Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Public Health, 30 May 2022
Sec. Public Health Policy
This article is part of the Research Topic Recovery, resilience, and innovation of Public Health systems during the COVID-19 pandemic View all 6 articles

Analyzing Risk Communication, Trust, Risk Perception, Negative Emotions, and Behavioral Coping Strategies During the COVID-19 Pandemic in China Using a Structural Equation Model

\nJunwang Gu
Junwang Gu*Rong HeRong HeXuanhui WuXuanhui WuJing TaoJing TaoWenhui YeWenhui YeChunmei WuChunmei Wu
  • School of Public Health and Health Management, Gannan Medical University, Ganzhou, China

Objective: Risk communication and the degree of trust are major factors that affect the public's behavioral coping strategies and play an important role in emergency risk management. However, the internal formation mechanism involved in the public's psychological behavior remains unclear. This study aimed to investigate the association among risk communication, trust, risk perception, negative emotions, and behavioral coping strategies during the coronavirus disease 2019 (COVID-19) pandemic, and to identify and quantify the factors that influence public behavior.

Methods: We launched an online survey through social media from April to July 2020 in China. Relevant data were elicited using a self-designed questionnaire that mainly examined respondent characteristics, risk communication, trust, risk perception, negative emotions, protective coping behavior, and excessive coping behavior in the context of the COVID-19 pandemic. A total of 735 valid responses were obtained. A structural equation model was then used to explore relationship pathways among the components.

Results: The higher the degree of risk communication (β = −0.10, p < 0.05) and trust (β = −0.22, p < 0.001), the lower the public risk perception. Risk communication and trust had a direct effect on public behavioral coping strategies during the COVID-19 pandemic. The higher the level of risk communication (β = 0.14, p < 0.001) or trust (β = 0.48, p < 0.001), the more likely it was that this would encourage the public to adopt protective coping behaviors, while the public was less likely to engage in excessive coping behaviors as the degree of trust increased (β = −0.12, p < 0.01). Risk perception influenced by poor risk communication and trust generated negative emotions (β = 0.31, p < 0.001), and such negative emotions further positively influenced public behavioral coping strategies (whether protective [β = 0.09, p < 0.05] or excessive [β = 0.24, p < 0.001] behaviors).

Conclusion: Risk communication, trust, risk perception, and negative emotions were significantly directly or indirectly related to public behavior. The findings provide useful information for emergency risk management and a theoretical basis for follow-up research on public coping behavior during the COVID-19 pandemic.

Introduction

Coronavirus disease 2019 (COVID-19), a fulminant infectious disease, has been sweeping the globe, causing extensive losses of life and generating significant worldwide concern (1). The COVID-19 pandemic has exposed global weaknesses in infection control practices, especially in the operational efficiency of health emergency services (2). Thus, there is an urgent need to establish a higher-level emergency management system when facing new challenges during such demanding public health emergencies.

Public risk perception (RP) changes with time, which is an important consideration in public health emergencies and risk management decision-making (3), while its role can be easily overlooked in the construction of emergency management systems. RP is defined as the subjective judgment people make about the characteristics and severity of risks (4), which was likely to be influenced by many factors like personal characteristics, health literacy during a crisis such as the COVID-19 pandemic (57).

The COVID-19 pandemic is deeply affecting all aspects of societies, this continuous long-term battle with the pandemic not only poses a huge burden on healthcare but also affects our daily life, social interactions, and work performance (8). COVID-19 information rapidly changes, in response, WHO developed risk communication (RC) and community engagement to help support efforts to promote uniform messaging for COVID-19 crisis (9). RC, according to a WHO guideline, is “the real-time exchange of information, advice and opinions between experts, community leaders, officials and the people who are at risk and is an integral part of any emergency response” (10). Therefore, from a public health perspective, RC is a critical factor for emergency management. It has been reported that RC may have direct and indirect positive effects on protective behaviors (11). Furthermore, RP mediates the relationship between RC and protective behaviors (11). A qualitative study revealed that when public RP was influenced by relevant information, the public's protective behavior was subsequently modified (12). The massively developing social network phenomenon has become the main dissemination platform for COVID-19 information and has attracted significant attention from emergency management agencies. One survey showed that the frequency of media use was positively associated with higher RP (13). Social media has become an important channel by which public could seek and share a staggering amount of health-related information and more so during the outbreak of COVID-19 (14). Social media exposure to COVID-19 information influences the adoption of preventive attitudes and behaviors by shaping RP (15). Informational support from the media plays one of the most critical role in facilitating individuals' adoption of compliance with anti-pandemic behavior (16). While there has been increasing attention to the role of social media during Covid-19 crisis, little is known about how social media can affect RP and public behaviors.

Trust is a key factor that encourages people to comply with public health regulations. One online survey indicated that higher trust in governmental organizations was linked to greater compliance in terms of adopting protective behaviors during the second wave of the Covid-19 outbreak (17). People with high social trust perceive more risks than do those with low social trust (18). Other factors, such as knowledge (6) and gender (7) are also important influencing factors in shaping public RP.

