Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Public Health, 22 July 2022
Sec. Public Health Education and Promotion

Psychographic segmentation to identify higher-risk teen peer crowds for health communications: Validation of Virginia's Mindset Lens Survey

\nCarolyn A. Stalgaitis
Carolyn A. Stalgaitis1*Jeffrey W. Jordan
Jeffrey W. Jordan1*Mayo DjakariaMayo Djakaria1Daniel J. SaggeseDaniel J. Saggese2Hannah Robbins BruceHannah Robbins Bruce2
  • 1Rescue Agency, San Diego, CA, United States
  • 2Virgina Foundation for Healthy Youth, Richmond, VA, United States

Audience segmentation is necessary in health communications to ensure equitable resource distribution. Peer crowds, which are macro-level teen subcultures, are effective psychographic segments for health communications because each crowd has unique mindsets, values, norms, and health behavior profiles. These mindsets affect behaviors, and can be used to develop targeted health communication campaigns to reach those in greatest need. Though peer crowd research is plentiful, no existing peer crowd measurement tool has been formally validated. As such, we developed and validated Virginia's Mindset Lens Survey (V-MLS), a mindset-based teen peer crowd segmentation survey to support health communication efforts. Using an online convenience sample of teens (N = 1,113), we assessed convergent and discriminant validity by comparing the V-MLS against an existing, widely-used peer crowd survey (I-Base Survey®) utilizing a multi-trait multi-method matrix. We also examined the V-MLS's predictive ability through a series of regressions using peer crowd scores to predict behaviors, experiences, and traits relevant to health communication campaign planning. The V-MLS demonstrated reliability and convergent and discriminant validity. Additionally, the V-MLS effectively distinguished teen peer crowds with unique health behaviors, experiences, and personal traits. When combined with appropriate information processing and campaign development frameworks, this new tool can complement existing instruments to inform message framing, tone, and style for campaigns that target at-risk teens to increase campaign equity and reach.

Introduction

Health inequities are persistent. One challenge of equitably administering public health services lies in identifying population segments who are at elevated risk, and understanding how to reach them effectively with resources that meet their needs. Audience segmentation, the process of identifying homogeneous subgroups within heterogeneous populations, is necessary in public health communications to increase equity by allowing campaign planners to create content that is tailored to a higher-risk audience's needs and delivered via targeted media channels to reach them effectively (1). Though public health has slowly incorporated segmentation into campaign development and delivery, the field too often segments based on demographics such as gender, race, or ethnicity alone, which is insufficient and can lead to stereotyping, exclusion, and the reinforcement of disparities (24).

Recently, public health and social marketers have embraced the use of psychographics such as mindsets, values, and worldviews to segment audiences. Promising results indicate that using psychographics alone or in combination with demographics leads to more clearly-defined audience segments than demographics alone (27). In particular, psychographic segmentation identifies audiences with unique behavioral needs who can be reached via specific media channels (13, 6, 7). Additionally, psychographic segmentation can inform campaign development in a way that demographic segmentation alone cannot, as the worldviews, values, and mindsets that define psychographic segments can be incorporated into message framing and content to increase persuasion (8).

Psychographic audience segmentation is effective because it creates groups based on shared characteristics that directly influence behavior, rather than demographic characteristics that may only proxy direct behavioral influences (5, 8). Though a range of approaches can be used in psychographic segmentation, many cutting-edge public health communication campaigns have segmented teens and young adults using peer crowds. Peer crowds are macro-level, reputational subcultures with shared values, interests, media preferences, visual cues, and behavioral norms (9, 10). Broader than the peer groups with which a young person interacts on a daily basis, peer crowds represent shared culture and identity across communities (9, 11). Peer crowds are ideal for health communication audience segmentation because campaign content can address the specific behavioral norms of a targeted peer crowd, using the crowd's shared values and mindsets to increase message relevance and tailoring visual cues to increase appeal and information processing. Peer crowds also tap into a key factor in teen and young adult behavior—social influence (12)—by identifying groups of youth with shared norms who can be reached together and whose behavior influences each other. Finally, peer crowd campaigns can be delivered efficiently by focusing resources on media channels where crowd members naturally gather, increasing campaign exposure among the target crowd without increasing campaign waste (13, 14).

Peer crowds fulfill a symbolic role within adolescent culture, each representing a particular set of values and lifestyles that a youth can embrace or reject as they explore their place within the social environment (1517). Crowds form through both selection (individuals with shared values, interests, and experiences seeking out each other) and socialization (crowd members reinforcing shared culture, mindsets, and behaviors as a means of maintaining identity and belonging) (18, 19). Peer crowds influence behavior by providing a prototype of normative crowd behavior, which a young person may emulate to signal to others their identity as a member of that crowd (2022).

One way of understanding the role of values and mindset in adolescent peer crowd culture is through the mindsponge model (23, 24). Though initially developed to explain processes of acculturation and cross-cultural business (23), the mindsponge model has been applied to describe other psychosocial phenomenon (24) and can be a useful way to think about how a person's values and mindsets evolve during adolescence. As peers overtake the family as a teen's primary source of socialization and culture (2527), the values and mindsets that the youth learned from their family may be challenged by new values held by their peers. The relevance, compatibility, and costs/benefits of these new values will influence if and how a teen incorporates these values into their comfort zone and eventually their mindset, as well as which previously-held values they may drop (23, 24). The values that pass a teen's filters will become part of their core beliefs, affecting behavior and which values they may accept in the future (24). This dynamic process may explain how youth develop peer crowd identifications via adoption of a crowd's cultural values, how a teen's peer crowd identification may evolve over time, and why peer crowds tend to fade in relevance in adulthood as other cultural contexts with their own value sets such as the workplace and family become a person's primary sources of socialization.

Peer crowds have been the subject of cross-disciplinary research for decades (Table 1). Research in the U.S. and in other, primarily Western, countries consistently identifies similar teen peer crowds with particular values, mindsets, interests, and behaviors across settings and methods. The Hip Hop and Alternative peer crowds reliably demonstrate elevated risk across a range of health topics including tobacco use, substance use, and mental health. Other crowds such as Country and Popular tend to have moderate risk levels, while Mainstream is typically at low risk across health topics.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Common teen peer crowds (11, 18, 20, 2854).

Peer crowds provide a purposeful means of audience segmentation that maximizes equity by allowing communication campaigns to create tailored content delivered via targeted channels to higher-risk crowds (13). Experimental studies indicate that peer crowd-matched educational materials can influence behavioral attitudes and beliefs (5557). In real-world applications (see Supplementary Figure 1 for examples), youth and young adult peer crowd-targeted campaigns have been associated with high levels of awareness, positive receptivity, and desired behavior change (14, 5863). Peer crowd segmentation for public health communication represents a promising means of increasing equity by focusing resources on crowds with disproportionate risk. However, this approach requires both an effective and practical means of distinguishing members of a given crowd, and an understanding of the values, interests, visual cues, and preferred media channels of the crowd to inform message content, framing, style, and placement.

Despite convergence across studies, no “gold standard” exists for measuring peer crowd identity (64). Existing methods each have benefits and drawbacks for audience segmentation purposes. Ethnographic methods, wherein researchers observe and interview teens in a single community to understand the prevalent crowds, provide detailed portraits of peer crowds but are not sufficiently generalizable or scalable to be used in campaign audience segmentation (50, 6568). Other studies use a peer-nominated social-type rating approach, whereby students at a given school place their classmates into peer crowds (16, 20, 43). This approach also lacks scalability and generalizability as it requires students to know each other sufficiently well to assign crowd affiliation, and agreement between raters is often low (43, 64). Furthermore, peer ratings focus on others' perceptions of an individual's crowd rather than the individual's self-perception, which is of greater relevance to public health communication segmentation as it more directly influences behavior (31) and avoids stereotyping.

