development in HIV-positive children in South Africa and Malawi: a community-based follow-up comparison study. Child Care Health Dev. (2018) 44:89–98. doi: 10.1111/cch.12533

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

23. Phillips, N, Amos, T, Kuo, C, Hoare, J, Ipser, J, Thomas, KG, et al. HIV-associated cognitive impairment in perinatally infected children: a Meta-analysis. Pediatrics. (2016) 138:e20160893. doi: 10.1542/peds.2016-0893

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

24. Kandawasvika, GQ, Kuona, P, Chandiwana, P, Masanganise, M, Gumbo, FZ, Mapingure, MP, et al. The burden and predictors of cognitive impairment among 6- to 8-year-old children infected and uninfected with HIV from Harare, Zimbabwe: a cross-sectional study. Child Neuropsychol. (2015) 21:106–20. doi: 10.1080/09297049.2013.876493

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

25. Kitsao-Wekulo, P, Holding, P, Taylor, HG, Abubakar, A, Kvalsvig, J, and Connolly, K. Nutrition as an important mediator of the impact of background variables on outcome in middle childhood. Front Hum Neurosci. (2013) 7:713. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2013.00713

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

26. Ajayi, OR, Matthews, GB, Taylor, M, Kvalsvig, JD, Davidson, L, Kauchali, S, et al. Structural equation modeling of the effects of family, preschool, and stunting on the cognitive development of school children. Front Nutr. (2017) 4:17. doi: 10.3389/fnut.2017.00017

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

27. Ruel, T, Boivin, M, Boal, H, Bangirana, P, Charlebois, E, Havlir, D, et al. Neurocognitive and motor deficits in HIV-infected Ugandan children with high CD4 cell counts. Clin Infect Dis. (2012) 54:1001–9. doi: 10.1093/cid/cir1037

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

28. Cohen, S, ter Stege, JA, Geurtsen, GJ, Scherpbier, HJ, Kuijpers, TW, Reiss, P, et al. Poorer cognitive performance in perinatally HIV-infected children versus healthy socioeconomically matched controls. Clin Infect Dis. (2015) 60:1111–9. doi: 10.1093/cid/ciu1144

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

29. Lezak, MD, Howieson, DB, and Loring, DW. Neuropsychological Assessment. 4th ed. New York, NY: Oxford University Press (2004).

Google Scholar

30. American Psychiatric Association. Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders. 5th ed. Washington DC: American Psychiatric Association (2013).

Google Scholar

31. Alam, MA, Richard, SA, Fahim, SM, Mahfuz, M, Nahar, B, Das, S, et al. Impact of early-onset persistent stunting on cognitive development at 5 years of age: results from a multi-country cohort study. PLoS One. (2020) 15:e0227839. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0227839

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

32. Neary, J, Langat, A, Singa, B, Kinuthia, J, Itindi, J, Nyaboe, E, et al. Higher prevalence of stunting and poor growth outcomes in HIV-exposed uninfected than HIV-unexposed infants in Kenya. AIDS. (2022) 36:605–10. doi: 10.1097/QAD.0000000000003124

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

33. McHenry, MS, Apondi, E, Ayaya, SO, Yang, Z, Li, W, Tu, W, et al. Growth of young HIV-infected and HIV-exposed children in western Kenya: a retrospective chart review. PLoS One. (2019) 14:e0224295. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0224295

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

34. Gur, RC, Richard, J, Calkins, ME, Chiavacci, R, Hansen, JA, Bilker, WB, et al. Age group and sex differences in performance on a computerized neurocognitive battery in children age 8-21. Neuropsychology. (2012) 26:251–65. doi: 10.1037/a0026712

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

35. Ramful, A, and Lowrie, T. Spatial Visualisation and Cognitive Style: How do Gender Differences Play Out? In: M Marshman, V Geiger, and A Bennison, editors. Mathematics Education in the Margins. Sunshine Coast: Mathematics Education Research Group of Australasia (2015). 508–15.