Currently, as part of an overall consideration of risk control, governments from different nations facing the spread of the global COVID-19 pandemic are imposing strict and severe mitigation measures to influence people's behaviors (19, 20). RP has been a major factor affecting people's behavioral coping strategies during the COVID-19 pandemic and has had the greatest impact on adherence to preventive measures (2123). The perception of health risks plays a key role in the response to health emergencies, affecting risk management (4) and individual behaviors (24, 25). Based on prior knowledge or social observation, it could be hypothesized that high-level RP would cause the public to overreact to the crisis, further producing irrational and excessive behaviors, such as hoarding (26), panic buying (27), and a bandwagon effect led by rumors (herding behavior) (28, 29); while low-level RP would not be conducive to the public adopting positive self-protection behaviors, such as face-mask usage (30), social distancing (31, 32), and appropriate hygiene measures (33), leading to an aggravated risk of epidemic spread. Such a hypothesis requires investigation, which we undertook in this study.

The COVID-19 outbreak has brought increasingly psychological distress to the worldwide population (34, 35). Some scholars (3537) confirmed the results and underlying mechanism of studies on the Italian population who reported high concomitance of anxiety symptomatology and sleep disorders during the COVID-19 pandemic, with an increased risk of post-traumatic stress disorder symptoms (PTSD) occurrence. Determining the susceptibility factors affecting the emotional response to COVID-19 is of critical importance for improving psychologically based epidemic-crisis interventions. Research has shown that RP is positively related to depressive states (38), and a higher RP concerning COVID-19 is notably associated with less positive or more negative emotions (NE) (39). NE, such as worry, fear, and anxiety, have been consistently associated with RP and could be significant indicators of RP (40). Studies have shown that people who perceive more risks also report higher levels of anxiety (41). Public RP increases the public's state of anxiety and then increases their willingness to panic buy (29). The above research suggests that RP may have a direct effect on public emotion during the current pandemic, and that COVID-19-related NE may be a key explanatory factor linking public behavioral coping strategies and RP.

Over recent decades, many behavioral risk management studies have focused on external influencing factors, such as RC, trust, and personal characteristics, but have not elucidated the internal mechanisms involved. Therefore, to further explore the internal formation mechanism involved in the public's psychological behavior during the COVID-19 pandemic, a relationship model comprising influencing factors such as public RP, NE, and behavioral coping strategies was constructed using a structural equation model (SEM) (Figure 1).

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Structural equation model hypothesis. RC, Risk communication; DT, degree of trust; RP, risk perception; NE, negative emotions; PCB, protective coping behavior; ECB, excessive coping behavior.

Accordingly, we will assess the relationships among influencing factors such as RC, trust, public RP, NE, and behavioral coping strategies (i.e., protective coping behavior [PCB], excessive coping behavior [ECB]); the mediating effect of NE on RP and behavioral coping strategies; and the effects of these influencing factors on public behavioral coping strategies when faced with COVID-19 from a psychological perspective (11, 17, 18, 21, 39). Our study is intended to provide a theoretical basis for better targeted epidemic-crisis interventions.

Materials and Methods

Respondents

We launched an online survey through social media, including Tencent WeChat or QQ and Sina Weibo, that was available from April to July 2020 in China. A total of 781 questionnaires were distributed, and incomplete or erroneously completed questionnaires were excluded. Finally, 735 valid responses were obtained with an efficiency recovery rate of 94%.

Questionnaire

Data were elicited using a self-designed questionnaire in line with the relevant literature. The questionnaire consisted of three parts: (1) basic personal information of the respondents who participated in the survey, (2) each measurement scale, and (3) other items such as risk communication mediums. After the questionnaire was piloted, some questions were excluded.

The study protocol was approved by the ethics review board of Gannan Medical University, and the study was conducted in accordance with the ethical guidelines of the Declaration of Helsinki and relevant policies in China. Informed consent forms obtained from the participants stated that the questionnaire was to be completed anonymously and that the information provided was confidential.

Measurement

Risk Communication (RC)

RC, as a key link in the entire process of emergency response, is defined as the exchange of real-time information, advice, and opinions between experts and people facing threats to their health, economic, or social well-being (10). Social media has become the main method to ensure appropriate RC and plays a critical role in sharing and transmitting risk information in time and effectively (42). In this study, we used a self-designed scale based on the relevant literature (26), namely, a 5-point Likert scale, where 1 corresponded to “totally disagree” and 5 to “totally agree” for the following three questions: (RC01) “Do you think media reports affected your judgment of the COVID-19 pandemic?;” (RC02) “Do you believe media reports on COVID-19 are true?;” and (RC03) “Do you believe that government departments are timely and transparent in the release of pandemic information?”

Degree of Trust (DT)

Trust involves an overall positive expectation concerning the worthiness of words, promises, and statements of either another person (interpersonal trust) or an institution (social trust). Based on previous literature (43, 44), we considered that COVID-19-related trust could be effectively measured in terms of beliefs related to “competency – having technical proficiency.” Specifically, in our study, the degree of trust (DT) was measured as the perceived or expected trustworthiness of others (e.g., government, medical workers), which was assessed using a 5-point Likert scale where one corresponded to “very distrusting” and 5 to “very trusting” for the following 4 questions: (DT01) “Can the pandemic be effectively controlled?;” (DT02) “What is your DT in the pandemic control ability of medical workers?;” (DT03) “What is your DT in your self-protection capabilities?;” and (DT04) “What is your DT in the government's ability to prevent and control the pandemic?”