Other approaches directly measure an individual's identity by asking youth to select their peer crowd affiliation from a list of names. Iterations of this approach include forcing selection of a single crowd, allowing selection of multiple crowds, or indicating degree of affiliation with multiple crowds, and may or may not include detailed crowd descriptions (2832, 34, 4245, 47, 52, 5457, 6971). For example, the Peer Crowd Questionnaire and College Peer Crowd Questionnaire ask participants to confirm if each crowd from an established list exists in their area (e.g., Jocks, Brains, Burnouts, Populars, Non-Conformists, or None/Average), and with which of the crowds they identify (18, 33, 35, 38, 7275). Though more scalable for audience segmentation, using crowd names suffers from desirability and other biases related to the names and descriptions used. Colloquial crowd names often differ across communities, and individuals may avoid selecting crowd names they perceive as undesirable or having negative connotations even if they personally identify with the group that the name was intended to represent (15, 31, 43, 64). Additionally, forcing youth to select a single crowd fails to account for the multifaceted nature of identity, as many individuals identify with more than one crowd (31, 32, 34, 37, 45). Finally, name-based self-reports provide none of the insight needed to create a targeted campaign such as a crowd's values, worldviews, mindsets, or visual cues, leaving campaign planners without the insights necessary to tailor content.

To address shortcomings of direct measures, indirect measures ask youth to report characteristics associated with peer crowds and assign affiliation using these data. Several studies have collected data on activities (e.g., studying, athletics, partying) or adjective pairs (e.g., quiet/loud, popular/unpopular, athletic/not athletic) and applied cluster analysis techniques to assign youth to peer crowd clusters (7680). Though a cluster analysis approach creates homogeneous segments, it is unclear if the clusters reflect interactional crowds with shared behavioral norms, making application to health communication campaigns limited. Other indirect approaches tap into the visual cues of peer crowd identity (11, 53). One example, the I-Base Survey®, includes a grid of photos of unknown youth with each photo representing a single peer crowd based on qualitative research (36, 81). Respondents select photos that would and would not fit with their group of friends, resulting in a score indicating strength of identification with each crowd (11, 13, 37, 40, 41, 5863, 82). While formal validation studies of the I-Base Survey have not been published, the instrument has been used extensively across U.S. communities and over time with consistent results, indicating the I-Base Survey has a level of applied validity and utility in audience segmentation (11, 13, 36, 37, 40, 41, 8285). This approach overcomes issues associated with asking individuals directly and forcing identification with a single crowd, and provides insight into the visual cues that should be applied to a communication campaign to attract the attention of the targeted crowd. However, photos must be updated regularly to stay abreast of fashion trends and must be adjusted to reflect the racial and ethnic demographics of the community.

While several approaches exist to measure peer crowd identification, to date there is no formally validated, scalable, and replicable peer crowd segmentation tool. With this in mind, we developed and validated a mindset-based teen peer crowd measurement tool that addresses many of the shortcomings of existing instruments, and more directly measures the psychographics that make peer crowds unique segments that can be effectively targeted in health communications. We focused on values and mindsets as these characteristics influence behavior, are predictive of health risk, and can effectively segment audiences (23, 24, 8689). To develop the instrument, we conducted formative research leading to the large-scale validation survey described in detail here. The result is a survey that effectively segments teens into distinct psychographic subgroups with unique health risk profiles, while also providing insight into the values and mindsets that should be incorporated into a health communication campaign targeting any given crowd. As the study described herein was conducted in Virginia, we refer to the instrument as Virginia's Mindset Lens Survey (V-MLS). However, as peer crowds transcend geographic boundaries, we believe V-MLS findings are likely generalizable across the U.S. and anticipate future work to establish the survey's applicability outside the state. Additionally, future cross-cultural adaptation work could ensure applicability of the instrument in other countries, where cultural differences require care to ensure local crowds and values are reflected accurately. The V-MLS represents an evolution in peer crowd measurement methods that can complement existing strategies and tools to identify, target, and tailor efforts to higher-risk segments to address health disparities.

Materials and methods

Study approach and design

Prior to data collection, we identified key parameters for instrument creation. First, we sought to measure identification with the peer crowds commonly identified in existing literature across different geographies, times, and methodologies (Table 1). We selected the I-Base Survey as our comparison instrument, given the widespread use of this survey both to understand peer crowd risk (11, 13, 36, 37, 40, 41, 82) and to create and evaluate peer crowd-targeted communication campaigns (5863). Therefore, our new instrument endeavored to measure identification with the five peer crowds measured in the U.S. teen I-Base Survey—Alternative, Country, Hip Hop, Mainstream, and Popular. Second, it was critical that the new instrument both measure strength of peer crowd identification as well as allow for assignment of individuals to a single peer crowd based on their strongest identification. This would allow us to reflect the multifaceted nature of teen identity and values-adoption, and also apply a range of analytical approaches to the data (9). Third, we wanted to improve upon existing instruments by creating a standardized survey less impacted by geographic and demographic differences. Fourth, the instrument needed to distinguish crowds with distinct health behavior profiles without asking directly about behavior, to avoid building a segmentation instrument around a particular health behavior rather than psychographics.

Study design was influenced by the methods we selected to examine instrument validity and predictive ability. Using a multi-trait multi-method matrix, we aimed to assess convergent validity, a metric of whether each subscale of the V-MLS measured the crowd we believed it measured, and discriminant validity, a metric of whether each crowd subscale did not measure identification with other crowds (90, 91). To complete the matrix, we examined the correlation between two methods (I-Base Survey and V-MLS) that measured the same set of traits (identification with five peer crowds). As such, participants completed the I-Base Survey during screening, and the V-MLS immediately after qualifying, providing assent/consent, and beginning the full questionnaire. Participants then completed a series of questions about health behavior, adversity, resilience, and personal traits to assess the ability of the V-MLS to predict these characteristics, which were selected based on their relevance to the creation, framing, and tone of health communication campaigns. Details on specific survey measures are provided below.

Data collection

Data were collected in August-October 2020 from an online convenience sample of Virginia teens ages 13–18 (N = 1,113). Participants were recruited through paid social media advertisements (n = 876), outreach to previous study participants (n = 209), and snowball sampling (n = 28). Advertisements were placed on Instagram and Facebook using age and geo-targeting to minimize potential for ineligible individuals to encounter the screener. For snowball sampling, youth who completed a valid screener and questionnaire were asked to share the survey link with friends. All individuals completed an online screener survey to determine eligibility (age 13–18, Virginia resident). The screener survey also included the I-Base Survey to allow us to monitor recruitment to ensure relatively even representation of each crowd in the sample. Eligible individuals provided electronic assent/consent and were emailed a parental notification form before beginning the questionnaire. Individuals who completed a valid questionnaire received an electronic gift card incentive; the incentive amount started at $10 (n = 478), and increased to $15 (n = 509) then $20 (n = 126) to encourage survey completion. The study protocol and assent/consent procedures were reviewed and approved by Advarra IRB (Pro00038832).

Measures

To enable completion of the multi-trait multi-method matrix for validity assessment, participants completed both the I-Base Survey (during screening) and the V-MLS (in the questionnaire). Participants completed the I-Base Survey by viewing a grid of teen photos and selecting three that best and three that least fit with their group of friends, then repeating the process with a second grid (11, 36). Each photo represented a single peer crowd based on prior qualitative research, and participants earned positive points for the peer crowds of photos selected as best fit and negative points for the crowds of photos selected as least fit. This resulted in a peer crowd score ranging from−12 to 12 for each of the five measured crowds (Alternative, Country, Hip Hop, Mainstream, Popular) for each participant. Additionally, participants were assigned to a single exclusive peer crowd based on their highest score. Participants completed the version of the I-Base Survey that was used in the 2019 Virginia Youth Survey (VYS), the state's implementation of the Youth Risk Behavior Survey (YRBS) (92).