Google Scholar

36. Jesson, J, Schomaker, M, Malasteste, K, Wati, DK, Kariminia, A, Sylla, M, et al. Stunting and growth velocity of adolescents with perinatally acquired HIV: differential evolution for males and females. A multiregional analysis from the IeDEA global paediatric collaboration. J Int AIDS Soc. (2019) 22:e25412. doi: 10.1002/jia2.25412

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

37. Weis, M, Heikamp, T, and Trommsdorff, G. Gender differences in school achievement: the role of self-regulation. Front Psychol. (2013) 4:4. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00442

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

38. Casale, D, and Desmond, C. Recovery from stunting and cognitive outcomes in young children: evidence from the south African birth to twenty cohort study. J Dev Orig Health Dis. (2016) 7:163–71. doi: 10.1017/S2040174415007175

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

39. Buttelmann, F, and Karbach, J. Development and plasticity of cognitive flexibility in early and middle childhood. Front Psychol. (2017) 8:1040. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2017.01040

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

40. Lazonder, AW, Janssen, N, Gijlers, H, and Walraven, A. Patterns of development in Children’s scientific reasoning: results from a three-year longitudinal study. J Cogn Dev. (2021) 22:108–24. doi: 10.1080/15248372.2020.1814293

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

41. Sachdev, P, Blacker, D, Blazer, D, Ganguli, M, Jeste, D, Paulsen, J, et al. Classifying neurocognitive disorders: the DSM-5 approach. Nat Rev Neurol. (2014) 10:634–42. doi: 10.1038/nrneurol.2014.181

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

42. Diamond, A. Executive functions. Annu Rev Psychol. (2013) 64:135–68. doi: 10.1146/annurev-psych-113011-143750

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

43. Maina, RW, Abubakar, A, Miguel, PG, van de Vijver, FJR, and Kumar, M. Standardization of the computerized battery for neuropsychological evaluation of children (BENCI) in an urban setting, in Kenya: a study protocol. BMC Res Notes. (2019) 12:799. doi: 10.1186/s13104-019-4830-y

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

44. Statistics KNBo (2018). Basic report on well-being in Kenya: based on the 2015/16 Kenya integrated household budget survey (KIHBS). Kenya National Bureau of Statistics (KNBS); Report No.: 978-9966-102-02-7.

Google Scholar

45. Ministry of Planning and National Development. Basic report on well-being in Kenya based on Kenya integrated household budget survey- 2005/06. 2007. Report no.: 9966–767–08-8.

Google Scholar

46. Fasfous, AF, Peralta-Ramirez, MI, Pérez-Marfil, MN, Cruz-Quintana, F, Catena-Martinez, A, and Pérez-García, M. Reliability and validity of the Arabic version of the computerized battery for neuropsychological evaluation of children (BENCI). Child Neuropsychol. (2015) 21:210–24. doi: 10.1080/09297049.2014.896330

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

47. World Health Organisation. Training Course on Child Growth Assessment. Geneva: WHO (2008).

Google Scholar

48. World Health Organisation. Growth reference data for 5-19 years 2013. Available at: www.who.int/growthref/en/

Google Scholar

49. Schumacker, RE. Teacher's corner: conducting specification searches with Amos. Struct Equ Model Multidiscip J. (2006) 13:118–29. doi: 10.1207/s15328007sem1301_6

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

50. Tarka, P. An overview of structural equation modeling: its beginnings, historical development, usefulness and controversies in the social sciences. Qual Quant. (2018) 52:313–54. doi: 10.1007/s11135-017-0469-8

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

51. Hu, L, and Bentler, PM. Cutoff criteria for fit indexes in covariance structure analysis: conventional criteria versus new alternatives. Struct Equ Model. (1999) 6:1–55. doi: 10.1080/10705519909540118

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

52. Moshagen, M, and Erdfelder, E. A new strategy for testing structural equation models. Struct Equ Model Multidiscip J. (2016) 23:54–60. doi: 10.1080/10705511.2014.950896

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

53. Stanaway, JD, Afshin, A, Gakidou, E, Lim, SS, Abate, D, Abate, KH, et al. Global, regional, and national comparative risk assessment of 84 behavioural, environmental and occupational, and metabolic risks or clusters of risks for 195 countries and territories, 1990–2017: a systematic analysis for the global burden of disease study 2017. Lancet. (2018) 392:1923–94. doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736(18)32225-6

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

54. Nigussie, J, Girma, B, Molla, A, Mareg, M, and Mihretu, E. Under-nutrition and associated factors among children infected with human immunodeficiency virus in sub-Saharan Africa: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Arch Public Health. (2022) 80:19. doi: 10.1186/s13690-021-00785-z