Risk Perception (RP)

Owing to different risk scenarios and research methods, the RP dimension has not been consistently defined; however, the controllability of risk is the most representative dimension employed in different fields, for example, the field of natural disasters and food safety (45). Slovic (4) claimed that people's RP of crisis events can be measured from the two dimensions of familiarity and controllability. In this study, we chose controllability as the RP measurement index, with five aspects of risk events investigated, namely, etiology, transmission, cure, preventive measures, and prognosis. RP was assessed using a 5-point Likert scale for the following five questions: “In terms of the controllability of the COVID-19 pandemic, what is your RP concerning its etiology (RP01), transmission (RP02), cure (RP03), preventive measures (RP04), and prognosis (RP05)?” (Responses ranging from: 1 “totally controllable” to 5 “totally uncontrollable”).

Negative Emotions (NE)

NE were assessed using a 5-point Likert scale (ranging from 1 “never” to 5 “always”) in relation to the following questions: “What was your worry (NE01), fear (NE02), and anxiety (NE03) frequency during the COVID-19 pandemic?”

Protective Coping Behavior (PCB)

PCB was measured using a 5-point Likert scale (ranging from 1 “never” to 5 “always”) in relation to the following questions: (PCB01) “Did you reduce contact with others during the pandemic?;” (PCB02) “Did you always wear a mask when you went out during the pandemic?;” (PCB03) “Did you try to avoid going to crowded places for activities during the pandemic?;” (PCB04) “Did you increase the amount of handwashing during the pandemic?;” (PCB05) “Did you open windows more often for ventilation during the pandemic?;” (PCB06) “Did you reduce the number of dinner parties held during the pandemic?;” (PCB07) “Did you increase exercise during the pandemic?;” (PCB08) “Were you more proactive in paying attention to and seeking health information during the pandemic?;” (PCB09) “Did you remind your family or friends to take measures to prevent and treat infection due to COVID-19 during the pandemic?;” and (PCB10) “Did you try to avoid contact with wild animals during the epidemic?”

Excessive Coping Behavior (ECB)

Based on social observations, ECB was assessed using three multiple choice questions, as follows: (ECB01) “Did you panic buy during the pandemic? If yes, please indicate whether any of the following items were involved: personal protective equipment, disinfectant, antiviral drugs, other directly pandemic-related purchases, or none of these;” (ECB02) “Did you hoard during the pandemic? If yes, please indicate whether any of the following items were involved: personal protective equipment, disinfectant, antiviral drugs, other directly pandemic-related purchases, or none of these;” (ECB03) “Were you influenced by a bandwagon effect led by rumor (herding behavior)? If yes, please indicate whether any of the following was involved: taking Shuanghuanglian (a drug with no preventive effect) to prevent COVID-19, sterilizing with white vinegar, taking other antiviral drugs while not been infected, other similar behavior, or none of these.” One point was assigned for each type of behavior, with points ranging from 0–4 points for each item.

Statistics

For statistical analysis, descriptive measures were performed using IBM SPSS (version 20.0, USA) software to summarize the principal results. Confirmatory factor analysis (CFA), assessment of normality, and SEM analyses were performed using SPSS AMOS (version 17.0, IBM Analytics, USA) software. Statistical significance was set at p < 0.05.

Compared with other methods [e.g., traditional multivariate statistical analysis (46), artificial neural network model (47)], SEM can construct hypothetical paths before data analysis, further perform CFA, and analyze the structural relationship between potential variables. This sample size of 735 was considered adequate for our SEM analyses, as the minimum satisfactory sample size recommended for performing a SEM procedure is 200 participants (48). For questionnaire validity and reliability, CFA testing, the index of composite reliability (CR) and average variance extracted (AVE), and Cronbach's alpha (α) were employed. The χ2/df, confirmatory factor index (CFI), expected cross validation index (ECVI), incremental fit index (IFI), normed fit index (NFI), parsimony comparative fit index (PCFI), root mean squared error of approximation (RMSEA) were adopted to determine whether the models fit the data (the evaluation Standard were showed in Table 4).

Results

Respondent Characteristics

The characteristics of the respondents are set out in Table 1. A total of 341 (46.4%) male participants and 394 (53.6%) female participants, mainly aged 18–24 years (72.5%), were included in this survey. The educational background of the respondents was mainly bachelor's degree or above (60.3%). Approximately 39.3% of the respondents had a medical background, and 61.9% of the respondents were students. Of the respondents, 30.1% had an annual income of <30,000 Yuan, and 39.5% had an income of 30,000–80,000 Yuan. Of the participants, 86.3% lived in rural areas during the COVID-19 pandemic, while the rest (13.7%) lived in urban areas.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Characteristics of respondents.

A wide range of sources of information and knowledge about COVID-19 was identified, mainly comprising the Internet in terms of news websites and forums (80.3%), followed by television news and newspapers (68.4%), health educators in hospitals or communities (43.3%), family or friends (36.9%), official agencies such as health administration departments and Centers for Disease Control and Prevention (26.1%), and others (5.0%). COVID-19 pandemic information media comprised official media, such as government documents and press conferences (70.7%); social networks, for example, Sina Weibo, Tencent WeChat, or QQ (69.4%); traditional media such as radio, television, and newspapers (55.8%); search engines, such as Baidu and Google (51.2%); and others (8.2%).