The V-MLS was developed through three phases of formative research in Virginia (Figure 1). In Fall 2019 we conducted 12 focus groups with 112 teens to test initial hypotheses surrounding peer crowd values and mindsets. In Spring 2020 we fielded an online survey with 1,043 youth to test potential survey questions with a larger sample. Finally, in Summer 2020 we conducted 42 cognitive testing interviews to identify potential sources of measurement bias and ensure the instrument was acceptable and easy to complete (93). This process resulted in the seven-question V-MLS examined here. Each question of the V-MLS presented five response options (one hypothesized per peer crowd) and participants selected the #1 and #2 options that best described themselves or their friends. The V-MLS was completed immediately after screening and assent/consent, with question and response option order randomized within the section. We scored participants' responses by assigning 2 points for the peer crowd of responses selected as rank #1 and 1 point for the crowd of responses selected as rank #2 (with the exception of Alternative and Mainstream peer crowd responses, which earned participants 0.5 points when selected as rank #2 to counter desirability biases associated with some of the responses for these crowds). Participants received a score from 0–14 for each crowd, and we also assigned them to a single crowd based on their highest score. Details of final question wording for the V-MLS and administration support are available upon request. Questions covered core personal values and mindsets expressed by formative research participants who identified with each peer crowd: Alternative (creativity/arts, activism, individuality, challenging norms), Country (personal liberty/rights, simple living, connection to outdoors, patriotism), Hip Hop (overcoming struggles, grinding/hustling, confidence, strength), Mainstream (helping others, rules/stability, learning/personal growth, achievement), and Popular (excitement, socializing, outgoing, living life to the fullest).

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Development of the Virginia's Mindset Lens Survey.

To assess the V-MLS's capacity to segment peer crowds with unique health behaviors and personal traits relevant to the development of equitable health communication campaigns, participants then completed a series of behavior, experience, and trait questions. Using questions from the 2019 VYS (92), participants reported the number of days in the past 30 on which they had used cigarettes, little cigars and cigarillos (LCCs), smokeless tobacco, vapes, alcohol, and marijuana (dichotomized to yes/no), and reported if they had felt sad or hopeless for two or more weeks in the past year (yes/no). Behavior questions were selected based on having consistent associations with specific peer crowds (11, 40, 41).

Participants also completed measures of adverse childhood experiences (ACEs) as well as resilience, an individual's ability to bounce back following trauma. ACEs and resilience have been linked to a range of health outcomes (9496), and peer crowds report differential levels of ACEs (40). We measured ACEs using an adapted version of the National Survey of Children's Health scale used previously to examine ACEs among teen peer crowds (40). Participants reported if they had experienced each of nine events, earning a score from 0–9 representing how many they had experienced: difficulty getting by on family's income, parental divorce/separation, parental incarceration, parental death, household mental illness, household substance misuse, household violence, neighborhood violence, and being treated unfairly because of race or ethnicity. Resilience was measured using the Brief Resilience Scale (BRS), a validated six-item scale that results in a score from 1–5, with a higher score indicating greater resilience (97).

Additionally, participants completed two validated personal trait scales. Participants completed the social prioritization index (SPI), a measure of the importance of socializing in an individual's life which is associated with tobacco use (83, 98). We used the eight-item abbreviated SPI, which results in a score from 0–10 with a higher score indicating greater social prioritization (98). Participants also completed the Brief Sensation Seeking Scale (BSSS) (99), an eight-item scale measuring sensation seeking, a predictor of risky behavior (100, 101). BSSS scores ranged from 1–5, with a higher score indicating greater sensation-seeking.

Finally, we collected demographic data including age in years; gender (“female”; “male”; and “transgender” and “a different identity” collapsed to “another identity” for analysis); region of Virginia (Northern, Central, Southwest, or Southeast, determined via zip code); and race/ethnicity (categorized as Hispanic and non-Hispanic white, Black, Asian-Pacific Islander, and other/multiracial for analysis).

Analysis

To ensure data integrity, we conducted extensive fraud reviews during and after data collection. Age and geo-targeting of social media ads limited study exposure to those most likely to qualify, and eligibility criteria were not stated in the ads or during screening to reduce fraudulent survey attempts. Additionally, participants did not learn their eligibility status until they had completed the entire screener to reduce attempts at gaming the survey. Data quality checks identified and removed duplicate, low quality, and fraudulent cases. All analyses were conducted using SPSS Statistics Subscription for Mac (IBM Corp., Armonk, NY, United States). We first examined frequencies and means to describe the study sample.

We assessed convergent and discriminant validity by comparing scores from the V-MLS and I-Base Survey using a multi-trait multi-method matrix. This approach examines the reliability of each peer crowd score measure as well as all correlations between crowd scores measured by the same and different surveys. To complete the matrix, we first examined reliability using the OMEGA macro for SPSS to calculate McDonald's omega (ω) for each peer crowd for the I-Base Survey and V-MLS (102, 103). We then examined Pearson correlation coefficients between all I-Base Survey and V-MLS peer crowd scores. Assessments for convergent validity focused on the correlation coefficients between I-Base Survey and V-MLS scores for a single crowd. Convergent validity was indicated if these coefficients were statistically significant and positive, demonstrating convergence between the two surveys for that crowd. Assessments for discriminant validity focused on the remaining correlation coefficients in the matrix, representing the associations between scores for two different crowds. Discriminant validity was indicated if these correlation coefficients were non-significant, statistically significant and negative, or statistically significant and positive but weaker than the convergent validity coefficients for the same crowds.

We assessed predictive ability by using I-Base Survey and V-MLS scores independently as predictors of health behavior, ACEs, resilience, and personal traits via a series of logistic regressions (health behaviors) and linear regressions (all other characteristics). All regressions controlled for age, gender, and race/ethnicity. We compared results for the two instruments to examine the V-MLS's predictive ability compared to the established I-Base Survey for these characteristics that are key to health communication campaign tailoring. This allowed us to understand if the V-MLS successfully segmented teens into distinct crowds with unique profiles that could be effectively reached with a uniquely tailored and targeted campaign.

Results

Sample descriptives are presented in Table 2. The racial/ethnic distribution of the sample mirrored census data for Virginia, and just over half identified as female. The proportion of the sample scoring highest for each peer crowd was largely similar between the I-Base Survey and V-MLS. Health behavior responses mirrored those reported in the 2019 VYS (104).

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Participant characteristics.

We assessed convergent and discriminant validity using a multi-trait multi-method matrix (Table 3). Reliability, shown in the matrix diagonal, was consistently higher for the V-MLS than the I-Base Survey, with McDonald's ω values for the new survey (0.56–0.77) falling in or near the acceptable range for scales with fewer than 10 items (0.60 or higher) (105). Correlations between I-Base Survey and V-MLS scores for a given peer crowd were statistically significant and positive for all five crowds (convergent validity), ranging from 0.17 (Hip Hop) to 0.55 (Country). With one exception, all other correlations in the matrix (discriminant validity) were negative, not statistically significant, or positive but weaker than the corresponding convergent validity correlations. The correlation coefficient between I-Base Survey Hip Hop score and V-MLS Popular score was 0.18 (p < 0.001), which was weaker than the coefficient between the two Popular scores (0.26, p < 0.001) but marginally stronger than the coefficient between the two Hip Hop scores (0.17, p < 0.001). Overall, the V-MLS demonstrated convergent and discriminant validity.