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

55. Wedderburn, CJ, Evans, C, Yeung, S, Gibb, DM, Donald, KA, and Prendergast, AJ. Growth and neurodevelopment of HIV-exposed uninfected children: a conceptual framework. Curr HIV/AIDS Rep. (2019) 16:501–13. doi: 10.1007/s11904-019-00459-0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

56. Wang, Q. The cultural Foundation of Human Memory. Annu Rev Psychol. (2021) 72:151–79. doi: 10.1146/annurev-psych-070920-023638

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

57. Walker, SP, Chang, SM, Powell, CA, and Grantham-McGregor, SM. Effects of early childhood psychosocial stimulation and nutritional supplementation on cognition and education in growth-stunted Jamaican children: prospective cohort study. Lancet. (2005) 366:1804–7. doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736(05)67574-5

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

58. Chen, Q. Impacts of late school entry on Children's cognitive development in rural northwestern China—does preprimary education matter? Asia Pacific Policy Stud. (2017) 4:586–601. doi: 10.1002/app5.192

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

59. Aduda, PO, Kodero, HMN, and Sichari, M. The effect of class repetition on the academic performance of pupils in lower primary schools in Homa-Bay Sub-County. Int J Educ Res. (2019) 5:37–54. doi: 10.53555/es.v5i9.1126

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

60. Hong, G, and Raudenbush, SW. Effects of kindergarten retention policy on Children's cognitive growth in Reading and mathematics. Educ Eval Policy Anal. (2005) 27:205–24. doi: 10.3102/01623737027003205

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

61. Wu, W, West, SG, and Hughes, JN. Effect of retention in first grade on Children's achievement trajectories over 4 years: a piecewise growth analysis using propensity score matching. J Educ Psychol. (2008) 100:727–40. doi: 10.1037/a0013098

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

62. Glick, P, and Sahn, DE. Early academic performance, grade repetition, and school attainment in Senegal: a panel data analysis. World Bank Econ Rev. (2010) 24:93–120. doi: 10.1093/wber/lhp023

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

63. Intiful, FD, Abdulai, H, Nyarko, R, Tette, E, and Asante, M. Malnutrition in HIV infected children on antiretroviral drugs in a cohort of Ghanaian children. Heliyon. (2021) 7:e08636. doi: 10.1016/j.heliyon.2021.e08636

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

64. Halpern, DF. Sex Differences in Cognitive Abilities. 4th ed. Milton Park: Taylor & Francis (2013).

Google Scholar

65. Scheiber, C, Reynolds, M, Hajovsky, D, and Kaufman, A. Gender differences in achievement in a large, nationally representative sample of children and adolescents. Psychol Sch. (2015) 52:335–48. doi: 10.1002/pits.21827

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

66. Vedechkina, M, and Borgonovi, F. A review of evidence on the role of digital Technology in Shaping Attention and Cognitive Control in children. Front Psychol. (2021) 12:611155. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2021.611155

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

67. Grammer, JK, Coffman, JL, Ornstein, PA, and Morrison, FJ. Change over time: conducting longitudinal studies of Children's cognitive development. J Cogn Dev. (2013) 14:515–28. doi: 10.1080/15248372.2013.833925

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: stunting, mediation, HIV, lower school students, executive functioning, reasoning, flexibility, lower & middle-income countries

Citation: Maina R, He J, Abubakar A, Perez-Garcia M, Kumar M and Wicherts JM (2023) The effects of height-for-age and HIV on cognitive development of school-aged children in Nairobi, Kenya: a structural equation modelling analysis. Front. Public Health. 11:1171851. doi: 10.3389/fpubh.2023.1171851

Received: 27 February 2023; Accepted: 31 May 2023;
Published: 21 June 2023.

Edited by:

Judie Arulappan, Sultan Qaboos University, Oman

Reviewed by:

Mei Tan, Columbia University, United States
Patricia Kitsao-Wekulo, African Population and Health Research Center (APHRC), Kenya

Copyright © 2023 Maina, He, Abubakar, Perez-Garcia, Kumar and Wicherts. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Rachel Maina, Ui5XLk1haW5hQHV2dC5ubA==;;cmFjaGVsLm1haW5hQGFrdS5lZHU=;;bWFpbmEucmFjaGVsQGdtYWlsLmNvbQ==

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.