Confirmatory Factor Analysis (CFA)

We tested the affiliation between the measurement and latent variables using CFA. Some items were removed because of low loading values or cross loadings. Specifically, the items RC01, PCB07, PCB01, and ECB03 with standardized factor loadings <0.6 were deleted step-by-step. Items PCB03, PCB10, and PCB08 were deleted when a strong and significant correlation was found between sets of variables (PCB03 and PCB06, PCB10 and PCB06, PCB08, and PCB09) in terms of the latent variable PCB based on modification indices. The items in the final model are shown in Table 2, which includes 6 latent variables and 21 measurement variables.

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Structural validity and reliability.

The model fit of χ2/df = 2.427 ≤ 3, RMSEA = 0.044 ≤ 0.05, estimated after CFA, showed that the scale had adequate structural validity. The AVE values of each latent variable were all >0.5 (49), with CR ≥0.7 (46): RC (α = 0.771, CR = 0.774, AVE = 0.632); DT (α = 0.900, CR = 0.904, AVE = 0.703); RP (α = 0.902, CR = 0.902, AVE = 0.649); NE (α = 0.867, CR = 0.870, AVE = 0.692); PCB (α = 0.847, CR = 0.854, AVE = 0.543); and ECB (α = 0.800, CR = 0.807, AVE = 0.677), indicating that each variable had good convergent validity and adequate reliability (α ≥ 0.7) (50). As shown in Table 3, the inter-factor correlation coefficients were ≤0.60, and the square roots of the AVE values were higher than the correlation coefficients, indicating that the discriminant validity of the factors was verified. The results of the assessment of normality (Table 2) showed that all measurement variables conformed to a normal distribution when the absolute value of skew and kurtosis were ≤ the corresponding critical ratio, in accordance with the requirements of the maximum likelihood method. Hence, we computed the SEM adopting maximum likelihood estimation.

TABLE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. Discriminant validity.

Structural Equation Model (SEM) Testing

To test our hypothesis, we constructed a structural equation model (Figure 2) with RC and DT as the exogenous variables, and PP, EM, PCB, and ECB as the endogenous latent variables (χ2/df = 2.408, RMSEA = 0.044). Path H7 (RC-ECB) was deleted because the path coefficient was significantly small (β = 0.07, p > 0.05). The final SEM is shown in Figure 3, with χ2/df = 2.406 and RMSEA = 0.044 (Table 4), indicating that the final model had an adequate fit for the observable data.

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Model 1: Preliminary fit results of the theoretical model. *p < 0.05; **p < 0.01; ***p < 0.001. RC, risk communication; DT, degree of trust; RP, risk perception; NE, negative emotions; PCB, protective coping behavior; ECB, excessive coping behavior. The variables e1, e2,..., e25 represent the structure residuals.

FIGURE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 3. Model 2: The final structural equation model. *p < 0.05; **p < 0.01; ***p < 0.001. RC, risk communication; DT, degree of trust; RP, risk perception; NE, negative emotions; PCB, protective coping behavior; ECB, excessive coping behavior. The variables e1, e2,..., e25 represent the structure residuals.

TABLE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Table 4. Structural equation model fitting index.

After verifying and correcting the model, the paths were tested. Overall, our results (Table 5) were consistent with our hypothesis: (1) RC and DT were influencing factors of RP; the higher the degree of RC (β = −0.10, p < 0.05) or DT (β = −0.22, p < 0.001), the lower the public RP; (2) RC and DT had a direct effect on public behavioral coping strategies during the COVID-19 pandemic. The higher the level of RC (β = 0.14, p < 0.001) or DT (β = 0.48, p < 0.001), the more likely it was that the public were encouraged to adopt PCB, whereas the public were less likely to engage in ECB as DT increased during the COVID-19 pandemic (β = −0.12, p < 0.01). (3) RC and DT had an indirect effect on public behavioral coping strategies during the COVID-19 pandemic, and psychological factors may be an intermediary variable in relation to behavioral coping strategies stimulated by RC and DT. Specifically, RP as influenced by RC and DT could positively generate NE (β = 0.31, p < 0.001), which would further positively influence public behavioral coping strategies (whether protective [β = 0.09, p < 0.05] or excessive [β = 0.24, p < 0.001] behaviors). It was clear that NE had a stronger direct effect on ECB.

TABLE 5
www.frontiersin.org

Table 5. Standardized effects between variables (β).

Discussion

The results showed that the nine direct paths of the model hypothesis, except for the path between RC and ECB, reached significance. The results of the model analysis are discussed below according to the standardized path coefficients and load coefficients of each component.