TABLE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. Multi-trait multi-method matrix for I-Base Survey and V-MLS.

To examine predictive ability, we conducted a series of regressions using I-Base Survey and V-MLS peer crowd scores to separately predict behavior, ACEs, resilience, and personal traits while controlling for demographics (Table 4). Results for the two surveys largely aligned. Higher Hip Hop scores on both surveys were associated with significantly increased odds of current LCC, vape, and marijuana use, and higher ACE scores. For Alternative, higher scores on both surveys were associated with significantly increased odds of current marijuana use and feeling sad or hopeless; reduced odds of current smokeless tobacco use; higher ACE and sensation-seeking scores; and lower resilience scores. Higher Popular scores on both surveys were associated with increased odds of current alcohol use, higher SPI scores, and lower ACE scores. For Country, higher scores on both surveys were associated with significantly increased odds of current cigarette and smokeless tobacco use, reduced odds of feeling sad or hopeless, and lower sensation-seeking scores. Higher Mainstream scores on both surveys were associated with significantly reduced odds for all behaviors except feeling sad, and with lower ACE, SPI, and sensation-seeking scores. Similar patterns across surveys demonstrated that the V-MLS has sufficient capability to predict key characteristics associated with peer crowds that should inform targeted health communication campaign development.

TABLE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Table 4. Peer crowd scores as predictors of behavior, experiences, and personal traits.

Discussion

The current manuscript describes validation and predictive ability testing for the new Mindset Lens Survey in Virginia, a mindset-based teen peer crowd segmentation survey that groups teens into crowds with distinct behavioral and psychographic characteristics. The V-MLS represents an evolution in peer crowd measurement for health communications segmentation as it more directly segments youth based on the values and mindsets that may influence peer crowd behavior. By using psychographics to segment teens into groups with real-world social influence, the V-MLS provides health marketers with the audience insights necessary to create targeted, tailored health campaigns that can more equitably address disparities.

The V-MLS demonstrated reliability and convergent and discriminant validity, elements that to our knowledge have not been reported for existing peer crowd measurement tools. Importantly, peer crowd risk profiles from the V-MLS align with existing literature, further reinforcing the validity of the new tool. Specifically, associations previously reported between the Alternative peer crowd and marijuana use, mental health struggles, and ACEs; the Country peer crowd and smokeless tobacco; the Hip Hop peer crowd and tobacco use, substance use, and ACEs; the Mainstream peer crowd and reduced risk across behaviors; and the Popular peer crowd and vaping, alcohol, and reduced ACEs were also observed when using the V-MLS (11, 28, 34, 35, 40, 41, 45, 47).

As with any new instrument, while the V-MLS demonstrates strong reliability and validity, it does not exactly match results derived from existing methods. In the regressions assessing predictive ability, there were several instances in which one of the survey tools produced a statistically significant result while the other did not. In most of these cases, however, the non-significant result trended in the same direction as the significant result, indicating the disparity may be due to power limitations. Generally, results of regression analyses indicate that the V-MLS is effective at identifying crowds with distinct behaviors, experiences, and traits, information that can inform health communication campaign development. In fact, results may indicate that we created a survey that more directly taps into the core psychographic characteristics that differentiate peer crowds, compared to indirect methods. Upcoming research efforts applying the instrument to representative teen surveys in Virginia and other states will allow us to further explore this issue and to demonstrate its generalizability to U.S. states beyond Virginia.

Based on the strength of our findings, we believe the V-MLS represents progress in peer crowd measurement methods and will be a useful tool for health communication campaign segmentation. The instrument is brief, acceptable to respondents, and can easily be appended to ongoing data collections such as representative surveys to inform audience segmentation. It allows youth to pick a primary and secondary response for each question, adding depth to our understanding by providing insight into the intensity of agreement with the values and the peer crowds they represent. Additionally, the V-MLS brings researchers one step closer to directly measuring the core characteristics that differentiate peer crowds—values, mindsets, and worldviews—making it a more efficient means of gathering insightful peer crowd data. The V-MLS provides information critical to effective health communication by revealing the values and mindsets that characterize each peer crowd and that should be incorporated into message framing for a peer crowd-targeted campaign (9, 14, 83). Per the mindsponge model, the values that pass through a teen's filters and comfort zone to reach their core beliefs will influence their behavior and reinforce acceptance of similar values and mindsets in the future (23, 24). By framing health messages based on the values and interests shared by teens within higher-risk audience segments, campaigns can increase the effectiveness of their messages through greater tuning in from the right audience, stronger message receptivity once processed, and ultimately a greater impact on behavior (9). These improvements will increase a campaign's ability to persuade those at higher risk, making the campaign's outcomes more equitable.

While critical to successful message development, information provided by the V-MLS does not address other important elements of campaign development such as the look, style, and feel of the campaign or the media channels where content should be placed (9, 14). As such, we believe that peer crowd visual cue data such as that provided by the I-Base Survey or qualitative data collection remains important to inform the style of campaign ads and actors, while data on media channel preferences should also be collected to ensure tailored content efficiently reaches the target crowd (83). Together, these data can provide the detailed portrait of teen peer crowds necessary to inform targeted health communication campaigns. Additionally, health communication campaigns should still rely on information processing frameworks such as 3D creativity management theory or prototype-willingness model, and campaign design frameworks such as Social Branding or social marketing benchmark criteria to translate audience insights into effective campaigns (14, 63, 106108).

Limitations

We have several limitations to note. Because data come from a convenience sample of Virginia teens, results may not generalize beyond the current sample or state. However, given consistent similarities in peer crowds observed across states (911, 37, 41), we anticipate the instrument will perform effectively in other U.S. states. Cross-cultural adaptation to reflect local crowds and crowd values would be necessary to ensure applicability to other countries, where crowds and their interests may differ (29, 31, 32, 39, 4447, 49). Some McDonald's ω values for the V-MLS were lower than typically expected for validated instruments, though still acceptable given the small number of survey items included in the final instrument and the instrument's design (105). In order to complete our validation analyses, we had to compare the V-MLS to an existing peer crowd measurement tool and selected the I-Base Survey for this task. Though the I-Base Survey does not have published validation results, we felt this was the best available comparison point as no “gold standard” instrument exists and the I-Base Survey has been used extensively with consistent findings, demonstrating applied validity (11, 13, 36, 37, 41, 82). As our data were cross-sectional we could not track changes in values over time, but future work could seek to extend the application of the mindsponge model to peer crowds by exploring how peer crowd values enter and leave a teen's core beliefs and the impact this has on behavior.

We observed a significant, positive association between I-Base Survey Hip Hop score and V-MLS Popular score, which was marginally stronger than the association between the two Hip Hop scores. Additionally, the correlation between the two Hip Hop scores was relatively lower than other intra-crowd correlations. We believe this reflects the increasing overlap between the Hip Hop and Popular crowds—particularly in terms of personal style as captured via the I-Base Survey—which has emerged due to the rapid increase in popularity of hip hop music and culture in the past decade (109). As the Popular crowd are trendsetters who adopt the latest fashion and music styles, the rise of hip hop culture's popularity has blurred stylistic differences between the crowds, but may not have impacted crowd values and mindsets. We feel this concern is outweighed by the strong predictive power of V-MLS scores for Hip Hop and Popular. Combined with the instrument's focus on values and mindsets that directly impact behavior, findings may indicate the V-MLS more effectively differentiates these two related crowds than prior instruments.