Government regulation of the public's RP based on the current pandemic situation is a key part of emergency risk management. Our results showed that RC and DT had a direct negative effect on RP, indicating that those responsible for risk management need to use relevant media to convey appropriate guidance to inform public opinion; promote RC; and strengthen the public's DT in self-protection, medical workers, and the government, thereby more effectively regulating the public's RP. The spread of pandemic information can significantly affect the public's RP, and the media largely construct the public's perception of risk. In the early stages of prevention and control of the pandemic, people used various media to obtain relevant information and to understand the development of the pandemic and how society in general was responding. The results showed that the largest source of public information or knowledge about COVID-19 was the Internet (80.3%), which may be partly related to the increase in internet use during the pandemic. Research has shown that 46.8% of 6,416 Chinese people increased their dependence on the Internet and 16.6% had longer hours of internet use during the COVID-19 pandemic (51). However, as the public began to focus attention on the pandemic, media-related information became complex and extensive and often contained rumors mixed in with more factual reporting. The pandemic has accelerated an already-growing trend in users' interest in fake news sources (52). Therefore, it is necessary to disseminate scientific information in real time to respond to public health emergencies, and rapid sharing of scientific information is an effective way to reduce public panic about public health emergencies. The information media mainly comprised official media, such as government documents and press conferences (70.7%) and social networks (69.4%), indicating that emergency management authorities should pay attention to the information transmission function of official channels and new mass media. Research has shown that unofficial social media causes urban citizens to experience higher levels of panic than official media (53). The government's posting and media posting can significantly contribute to a more informed public perception of risk issues; therefore, the government and media must be vigilant in providing credible risk-related information (54).

Faced with a pandemic, PCB will substantially influence the effectiveness of risk control measures performed by the government (19, 20); thus, identifying the factors affecting public behavior is critical to emergency management. Our results showed that RC and DT had direct and indirect effects on public behavioral coping strategies during the COVID-19 epidemic. Our results were consistent with the previous literature (12, 13, 17). RC directly and positively affected the public's PCB; the higher the degree of RC, the more likely the public were to adopt PCB. However, the results did not show that RC had a direct effect on ECB, indicating that PCB and ECB undertaken by the public were not mutually exclusive as both could be engaged in simultaneously.

The results showed that DT in self-protection capabilities, medical workers, and the government's ability in relation to effective control of the pandemic had a direct positive effect on PCB and a negative effect on ECB, suggesting that building social trust is a key measure for promoting effective public awareness in terms of epidemic prevention.

Public behavior was also driven by intrinsic psychological factors (34). An online survey in the Italian population indicated that the COVID-19 pandemic may be an influential risk factor for the development of psychological diseases (35). The COVID-19 pandemic could be considered as a traumatic event (37), which had clearly affected the mental health of the public, particularly closely linked to negative emotions (39), sleep disorder (36), and PTSD (37). However, previous studies have mostly focused on the effect of influencing factors, such as RC and trust on public behavior, the psychological mechanisms and relationships involved have rarely been discussed. In our study, we not only focused on the direct effects of influencing factors on public behavior, but also on the indirect effects of psychological factors. On the one hand, external influencing factors, such as RC and trust, could influence public psychological factors, such as public RP and emotion, which in turn could affect public behavior. However, the indirect effect appears to be relatively small. On the other hand, RP can positively affect public behavior, possibly through the mediating effect of NE. Interestingly, with the increase in RP, NE also grew, which had the effect of not only promoting public PCB but also of promoting ECB, indicating that in risk control, attention should be paid to public emotional stability and to avoiding too high or too low RP.

In conclusion, our hypothesis was verified. We used an SEM to evaluate the internal potential relationships between influencing factors and coping behaviors of the public facing COVID-19 risk. Compared with conventional statistical methods, the analysis results of the SEM were likely to be more comprehensive as the theoretical model constructed in this study had an excellent fit, as indicated by the various fit indices. The results showed that RC, DT, RP, and NE were significantly directly or indirectly related to public behavior. The reported findings provide useful information for emergency risk management and a theoretical basis for follow-up research on public coping behavior during a pandemic. Furthermore, the research results on the relevant formation mechanisms involved and the relationship between factors and behaviors are likely to be useful in guiding subsequent research.

However, this study had several limitations. First, due to the pandemic, the questionnaire was applied using convenience sampling on the Internet so that only certain types of respondents may have become involved. Specifically, the respondents were all internet users, were often relatively young, and could not be considered representative of the entire population. However, considering the high internet penetration rate in China (71.6%) and that the proportion of mobile internet users was reported to be 99.6% (as of June 2021) (55), we consider that this limitation is unlikely to have led to excessive distortion. Second, our theory (Figure 3) is verified by investigation only in China and limited in scope due to its focus on mainly young people. However, considering that the basis of our hypothesis come from the investigation and research of different countries as seen in Introduction (5, 6, 15, 25, 35), our study could be extended to accommodate a wider range of applications and provides more opportunities for further investigation. Third, although our questionnaire was self-designed based on the latest relevant literature, it inevitably contained some biases. In this study, we explored a public behavior model from the perspective of psychological factors only. Further studies should be conducted to improve the current questionnaire design to better capture other potential factors. For example, according to the relevant literature (4, 35, 36), we could speculate that factors like sleep quality may have the mediating effect in psychological behavior model during COVID-19 pandemic. Besides, additional considerations could be included, such as personal characteristics, health literacy, social environment, and other aspects (57), in ongoing model research for the public behavior model designed further optimized in future.

Data Availability Statement

The original contributions presented in the study are included in the article/supplementary material, further inquiries can be directed to the corresponding author/s.