We also must acknowledge that our approach was more confirmatory than exploratory. We did not start with a blank slate and rely on a methodology such as cluster analysis to create new crowds. Instead, we started with a concept of five teen peer crowds based on the literature. We believe this approach was warranted as we aimed to improve upon available methods rather than reinvent decades of convergent research. Additionally, during early qualitative data collection we included participants who did not fall neatly into a single existing peer crowd to allow for the potential emergence of new crowds. However, evidence did not support adding a new crowd to those previously observed. Our chosen approach ensured that the V-MLS identifies groups of youth who not only share characteristics but also influence each others' behavior through social norms, a critical feature if the instrument is to be useful in health communications segmentation. A cluster analysis would not have guaranteed the creation of crowds with meaningful shared norms and culture, which led us to adopt the approach described herein.

Conclusions

This manuscript describes the validity and predictive ability of Virginia's Mindset Lens Survey, a new mindset-based teen peer crowd segmentation survey that complements existing methods to provide audience insights necessary to create peer crowd-targeted health communication campaigns. The V-MLS addresses many of the shortcomings of previous peer crowd measurement tools when applied to audience segmentation, and demonstrates reliability and validity. The survey is a brief and practical instrument that can be appended to surveillance surveys and, when combined with relevant information processing and campaign design frameworks, can inform the design and tailoring of health campaigns for higher-risk teens alongside complementary instruments providing visual cue and media use data. Future research seeks to replicate and extend findings in representative samples in Virginia and other U.S. states, explore cross-cultural adaptation opportunities, and apply the survey to real-world health communication campaign development.

Data availability statement

The raw data supporting the conclusions of this article will be made available by the authors, without undue reservation.

Ethics statement

The studies involving human participants were reviewed and approved by Advarra IRB. Written informed consent from the participants' legal guardian/next of kin was not required to participate in this study in accordance with the national legislation and the institutional requirements.

Author contributions

All authors conceived of and designed the research study. CS and MD implemented the research. CS analyzed the data and wrote the first draft. All authors contributed to article revisions and approved the submitted version.

Funding

This research was funded by the Virginia Foundation for Healthy Youth.

Acknowledgments

The authors would like to thank Priscilla Fernandez, Erika Redke, Jensen Saintilien, Hadley Scharer, Rebeca Mahr, Lisa Ott, Delaney Kipple, Molly Barry, Deepna Anand, and Molly Moran for their assistance with data collection, and Shiloh Beckerley for her feedback on an early draft of the manuscript.

Conflict of interest

Authors CS, JJ, and MD are employed by Rescue Agency, a research and campaign contractor for the Virginia Foundation for Healthy Youth, the study sponsor and employer of authors DS and HB. Rescue Agency holds the proprietorship and trademark for the I-Base Survey and Mindset Lens Survey used in this manuscript. As employees of the funding agency, the Virginia Foundation for Healthy Youth, DS and HB were involved in study design and manuscript writing.

Publisher's note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

Supplementary material

The Supplementary Material for this article can be found online at: https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fpubh.2022.871864/full#supplementary-material

Abbreviations

ACE, adverse childhood experiences; AOR, adjusted odds ratio; API, Asian-Pacific Islander; BRS, Brief Resilience Scale; BSSS, Brief Sensation Seeking Scale; LCC, little cigars and cigarillos; NH, non-Hispanic; SPI, Social Prioritization Index; V-MLS, Virginia's Mindset Lens Survey; VYS, Virginia Youth Survey; YRBS, Youth Risk Behavior Survey.

References

1. Slater MD. Theory and method in health audience segmentation. J Health Commun. (1996) 1:267–84. doi: 10.1080/108107396128059

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

2. Boslaugh SE, Kreuter MW, Nicholson RA, Naleid K. Comparing demographic, health status and psychosocial strategies of audience segmentation to promote physical activity. Health Educ Res. (2005) 20:430–8. doi: 10.1093/her/cyg138

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

3. Wills J, Crichton N, Lorenc A, Kelly M. Using population segmentation to inform local obesity strategy in England. Health Promot Int. (2015) 30:658–66. doi: 10.1093/heapro/dau004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

4. Schuster L, Kubacki K, Rundle-Thiele S. A theoretical approach to segmenting children's walking behaviour. Young Consum. (2015) 16:159–71. doi: 10.1108/YC-07-2014-00461

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

5. Kitunen A, Rundle-Thiele S, Kadir M, Badejo A, Zdanowicz G, Price M. Learning what our target audiences think and do: extending segmentation to all four bases. BMC Public Health. (2019) 19:382. doi: 10.1186/s12889-019-6696-2

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

6. Kazbare L, van Trijp HCM, Eskildsen JK. A-priori and post-hoc segmentation in the design of healthy eating campaigns. J Mark Commun. (2010) 16:21–45. doi: 10.1080/13527260903342712

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

7. Maibach EW, Maxfield A, Ladin K, Slater M. Translating health psychology into effective health communication: the American healthstyles audience segmentation project. J Health Psychol. (1996) 1:261–77. doi: 10.1177/135910539600100302

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

8. Hardcastle SJ, Hagger MS. Psychographic profiling for effective health behavior change interventions. Front Psychol. (2016) 6:1988. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2015.01988

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

9. Moran MB, Walker MW, Alexander TN, Jordan JW, Wagner DE. Why peer crowds matter: incorporating youth subcultures and values in health education campaigns. Am J Public Health. (2017) 107:389–95. doi: 10.2105/AJPH.2016.303595

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

10. Sussman S, Pokhrel P, Ashmore RD, Brown BB. Adolescent peer group identification and characteristics: a review of the literature. Addict Behav. (2007) 32:1602–27. doi: 10.1016/j.addbeh.2006.11.018

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

11. Jordan JW, Stalgaitis CA, Charles J, Madden PA, Radhakrishnan AG, Saggese D. Peer crowd identification and adolescent health behaviors: results from a statewide representative study. Health Educ Behav. (2019) 46:40–52. doi: 10.1177/1090198118759148

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

12. Blakemore S-J, Mills K. Is adolescence a sensitive period for sociocultural processing? Annu Rev Psychol. (2014) 65:187–207. doi: 10.1146/annurev-psych-010213-115202

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

13. Ling PM, Holmes LM, Jordan JW, Lisha NE, Bibbins-Domingo K. Bars, nightclubs, and cancer prevention: new approaches to reduce young adult cigarette smoking. Am J Prev Med. (2017) 53:S78–85. doi: 10.1016/j.amepre.2017.03.026

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

14. Stalgaitis CA, Jordan JW, Djakaria M, Saggese DJ. Social branding framework to address smoking among alternative peer crowd teens. J Soc Mark. (2021) 11:189–203. doi: 10.1108/JSOCM-02-2020-0021

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

15. Brown BB, Mory MS, Kinney D. “Casting Adolescent Crowds in a Relational Perspective: Caricature, Channel, and Context,” In: Montemayo R, Adams GR, Gullotta TP, editors. Personal Relationships During Adolescence. Thousand Oaks, CA: SAGE Publications (1994). p. 123–67.