Ethics Statement

The studies involving human participants were reviewed and approved by Gannan Medical University. Written informed consent to participate in this study was provided by the participants' legal guardian/next of kin.

Author Contributions

JG and RH conceived this study and designed the questionnaire. JG performed data analysis, interpreted the results, and wrote the paper. RH, XW, JT, and WY collected the relevant data. CW contributed to the interpretation and revision of the paper. All authors have read and approved the final manuscript.

Funding

This work was supported by Humanities and Social Sciences Project of Jiangxi Colleges and Universities (No. JC21204) and the Social Science project of Ganzhou (No. 2021-018-0008).

Conflict of Interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher's Note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

Acknowledgments

We would like to thank Editage (www.editage.com) for English language editing.

References

1. WHO. Coronavirus disease (COVID-19) Weekly Epidemiological Update and Weekly Operational Update (2021). Available online at: https://www.who.int/emergencies/diseases/novel-coronavirus-2019/situation-reports (accessed November 16, 2021).

2. Cao Y, Shan J, Gong Z, Kuang J, Gao Y. Status and challenges of public health emergency management in China Related to COVID-19. Front Public Health. (2020) 8:250. doi: 10.3389/fpubh.2020.00250

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

3. Duan T, Sun Z, Shi G. Sustained Effects of Government Response on the COVID-19 infection rate in china: a multiple mediation analysis. Int J Environ Res Public Health. (2021) 18:2422. doi: 10.3390/ijerph182312422

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

4. Slovic P. Perception of risk. Science. (1987) 236:280–5. doi: 10.1126/science.3563507

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

5. Lu P, Kong D, Shelley M. Risk perception, preventive behavior, and medical care avoidance among american older adults during the COVID-19 PANDEMIC. J Aging Health. (2021) 33:577–84. doi: 10.1177/08982643211002084

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

6. James K, Thompson C, Chin-Bailey C, Davis KD, Nevins DH, Walters D. COVID-19 related risk perception among taxi operators in Kingston and St. Andrew, Jamaica J Transp Health. (2021) 22:101229. doi: 10.1016/j.jth.2021.101229

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

7. Rana IA, Bhatti SS, Aslam AB, Jamshed A, Ahmad J, Shah AA. COVID-19 risk perception and coping mechanisms: does gender make a difference? Int J Disaster Risk Reduct. (2021) 55:102096. doi: 10.1016/j.ijdrr.2021.102096

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

8. Omura T, Araki A, Shigemoto K, Toba K. Geriatric practice during and after the COVID-19 pandemic. Geriatr Gerontol Int. (2020) 20:735–7. doi: 10.1111/ggi.13958

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

9. WHO. Risk communication and community engagement readiness and response to coronavirus disease (COVID-19): interim guidance, 19 March 2020 (2020). Available online at: https://www.who.int/publications/i/item/risk-communication-and-community-engagement-readiness-and-initial-response-for-novel-coronaviruses (accessed November 10, 2021).

10. WHO. Communicating Risk in Public Health Emergencies: a WHO Guideline for Emergency Risk Communication (ERC) Policy and Practice. Geneva: World Health Organization (2017).

11. Heydari ST, Zarei L, Sadati AK, Moradi N, Akbari M, Mehralian G, et al. The effect of risk communication on preventive and protective behaviours during the COVID-19 outbreak: mediating role of risk perception. BMC Public Health. (2021) 21:54. doi: 10.1186/s12889-020-10125-5

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

12. Siddiqui S, Qamar AH. Risk perception and protective behavior in the context of COVID-19: a qualitative exploration. Asian Bioeth Rev. (2021) 13:1–20. doi: 10.1007/s41649-021-00181-3

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

13. Ju Y, You M. It's Politics, Isn't It? Investigating direct and indirect influences of political orientation on risk perception of COVID-19. Risk Anal. (2022) 42:56–68. doi: 10.1111/risa.13801

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

14. Han X, Wang J, Zhang M, Wang X. Using social media to mine and analyze public opinion related to COVID-19 in China. Int J Environ Res Public Health. (2020) 17:2788. doi: 10.3390/ijerph17082788

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

15. Zeballos Rivas DR, Lopez Jaldin ML, Nina Canaviri B, Portugal Escalante LF, Alanes Fernandez AMC, Aguilar Ticona JP. Social media exposure, risk perception, preventive behaviors and attitudes during the COVID-19 epidemic in La Paz, Bolivia: a cross sectional study. PLoS ONE. (2021) 16:e0245859. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0245859

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

16. Fu L, Wang X, Deng S, Zhang Q, Liu Y. Informational support, risk perception, anti-pandemic motivation and behavior: a longitudinal study in China. Curr Psychol. (2021) 1–15. doi: 10.1007/s12144-021-02071-1. [Epub ahead of print].

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

17. Scandurra C, Bochicchio V, Dolce P, Valerio P, Muzii B, Maldonato NM. Why people were less compliant with public health regulations during the second wave of the Covid-19 outbreak: the role of trust in governmental organizations, future anxiety, fatigue, and Covid-19 risk perception. Curr Psychol. (2021) 1–11. doi: 10.1007/s12144-021-02059-x. [Epub ahead of print].