Google Scholar

16. Dolcini MM, Adler NE. Perceived competencies, peer group affiliation, and risk behavior among early adolescents. Health Psychol. (1994) 13:496–506. doi: 10.1037/0278-6133.13.6.496

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

17. Stone MR, Barber BL, Eccles JS. We knew them when: sixth grade characteristics that predict adolescent high school social identities. J Early Adolesc. (2008) 28:304–28. doi: 10.1177/0272431607312743

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

18. Mackey ER, La Greca AM. Does this make me look fat? Peer crowd and peer contributions to adolescent girls' weight control behaviors. J Youth Adolesc. (2008) 37:1097–110. doi: 10.1007/s10964-008-9299-2

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

19. Simons-Morton BG, Farhat T. Recent findings on peer group influences on adolescent smoking. J Prim Prev. (2010) 31:191–208. doi: 10.1007/s10935-010-0220-x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

20. Brown BB, Von Bank H, Steinberg L. Smoke in the looking glass: effects of discordance between self- and peer rated crowd affiliation on adolescent anxiety, depression and self-feelings. J Youth Adolesc. (2008) 37:1163–77. doi: 10.1007/s10964-007-9198-y

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

21. Berger J, Rand L. Shifting signals to help health: using identity signaling to reduce risky health behaviors. J Consum Res. (2008) 35:509–18. doi: 10.1086/587632

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

22. In: Turner JC, Hogg MA, Oakes PJ, Reicher SD, Wetherell MS editors. Rediscovering the Social Group: A Self-Categorization Theory. Oxford, UK: Basil Blackwell (1987).

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

23. Vuong QH, Napier NK. Acculturation and global mindsponge: An emerging market perspective. Int J Intercult Relat. (2015) 49:354–67. doi: 10.1016/j.ijintrel.2015.06.003

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

24. Nguyen MH, Le TT, Nguyen HKT, Ho MT, Nguyen HTT, Vuong QH. Alice in suicideland: exploring the suicidal ideation mechanism through the sense of connectedness and help-seeking behaviors. Int J Environ Res Public Health. (2021) 18:3681. doi: 10.3390/ijerph18073681

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

25. Arnett JJ. Emerging adulthood: what is it, and what is it good for? Child Dev Perspect. (2007) 1:68–73. doi: 10.1111/j.1750-8606.2007.00016.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

26. Brechwald WA, Prinstein MJ. Beyond homophily: a decade of advances in understanding peer influence processes. J Res Adolesc. (2011) 21:166–79. doi: 10.1111/j.1532-7795.2010.00721.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

27. Erikson EH. Identity: Youth and Crisis. New York: WW Norton & Co. (1994).

Google Scholar

28. Barber BL, Eccles JS., Stone MR. Whatever happened to the Jock, the Brain, and the Princess?: Young adult pathways linked to adolescent activity involvement and social identity. J Adolesc Res. (2001) 16:429–55. doi: 10.1177/0743558401165002

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

29. Bešić N, Kerr M. Punks, goths, and other eye-catching peer crowds: do they fulfill a function for shy youths? J Res Adolesc. (2009) 19:113–21. doi: 10.1111/j.1532-7795.2009.00584.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

30. Daddis C. Adolescent peer crowds and patterns of belief in the boundaries of personal authority. J Adolesc. (2010) 33:699–708. doi: 10.1016/j.adolescence.2009.11.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

31. Delsing MJMH. ter Bogt TFM, Engels RCME, Meeus WHJ. Adolescents' peer crowd identification in the Netherlands: structure and associations with problem behaviors. J Res Adolesc. (2007) 17:467–80. doi: 10.1111/j.1532-7795.2007.00530.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

32. Doornwaard SM, Branje S, Meeus WHJ. ter Bogt TFM. Development of adolescents' peer crowd identification in relation to changes in problem behaviors. Dev Psychol. (2012) 48:1366–80. doi: 10.1037/a0026994

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

33. Fleischman K, Hains AA. Peer crowd affiliation, adherence, perceived support, and metabolic control in T1DM youth. J Child Health Care. (2021) 25:240–52. doi: 10.1177/1367493520924875

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

34. Fuqua JL, Gallaher PE, Unger JB, Trinidad DR, Sussman S, Ortega E, et al. Multiple peer group self-identification and adolescent tobacco use. Subst Use Misuse. (2012) 47:757–66. doi: 10.3109/10826084.2011.608959

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

35. La Greca AM, Prinstein MJ, Fetter MD. Adolescent peer crowd affiliation: linkages with health-risk behaviors and close friendships. J Pediatr Psychol. (2001) 26:131–43. doi: 10.1093/jpepsy/26.3.131

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

36. Lee YO, Jordan JW, Djakaria M, Ling PM. Using peer crowds to segment black youth for smoking intervention. Health Promot Pract. (2014) 15:530–7. doi: 10.1177/1524839913484470

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

37. Navarro MA, Stalgaitis CA, Walker MW, Wagner DE. Youth peer crowds and risk of cigarette use: the effects of dual peer crowd identification among Hip Hop youth. Addict Behav Rep. (2019) 10:100204. doi: 10.1016/j.abrep.2019.100204

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

38. Prinstein MJ, La Greca AM. Peer crowd affiliation and internalizing distress in childhood and adolescence: a longitudinal follow-back study. J Res Adolesc. (2002) 12:325–51. doi: 10.1111/1532-7795.00036

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

39. Sim TN, Yeo GH. Peer crowds in Singapore. Youth Soc. (2012) 44:201–16. doi: 10.1177/0044118X10388220

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

40. Stalgaitis CA, Wagner DE, Djakaria M, Jordan JW. Understanding adversity and peer crowds to prevent youth health risks. Am J Health Behav. (2019) 43:767–80. doi: 10.5993/AJHB.43.4.10

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

41. Stalgaitis CA, Navarro MA, Wagner DE, Walker MW. Who uses tobacco products? Using peer crowd segmentation to identify youth at risk for cigarettes, cigar products, hookah, and e-cigarettes. Subst Use Misuse. (2020) 55:1045–53. doi: 10.1080/10826084.2020.1722698

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

42. Sussman S, Dent CW, Stacy AW, Burciaga C, Raynor A, Turner GE, et al. Peer-group association and adolescent tobacco use. J Abnorm Psychol. (1990) 99:349–52. doi: 10.1037/0021-843X.99.4.349

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

43. Urberg KA, Degirmencioglu SM, Tolson JM, Halliday-Scher K. Adolescent social crowds: measurement and relationship to friendships. J Adolesc Res. (2000) 15:427–45. doi: 10.1177/0743558400154001

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

44. van der Rijt GAJ, Haenens LSJ, van Straten P. Smoking and other substance use as distinct features of teenage subcultures. J Adolesc Health. (2002) 31:433–5. doi: 10.1016/S1054-139X[02]00394-4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

45. Verkooijen KT, de Vries NK, Nielsen GA. Youth crowds and substance use: the impact of perceived group norm and multiple group identification. Psychol Addict Behav. (2007) 21:55–61. doi: 10.1037/0893-164X.21.1.55

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

46. Young R, Sproeber N, Groschwitz RC, Preiss M, Plener PL. Why alternative teenagers self-harm: exploring the link between non-suicidal self-injury, attempted suicide and adolescent identity. BMC Psychiatry. (2014) 14:137. doi: 10.1186/1471-244X-14-137

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

47. Young R, Sweeting H, West P. Prevalence of deliberate self harm and attempted suicide within contemporary Goth youth subculture: longitudinal cohort study. BMJ. (2006) 332:1058–61. doi: 10.1136/bmj.38790.495544.7C

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

48. Staten LK, Birnbaum AS, Jobe JB, Elder JP. A typology of middle school girls: audience segmentation related to physical activity. Health Educ Behav. (2006) 33:66–80. doi: 10.1177/1090198105282419

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

49. Brady P. Jocks, Teckers. Nerds: the role of the adolescent peer group in the formation and maintenance of secondary school institutional culture. Discourse Stud Cult Polit Educ. (2004) 25:351–64. doi: 10.1080/0159630042000247926

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

50. Garner R, Bootcheck J, Lorr M, Rauch K. The adolescent society revisited: cultures, crowds, climates, and status structures in seven secondary schools. J Youth Adolesc. (2006) 35:1023–35. doi: 10.1007/s10964-006-9060-7

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

51. Kinney DA. From “headbangers” to “hippies”: delineating adolescents' active attempts to form an alternative peer culture. New Dir Child Adolesc Dev. (1999) 84:21–35. doi: 10.1002/cd.23219998404

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

52. Sussman S, Dent CW, McCullar WJ. Group self-identification as a prospective predictor of drug use and violence in high-risk youth. Psychol Addict Behav. (2000) 14:192–6. doi: 10.1037/0893-164X.14.2.192

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

53. Pivnick LK, Gordon RA, Crosnoe R. Crowd sourcing: do peer crowd prototypes match reality? Soc Psychol Q. (2020) 83:272–93. doi: 10.1177/0190272520936228

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

54. Stapleton J, Turrisi R, Hillhouse J. Peer crowd identification and indoor artificial UV tanning behavioral tendencies. J Health Psychol. (2008) 13:940–5. doi: 10.1177/1359105308095068

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

55. Moran MB, Murphy ST, Sussman S. Campaigns and cliques: variations in effectiveness of an antismoking campaign as a function of adolescent peer group identity. J Health Commun. (2012) 17:1215–31. doi: 10.1080/10810730.2012.688246

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

56. Moran MB, Sussman S. Translating the link between social identity and health behavior into effective health communication strategies: an experimental application using antismoking advertisements. Health Commun. (2014) 29:1057–66. doi: 10.1080/10410236.2013.832830

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

57. Moran MB, Sussman S. Changing attitudes toward smoking and smoking susceptibility through peer crowd targeting: more evidence from a controlled study. Health Commun. (2015) 30:521–4. doi: 10.1080/10410236.2014.902008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

58. Wagner DE, Fernandez P, Jordan JW, Saggese DJ. Freedom from chew: using social branding to reduce chewing tobacco use among country peer crowd teens. Health Educ Behav. (2019) 46:286–94. doi: 10.1177/1090198118806966

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

59. Guillory J, Henes A, Farrelly MC, Fiacco L, Alam I, Curry L, et al. Awareness of and receptivity to the fresh empire tobacco public education campaign among hip hop youth. J Adolesc Health. (2020) 66:301–7. doi: 10.1016/j.jadohealth.2019.09.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

60. Ling PM, Lisha NE, Neilands TB, Jordan JW. Join the commune: a controlled study of social branding influencers to decrease smoking among young adult hipsters. Am J Health Promot. (2020) 34:754–61. doi: 10.1177/0890117120904917

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

61. Kalkhoran S, Lisha NE, Neilands TB, Jordan JW, Ling PM. Evaluation of bar and nightclub intervention to decrease young adult smoking in New Mexico. J Adolesc Health. (2016) 59:222–9. doi: 10.1016/j.jadohealth.2016.04.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

62. Fallin A, Neilands TB, Jordan JW, Hong JS, Ling PM. Wreaking “havoc” on smoking: social branding to reach young adult “partiers” in Oklahoma. Am J Prev Med. (2015) 48:S78–85. doi: 10.1016/j.amepre.2014.09.008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

63. Ling PM, Lee YO, Hong J, Neilands TB, Jordan JW, Glantz SA. Social branding to decrease smoking among young adults in bars. Am J Public Health. (2014) 104:751–60. doi: 10.2105/AJPH.2013.301666

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

64. Cross JR, Fletcher KL. The challenge of adolescent crowd research: defining the crowd. J Youth Adolesc. (2009) 38:747–64. doi: 10.1007/s10964-008-9307-6

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

65. Buff SA. “Greasers, Dupers and Hippies: Three Responses to the Adult World,” In: Howe LK, editor. The White Majority. New York, NY: Random House (1970). p. 60–77.

Google Scholar

66. Larkin RW. Suburban Youth in Cultural Crisis. New York, NY: Oxford University Press. (1979).

Google Scholar

67. Varenne H. “Jocks and Freaks: The Symbolic Structure of the Expression of Social Interaction among American Senior High School Students,” In: Spindler G, editor. Doing the Ethnography of Schooling. New York, NY: Rinehart & Winston (1982). p. 210–35.

Google Scholar

68. Eckert P. Jocks & Burnouts: Social Categories and Identity in the High School. New York, NY: Teachers College. (1989).

Google Scholar

69. Mosbach P, Leventhal H. Peer group identification and smoking: implications for intervention. J Abnorm Psychol. (1988) 97:238–45. doi: 10.1037/0021-843X.97.2.238

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

70. Van Zalk N, Van Zalk MHW, Kerr M. Socialization of social anxiety in adolescent crowds. J Abnorm Child Psychol. (2011) 39:1239–49. doi: 10.1007/s10802-011-9533-3

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

71. Mak A, Heaven P, Rummery A. The role of group identity and personality domains as indicators of self-reported delinquency. Psychol Crime Law. (2003) 9:9–18. doi: 10.1080/10683160308144

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

72. Chinopfukutwa VS, Hektner JM. Peer crowd affiliations as predictors of prosocial and risky behaviors among college students. J Am Coll Health. (2022) 70:1231–40. doi: 10.1080/07448481.2020.1790574

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

73. Hopmeyer A, Medovoy T. Emerging adults' self-identified peer crowd affiliations, risk behavior, and social–emotional adjustment in college. Emerg Adulthood. (2017) 5:143–8. doi: 10.1177/2167696816665055

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

74. Hopmeyer A, Troop-Gordon W, Medovoy T, Fischer J. Emerging adults' self-identified peer crowd affiliations and college adjustment. Soc Psychol Educ. (2017) 20:643–67. doi: 10.1007/s11218-017-9390-1

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

75. Wax A, Hopmeyer A, Dulay PN, Medovoy T. Commuter college student adjustment: peer crowd affiliation as a driver of loneliness, belongingness, and risk behaviors. Emerg Adulthood. (2019) 7:363–9. doi: 10.1177/2167696818781128

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

76. Kuh GD, Hu S, Vesper N. “They shall be known by what they do”: an activities-based typology of college students. J Coll Stud Dev. (2000) 41:228–44.

Google Scholar

77. Sessa FM. Peer crowds in a commuter college sample: the relation between self-reported alcohol use and perceived peer crowd norms. J Psychol. (2007) 141:293–306. doi: 10.3200/JRLP.141.3.293-306

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

78. Strouse DL. Adolescent crowd orientations: a social and temporal analysis. New Dir Child Adolesc Dev. (1999) 84:37–54. doi: 10.1002/cd.23219998405

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

79. Youniss J, Mclellan JA, Mazer B. Voluntary service, peer group orientation, and civic engagement. J Adolesc Res. (2001) 16:456–68. doi: 10.1177/0743558401165003

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

80. Erskine M, Kier C, Leung A, Sproule R. Peer crowds, work experience, and financial saving behaviour of young Canadians. J Econ Psychol. (2006) 27:262–84. doi: 10.1016/j.joep.2005.05.005

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

81. Wagner DE, Walker M, Stalgaitis CA, Hoffman L, Garibay L, Alexander T, et al. Measuring peer crowds and peer crowd tobacco use. In: Society for Personality and Social Psychology Annual Convention Conference. San Diego, CA: Society for Personality and Social Psychology (2016).