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

18. Siegrist M, Luchsinger L, Bearth A. The impact of trust and risk perception on the acceptance of measures to reduce COVID-19 cases. Risk Anal. (2021) 41:787–800. doi: 10.1111/risa.13675

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

19. Sun CX, He B, Mu D, Li PL, Zhao HT Li ZL, et al. Public awareness and mask usage during the COVID-19 epidemic: a survey by China CDC new media. Biomed Environ Sci. (2020) 33:639–45. doi: 10.3967/bes2020.085

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

20. Al-Bsheish M, Jarrar M, Scarbrough A. A Public safety compliance model of safety behaviors in the age of the COVID-19 pandemic. Inquiry. (2021) 58:469580211031382. doi: 10.1177/00469580211031382

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

21. Roshanshad R, Roshanshad A, Molavi Vardanjani H, Mashhadiagha A, Mobarakabadi M, Hoveidaei A, et al. Risk perception, attitude, and practice related to COVID-19: a cross-sectional study among 1,085 Iranian healthcare workers. Ann Med Surg. (2021) 70:102865. doi: 10.1016/j.amsu.2021.102865

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

22. Yang XY, Gong RN, Sassine S, Morsa M, Tchogna AS, Drouin O, et al. Risk perception of COVID-19 infection and adherence to preventive measures among adolescents and young adults. Children. (2020) 7:311. doi: 10.3390/children7120311

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

23. Tsai FJ, Yang HW, Lin CP, Liu JZ. Acceptability of COVID-19 Vaccines and protective behavior among adults in Taiwan: associations between risk perception and willingness to vaccinate against COVID-19. Int J Environ Res Public Health. (2021) 18:5579. doi: 10.3390/ijerph18115579

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

24. Krewski D, Slovic P, Bartlett S, Flynn J, Mertz CK. Health risk perception in Canada I: Rating hazards, sources of information and responsibility for health protection. Human Ecol Risk Assess: Int J. (1995) 1:117–32. doi: 10.1080/10807039509379997

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

25. Wang X, Lin L, Xuan Z, Xu J, Wan Y, Zhou X. Risk communication on behavioral responses during COVID-19 among general population in China: a rapid national study. J Infect. (2020) 81:911–22. doi: 10.1016/j.jinf.2020.10.031

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

26. Abrams EM, Greenhawt M. Risk communication during COVID-19. J Allergy Clin Immunol Pract. (2020) 8:1791–4. doi: 10.1016/j.jaip.2020.04.012

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

27. Li JB, Zhang R, Wang LX, Dou K. Chinese public's panic buying at the beginning of COVID-19 outbreak: the contribution of perceived risk, social media use, and connection with close others. Curr Psychol. (2021) 1-10. doi: 10.1007/s12144-021-02072-0. [Epub ahead of print].

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

28. Dai B, Fu D, Meng G, Liu B, Li Q, Liu X. The effects of governmental and individual predictors on COVID-19 protective behaviors in China: a path analysis model. Public Adm Rev. (2020) 80:797–804. doi: 10.1111/puar.13236

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

29. Lee YC, Wu WL, Lee CK. How COVID-19 triggers our herding behavior? Risk perception, state anxiety, and trust. Front Public Health. (2021) 9:587439. doi: 10.3389/fpubh.2021.587439

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

30. Rui JR, Yang K, Chen J. Information sources, risk perception, and efficacy appraisal's prediction of engagement in protective behaviors against COVID-19 in China: repeated cross-sectional survey. JMIR Hum Factors. (2021) 8:e23232. doi: 10.2196/23232

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

31. Xie K, Liang B, Dulebenets MA, Mei Y. The impact of risk perception on social distancing during the COVID-19 pandemic in China. Int J Environ Res Public Health. (2020) 17:6256. doi: 10.3390/ijerph17176256

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

32. Trifiletti E, Shamloo SE, Faccini M, Zaka A. Psychological predictors of protective behaviours during the Covid-19 pandemic: theory of planned behaviour and risk perception. J Community Appl Soc Psychol. (2021). doi: 10.1002/casp.2509. [Epub ahead of print].

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

33. Barrett C, Cheung KL. Knowledge, socio-cognitive perceptions and the practice of hand hygiene and social distancing during the COVID-19 pandemic: a cross-sectional study of UK university students. BMC Public Health. (2021) 21:426. doi: 10.1186/s12889-021-10461-0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

34. Qiu J, Shen B, Zhao M, Wang Z, Xie B, Xu Y, et al. nationwide survey of psychological distress among Chinese people in the COVID-19 epidemic: implications and policy recommendations. Gen Psychiatr. (2020) 33:e100213. doi: 10.1136/gpsych-2020-100213

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

35. Favieri F, Forte G, Tambelli R, Casagrande M. The Italians in the time of coronavirus: psychosocial aspects of the unexpected COVID-19 pandemic. Front Psychiatry. (2021) 12:551924. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2021.551924

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

36. Casagrande M, Forte G, Tambelli R, Favieri F. The coronavirus pandemic: a possible model of the direct and indirect impact of the pandemic on sleep quality in Italians. Nat Sci Sleep. (2021) 13:191–9. doi: 10.2147/NSS.S285854

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

37. Forte G, Favieri F, Tambelli R, Casagrande M. COVID-19 pandemic in the Italian population: validation of a post-traumatic stress disorder questionnaire and prevalence of PTSD symptomatology. Int J Environ Res Public Health. (2020) 17:4151. doi: 10.3390/ijerph17114151

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

38. Zhong Y, Liu W, Lee TY, Zhao H, Ji J. Risk perception, knowledge, information sources and emotional states among COVID-19 patients in Wuhan, China. Nurs Outlook. (2021) 69:13–21. doi: 10.1016/j.outlook.2020.08.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

39. Han Q, Zheng B, Agostini M, Belanger JJ, Gutzkow B, Kreienkamp J, et al. Associations of risk perception of COVID-19 with emotion and mental health during the pandemic. J Affect Disord. (2021) 284:247–55. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2021.01.049

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

40. Qianlan Y, Ying L, Aibin C, Xiangrui S, Wenpeng C, Guanghui D, et al. Risk perception and emotion reaction of Chinese health care workers varied during COVID-19: a repeated cross-sectional research. Int J Public Health. (2021) 66:613057. doi: 10.3389/ijph.2021.613057

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

41. Liu M, Zhang H, Huang H. Media exposure to COVID-19 information, risk perception, social and geographical proximity, and self-rated anxiety in China. BMC Public Health. (2020) 20:1649. doi: 10.1186/s12889-020-09761-8

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

42. Slovic P. The Perception of Risk. London: Earthscan Publications (2000).

43. Meredith LS, Eisenman DP, Rhodes H, Ryan G, Long A. Trust influences response to public health messages during a bioterrorist event. J Health Commun. (2007) 12:217–32. doi: 10.1080/10810730701265978

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

44. Eisenman DP, Williams MV, Glik D, Long A, Plough AL, Ong M. The public health disaster trust scale: validation of a brief measure. J Public Health Manag Pract. (2012) 18:E11–8. doi: 10.1097/PHH.0b013e31823991e8

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

45. Ueland O, Gunnlaugsdottir H, Holm F, Kalogeras N, Leino O, Luteijn JM, et al. State of the art in benefit-risk analysis: consumer perception. Food Chem Toxicol. (2012) 50:67–76. doi: 10.1016/j.fct.2011.06.006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

46. Hair J, Anderson R, Tatham R, Black W. Multivariate Data Analysis, 5th Ed. Upper Saddle River, NJ: Prentice-Hall (1998).

47. Dhiman G, Kumar V. Spotted hyena optimizer: a novel bio-inspired based metaheuristic technique for engineering applications. Adv Eng Software. (2017) 114:48–70. doi: 10.1016/j.advengsoft.2017.05.014

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

48. Marsh HW, Hau KT, Balla JR, Grayson D. Is more ever too much? The number of indicators per factor in confirmatory factor analysis multivariate. Behav Res. (1998) 33:181–220. doi: 10.1207/s15327906mbr3302_1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

49. Bagozzi RP, Yi Y. On the evaluation of structural equation models. J Aca Mark Sci. (1988) 16:74–94. doi: 10.1007/bf02723327

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

50. Nunnally J, Bernstein I. Psychometric Theory. Vol 5. (1994).

51. Sun Y, Li Y, Bao Y, Meng S, Sun Y, Schumann G, et al. brief report: increased addictive internet and substance use behavior during the COVID-19 pandemic in China. Am J Addict. (2020) 29:268–70. doi: 10.1111/ajad.13066

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

52. Rovetta A. The impact of COVID-19 on conspiracy hypotheses and risk perception in italy: infodemiological survey study using google trends. JMIR Infodemiol. (2021) 1:e29929. doi: 10.2196/29929

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

53. Wu G, Deng X, Liu B. Managing urban citizens' panic levels and preventive behaviours during COVID-19 with pandemic information released by social media. Cities. (2022) 120:103490. doi: 10.1016/j.cities.2021.103490

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

54. Zhu H, Liu K. Capturing the interplay between risk perception and social media posting to support risk response and decision making. Int J Environ Res Public Health. (2021) 18:5220. doi: 10.3390/ijerph18105220

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

55. CNNIC. The 48th Statistical Report on China's Internet Development (2021). Available online at: http://www.cnnic.cn/hlwfzyj/hlwxzbg/hlwtjbg/202109/t20210915_71543.htm (accessed December 22, 2021).

Keywords: emergency risk management, influencing factors, risk perception, negative emotions, behavioral coping strategies, structural equation model

Citation: Gu J, He R, Wu X, Tao J, Ye W and Wu C (2022) Analyzing Risk Communication, Trust, Risk Perception, Negative Emotions, and Behavioral Coping Strategies During the COVID-19 Pandemic in China Using a Structural Equation Model. Front. Public Health 10:843787. doi: 10.3389/fpubh.2022.843787

Received: 26 December 2021; Accepted: 25 April 2022;
Published: 30 May 2022.

Edited by:

Albert Prats-Uribe, University of Oxford, United Kingdom

Reviewed by:

Maria Casagrande, Sapienza University of Rome, Italy
Gaurav Dhiman, Government Bikram College of Commerce Patiala (Punjab), India

Copyright © 2022 Gu, He, Wu, Tao, Ye and Wu. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Junwang Gu, Z2pfb25lJiN4MDAwNDA7Z211LmVkdS5jbg==

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.