Google Scholar

82. Lisha NE, Jordan JW, Ling PM. Peer crowd affiliation as a segmentation tool for young adult tobacco use. Tob Control. (2016) 25:i83–9. doi: 10.1136/tobaccocontrol-2016-053086

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

83. Stalgaitis CA, Djakaria M, Jordan JW. The vaping teenager: understanding the psychographics and interests of adolescent vape users to inform health communication campaigns. Tob Use Insights. (2020) 13:1–15. doi: 10.1177/1179173X20945695

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

84. Lee YO, Curry LE, Fiacco L, Henes A, Farrelly MC, Nonnemaker JM, et al. Peer crowd segmentation for targeting public education campaigns: Hip Hop youth and tobacco use. Prev Med Rep. (2019) 14:100843. doi: 10.1016/j.pmedr.2019.100843

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

85. Walker MW, Navarro MA, Hoffman L, Wagner DE, Stalgaitis CA, Jordan JW. The Hip Hop peer crowd: an opportunity for intervention to reduce tobacco use among at-risk youth. Addict Behav. (2018) 82:28–34. doi: 10.1016/j.addbeh.2018.02.014

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

86. Liu H, Yu S, Cottrell L, Lunn S, Deveaux L, Brathwaite NV, et al. Personal values and involvement in problem behaviors among Bahamian early adolescents: a cross-sectional study. BMC Public Health. (2007) 7:135. doi: 10.1186/1471-2458-7-135

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

87. Borca G, Rabaglietti E, Roggero A, Keller P, Haak E, Begotti T. Personal values as a mediator of relations between perceived parental support and control and youth substance use. Subst Use Misuse. (2017) 52:1589–601. doi: 10.1080/10826084.2017.1293103

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

88. Yasuma N, Watanabe K, Matsunaga A, Nishi D, Kawakami N. Personal values in adolescence and suicidality: a cross-sectional study based on a retrospective recall. BMC Psychiatry. (2019) 19:214. doi: 10.1186/s12888-019-2194-4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

89. Szakály Z, Balogh P, Jasák H, Szabó S, Szente V. The influence of personal values on the transition towards health-conscious diet: a segmentation approach. J Food Nutr Res. (2014) 2:476–84. doi: 10.12691/jfnr-2-8-8

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

90. Campbell DT, Fiske DW. Convergent and discriminant validation by the multitrait-multimethod matrix. Psychol Bull. (1959) 56:81–105. doi: 10.1037/h0046016

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

91. Carmines EG, Zeller RA. Reliability and Validity Assessment. Thousand Oaks, CA: SAGE Publications. (1979).

Google Scholar

92. Virginia Department of Health. 2019 Virginia High School Youth Survey (2019). https://www.vdh.virginia.gov/content/uploads/sites/69/2019/09/2019VAH-with-IBase.pdf (accessed Feb 2, 2021).

Google Scholar

93. Boness CL, Sher KJ. The case for cognitive interviewing in survey item validation: a useful approach for improving the measurement and assessment of substance use disorders. J Stud Alcohol Drugs. (2020) 81:401–4. doi: 10.15288/jsad.2020.81.401

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

94. Bellis MA, Hughes K, Ford K, Hardcastle KA, Sharp CA, Wood S, et al. Adverse childhood experiences and sources of childhood resilience: a retrospective study of their combined relationships with child health and educational attendance. BMC Public Health. (2018) 18:792. doi: 10.1186/s12889-018-5699-8

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

95. Hughes K, Bellis MA, Hardcastle KA, Sethi D, Butchart A, Mikton C, et al. The effect of multiple adverse childhood experiences on health: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Lancet Public Health. (2017) 2:e356–66. doi: 10.1016/S2468-2667(17)30118-4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

96. Duke NN. Adolescent adversity and concurrent tobacco, alcohol, and marijuana use. Am J Health Behav. (2018) 42:85–99. doi: 10.5993/AJHB.42.5.8

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

97. Smith BW, Dalen J, Wiggins K, Tooley E, Christopher P, Bernard J. The brief resilience scale: assessing the ability to bounce back. Int J Behav Med. (2008) 15:194–200. doi: 10.1080/10705500802222972

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

98. Lisha NE, Neilands TB, Jordan JW, Holmes LM, Ling PM. The social prioritization index and tobacco use among young adult bar patrons. Health Educ Behav. (2016) 43:641–7. doi: 10.1177/1090198115621867

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

99. Hoyle RH, Stephenson MT, Palmgreen P, Lorch EP, Donohew RL. Reliability and validity of a brief measure of sensation seeking. Personal Individ Differ. (2002) 32:401–14. doi: 10.1016/S0191-8869(01)00032-0

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

100. Patrick ME, Schulenberg JE. Prevalence and predictors of adolescent alcohol use and binge drinking in the United States. Alcohol Res Curr Rev. (2013) 35:193–200.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

101. Wellman RJ, Dugas EN, Dutczak H, O'Loughlin EK, Datta GD, Lauzon B, et al. Predictors of the onset of cigarette smoking: a systematic review of longitudinal population-based studies in youth. Am J Prev Med. (2016) 51:767–78. doi: 10.1016/j.amepre.2016.04.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

102. McDonald RP. Test Theory: A Unified Treatment. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum. (1999).

Google Scholar

103. Hayes AF, Coutts JJ. Use Omega rather than Cronbach's Alpha for estimating reliability. But… Commun Methods Meas. (2020) 14:1–24. doi: 10.1080/19312458.2020.1718629

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

104. Virginia Department of Health. 2019 Youth Risk Behavior Survey Results: Virginia High School Survey, Summary Tables - Weighted Data (2020). Available online at: https://www.vdh.virginia.gov/content/uploads/sites/69/2020/06/2019VAH-Summary-Tables.pdf (accessed February 3, 2021).

Google Scholar

105. Loewenthal KM, Lewis CA. An Introduction to Psychological Tests and Scales. 3rd ed London, UK: Routledge. (2018).

Google Scholar

106. French J, Blair-Stevens C. “Key Concepts and Principles of Social Marketing,” In: French J, Blair-Stevens C, McVey D, Merritt R, editors. Social Marketing and Public Health: Theory and Practice. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press (2010). p. 29–43.

Google Scholar

107. Vuong QH, Le TT, La VP, Nguyen HTT, Ho MT, Van Khuc Q, et al. Covid-19 vaccines production and societal immunization under the serendipity-mindsponge-3D knowledge management theory and conceptual framework. Humanit Soc Sci Commun. (2022) 9:1–12. doi: 10.1057/s41599-022-01034-6

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

108. Gerrard M, Gibbons FX, Houlihan AE, Stock ML, Pomery EA. A dual-process approach to health risk decision making: the prototype willingness model. Dev Rev. (2008) 28:29–61. doi: 10.1016/j.dr.2007.10.001

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

109. Reuters. Hip hop and R&B surpass rock as biggest U.S. music genre. (2018). Available online at: https://www.reuters.com/article/us-music-2017-idUSKBN1ET258 (accessed March 28, 2021).

Google Scholar

Keywords: peer crowd, adolescent, segmentation, health communication, equity, validation, psychographic

Citation: Stalgaitis CA, Jordan JW, Djakaria M, Saggese DJ and Bruce HR (2022) Psychographic segmentation to identify higher-risk teen peer crowds for health communications: Validation of Virginia's Mindset Lens Survey. Front. Public Health 10:871864. doi: 10.3389/fpubh.2022.871864

Received: 08 February 2022; Accepted: 27 June 2022;
Published: 22 July 2022.

Edited by:

Maria Elena Villar, Florida International University, United States

Reviewed by:

Quan-Hoang Vuong, Phenikaa University, Vietnam
Minh-Hoang Nguyen, Ritsumeikan Asia Pacific University, Japan

Copyright © 2022 Stalgaitis, Jordan, Djakaria, Saggese and Bruce. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Carolyn A. Stalgaitis, Y2Fyb2x5bi5zdGFsZ2FpdGlzQGdtYWlsLmNvbQ==; Jeffrey W. Jordan, amVmZkByZXNjdWVhZ2VuY3kuY29t

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.